Life Between Clocks: Daily Temporal Patterns of Human Chronotypes

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Life Between Clocks: Daily Temporal Patterns of Human Chronotypes 10.1177/0748730402239679JOURNALRoenneberg OF et BIOLOGICALal. / LIFE BETWEENARTICLE RHYTHMS CLOCKS / February 2003 Life between Clocks: Daily Temporal Patterns of Human Chronotypes Till Roenneberg,*,1 Anna Wirz-Justice,† and Martha Merrow* *Centre for Chronobiology, Institute for Medical Psychology, Munich, Germany, †Centre for Chronobiology, Psychiatric University Clinic, Basel, Switzerland Abstract Human behavior shows large interindividual variation in temporal organization. Extreme “larks” wake up when extreme “owls” fall asleep. These chronotypes are attributed to differences in the circadian clock, and in animals, the genetic basis of similar phenotypic differences is well established. To better understand the genetic basis of temporal organization in humans, the authors developed a questionnaire to document individual sleep times, self-reported light exposure, and self-assessed chronotype, considering work and free days separately. This report summarizes the results of 500 questionnaires completed in a pilot study. Individual sleep times show large differences between work and free days, except for extreme early types. During the workweek, late chronotypes accumulate considerable sleep debt, for which they compensate on free days by lengthening their sleep by several hours. For all chronotypes, the amount of time spent outdoors in broad daylight significantly affects the timing of sleep: Increased self-reported light exposure advances sleep. The timing of self- selected sleep is multifactorial, including genetic disposition, sleep debt accumu- lated on workdays, and light exposure. Thus, accurate assessment of genetic chronotypes has to incorporate all of these parameters. The dependence of human chronotype on light, that is, on the amplitude of the light:dark signal, fol- lows the known characteristics of circadian systems in all other experimental organisms. Our results predict that the timing of sleep has changed during industrialization and that a majority of humans are sleep deprived during the workweek. The implications are far ranging concerning learning, memory, vigi- lance, performance, and quality of life. Key words circadian, clock, human, light, sleep, chronotype Our daily life is organized by three different clocks: ings time. When shielded from the solar and the social a solar clock, providing light and warmer tempera- clock (constant conditions), the biological clock “runs tures during the day; a social clock, which we see or free.” In real life, however, circadian clocks are usually hear first thing on a working day; and a biological synchronized (entrained) to the 24 h of the solar clock clock, which we sense most vividly when jet lagged, (Rajaratnam and Arendt, 2001; Roenneberg and Fos- during shift work, or when adjusting to daylight sav- ter, 1997). The major environmental signal for entrain- 1. To whom all correspondence should be addressed: Centre for Chronobiology,Institute for Medical Psychology,Munich, Ger- many; e-mail: [email protected]. JOURNAL OF BIOLOGICAL RHYTHMS, Vol. 18 No. 1, February 2003 80-90 DOI: 10.1177/0748730402239679 © 2003 Sage Publications 80 Downloaded from jbr.sagepub.com at COLUMBIA UNIV on April 14, 2014 Roenneberg et al. / LIFE BETWEEN CLOCKS 81 ment (zeitgeber) is light that, in mammals, can reach tally measured timing of the individual’s circadian the biological clock only through the eyes (for refer- rhythms, for example, temperature, melatonin, or ences, see Roenneberg and Merrow, 2002). cortisol (Bailey and Heitkemper, 2001; Duffy et al., The circadian clock controls physiology at many 1999, 2001), the genetics behind “morningness” and levels, from gene expression to complex behaviors “eveningness” are far from straightforward (Duffy (e.g., sleep and performance). As with other genetic and Czeisler, 2002; Duffy et al., 1999; Vink et al., 2001). traits, circadian properties depend on specific alleles We have developed a new questionnaire to quanti- of genes (clock genes; for review, see Young and Kay, tatively assess the timing of sleep within the 24-h day 2001). In a given population, free-running periods are (sleep phase) using simple questions (see the Methods distributed around a species-specific mean both in section). The results of this pilot study show that animals (Pittendrigh and Daan, 1976) and in humans although indicated sleep times correlate well with (e.g., Dijk and Lockley, 2002; Klerman, 2001; Wever, chronotype self-assessment, they are very different 1979). These circadian differences can also be seen between workdays and free days. Most important, when the clock is entrained because the phase angle phase and duration of sleep on free days strongly between dawn and activity onset depends on the indi- depend on (1) the amount of sleep obtained during the vidual free-running period (Aschoff, 