Gossip in Evolutionary Perspective

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Gossip in Evolutionary Perspective Review of General Psychology Copyright 2004 by the Educational Publishing Foundation 2004, Vol. 8, No. 2, 100–110 1089-2680/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1089-2680.8.2.100 Gossip in Evolutionary Perspective R. I. M. Dunbar University of Liverpool Conversation is a uniquely human phenomenon. Analyses of freely forming conver- sations indicate that approximately two thirds of conversation time is devoted to social topics, most of which can be given the generic label gossip. This article first explores the origins of gossip as a mechanism for bonding social groups, tracing these origins back to social grooming among primates. It then asks why social gossip in this sense should form so important a component of human interaction and presents evidence to suggest that, aside from servicing social networks, a key function may be related explicitly to controlling free riders. Finally, the author reviews briefly the role of social cognition in facilitating conversations of this kind. For reasons that are not entirely clear, gossip To be able to make this claim, I need first to has acquired a decidedly shady reputation. It is step back in evolutionary time to what we might seen as malicious, destructive, and largely rep- see as the ancestral state from which modern rehensible. Describing a person as an “old gos- humans sprang. This is the nature of social sip” implies someone with more time on their relationships that pertain among our primate hands than they know what to do with, too cousins. I then argue that language evolved as a much hanging over the garden gate waiting for mechanism for bonding large social groups, and some passerby to pause for idle chat. To engage that it does so precisely because it allows us to in gossip is to speak ill of one’s fellows, to exchange information about the state of our interfere with the smooth running of the social social networks. In this context, the problem of relationships within which we are all embed- free riders (those who take the benefits of soci- ded: in a word, to undermine the very fabric of ality without paying the costs) is a central issue society. Yet, the term gossip itself did not orig- for which gossip provides a particularly power- inally have that meaning. It meant simply the ful mechanism of control. activity that one engaged in with one’s “god- sibs,” one’s peer group equivalent of godpar- Origins ents: in other words, those with whom one was especially close. Humans are members of the order primates, a Whether it is in fact the case that tale-telling large and diverse group of mammals of very and tittle-tattle are all that we do with our near- ancient lineage. We belong to that subgroup of est and dearest is, perhaps, a moot point. I want, primates known as the catarrhines, the Old instead, to focus on the broader nature of this World monkeys and apes. We share with these activity and argue that gossiping (though per- monkeys and apes a deep sociality that is pred- haps not gossip in its contemporary malicious icated on relatively (by comparison with other form) is the core of human social relationships, mammals and birds) advanced forms of social indeed of society itself. Without gossip, there cognition. This is represented in humans by would be no society. In short, gossip is what such phenomena as theory of mind (Astington, makes human society as we know it possible. 1993; Whiten, 1991; Wimmer & Perner, 1983) and more advanced forms of the intentional stance (Kinderman, Dunbar, & Bentall, 1998), which appear to be unique to humans (Call & Correspondence concerning this article should be ad- Tomasello, 1999; Tomasello & Call, 1998). dressed to R. I. M. Dunbar, Economic and Social Science (The “intentional stance” is the phenomenon of Research Council Research Centre in Economic Learning and Social Evolution, School of Biological Sciences, Uni- interpreting behavior in terms of the belief versity of Liverpool, Crown Street, Liverpool L69 7ZB, states of the mind that is behind the behavior, England. E-mail: [email protected] something that seems to be especially charac- 100 SPECIAL ISSUE: GOSSIP IN EVOLUTIONARY PERSPECTIVE 101 teristic of humans [Dennett, 1983].) Social cog- Living together exposes animals to a number of nition in these respects is based on the reflex- stresses that include disturbances to feeding ively hierarchical phenomenon of mind-read- when one animal displaces another from a feed- ing, the ability reflected in the claim that I ing site, harassment by more dominant individ- suppose that you believe that I want you to think uals, and the generally disruptive effects that that X is the case. arise from the fact that animals in social groups The extent to which other monkeys and apes are obliged to coordinate their behavior in ways share these particular capacities is not germane that are not always ideal for each individual. to the thesis