Freeman’S Review

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Freeman’S Review Karl Pribram’s Book 1 Walter Freeman’s Review BOOK REVIEW Title: The Form Within: My Point of View Author: Karl H Pribram MD Publisher: Westport CT: Prospecta Press ISBN: 978-1935212-80-5 Date: 2013 Reviewer: Walter J Freeman MD Dept of Molecular & Cell Biology University of California at Berkeley Berkeley CA 94720 USA Phone 1-510-642-4220 [email protected] Length: 1,997 words, 4 pages 22 March 2014 Cognitive neuroscience is in disarray. The neural mechanisms of locomotion, navigation, and manipulation of objects are well in hand, as witnessed by advances in information technology, robotics and space exploration. What we lack is an understanding of the mechanisms of mind. The urgency of perceived need is attested by the massive funding allocated for brain research in programs in Europe, North America and Asia. The programs are modeled on the Manhattan Project and the Human Genome Project, despite the difference that while the theories of nuclear fission and of the double helix were well enough developed to support large-scale engineering applications, in contrast, neuroscientists cannot tell us how minds work. For examples, we do not understand how brains make and use symbols, or how natural languages work, or how to solve the framing problem, or how to illuminate the mysteries of consciousness. At best we can enhance our existing techniques to increase our rates of data collection and facilitate our large-scale storage and retrieval by data mining for analysis using existing theory. Karl Pribram believes there is a better way than megaprojects to learn how minds work. As a neurosurgeon he believes that the answers must come from studies of brain activities as and when the subjects are performing cognitive tasks with normal brains or brains that are modified by disease or by surgical intervention. At the age of 94 he has written in conversational style an amazingly clear, voluminously detailed, yet easily accessible description of his experiments over the past seven decades in neurocognition by man and animals. He insists that his book is not an autobiography; rather he has written an intellectual history in which he describes his thoughts and conjectures, and those of his students and colleagues, as they planned their experiments and analyzed their results. Thereby he enables his readers to experience what it is like to be in the laboratory as scientific facts emerge. He introduces personalities to match the legendary achievements of major actors in the growth of neuroscience in the 20th century. This is his valuable legacy, which will inform the next two or three generations in the on-going search for solutions to the mysteries of mind. When Karl came on the scene in the 1940s, brain-mind research worldwide was disorganized and in conflict or isolation. Earlier, in the second half of the nineteenth century, there had been enthusiastic unity in adoption of a thermodynamic model advanced by Hermann Helmholtz and colleagues, which was based on the thermodynamic concept of “nerve energy” and “nerve force”. This conceptual advance enabled scientists and clinicians of all persuasions to replace mystical “animal spirits” with something tangible and measureable and expel all vestiges of “vitalism” from science. However, during the scientific revolution at the end of the century, the consensus disintegrated, when biophysicists showed that “nerve energy” is not conserved and cannot be measured. Lacking a shared foundation in science, neurologists and psychiatrists on the whole stopped talking with each other Karl Pribram’s Book 2 Walter Freeman’s Review and formed diverse schools. Among these schools were the behaviorists who denied altogether that mind was subject to scientific investigation. On the other side, Sigmund Freud abandoned brain science and concentrated on the psychology of mind, as Karl discusses in his book. The importance of Karl’s illuminating recollections stems from his strong conviction of the necessity for studies simultaneously of the properties of brain and mind. As Karl relates in his book, after he completed his training in medicine and neurosurgery, he engaged in neurocognitive research on primates under Karl Lashley, the leading neuropsychologist of the era, who became famous for his ultimately failed search for the location of memories (“engrams”) in brains. He immersed himself in the rich heritage of neurological, psychiatric and bio-behavioral research accreting through the two world wars and the depression, most notably the theory of “central excitatory state (c.e.s.) of Sir Charles Sherrington (1929), the “Gestalt” theory of Wolfgang Köhler (1941), and the “mass action” theory of Karl Lashley (1952). Their data and concepts constituted the paradigm of “neural fields” that Karl has pursued now into the 21st century. With characteristic flair he often used holography to demonstrate the principle of distributed memory by simply removing the condenser from his slide projector and recovering the same image from all parts of the unfocused field. At this juncture Karl collaborated