1979; Duffy et al., workweek and (2) the amount of time subjects spend 2001; Roenneberg and Merrow, 2000). Human prefer- outdoors in daylight. Thus, timing of sleep is a good ences in the timing of sleep and wake (called “chrono- indicator for the underlying chronotype when cor- types,” such as “larks” and “owls”) are, at least partly, rected for these factors. based on genetics (Ebisawa et al., 2001; Jones et al., 1999; Katzenberg et al., 1998; Toh et al., 2001). Chronotypes have been assessed mainly by ques- METHODS tionnaires designed to associate individuals to ten- dencies that were coined “morningness” or This article describes the results of an ongoing “eveningness” (Horne and Östberg, 1976). The ques- study, based on the first 500 completed questionnaires tions used are mostly subjective, relating sleep and (see Fig. 1), which were distributed in Germany and activity times to a personal “feeling best rhythm” Switzerland in high schools, universities, and the gen- (Horne and Östberg, 1976) or to the habits of others eral population mainly between March and June (shift (e.g., “I get up later than most people”) (Smith et al., workers were excluded; for age and gender distribu- 2002), or they ask subjects to assess hypothetical situa- tion, see Table 1). All questionnaires were filled in vol- tions (e.g., “Approximately what time would you get untarily,and subjects were informed about confidenti- up if you were entirely free to plan your day?”) ality and data handling on a cover page (not shown). (Terman and White, 2001). These questionnaires yield We continue to distribute and evaluate the question- plausible results, yet oddly enough, in terms of “real” naire in a project that searches for extreme behavior, they do not explicitly assess free days and chronotypes and their families, and the results pre- workdays separately nor do they ask for actual sleep sented here prove to be extremely stable. This first times (Putilov, 2000) or exposure to outdoor light. report concentrates on the temporal distribution of These details have, so far, been addressed only in a daily behavior and therefore does not address all questionnaire assessing seasonality (Wirz-Justice questions. The temporal structure of daily life was et al., 1996, 2003). assessed separately for workdays and free days. Sub- A growing number of studies investigate the jects are explicitly asked to describe their behavior for genetic basis of the human circadian clock and its rela- normal circumstances, that is, without partying. In tionship to abnormal sleep times using the existing addition, subjects self-rated their chronotype category questionnaires (e.g., Ebisawa et al., 2001; Jones et al., based on a description (0 = extreme early,1=moderate 1999; Katzenberg et al., 1998). However, determina- early,2=slight early,3=normal,4=slight late,5=moder- tion of circadian genes in experimental organisms ate late, 6 = extreme late; see questionnaire in Fig. 1). stems from the excellent ability to quantify circadian A preliminary validation was carried out in a ran- outputs (e.g., period length). Thus, successful genetic dom sample of subjects (N = 30) who kept a sleep log research into the human clock also requires a quantita- for 5 weeks after completing the questionnaire. Actual tive description of circadian traits. Although and indicated sleep times on both workdays and free “morningness” scores correlate with the experimen- days correlated highly (p < 0.0001) with slopes around Downloaded from jbr.sagepub.com at COLUMBIA UNIV on April 14, 2014 82 Self assessment Munich ChronoType Questionnaire (MCTQ) After you have answered the preceding questions, you should have a feeling to which chronotype (time-of-day-type) you belong to. If for example, you like (and manage) to sleep quite a bit longer on free days than on workdays, or if you cannot get out of bed on Monday mornings, even Please enter your age, gender, etc.. This information is important for our evaluations without a Sunday-night-party, then you are more a late type. If, however, you regularly wake up and feel perky once you jump out of bed, and if you would rather go to bed early than to an eve- Age: _________ female male Height ______ Weight _______ ning concert then you are an early type. In the following questions, you should categorise your- self and your family members. On work days … Please tick only one possibility! I have to get up at… _________o ’ c l o c k Description of categories: extreme early type = 0 I need… _________m i n t o w a k e u p moderate early type = 1 I regularly wake up… before the alarm with the alarm slight early type = 2 From… _________o ’ c l o c k I am fully awake normal type = 3 At around…
Recommended publications
  • Single-Cell Analysis on the Biological Clock Using Microfluidic
