How Does the Brain Learn Environmental Structure? Ten Core Principles for Understanding the Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Statistical Learning T

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

How Does the Brain Learn Environmental Structure? Ten Core Principles for Understanding the Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Statistical Learning T Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 112 (2020) 279–299 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neubiorev How does the brain learn environmental structure? Ten core principles for understanding the neurocognitive mechanisms of statistical learning T Christopher M. Conway* Center for Childhood Deafness, Language, and Learning, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Omaha, NE, United States ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Keywords: Despite a growing body of research devoted to the study of how humans encode environmental patterns, there is Statistical learning still no clear consensus about the nature of the neurocognitive mechanisms underpinning statistical learning nor Implicit learning what factors constrain or promote its emergence across individuals, species, and learning situations. Based on a Sequential learning review of research examining the roles of input modality and domain, input structure and complexity, attention, Artificial grammar learning neuroanatomical bases, ontogeny, and phylogeny, ten core principles are proposed. Specifically, there exist two sets of neurocognitive mechanisms underlying statistical learning. First, a “suite” of associative-based, auto- matic, modality-specific learning mechanisms are mediated by the general principle of cortical plasticity, which results in improved processing and perceptual facilitation of encountered stimuli. Second, an attention-depen- dent system, mediated by the prefrontal cortex and related attentional and working memory networks, can modulate or gate learning and is necessary in order to learn nonadjacent dependencies and to integrate global patterns across time. This theoretical framework helps clarify conflicting research findings and provides the basis for future empirical and theoretical endeavors. 1. Introduction across different perceptual modalities (e.g., vision vs. audition) or domains (e.g., language vs. music)? Does the learning of patterns in Many events in our daily existence occur not completely randomly one modality or domain involve the same neurocognitive mechan- or haphazardly, but with a certain amount of structure, regularity, and isms as learning in a different modality or domain? predictability. Because of the ubiquitous presence of structured patterns 2) Input structure and complexity: What mechanisms underpin the in human action, perception, and cognition, the ability to process and learning of different types of input patterns, such as associations represent these patterns is of paramount importance. This type of between adjacent or co-occurring elements to more complex structured pattern learning – which is likely a crucial foundational “global” patterns that require integration of information over longer ability of all higher-level organisms, and possibly of many lower-level time-scales? ones as well – has been studied under the guise of different terms for 3) Role of attention. To what extent are attention and related cognitive what arguably tap into aspects of the same underlying construct, in- processes necessary for statistical learning to occur? In turn, does cluding “implicit learning” (A.S. Reber, 1967), “sequence learning” the outcome of learning modulate attention? (Nissen and Bullemer, 1987), “sequential learning” (Conway and 4) Neural bases. What is the underlying neuroanatomy of statistical Christiansen, 2001), and “statistical learning” (Saffran et al., 1996). learning? Is there a single, common learning and processing net- Despite gains made in understanding how humans and other or- work? Or are there different sets of regions or networks that are used ganisms learn patterned input, we are still far from an understanding of for different types of learning situations? the neurocognitive mechanisms underlying learning and what factors 5) Ontogenetic constraints. How does statistical learning emerge and constrain its emergence across individuals, species, and learning si- change across the lifespan? Do different aspects of learning have tuations. What is needed is an integration of research findings across six different developmental trajectories? key areas that have generally been