with Köhler by testing Köhler’s hypothesis that perception operates by cortex constructing representations of stimuli by making images of them in “geometric isomorphism”. Köhler made a fatal category error when he identified the perceptual vector fields of the Gestalts with the scalar DC electric fields of the EEG, and replaced “nerve energy” of thermodynamics with “electric energy” of the EEG. Karl reports in his book that he and Roger Sperry among others quickly disproved the hypothesis that the EEG extracellular electric fields of ionic current played any causal role in Gestalt formation. More recently Karl has explored other aspects of the use of EEG and ECoG, but in the 1950s the damage was done. The EEG was relegated to the status of noise far removed from the primacy of action potentials, and was treated as “the roar of a crowd at a football team.” Field theory was banished from mainstream neuroscience. The isolation was compounded by the historical fact, which was described by Thomas Kuhn in his path-finding analysis, “The Structure of Scientific Revolutions” (1962), that work in one paradigm cannot disprove another, because paradigms differ in axioms, methods, classic experiments, rules of evidence, and heroes. One replaces another when the other becomes paralyzed with paradoxes and intractable failures that the one resolves. The process requires a generation to die and be replaced. I know this history because I lived through a significant part of it. Karl was my teacher in the Yale School of Medicine in 1950 and read my thesis. He has been my mentor ever since. I am grateful for his leadership and friendship. Hence conflict within neuroscience persists. Beginning in the 1950s mainstream neuroengineers and systems analysts modified the 19th century thermodynamic model by replacing “nerve energy” with “information” (Freeman, 2007). Thereby they created a new paradigm of “neural pulses”. Every textbook of neurophysiology in that era stated that action potentials were the sole means of communication between neurons in the brain. With a single chop, engineers and neuroscientists alike discarded half the electrical activity of the brain. With great confidence they revised the “nerve energy” paradigm by replacing it with it with “information”. They located information sources and sinks in the body and the environment. They conceived the sensory receptors as transducing energy from the environment and coding it as “ information” in trains of pulses. They replaced the “channels” of nerve tracts that carried “nerve energy” with single axons and dendrites like wires, which they represented with drawings of Golgi stains done by neuroanatomists Ramon y Cajal and his last graduate student Rafael Lorente de Nó. They modeled “information processing” by treating action potentials as binary digits that served as symbols in symbolic dynamics. They adopted Claude Shannon’s information theory despite Shannon’s vociferous opposition to application of his theory to problems in semantics. These diverse developments in brain theory in the 1950s coincided with the perfection of engineering techniques for recording action potentials from the brains of animals and humans who were fully alert and engaged in cognitive tasks. Neuroscientists flocked to the new Karl Pribram’s Book 3 Walter Freeman’s Review paradigm of “neural pulses”, much as they had to the doctrine of “nerve energy” a century before, while the paradigm of “neural fields” fell into relative obscurity. Disunity prevails because a strict interpretation of the Neuron Doctrine excludes field effects and insists that the pulse is the sole carrier of neural information, despite numerous discoveries of spikeless neurons, gap junctions, ephapsis, etc. in the past six decades. The action potential persists as the gold standard for observation and interpretation of neural activity. What is at issue may seem to resemble the wave-particle duality in quantum theory, but the disagreement is epistemological, not ontological, theoretical and not experimental. For the pulse paradigm the firing of a neuron is conceived as a “representation” of an action, object, person or “concept” according to Rodgrico Quian Quiroga (2013). The sensory information from the environment is combined with information stored in memory and condensed into a point in space- time. The pulse is treated as a symbol of what it represents. The length of a train of pulses represents the degree of certainty of the retrieved information. Information processing is by symbolic dynamics. For the field paradigm the firing of a neuron is seen as a random instantiation of a pulse in a time- space window over an interactive neural population. The population is creating a wave function that is carried by all of the neurons by time-sharing. The firing looks random in the recording of individual pulse trains, but it is seen as synchronized in multiple pulse trains recorded simultaneously or by correlation with the local field potential. The wave function is defined by integration over time-space of the impact of the transmitted pulse on all neurons to which the axon carries the pulse.