of this article. The issue is more Because the consequences of predation are so that Old World monkey and ape social relation- final for the individual, the costs of sociality ships seem to be underpinned by relatively so- must be held in check to minimize their impact; phisticated forms of social cognition that find otherwise, the centrifugal forces of individuals’ their fullest expression in the reflexive multi- selfish demands will rapidly and inevitably re- level hierarchy of intentionality that we find in sult in the dispersal of the group. Sociality, in living humans. I return to this particular issue in short, demands compromise on one’s personal, the final section; first, I simply want to establish short-term objectives so that one gains in the why it is that the capacity to exchange informa- longer term through a greatly reduced risk of tion (i.e., language) evolved at all in humans. falling victim to a predator. Anthropoid primate societies (and especially The primate solution to this problem (essen- those of the Old World monkeys and apes) are tially the need to balance short-term interests characterized by an intensity of sociality that is against the longer term gains to be had through not as conspicuous in other species. These spe- group living) is the formation of alliances. Of- cies are deeply social; they are social in a sense ten, these alliances are deeply rooted in matri- that is all too readily apparent to anyone who lineal relationships (mothers and daughters, sis- has ever taken the trouble to spend any time ters). These relationships work (and are crucial observing their behavior. This intensity of soci- in the life histories of the animals themselves) ality depends on two key phenomena. One is the because they involve a strong element of trust ability to understand something of the workings and commitment. An alliance member can be of another’s mind. By this, I do not mean to relied on to come to one’s aid at the crucial suggest that monkeys and apes understand that moment when one is under attack. That sense of other individuals have minds in the way that we obligation is created through social grooming. humans take this for granted. Theirs is more the We do not really understand how grooming ethologist’s skill at reading behavior (Cheney & makes this possible, though we do know that Seyfarth, 1990). In effect, they are good at grooming is extremely effective at releasing understanding correlations in behavior: that in- endorphins (endogenous opiates produced nat- dividuals behave in certain consistent ways that urally by the brain as part of the body’s pain can vary widely from one individual to another, control system; Keverne, Martensz, & Tuite, that others’ behavior can be deliberately manip- 1989). The flood of opiates triggered by being ulated to one’s own advantage. groomed (and perhaps even by the act of The second feature underpinning primate so- grooming itself) generates a sense of relaxation ciality is the use of trust and (in a weak sense) (grooming lowers heart rate, reduces signs of obligation to ensure that relationships work ef- nervousness such as scratching, and can so relax fectively and do the job they are intended to do. the groomee that it may even fall asleep; For monkeys and apes, predators are the single Goosen, 1981). most important factor influencing group size: We are ourselves familiar with these effects, As a species’ ecological habits expose it to because we show a striking preference for re- increased risk of attack by predators, so its sorting to old-fashioned primate grooming in group size must be proportionately larger to our more intimate relationships; here language protect its members (for reviews, see Dunbar, is an inadequate means for communicating deep 1988, 1996a). Although protection against pred- inner feelings (and especially emotions), and we ators can be seen as clearly advantageous for often resort to physical contact forms of com- primates, it is important not to lose sight of the munication (rubbing, stroking, patting, and pet- fact that sociality has costs as well as benefits. ting) that are extremely effective at triggering 102 DUNBAR the release of endorphins. As the endorphins lent to the number of individuals one knows triggered by these behaviors begin to flood the personally; Dunbar, 1993; Hill & Dunbar, in body, we experience a rising sense of warmth, a press). At some point in our evolutionary his- feeling of peace with the world, of well-being tory, hominid groups began to push against the toward those with whom we share such experi- ceiling on group size. The only way they could ences of intimacy. The effect is instantaneous have broken through this ceiling so as to live in and direct: The physical stimulation of touch groups larger than about 80 individuals was to tells us more about the inner feelings of the find an alternative mechanism for bonding in “groomer,” and in a more direct way, than any which the available social time was used more words could possibly do.