treated in isolation: 6) Phylogenetic constraints. Which aspects of statistical learning are shared versus unique across different animal species? What drives 1) Input modality and domain: How does learning proceed for inputs variation or differences across species? ⁎ Corresponding author at: Brain, Learning, and Language Lab, Center for Childhood Deafness, Language, and Learning, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Lied Learning and Technology Center, 555N. 30th St., Omaha, NE 68131, United States. E-mail address: [email protected]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.01.032 Received 8 August 2019; Received in revised form 22 January 2020; Accepted 25 January 2020 Available online 01 February 2020 0149-7634/ © 2020 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. C.M. Conway Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 112 (2020) 279–299 Although a number of theoretical perspectives exist (e.g., Arciuli, Of course, even the early research on implicit learning did not occur 2017; Aslin and Newport, 2012; Daltrozzo and Conway, 2014; in a vacuum. Behaviorist approaches to associative learning and con- Forkstam and Petersson, 2005; Frost et al., 2015; Janacsek and Nemeth, ditioning provided an important historical context for the implicit 2012; Keele et al., 2003; Perruchet and Pacton, 2006; Pothos, 2007; P.J. learning work (e.g., see Pearce and Bouton, 2001; Rescorla and Wagner, Reber, 2013; A.S. Reber, 2003; Savalia et al., 2016; Seger, 1994; 1972). Gureckis and Love (2007) in fact argued that much of what the Thiessen and Erickson, 2013), currently none of them sufficiently ad- field has been studying under the guise of statistical learning is embo- dress all of these questions. In this paper, we begin by defining in more died by behaviorist principles of conditioning and associative learning detail what is meant by “statistical learning” and how it relates to other (c.f., Goddard, 2018). Additional neurophysiological antecedents of similarly-used terms. A lack of clarity and consensus in regards to ter- statistical learning include Hebb’s principles of learning and plasticity minology has proven to be a barrier for integrating findings across (i.e., the “Hebbian learning rule”; see Cooper, 2005; Hebb, 1949) and different research areas; furthermore, the use of certain terms denotes the demonstration that the development of primary visual cortex de- premature assumptions about what the underlying mechanisms are that pends on environmental experience (e.g., Blakemore and Cooper, characterize the construct of interest. Following this discussion, we 1970). While acknowledging these important precursors, this review provide a selective review and synthesis of research related to the six focuses primarily on the findings from the implicit learning and sta- areas described above. Then, based on this review, we outline ten core tistical learning literatures per se. principles that arise from an integration of the reviewed research and For simplicity the term “statistical learning” is used in the re- provide the beginnings of what could be construed as a unified theory mainder of this paper to refer to incidental learning of structured pat- of statistical learning. To preview, we propose there exist two primary terns encountered in the environment. To constrain and operationalize sets of neurocognitive mechanisms – one based on the general principle the definition of statistical learning and provide added focus to this of cortical plasticity and the other a specialized neural system that can review, we delineate the task or situational characteristics of interest. provide top-down modulation of learning – with each affected and Specifically, we propose three orthogonal dimensions that can help constrained by different factors in different ways. Only by taking into clarify the construct of statistical learning. These dimensions include: account the operation of these two mechanisms will we understand the the level of structure present in input (i.e., random versus heavily neurocognitive bases of statistical learning and how they are con- structured sequences); the amount of exposure that is involved (i.e., a strained by factors such as input modality, complexity, ontogeny, and single exposure versus multiple instances); and the extent to which task phylogeny. situations provide explicit instruction or overt feedback (i.e., incidental versus intentional learning situations). These three dimensions are de- 2. Preliminary considerations picted graphically in Fig. 1. They create a “task space” containing