Recommended publications
  • Latinos: HCHS/SOL Results Neil Schneiderman10 Diabetes Care 2018;41:1501–1509 |
    Diabetes Care Volume 41, July 2018 1501 Hector M. Gonzalez,´ 1 Wassim Tarraf,2 Metabolic Syndrome and Priscilla Vasquez,´ 1 Ashley H. Sanderlin,3 Natalya I. Rosenberg,4 Sonia Davis,5 Neurocognition Among Diverse Carlos J. Rodr´ıguez,6 Linda C. Gallo,7 Bharat Thyagarajan,8 Martha Daviglus,4 Middle-Aged and Older Hispanics/ Tasneem Khambaty,9 Jianwen Cai,5 and Latinos: HCHS/SOL Results Neil Schneiderman10 Diabetes Care 2018;41:1501–1509 | https://doi.org/10.2337/dc17-1896 OBJECTIVE Hispanics/Latinos have the highest risks for metabolic syndrome (MetS) in the U.S. and are also at increased risk for Alzheimer disease. In this study, we exam- ined associations among neurocognitive function, MetS, and inflammation 1Department of Neurosciences and Shiley- ’ among diverse middle-aged and older Hispanics/Latinos. Marcos Alzheimer s Disease Research Center, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 2Institute of Gerontology and Department of RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODS Healthcare Sciences, Wayne State University, Cross-sectional data (2008–2011) from theHispanic Community Health Study/Study Detroit, MI of Latinos (HCHS/SOL) were analyzed to examine associations between neuro- 3Section of Gerontology and Geriatric Medicine, cognition and MetS among diverse Hispanics/Latinos (N = 9,136; aged 45–74 years). Department of Internal Medicine, Wake Forest School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, NC 4Institute for Minority Health Research, College RESULTS of Medicine, University of Illinois at Chicago, MetS status was associated with lower global neurocognition, mental status, verbal Chicago, IL learning and memory, verbal fluency, and executive function. Age significantly 5Collaborative Studies Coordinating Center, De- modified the associations between MetS and learning and memory measures.
    [Show full text]
  • 3Rd Annual Meeting of Women in Cognitive Science – Canada Wednesday July 4, 2018, at 3:00 Pm
    CSBBCS & EPS 2018 Meeting July 4-7 2018 St. John’s, NL Table of Contents Welcome ....................................................................................................................................................... 3 Statement of Inclusion .............................................................................................................................. 3 General Information ..................................................................................................................................... 4 Hotel Maps ................................................................................................................................................ 5 Pre-Conference Information ......................................................................................................................... 6 Sponsors ........................................................................................................................................................ 7 CSBBCS 2018 Award Information.................................................................................................................. 8 2018 Donald O. Hebb Distinguished Contribution Award Winner ........................................................... 8 2018 CSBBCS Vincent Di Lollo Early Career Award Winner .................................................................... 10 2017 CSBBCS/CPA CJEP Best Article Award Winner ............................................................................... 11 2018 Richard Tees Distinguished
    [Show full text]
  • Neurocognition of Major-Minor and Consonance-Dissonance
    Processing Major-Minor and Consonance-Dissonance 387 NEUROCOGNITION OF MAJOR-MINOR AND CONSONANCE-DISSONANCE PAULA VIRTALA &MARI TERVANIEMI even widened to infancy, and behavioral and brain stud- University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland ies have demonstrated many music-related auditory skills during the first months of life (Hannon, & Trainor MAJOR-MINOR AND CONSONANCE-DISSONANCE ARE 2007; Trainor & Corrigall, 2010; Trehub, 2010). Infants two profound elements of Western tonal music, and with limited exposure to music of their culture offer an have strong affective connotations for Western listeners. opportunity to study the earliest skills for music proces- This review summarizes recent evidence on the neu- sing, serving as candidates for innate universal abilities rocognitive basis of major-minor and consonance- which all music cultures build on. dissonance by presenting studies on their processing According to present understanding, the develop- and how it is affected by maturation, musical encul- ment of music processing is based on, first, early audi- turation, and music training. Based on recent findings tory skills that serve as building blocks for music in the field, it is proposed that both classifications, processing, and second, the process of musical encul- particularly consonance-dissonance, have partly turation that leads to facilitated processing of music of innate, biologically hard-wired properties. These prop- one’s own culture due to exposure without explicit erties can make them discriminable even for newborn training (Hannon & Trainor, 2007). This development infants and individuals living outside the Western is modified by several individual factors such as musical music culture and, to a small extent, reflect their affec- aptitude, cognitive abilities, and motivation, extending tive connotations in Western music.