    SINGLE-CELL ANALYSIS ON THE BIOLOGICAL CLOCK USING MICROFLUIDIC DROPLETS by ZHAOJIE DENG (Under the Direction of Leidong Mao and Jonathan Arnold) ABSTRACT Single-cell analysis has become crucial for uncovering the underlying mechanism for cell heterogeneity. Different biological questions pose different challenges for single-cell analysis. In order to answer questions like, whether a single-cell has a biological clock and how the clocks synchronize among cells to overcome the heterogeneity, continuous long-term measurement on large numbers of single-cells is required. However traditional measurement techniques usually involve measurement on millions of cells. My dissertation addresses these challenges by developing a microfluidic droplet platform capable of measuring the biological clock on >1000 Neurospora crassa single-cells for up to 10 days. The results show that in Neurospora crassa a single cell has the three major properties of a biological clock: a circadian oscillator, light entertainment, and temperature compensation and that single-cells synchronize their biological clock with each other possibly through quorum sensing. INDEX WORDS: Single-cell analysis, Microfluidic droplets, Neurospora crassa, Circadian rhythm, Biological clock, Stochastic noise, Synchronization SINGLE-CELL ANALYSIS ON THE BIOLOGICAL CLOCK USING MICROFLUIDIC DROPLETS by ZHAOJIE DENG B.A., Huazhong University of Science and Technology, China, 2008 M.S., Huazhong University of Science and Technology, China, 2011 A Dissertation Submitted to the Graduate Faculty
    [Show full text]
  • Challenging the Human Circadian Clock by Daylight Saving Time and Shift-Work
    Challenging the human circadian clock by Daylight Saving Time and Shift-Work Academic Dissertation (Doctor rerum naturalium) At the Centre for Chronobiology Institute for Medical Psychology Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich Written by Thomas Kantermann Born 26th of February, 1979 in Gütersloh Arbeit eingereicht am: 17.07.2008 1. Gutachter / Prüfer: Prof. Gisela Grupe 2. Gutachter / Prüfer: Prof. Benedikt Grothe 3. Prüfer: Prof. Susanne Foitzik 4. Prüfer: Prof. Gerhard Haszprunar Sondergutachter: Prof. Till Roenneberg Tag der mündlichen Prüfung: 15.12.2008 Für meine Schwester Stefanie „Probleme kann man niemals mit derselben Denkweise lösen, durch die sie entstanden sind.“ Albert Einstein dt.-amerikan. Physiker, 1921 Nobelpreis für Physik 1879 – 1955 Contents 1. INTRODUCTION ..................................................................................... 1 1.1. Biological (circa-) Rhythms..........................................................................................2 1.2. Sleep.............................................................................................................................4 1.2.1. Two Process Model of Sleep..................................................................................6 1.2.2. Circadian Rhythm Sleep Disorders, Sleepiness and Fatigue....................................7 1.3. The Internal Clock ........................................................................................................8 1.3.1. Phase of Entrainment – Chronotype .....................................................................11
    [Show full text]
  • Connecting Biological Rhythms with Patterns of Smartphone App Use
    Mobile Manifestations of Alertness: Connecting Biological Rhythms with Patterns of Smartphone App Use Elizabeth L. Murnane1, Saeed Abdullah1, Mark Matthews1, Matthew Kay2, Julie A. Kientz3, Tanzeem Choudhury1, Geri Gay1, Dan Cosley1 1Information Science, Cornell University, {elm236, sma249, mjm672, tkc28, gkg1, drc44}@cornell.edu 2Computer Science & Engineering, 3Human Centered Design & Engineering, University of Washington, {mjskay, jkientz}@uw.edu ABSTRACT such behavior is a reliable path to reaching high levels of Our body clock causes considerable variations in our behav- achievement and success [2]. ioral, mental, and physical processes, including alertness, Such attempts at optimizing performance rarely take into ac- throughout the day. While much research has studied technol- count both inter- and intra-individual variability in biological ogy usage patterns, the potential impact of underlying biologi- characteristics [84] — namely the “internal timing” of the cal processes on these patterns is under-explored. Using data body clock [73], which produces individually-variable fluc- from 20 participants over 40 days, this paper presents the first tuations in cognitive and physical performance. Similarly, study to connect patterns of mobile application usage with technologies aimed at supporting productivity are typically these contributing biological factors. Among other results, designed on assumptions that our capabilities over the course we find that usage patterns vary for individuals with differ- of a day are steady (or could be made steady). ent body clock types, that usage correlates with rhythms of alertness, that app use features such as duration and switching In reality, biological clocks influence our performance lev- can distinguish periods of low and high alertness, and that app els, which naturally rise and fall throughout the day [14].