a continuum of distributed points in which tasks (or situations) that are Statistical learning research began in earnest with the seminal study closer to the zero-point (0,0,0) can be thought of as being more char- by Saffran et al. (1996), who showed that 8-month-old infants were acteristic of statistical learning compared to tasks at the periphery (note sensitive to the statistical structure inherent in a short (2-minute) au- though, that technically speaking there is no actual zero-point as there ditory nonword speech stream. In this study, statistical structure was could always be a situation with more exposures, or more structure, etc. operationalized as the strength of transitional probabilities between and in that case the zero-point might be more appropriately regarded as adjacent syllables (i.e., the
Recommended publications
  • Neuronal Activity in Monkey Ventral Striatum Related to the Expectation of Reward
    The Journal of Neuroscience. December 1992. 72(12): 45954610 Neuronal Activity in Monkey Ventral Striatum Related to the Expectation of Reward Wolfram Schultz, Paul Apicella,” Eugenio Scarnati,b and Tomas Ljungbergc lnstitut de Physiologie, Universitk de Fribourg, CH-1700 Fribourg, Switzerland Projections from cortical and subcortical limbic structures The anatomical substrate underlying these functions may con- to the basal ganglia are predominantly directed to the ventral sist in the conjunction of afferents from limbic structures and striatum. The present study investigated how the expecta- mesencephalicdopamine neurons. Major limbic structures in tion of external events with behavioral significance is re- monkeys, such asthe anterior cingulate gyms, orbitofrontal cor- flected in the activity of ventral striatal neurons. A total of tex, and amygdala, project to the ventral striatum, including the 420 neurons were studied in macaque monkeys performing nucleus accumbens,in a particularly dense and interdigitating in a delayed go-no-go task. Lights of different colors in- fashion, whereas their projections to the dorsal striatum are structed the animal to do an arm-reaching movement or re- more sparseand scattered(Baleydier and Mauguiere, 1980; Par- frain from moving, respectively, when a trigger light was ent et al., 1983; Russchenet al., 1985; Selemonand Goldman- illuminated a few seconds later. Task performance was re- Rakic, 1985). The amygdala is involved in the association of inforced by liquid reward in both situations. A total of 60 external stimuli with primary and secondaryreinforcers for sus- ventral striatal neurons showed sustained increases of ac- taining performance in learning tasks (Gaffan and Harrison, tivity before the occurrence of individual task events.
    [Show full text]
  • Audiovisual Integration of Letters in the Human Brain
    Neuron, Vol. 28, 617±625, November, 2000, Copyright 2000 by Cell Press Audiovisual Integration of Letters in the Human Brain Tommi Raij,* Kimmo Uutela, and Riitta Hari dalities to allow them then to interact. Therefore, brain Brain Research Unit areas participating in, e.g., audiovisual integration would Low Temperature Laboratory be expected to show signs of (1) convergence (both Helsinki University of Technology auditory and visual stimuli should activate the same P.O. Box 2200 region) and (2) interaction (the activation evoked by au- FIN-02015-HUT diovisual stimulation should differ from the sum of uni- Espoo modally presented auditory and visual activations). Finland Our aim was to study the human brain's audiovisual integration mechanisms for letters, i.e., for stimuli that have been previously associated through learning. For Summary literate people, the alphabet is effortlessly transformed between the auditory and visual domains (and transmit- Letters of the alphabet have auditory (phonemic) and ted to the motor systems for speech and writing). Our visual (graphemic) qualities. To investigate the neural subjects received auditory, visual, and audiovisual let- representations of such audiovisual objects, we re- ters of the roman alphabet and were required to identify corded neuromagnetic cortical responses to audi- them, regardless of stimulus modality. Audiovisual let- torily, visually, and audiovisually presented single let- ters included matching letters, in which the auditory ters. The auditory and visual brain activations first and visual stimulus corresponded to each other based converged around 225 ms after stimulus onset and on previous experience, and nonmatching (randomly then interacted predominantly in the right temporo- paired) letters.