    [Show full text]
  • Dopamine Modulation of Learning and Memory in the Prefrontal Cortex: Insights from Studies in Primates, Rodents, and Birds
    Dopamine modulation of learning and memory in the prefrontal cortex: insights from studies in primates, rodents, and birds The MIT Faculty has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters. Citation Puig, M. Victoria, Jonas Rose, Robert Schmidt, and Nadja Freund. “Dopamine Modulation of Learning and Memory in the Prefrontal Cortex: Insights from Studies in Primates, Rodents, and Birds.” Front. Neural Circuits. 8 (August 5, 2014). As Published http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2014.00093 Publisher Frontiers Research Foundation Version Final published version Citable link http://hdl.handle.net/1721.1/90936 Terms of Use Creative Commons Attribution Detailed Terms http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ REVIEW ARTICLE published: 05 August 2014 doi: 10.3389/fncir.2014.00093 Dopamine modulation of learning and memory in the prefrontal cortex: insights from studies in primates, rodents, and birds M. Victoria Puig 1*, Jonas Rose 1,2 *, Robert Schmidt 3 and Nadja Freund 4 1 The Picower Institute for Learning and Memory, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA 2 Animal Physiology, Institute of Neurobiology, University of Tübingen,Tübingen, Germany 3 BrainLinks-BrainTools, Department of Biology, Bernstein Center Freiburg, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany 4 Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Tübingen,Tübingen, Germany Edited by: In this review, we provide a brief overview over the current knowledge about the role of Guillermo Gonzalez-Burgos, dopamine transmission in the prefrontal cortex during learning and memory. We discuss University of Pittsburgh, USA work in humans, monkeys, rats, and birds in order to provide a basis for comparison Reviewed by: across species that might help identify crucial features and constraints of the dopaminergic Onur Gunturkun, Ruhr University Bochum, Germany system in executive function.
    [Show full text]
  • Is Neurocognition Crucial in STEM Education in the Present Scenario? S.Amutha* Bharathidasan University, Tiruchirappalli *Corresponding Author: S
    2020 Review Article Journal of Cognitive Neuropsychology Vol.4 No.2 Is Neurocognition Crucial in STEM Education in the Present Scenario? S.Amutha* Bharathidasan University, Tiruchirappalli *Corresponding author: S. Amutha, Bharathidasan University, Tiruchirappalli, E-mail: [email protected] Tel No: +91 9443145648 1. Abstract Countries around the globe moving away from traditional rote and regurgitation learning to experiential learning. Science, Technology, Engineering and Mathematics are the subjects interlinked with each other. Most of the countries in the world prefer to adopt STEM (Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics) education because it infuses every part of our lives. Without Science and Technology our survival in this globe is questioned. Technology is constantly escalating into every aspect of our lives. Engineering is the basic subject for designing the houses, machineries, construction etc. Basically everyone need Mathematics in every occupation and every activity of lives. STEM based curriculum comprise of hands-on and minds-on activities for the student. By divulging students to explore STEM-related concepts, they will develop a passion towards it. Nevertheless of voluminous initiatives by all the countries, are on the way to impart STEM education to students due to their difference in perception, knowledge and memory. Neurocognition is the process which activates the cognitive process. Depending on the needs of the individual Neurocognitive strategies will restructure or modify their thought, feelings, perceptions and emotions. This article addresses the theoretical back drop of Neurocognition in education and how the issues related to educational performance can be solved with the help of Neurocognitive practice. 2. Keywords: Science, Technology, Engineering, Mathematics, (STEM) Brain, Neurocognition 3.