    [Show full text]
  • 7742.Full.Pdf
    Demasking biological oscillators: Properties and principles of entrainment exemplified by the Neurospora circadian clock Till Roenneberg*†, Zdravko Dragovic‡, and Martha Merrow§ *Centre for Chronobiology, Institute of Medical Psychology, Medical Faculty, University of Munich, Goethestrasse 31, D-80336 Munich, Germany; ‡Max Planck Institute for Cellular Biochemistry, Am Klopferspitz 18a, D-82152, Martinsried, Germany; and §Biological Center, University of Groningen, Kerklaan 30, P.O. Box 14, 9750 AA, Haren, The Netherlands Edited by Joseph S. Takahashi, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, and approved April 14, 2005 (received for review March 8, 2005) Oscillations are found throughout the physical and biological remains constant (in case of the circadian clock, the time of worlds. Their interactions can result in a systematic process of the human temperature minimum could be related to dawn). synchronization called entrainment, which is distinct from a simple 2. An oscillator may go through several cycles before it reaches stimulus-response pattern. Oscillators respond to stimuli at some stable entrainment (in the case of the circadian clock, we times in their cycle and may not respond at others. Oscillators can experience these ‘‘transients’’ during jetlag). also be driven if the stimulus is strong (or if the oscillator is weak); 3. When oscillators are submitted to entraining cycles of differ- i.e., they restart their cycle every time they receive a stimulus. ent periods (T), ⌿ can change systematically, ranging from Stimuli can also directly affect rhythms without entraining the relatively more delayed in short cycles to relatively more underlying oscillator (masking): Drivenness and masking are often advanced in long cycles (see examples in Fig.
    [Show full text]
  • General Information
    General Information Headquarters is at the Baytowne Conference Center, which is conveniently located within walking distance of all hotel rooms. SRBR Information Desk and Message Center is in the Foyer of the Baytowne Conference Center main level. The desk hours are as follows: Friday 5/21 2:00–6:00 PM Saturday 5/22 7:30 –11:00 AM 2:00–8:00 PM Sunday 5/23 7:00–11:00 AM 4:00–7:00 PM Monday 5/24 7:30–11:00 AM 4:00–6:00 PM Tuesday 5/25 8:00–11:00 AM 4:00–6:00 PM Wednesday 5/26 8:00–10:00 AM Messages can be left on the SRBR message board next to the registration desk. Meeting participants are asked to check the message board routinely for mail, notes, and telephone messages. Hotel check-in will be at the individual properties. Posters will be available for viewing in the Magnolia B/C/D/E rooms. Poster numbers 1–93 Sunday, May 23, 10:00 AM–10:30 PM Poster numbers 94–183 Monday, May 24, 10:00 AM–10:30 PM Poster number 184–275 Tuesday, May 25, 10:00 AM–10:30 PM All posters must be removed by 10:00 am on Wednesday, May 26. The Village of Baytowne Wharf—Indulge your senses at Sandestin’s charming Village of Baytowne Wharf, a picturesque pedestrian village overlooking the Choctawatchee Bay. Discover a unique collection of more than 40 specialty merchants ranging from quaint boutiques and intimate eateries to lively nightclubs, all set up against a backdrop of vibrant special events.