    [Show full text]
  • Boosting Learning Efficacy with Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation in Intact and Brain-Damaged Humans
    This Accepted Manuscript has not been copyedited and formatted. The final version may differ from this version. Research Articles: Behavioral/Cognitive Boosting learning efficacy with non-invasive brain stimulation in intact and brain-damaged humans F. Herpich1,2,3, F. Melnick, M.D.4 , S. Agosta1 , K.R. Huxlin4 , D. Tadin4 and L Battelli1,5,6 1Center for Neuroscience and Cognitive Systems@UniTn, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Corso Bettini 31, 38068 Rovereto (TN), Italy 2Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, 38068 Rovereto, Italy 3kbo Klinikum-Inn-Salzach, Gabersee 7, 83512, Wasserburg am Inn, Germany 4Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Flaum Eye Institute and Center for Visual Science, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA 5Berenson-Allen Center for Noninvasive Brain Stimulation and Department of Neurology, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Harvard Medical School, Boston, 02215 Massachusetts, USA 6Cognitive Neuropsychology Laboratory, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3248-18.2019 Received: 28 December 2018 Revised: 10 April 2019 Accepted: 8 May 2019 Published: 27 May 2019 Author contributions: F.H., M.D.M., K.H., D.T., and L.B. designed research; F.H., M.D.M., and S.A. performed research; F.H., M.D.M., and D.T. analyzed data; F.H., M.D.M., and L.B. wrote the first draft of the paper; M.D.M., S.A., K.H., D.T., and L.B. edited the paper; K.H., D.T., and L.B. wrote the paper. Conflict of Interest: The authors declare no competing financial interests. The present study was funded by the Autonomous Province of Trento, Call “Grandi Progetti 2012”, project “Characterizing and improving brain mechanisms of attention — ATTEND (FH, SA, LB), “Fondazione Caritro — Bando Ricerca e Sviluppo Economico” (FH), NIH (DT, MM and KRH: R01 grants EY027314 and EY021209, CVS training grant T32 EY007125), and by an unrestricted grant from the Research to Prevent Blindness (RPB) Foundation to the Flaum Eye Institute.
    [Show full text]
  • AN ANALYSIS of RIGHT- and LEFT-BRAIN THINKERS and CERTAIN STYLES of LEARNING by Steven D. Bielefeldt a Research Paper Submitted
    AN ANALYSIS OF RIGHT- AND LEFT-BRAIN THINKERS AND CERTAIN STYLES OF LEARNING BY Steven D. Bielefeldt A Research Paper Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Master of Science Degree Education Approved: 2 Semester Credits Ax I Dr. Robert Sedlak The Graduate School University of Wisconsin -Stout May 2006 The Graduate School University of Wisconsin -Stout Menomonie, WI Author: Bielefeldt, Steven D. Title: An Analysis of Right- and Left-Brain Thinkers and Certain Styles of Learning Graduate Degree I Major: MS Education Research Advisor: Robert Sedlak, Ph.D. Month I Year: May, 2006 Number of Pages: 29 Style Manual Used: American Psychological Association, sth edition ABSTRACT The purpose of this study was to analyze right- and left- brain thinkers and certain styles of learning (specifically visual, auditory, or kinesthetic) in college- level adult learners. This study includes data collected from approximately 100 adult learners with a survey, as well as a comprehensive review and analysis of literature concerning the brain, brain-based learning, and certain learning styles. Research-based evidence from the study will be used to ellcourage instructors to develop and use appropriate styles of teaching to enhance their student's educational experience. Table of Contents ABSTRACT ........................................................................................................... ii List of Tables ......................................................................................................... V Chapter I:
    [Show full text]
  • Neural Plasticity Underlying Visual Perceptual Learning in Aging
    brain research ] ( ]]]]) ]]]– ]]] Available online at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Neural plasticity underlying visual perceptual learning in aging Jyoti Mishraa,n, Camarin Rollea, Adam Gazzaleya,b,c,nn aDepartment of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA bDepartment of Physiology, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA cDepartment of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA article info abstract Article history: Healthy aging is associated with a decline in basic perceptual abilities, as well as higher-level Accepted 2 September 2014 cognitive functions such as working memory. In a recent perceptual training study using moving sweeps of Gabor stimuli, Berry et al. (2010) observed that older adults significantly Keywords: improved discrimination abilities on the most challenging perceptual tasks that presented Aging paired sweeps at rapid rates of 5 and 10 Hz. Berry et al. further showed that this perceptual fi Visual perception training engendered transfer-of-bene t to an untrained working memory task. Here, we Cognitive training investigated the neural underpinnings of the improvements in these perceptual tasks, as Perceptual learning assessed by event-related potential (ERP) recordings. Early visual ERP components time- Working memory locked to stimulus onset were compared pre- and post-training, as well as relative to a no- fi Transfer of benefit contact control group. The visual N1 and N2 components were signi cantly enhanced after ERP training, and the N1 change correlated with improvements in perceptual discrimination on the task. Further, the change observed for the N1 and N2 was associated with the rapidity of the perceptual challenge; the visual N1 (120–150 ms) was enhanced post-training for 10 Hz sweep pairs, while the N2 (240–280 ms) was enhanced for the 5 Hz sweep pairs.