    [Show full text]
  • Neurocognition in Psychometrically Defined College Schizotypy Samples: We Are NOT Measuring the ''Right Stuff''
    Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society (2013), 19, 1–14. Copyright E INS. Published by Cambridge University Press, 2013. doi:10.1017/S135561771200152X 1 Neurocognition in Psychometrically Defined College 2 Schizotypy Samples: We Are NOT Measuring the 3 ‘‘Right Stuff’’ 4 Charlotte A. Chun, Kyle S. Minor, AND Alex S. Cohen 5 Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, Louisiana 6 (RECEIVED November 11, 2011; FINAL REVISION October 4, 2012; ACCEPTED October 4, 2012) 7 Abstract 8 Although neurocognitive deficits are an integral characteristic of schizophrenia, there is inconclusive evidence as to 9 whether they manifest across the schizophrenia-spectrum. We conducted two studies and a meta-analysis comparing 10 neurocognitive functioning between psychometrically defined schizotypy and control groups recruited from a college 11 population. Study One compared groups on measures of specific and global neurocognition, and subjective and objective 12 quality of life. Study Two examined working memory and subjective cognitive complaints. Across both studies, the 13 schizotypy group showed notably decreased subjective (d 5 1.52) and objective (d 5 1.02) quality of life and greater 14 subjective cognitive complaints (d 5 1.88); however, neurocognition was normal across all measures (d’s , .35). Our 15 meta-analysis of 33 studies examining neurocognition in at-risk college students revealed between-group differences in 16 the negligible effect size range for most domains. The schizotypy group demonstrated deficits of a small effect size for 17 working memory and set-shifting abilities. Although at-risk individuals report relatively profound neurocognitive deficits 18 and impoverished quality of life, neurocognitive functioning assessed behaviorally is largely intact.
    [Show full text]
  • Neurocognitive and Social Cognitive Correlates of Formal Thought Disorder in Schizophrenia Patients
    University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs Staff Publications U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs 2006 Neurocognitive and social cognitive correlates of formal thought disorder in schizophrenia patients Kenneth L. Subotnik University of California - Los Angeles, [email protected] Keith H. Nuechterlein University of California - Los Angeles, [email protected] Michael F. Green University of California - Los Angeles William P. Horan University of California - Los Angeles, [email protected] Tasha M. Nienow University of California - Los Angeles See next page for additional authors Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/veterans Subotnik, Kenneth L.; Nuechterlein, Keith H.; Green, Michael F.; Horan, William P.; Nienow, Tasha M.; Ventura, Joseph; and Nguyen, Annie T., "Neurocognitive and social cognitive correlates of formal thought disorder in schizophrenia patients" (2006). U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs Staff Publications. 76. https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/veterans/76 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs Staff Publications by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. Authors Kenneth L. Subotnik, Keith H. Nuechterlein, Michael F. Green, William P. Horan, Tasha M. Nienow, Joseph Ventura, and Annie T. Nguyen This article is available at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/ veterans/76 Schizophrenia Research 85 (2006) 84–95 www.elsevier.com/locate/schres Neurocognitive and social cognitive correlates of formal thought disorder in schizophrenia patients Kenneth L.