    [Show full text]
  • Sleep and Chronotype in Adolescents
    Aus dem Institut für Medizinische Psychologie der Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München komm. Vorstand: Prof. Dr. Till Roenneberg Sleep and Chronotype in Adolescents - a Chronobiological Field-Study - Dissertation zum Erwerb des Doktorgrades der Medizin an der medizinischen Fakultät der Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität zu München vorgelegt von Stephanie Böhm aus Thuine 2012 Mit Genehmigung der Medizinschen Fakultät der Universität München Berichterstatter: Prof. Dr. Till Roenneberg Mitberichterstatter: Prof. Dr. Axel Steiger Prof. Dr. Brigitte Bondy Dekan: Prof. Dr. Dr. h.c. Maximilian Reiser, FACR, FRCR Tag der mündlichen Prüfung: 08.03.2012 Dedicated to Felix Table of Contents 1 Introduction ................................................................................................... 6 1.1 Chronobiology and Biological Rhythms ....................................................... 6 1.2 Circadian Rhythms in Humans ...................................................................... 7 1.2.1 Chronotype ..................................................................................................... 8 1.2.2 The Circadian Clock in Adolescence ............................................................ 10 1.3 Sleep ................................................................................................................ 11 1.3.1 Anatomy and Physiology of Sleep ................................................................ 12 1.3.2 Sleep-EEG and Sleep Stages .........................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Twilight Times: Light and the Circadian System
    Photochemistry and Photobiology, 1997, 66(5): 549-561 Invited Review Twilight Times: Light and the Circadian System Till Roenneberg*’ and Russell G. Foster2 ’Institute for Medical Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilian University, Munich, Germany and 2Department of Biology, Imperial College, London, UK Received 21 May 1997; accepted 31 July 1997 Whose twilights were more clear, than our mid-day. even if it is isolated from the rest of the brain (1). Long John Donne (1571-1631) before the primary circadian clock of mammals was identi- Of the progress of the soul fied in the SCN, the endogenous nature of circadian rhythms was recognized, first in plants (2-5) and then in many dif- ferent animal species (6). Identification has been based upon THE CIRCADIAN CLOCK the critical observation that when organisms are kept in iso- The spatial and temporal features of the environment have lation, void of any temporal cues, their daily routine contin- provided the abiotic selection pressures that have shaped the ues unabated, although the endogenous daily period (7)may evolution of life on earth. We are accustomed to accept that deviate from the external 24 h cycle (T), hence the term the spatial world offers specialized niches. However, we circadian (about 1 day). Such drifting rhythms are often rarely consider that the temporal structure of our planet of- termed “free-running’’ rhythms, and depending on the or- fers similar opportunities. Most organisms have evolved spe- ganism and on the nature of the constant conditions (e.g. cializations that allow them to exploit their environment in constant light or constant darkness), the endogenous period terms of both space and time, and this demands that indi- of the free-running rhythm can range from about 19 to 28 h.
    [Show full text]
  • Entrainment Concepts Revisited
    Entrainment Concepts Revisited Till Roenneberg,*,1 Roelof Hut,† Serge Daan,† and Martha Merrow† *Institute for Medical Psychology, University of Munich, Germany, †Centre for Behaviour and Neurosciences, University of Groningen, Haren, The Netherlands Abstract The traditional approaches to predict entrainment of circadian clocks by light are based on 2 concepts that were never successfully unified: the non- parametric approach assumes that entrainment occurs via discrete daily phase shifts while the parametric approach assumes that entrainment involves changes of the clock’s velocity. Here the authors suggest a new approach to predict and model entrainment. Unlike the traditional approaches, it does not assume a priori the mechanism of how the internal and external cycle lengths are matched (via phase shifts, velocity changes, or even other mechanisms). It is based on a circadian integrated response characteristic (CIRC) that describes how the cir- cadian system integrates light signals at different circadian phases, without specifying exactly when and how fast its progression is affected. Light around subjective dawn compresses the internal cycle; light around subjective dusk expands it. While the phase response curve (PRC) describes the results of experiments using light stimuli (of specified duration and intensity), the CIRC reflects how the system integrates any given light profile, be it single pulses or any form of light-dark cycle (from skeleton photoperiods to natural light pro- files). CIRCs are characterized by their shape (determining the extent of their dead zone) and their asymmetry (the ratio of its compressing and expanding portions). They are dimensionless (time/time), and their maximum is by defi- nition 1. To make predictions about entrainment, the CIRC is multiplied with the light intensity/sensitivity at any given time point.