    [Show full text]
  • The Visual Corticostriatal Loop Through the Tail of the Caudate: Circuitry and Function
    HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY ARTICLE published: 06 December 2013 SYSTEMS NEUROSCIENCE doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00104 The visual corticostriatal loop through the tail of the caudate: circuitry and function CarolA.Seger* Program in Molecular, Cellular, and Integrative Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, CO, USA Edited by: Although high level visual cortex projects to a specific region of the striatum, the tail of the Ahmed A. Moustafa, University of caudate, and participates in corticostriatal loops, the function of this visual corticostriatal Western Sydney, Australia system is not well understood. This article first reviews what is known about the anatomy Reviewed by: of the visual corticostriatal loop across mammals, including rodents, cats, monkeys, and Erick J. Paul, University of Illinois Urbana Champaign, USA humans. Like other corticostriatal systems, the visual corticostriatal system includes Greg Ashby, University of California, both closed loop components (recurrent projections that return to the originating cortical Santa Barbara, USA location) and open loop components (projections that terminate in other neural regions). *Correspondence: The article then reviews what previous empirical research has shown about the function Carol A. Seger, Program in of the tail of the caudate. The article finally addresses the possible functions of the closed Molecular, Cellular, and Integrative Neuroscience, Department of and open loop connections of the visual loop in the context of theories and computational Psychology, Colorado State models of corticostriatal function. University, Mail code 1876, Fort Collins, CO 80523, USA Keywords: striatum, caudate, category learning, basal ganglia, corticostriatal, recurrent neural network, e-mail: [email protected] reinforcement learning, Area TE INTRODUCTION forming both open and closed loops.
    [Show full text]
  • Latinos: HCHS/SOL Results Neil Schneiderman10 Diabetes Care 2018;41:1501–1509 |
    Diabetes Care Volume 41, July 2018 1501 Hector M. Gonzalez,´ 1 Wassim Tarraf,2 Metabolic Syndrome and Priscilla Vasquez,´ 1 Ashley H. Sanderlin,3 Natalya I. Rosenberg,4 Sonia Davis,5 Neurocognition Among Diverse Carlos J. Rodr´ıguez,6 Linda C. Gallo,7 Bharat Thyagarajan,8 Martha Daviglus,4 Middle-Aged and Older Hispanics/ Tasneem Khambaty,9 Jianwen Cai,5 and Latinos: HCHS/SOL Results Neil Schneiderman10 Diabetes Care 2018;41:1501–1509 | https://doi.org/10.2337/dc17-1896 OBJECTIVE Hispanics/Latinos have the highest risks for metabolic syndrome (MetS) in the U.S. and are also at increased risk for Alzheimer disease. In this study, we exam- ined associations among neurocognitive function, MetS, and inflammation 1Department of Neurosciences and Shiley- ’ among diverse middle-aged and older Hispanics/Latinos. Marcos Alzheimer s Disease Research Center, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 2Institute of Gerontology and Department of RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODS Healthcare Sciences, Wayne State University, Cross-sectional data (2008–2011) from theHispanic Community Health Study/Study Detroit, MI of Latinos (HCHS/SOL) were analyzed to examine associations between neuro- 3Section of Gerontology and Geriatric Medicine, cognition and MetS among diverse Hispanics/Latinos (N = 9,136; aged 45–74 years). Department of Internal Medicine, Wake Forest School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, NC 4Institute for Minority Health Research, College RESULTS of Medicine, University of Illinois at Chicago, MetS status was associated with lower global neurocognition, mental status, verbal Chicago, IL learning and memory, verbal fluency, and executive function. Age significantly 5Collaborative Studies Coordinating Center, De- modified the associations between MetS and learning and memory measures.