    [Show full text]
  • Computerized Neurocognitive Scanning: II
    Computerized Neurocognitive Scanning: II. The Profile of Schizophrenia Ruben C. Gur, Ph.D., J. Daniel Ragland, Ph.D., Paul J. Moberg, Ph.D., Warren B. Bilker, Ph.D., Christian Kohler, M.D., Steven J. Siegel, M.D., Ph.D., and Raquel E. Gur, M.D., Ph.D. Cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia is well established neurocognitive scan and clinical variables, including with neuropsychological batteries, which have assessed premorbid adjustment, onset age, illness duration, quality multiple domains indicating diffuse deficits especially in of life, and severity of negative symptoms. These processing related to frontotemporal systems. Two studies correlations were higher and more prevalent in women than are reported examining the feasibility of the computerized men, who showed correlations predominantly for speed neurocognitive scan to assess differential deficits in rather than accuracy. Neuroleptic exposure was associated schizophrenia. In Study 1, we tested 53 patients and 71 with poorer performance only for speed of memory controls with the traditional and computerized assessments processing, and in men, this association was seen only for counterbalanced in order. Both showed comparable typical neuroleptics. We conclude that the computerized generalized impairment in schizophrenia with differential neurocognitive scan can be applied reliably in people with deficits in executive functions and memory. The profile was schizophrenia, yielding data that support its construct and replicated in Study 2 in a new sample of 68 patients and 37 criterion validity. controls, receiving only the computerized scan. The [Neuropsychopharmacology 25:777–788, 2001] combined sample showed robust correlations between © 2001 American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. performance on both speed and accuracy measures of the Published by Elsevier Science Inc.
    [Show full text]
  • IGNITE): Protocol T ⁎ Kirk I
    Contemporary Clinical Trials 85 (2019) 105832 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Contemporary Clinical Trials journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/conclintrial Investigating Gains in Neurocognition in an Intervention Trial of Exercise (IGNITE): Protocol T ⁎ Kirk I. Ericksona,b, , George A. Grovea,Jeffrey M. Burnsc, Charles H. Hillmand,e, Arthur F. Kramerd,f, Edward McAuleyg, Eric D. Vidonic, James T. Beckera,h,i, Meryl A. Buttersh, Katerina Graya, Haiqing Huanga, John M. Jakicicj, M. Ilyas Kambohk, Chaeryon Kangl, William E. Klunkh, Phil Leem, Anna L. Marslanda, Joseph Mettenburgn, Renee J. Rogersj, Chelsea M. Stillmana, Bradley P. Suttonf, Amanda Szabo-Reedo, Timothy D. Verstynenp, Jennifer C. Watta, Andrea M. Weinsteinh, Mariegold E. Wollama a Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, USA. b Discipline of Exercise Science, College of Science, Health, Engineering and Education, Murdoch University, Australia c Department of Neurology, University of Kansas Medical Center, USA d Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, USA e Department of Physical Therapy, Movement, & Rehabilitation Sciences, Northeastern University, USA f Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, USA g Department of Kinesiology and Community Health, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, USA h Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, USA i Department of Neurology, University of Pittsburgh, USA j Department of Health and Physical Activity, University of Pittsburgh, USA
    [Show full text]
  • Executive Neurocognition, Memory Systems, and Borderline Personality Disorder
    Clinical Psychology Review 26 (2006) 346–375 Executive neurocognition, memory systems, and borderline personality disorder Eric A. Fertucka,T, Mark F. Lenzenwegerc, John F. Clarkind, Simone Hoermanne, Barbara Stanleya,b aNew York State Psychiatric Institute, Columbia University, College of Physicians and Surgeons, United States bJohn Jay College, City University of New York, United States cState University of New York at Binghamton, United States dWeill Medical College of Cornell University, United States eColumbia University Medical Center, United States Received 13 April 2004; accepted 9 May 2005 Abstract Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD) is a common, disabling, and burdensome psychiatric condition. It is characterized by turbulent fluctuations of negative emotions and moods, unstable and conflictual interpersonal relationships, an incoherent and often contradictory sense of self, and impulsive, potentially lethal self-injurious behaviors. The neurobehavioral facets of BPD have not been extensively studied. However, clinical theoreticians and researchers have proposed that the symptoms and behaviors of BPD are, in part, associated with disruptions in basic neurocognitive processes. This review summarizes and evaluates research that has investigated the relationship between executive neurocognition, memory systems, and BPD. Three historical phases of research are delineated and reviewed, and the methodological and conceptual challenges this body of investigation highlights are discussed. Laboratory-based assessment of executive neurocognition and memory systems is integral to an interdisciplinary approach to research in BPD. Such an approach holds promise in elucidating the T Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 212 543 6926; fax: +1 212 543 6946. E-mail address: [email protected] (E.A. Fertuck). 