    [Show full text]
  • Chronobiology and Psychiatry
    ARTICLE IN PRESS Sleep Medicine Reviews (2007) 11, 423–427 www.elsevier.com/locate/smrv GUEST EDITORIAL Chronobiology and psychiatry We can begin with the ancient Greeks, meander experience. It was as if there was suddenly a through Latin footnote-sprinkled chapters in the scientific language in which to express the clinical 1621 Anatomy of Melancholy, and dive into the observations, a framework for formulating experi- writings of late nineteenth century German psy- ments. Tom Wehr and I called it Pittendrighian chiatrists. All describe daily and seasonal cycles in psychiatry, attempting to understand underlying many aspects of mental illness. Depression in rules by which, for example, manic-depressive particular is linked to characteristic rhythm ab- patients shifted circadian phase earlier and later normalities: diurnal mood variation and early according to clinical state.8 According to our morning awakening, longer-term periodicities such theory, if we simply phase advanced the sleep– as regular manic-depressive cycles or seasonally wake cycle of a bipolar depressed patient, we linked episodes.1 But it was very difficult to could improve depression. Indeed, the effect of connect these clinical observations over the cen- this circadian manipulation was remarkable.9 The turies in any heuristic manner to an underlying patient’s diurnal rhythm of mood shifted day by biological timing system, lacking grounding in day across the window of waking, a kind of jet-lag circadian biology, a field which only began its with positive clinical response—but depre- modern development in the 1960s.2 One could ssion returned as she slowly re-entrained. We observe altered rhythms, but there was no theore- rephrased the Pittendrighian ‘‘internal coincidence tical construct to explain them.
    [Show full text]
  • SRBR 2008 Program Book
    20th Anniversary Meeting Society for Research on Biological Rhythms Program and Abstracts SRBR May 17–21, 2008 Sandestin Golf and Beach Resort Destin, Florida SOCIETY FOR RESEARCH ON BIOLOGICAL RHYTHMS I Executive Committee Animal Issues Committee Trainee Professional Development Day Martha Gillette, Ph.D., President Larry Morin, Ph.D., Chair Organizing Committee University of Illinois at Stony Brook University Urbana-Champaign Eric Bittman, Ph.D. Kenneth P. Wright Jr., Joseph Takahashi, Ph.D., President-elect University of Massachusetts Ph.D., Chair University of Colorado at Boulder Northwestern University Lance Kriegsfeld, Ph.D. Amita Sehgal, Ph.D., Treasurer University of California–Berkeley Nicolas Cermakian, Ph.D. McGill University University of Pennsylvania Laura Smale, Ph.D. David Weaver, Ph.D., Secretary Michigan State University Marian Comas University of Massachusetts Medical University of Groningen ChronoHistory Committee School Stephanie Crowley Members-at-Large Anna Wirz-Justice, Ph.D., Chair Brown University University of Basel Elizabeth B. Klerman, M.D., Ph.D. Charlotte Helfrich-Foerster, Ph.D. Josephine Arendt, Ph.D. Harvard Medical School University of Regensburg University of Surrey Luciano Marpegan, Ph.D. Martha Merrow, Ph.D. Eric Bittman, Ph.D. Washington University University of Groningen University of Massachusetts Michael J. Muskus Ignacio Provencio, Ph.D. Serge Daan, Ph.D. University of Missouri– University of Virginia University of Groningen Kansas City Ex-Officio Members Patricia DeCoursey, Ph.D. Ozgur Tataroglu University of South Carolina Heidelberg University Lawrence Morin, Ph.D., Comptroller Stony Brook University Jeffrey Hall, Ph.D. David Weaver, Ph.D. Brandeis University University of Massachusetts Medical William J. Schwartz, M.D., School Past President J.