    [Show full text]
  • Towards a Theory of Visual Learning
    Towards a Theory of Visual Learning Learning can be seen as a mental function that relies on the acquisition of knowledge (of different types and range) that is grounded in information – whether specific or perceived. What is learned is used as the basis of further learning, skills, values, belief systems, ideologies and competences. The visual learning process is one that can be seen to underpin others (cf. Ostensiveness: Piaget, 1953.) The assumptions that follow are drawn from current thinking about the relationships between what we see, what we remember and what we know. They help to explain why visual learning may be important, and how a range of technologies may contribute to these processes. What Science suggests Connections in the brain are constantly changing: they are not hardwired (Greenfield, 2003). The synapses relating to vision peak at around 10 months. The density of these synapses then declines and stabilises around 10 years of age. It is the pattern, however, rather than the number of connections that is most important. In terms of cognitive development there are ‘windows of time’ in the developing brain: critical periods for neural connections and pathways (Hubel & Weisel, 1981). The concept of plasticity is relevant here, in terms of the ways in which organisms adapt to environmental stimulus – in particular the brain, and the ways in which it adapts to stimuli (Maturana & Varela, 1981.) Plasticity refers to the ways in which brain structures can change to better cope with the environment: neurons or synapses can change their internal parameters in response to inputs and stimuli. The theory of neuroplasticity (Shaw & McEachern, 2001) describes the ways in which thinking, learning, and acting actually change both the brain’s physical structure and functional organization from top to bottom.
    [Show full text]
  • 3Rd Annual Meeting of Women in Cognitive Science – Canada Wednesday July 4, 2018, at 3:00 Pm
    CSBBCS & EPS 2018 Meeting July 4-7 2018 St. John’s, NL Table of Contents Welcome ....................................................................................................................................................... 3 Statement of Inclusion .............................................................................................................................. 3 General Information ..................................................................................................................................... 4 Hotel Maps ................................................................................................................................................ 5 Pre-Conference Information ......................................................................................................................... 6 Sponsors ........................................................................................................................................................ 7 CSBBCS 2018 Award Information.................................................................................................................. 8 2018 Donald O. Hebb Distinguished Contribution Award Winner ........................................................... 8 2018 CSBBCS Vincent Di Lollo Early Career Award Winner .................................................................... 10 2017 CSBBCS/CPA CJEP Best Article Award Winner ............................................................................... 11 2018 Richard Tees Distinguished
    [Show full text]
  • Interaction of Inferior Temporal Cortex with Frontal Cortex and Basal Forebrain: Double Dissociation in Strategy Implementation and Associative Learning
    The Journal of Neuroscience, August 15, 2002, 22(16):7288–7296 Interaction of Inferior Temporal Cortex with Frontal Cortex and Basal Forebrain: Double Dissociation in Strategy Implementation and Associative Learning David Gaffan,1 Alexander Easton,2 and Amanda Parker2 1Department of Experimental Psychology, Oxford University, Oxford OX1 3UD, United Kingdom, and 2School of Psychology, Nottingham University, Nottingham NG7 2RD, United Kingdom Macaque monkeys learned a strategy task in which two groups TSϩAMϩFX cannot be generally attributed to the partial tem- of visual objects needed to be treated differently, one with poral–frontal disconnection that this lesion creates, and there- persistent and one with sporadic object choices, to obtain food fore support the hypothesis that the amnesic effects of this rewards. After preoperative training, they were divided into two lesion are caused primarily by the disconnection of temporal surgical groups of three monkeys each. One group received cortex from ascending inputs from the basal forebrain. The crossed unilateral removals of frontal cortex and inferior tem- results also show that temporal–frontal interaction in strategy poral cortex (IT ϫ FC) and were severely impaired in performing implementation does not require those routes of temporal– the strategy task. The other group received bilateral transection frontal interaction that are interrupted in TSϩAMϩFX, and of anterior temporal stem, amygdala, and fornix (TSϩAMϩFX) therefore support the hypothesis that projections to other pos- and were unimpaired in performing the strategy task. Subse- terior cortical areas allow temporal and frontal cortex to interact quently the same animals were tested in visual object–reward with each other by multisynaptic corticocortical routes in strat- association learning.