0272-7358/$ - see front matter D 2005 Elsevier Ltd.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 CURRICULUM VITAE BJ Casey, Ph.D. January 1, 2021 A
    CURRICULUM VITAE BJ Casey, Ph.D. January 1, 2021 A. GENERAL INFORMATION Mailing address: Department of Psychology Yale University 2 Hillhouse Ave New Haven, CT 06511 Office location SSS 414D Office telephone: 203-432-7790 Office fax: 203-432-7172 Lab: 203-432-0228 Email: [email protected] Citizenship: USA B. EDUCATIONAL BACKGROUND BA Appalachian State University, 1977-1982 1982 Boone, NC MA Appalachian State University 1983-1984 1984 Boone, NC PhD University of South Carolina 1986-1990 1990 Columbia, SC C. PROFESSIONAL POSITIONS AND EMPLOYMENT Post-doctoral training Postdoctoral Fellow National Institute of Mental Health 1990-1992 Bethesda, MD Staff Fellow National Institute of Mental Health 1992-1994 Bethesda, MD Academic positions Assistant Professor University of Pittsburgh Medical Center 7/01/1994- Pittsburgh, PA 5/31/1999 Visiting Research Princeton University 7/01/1998- Collaborator Princeton, NJ 6/30/2006 Assistant Professor Weill Medical College of Cornell University 6/01/1999- in Psychiatry (interim) New York, NY 7/22/1999 Associate Professor Weill Medical College of Cornell University 6/01/1999- of Psychiatry New York, NY 5/31/2002 Professor of Psychology Weill Medical College of Cornell University 7/01/2002- in Psychiatry and New York, NY 6/30/2016 Neuroscience Sackler Professor and Weill Medical College of Cornell University 7/01/2002- Sackler Institute Director New York, NY 6/30/2016 Adjunct Professor The Rockefeller University 5/01/2013- New York, NY 4/30/2017 Adjunct Professor Weill Medical College of Cornell University 7/01/2016- New York, NY 6/30/2017 1 Professor of Psychology Yale University, New Haven CT 7/1/2016- Present Affiliated Professor The Justice Collaboratory, Yale University 10/1/2016- New Haven CT Present Affiliated Professor Interdepartmental Neuroscience Program 9/1/2016- Yale University, New Haven CT Present Guest Investigator The Rockefeller University 5/01/2017- New York, NY 4/30/20 D.
    [Show full text]
  • Schizophrenia Research 204 (2019) 133–137
    Schizophrenia Research 204 (2019) 133–137 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Schizophrenia Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/schres Language comprehension and neurocognition independently and concurrently contribute to formal thought disorder severity in schizophrenia Eric J. Tan ⁎, Susan L. Rossell Centre for Mental Health, Swinburne University of Technology, Melbourne, Australia Monash Alfred Psychiatry Research Centre, Monash University Central Clinical School, The Alfred Hospital, Melbourne, Australia St Vincent's Mental Health Service, St Vincent's Hospital, Melbourne, Australia article info abstract Article history: Formal thought disorder (FTD) in schizophrenia is a prevalent symptom that has a significant impact on patients Received 15 October 2017 but low remediation options. This is largely due to a still unclear aetiology, where both neurocognitive and lan- Received in revised form 20 July 2018 guage dysfunction have been shown to contribute. Given established relationships between neurocognition and Accepted 13 August 2018 language themselves, this study aimed to examine if language comprehension impairments have a significant ef- Available online 17 August 2018 fect on FTD severity independent of neurocognition. 54 schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder patients (M = 43.35, SD = 10.74) completed three measures of language comprehension along with the MATRICS Consensus Keywords: Schizophrenia Cognitive Battery and the D-KEFS Colour Word Interference Test. Symptomatology was assessed using the Posi- Formal thought disorder tive and Negative Symptom Scale and the Scale for the Assessment of Thought, Language and Communication. Neurocognition Hierarchical linear regression analyses revealed syntactic sentence processing had a significant, and independent, Language comprehension contribution to positive FTD severity above neurocognition, while semantic sentence processing and single word Syntax semantic processing did not.
    [Show full text]