    [Show full text]
  • Chronotype and Social Jetlag: a (Self-) Critical Review
    biology Review Chronotype and Social Jetlag: A (Self-) Critical Review Till Roenneberg 1,* , Luísa K. Pilz 1,2,3, Giulia Zerbini 1 and Eva C. Winnebeck 1 1 Institute of Medical Psychology, LMU Munich, 80336 Munich, Germany 2 Programa de Pós-Graduação em Psiquiatria e Ciências do Comportamento, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul (UFRGS), Porto Alegre 90035-003, Brazil 3 Laboratório de Cronobiologia e Sono, Hospital de Clínicas de Porto Alegre (HCPA), Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul (UFRGS), Porto Alegre 90035-903, Brazil * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +49-89-2180-75-650 Received: 7 May 2019; Accepted: 8 July 2019; Published: 12 July 2019 Abstract: The Munich ChronoType Questionnaire (MCTQ) has now been available for more than 15 years and its original publication has been cited 1240 times (Google Scholar, May 2019). Additionally, its online version, which was available until July 2017, produced almost 300,000 entries from all over the world (MCTQ database). The MCTQ has gone through several versions, has been translated into 13 languages, and has been validated against other more objective measures of daily timing in several independent studies. Besides being used as a method to correlate circadian features of human biology with other factors—ranging from health issues to geographical factors—the MCTQ gave rise to the quantification of old wisdoms, like “teenagers are late”, and has produced new concepts, like social jetlag. Some like the MCTQ’s simplicity and some view it critically. Therefore, it is time to present a self-critical view on the MCTQ, to address some misunderstandings, and give some definitions of the MCTQ-derived chronotype and the concept of social jetlag.
    [Show full text]
  • Clocks in Action: Exploring the Impact of Internal Time in Real Life
    Clocks in Action Exploring the impact of internal time in real life Céline Vetter Inaugural-Dissertation zur Erlangung des Doktorgrades der Philosophie an der Fakultät für Psychologie der Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München Vorgelegt von Céline Vetter aus München München, 7.Oktober 2010 Erstgutachter: Prof. Dr. Rainer Schandry Zweitgutachter: Prof. Dr. Till Roenneberg, Prof. Dr. Gisela Grupe Tag der mündlichen Prüfung: 3. Februar 2011 Table of Contents 1. GENERAL INTRODUCTION 8 1.1. THE CIRCADIAN CLOCK 9 1.1.1. A BRIEF HISTORY OF CLOCK RESEARCH 9 1.1.2. THE SUPRA-CHIASMATIC NUCLEUS (SCN) 11 1.1.3. SLEEP AND WAKE BEHAVIOUR 13 1.1.4. ENTRAINMENT 16 1.1.5. ZEITGEBER 19 1.1.6. CHRONOTYPE 23 1.2. THE CIRCADIAN CLOCK IN REAL LIFE 25 1.2.1. SOCIAL JETLAG 26 1.2.2. SHIFT WORK 27 1.2.3. ASSESSING CHRONOTYPE IN FIELD STUDIES 28 1.3. SCOPE OF THE RESEARCH 31 2. LIGHTS ON: TRACKING THE EFFECTS OF BLUE-ENRICHED LIGHT ON SLEEP, ACTIVITY AND WELLBEING IN OFFICE WORKERS 34 2.1. INTRODUCTION 34 2.1.1. THE EFFECT OF LIGHT ON HUMANS: LABORATORY STUDIES 35 2.1.2. THE INFLUENCE OF LIGHT IN OFFICE SETTINGS 37 2.1.3. RESEARCH AIM 37 2.2. MATERIALS AND METHODS 39 2.2.1. STUDY DESIGN 40 2.2.2. PARTICIPANTS 41 2.2.3. MATERIALS 42 2.2.4. DATA PROCESSING 43 2.2.5. STATISTICAL ANALYSES 45 2.3. RESULTS 46 2.3.1. SLEEP AND WAKE BEHAVIOUR 46 2.3.2. LOCO-MOTOR ACTIVITY 48 2.3.3.
    [Show full text]