    [Show full text]
  • Neurocognition of Major-Minor and Consonance-Dissonance
    Processing Major-Minor and Consonance-Dissonance 387 NEUROCOGNITION OF MAJOR-MINOR AND CONSONANCE-DISSONANCE PAULA VIRTALA &MARI TERVANIEMI even widened to infancy, and behavioral and brain stud- University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland ies have demonstrated many music-related auditory skills during the first months of life (Hannon, & Trainor MAJOR-MINOR AND CONSONANCE-DISSONANCE ARE 2007; Trainor & Corrigall, 2010; Trehub, 2010). Infants two profound elements of Western tonal music, and with limited exposure to music of their culture offer an have strong affective connotations for Western listeners. opportunity to study the earliest skills for music proces- This review summarizes recent evidence on the neu- sing, serving as candidates for innate universal abilities rocognitive basis of major-minor and consonance- which all music cultures build on. dissonance by presenting studies on their processing According to present understanding, the develop- and how it is affected by maturation, musical encul- ment of music processing is based on, first, early audi- turation, and music training. Based on recent findings tory skills that serve as building blocks for music in the field, it is proposed that both classifications, processing, and second, the process of musical encul- particularly consonance-dissonance, have partly turation that leads to facilitated processing of music of innate, biologically hard-wired properties. These prop- one’s own culture due to exposure without explicit erties can make them discriminable even for newborn training (Hannon & Trainor, 2007). This development infants and individuals living outside the Western is modified by several individual factors such as musical music culture and, to a small extent, reflect their affec- aptitude, cognitive abilities, and motivation, extending tive connotations in Western music.
    [Show full text]
  • Dopamine Modulation of Learning and Memory in the Prefrontal Cortex: Insights from Studies in Primates, Rodents, and Birds
    Dopamine modulation of learning and memory in the prefrontal cortex: insights from studies in primates, rodents, and birds The MIT Faculty has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters. Citation Puig, M. Victoria, Jonas Rose, Robert Schmidt, and Nadja Freund. “Dopamine Modulation of Learning and Memory in the Prefrontal Cortex: Insights from Studies in Primates, Rodents, and Birds.” Front. Neural Circuits. 8 (August 5, 2014). As Published http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2014.00093 Publisher Frontiers Research Foundation Version Final published version Citable link http://hdl.handle.net/1721.1/90936 Terms of Use Creative Commons Attribution Detailed Terms http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ REVIEW ARTICLE published: 05 August 2014 doi: 10.3389/fncir.2014.00093 Dopamine modulation of learning and memory in the prefrontal cortex: insights from studies in primates, rodents, and birds M. Victoria Puig 1*, Jonas Rose 1,2 *, Robert Schmidt 3 and Nadja Freund 4 1 The Picower Institute for Learning and Memory, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA 2 Animal Physiology, Institute of Neurobiology, University of Tübingen,Tübingen, Germany 3 BrainLinks-BrainTools, Department of Biology, Bernstein Center Freiburg, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany 4 Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Tübingen,Tübingen, Germany Edited by: In this review, we provide a brief overview over the current knowledge about the role of Guillermo Gonzalez-Burgos, dopamine transmission in the prefrontal cortex during learning and memory. We discuss University of Pittsburgh, USA work in humans, monkeys, rats, and birds in order to provide a basis for comparison Reviewed by: across species that might help identify crucial features and constraints of the dopaminergic Onur Gunturkun, Ruhr University Bochum, Germany system in executive function.
    [Show full text]