Recommended publications
  • Effects of Hierarchical Steepness on Grooming Patterns in Female Tibetan Macaques (Macaca Thibetana)
    fevo-09-631417 February 27, 2021 Time: 15:50 # 1 ORIGINAL RESEARCH published: 04 March 2021 doi: 10.3389/fevo.2021.631417 Effects of Hierarchical Steepness on Grooming Patterns in Female Tibetan Macaques (Macaca thibetana) Dong-Po Xia1,2*†, Xi Wang2,3†, Paul A. Garber4,5, Bing-Hua Sun2,3, Lori K. Sheeran6, Lixing Sun7 and Jin-Hua Li2,3* 1 School of Life Sciences, Anhui University, Hefei, China, 2 International Collaborative Research Center for Huangshan Biodiversity and Tibetan Macaque Behavioral Ecology, Hefei, China, 3 School of Resources and Environmental Engineering, Anhui University, Hefei, China, 4 Department of Anthropology and Program in Ecology, Evolution, and Conservation Biology, University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign, Urbana, IL, United States, 5 International Centre of Biodiversity and Primate Conservation, Dali University, Dali, China, 6 Department of Anthropology and Museum Studies, Central Washington Edited by: University, Ellensburg, WA, United States, 7 Department of Biological Sciences, Central Washington University, Ellensburg, Lucja A. Fostowicz-Frelik, WA, United States Institute of Paleobiology (PAN), Poland Hierarchical steepness, defined as status asymmetries among conspecifics living in the Reviewed by: Małgorzata Arlet, same group, is not only used as a main characteristic of animal social relationships, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poland but also represents the degree of discrepancy between supply and demand within Jundong Tian, Zhengzhou University, China the framework of biological market theory. During September and December 2011, *Correspondence: we studied hierarchical steepness by comparing variation in grooming patterns in two Dong-Po Xia groups of Tibetan macaques (Macaca thibetana), a primate species characterized by [email protected] a linear dominance hierarchy.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolution of Food Sharing Behavior and Social Grooming Networks in Desmodus Rotundus with Agent-Based Models
    IN PRESS, UC DAVIS McNAIR SCHOLARS JOURNAL 2019 Evolution of Food Sharing Behavior and Social Grooming Networks in Desmodus rotundus with Agent-Based Models Alery Tenorio† Faculty Mentor: Jeffrey Schank‡ College of Biological Sciences†, College of Letters and Science‡ The common vampire bat, Desmodus rotundus, is an example of a non-primate social animal that shares food. Females live in social groups of 8-12, with their offspring and perform social behaviors, such as food sharing and social grooming. Vampire bats share food with other females in their social group by regurgitating their bloodmeals to certain group members. Vampire bats groom certain group members. While social grooming has been studied, its contribution to fitness is unknown. This behavior often occurs before food sharing, possibly to identify cheats, who accept food sharing but do not share. Excluding mother-daughter interactions, social grooming and food sharing usually occurs within subgroups of social groups. These subgroups within social groups are highly associated group members of 2 to 4 and their offspring. While many studies focus on entire social groups or populations, this paper lays the groundwork for investigating whether social grooming is a mechanism for assessing whether bats will share blood meals. The proposed investigation will be conducted by producing agent-based models of vampire bat social groups. These models represent individuals that behave in essential ways like vampire bats and will allow us to characterize by emergent behavior and population-level impacts of behavior. Our goal is to determine whether blood-meal sharing can evolve given or understand of the physiology, behavior, and population structure of vampire bats.
    [Show full text]
  • Anatomy Was Groomed. During a Focal Animal Sample 1974). Combining the Three Sampling Procedures A
    3 Patterns and Functions of Grooming Behavior among the Common Indian Langur Monkey James J. McKenna For many primate species grooming is a predomin- These data suggest, particularly with respect to the ant and socially important behavior pattern diverse question of functions, that while grooming no doubt both in the form it can take and the social situations serves both social and hygienic functions, it does so in a within which it is initiated. While not the only behavior context apparently dictated -by social needs. It is con- that often cuts across different age and sex classes to cluded that social grooming among langur monkeys is promote cohesion, the tremendous amount of time highly integrated into almost all aspects of group life and energy invested by some species in this activity and, while not a reliable behavioral index to status suggests that as a bond-building mechanism social differentials between group members, it is important grooming has played an auxiliary if not complemen- in assessing role complexes. tary role in the evolution of primate sociality. Among primates social grooming is thought by in- Materials and Methods vestigators to provide useful information on many dif- The study group was housed in a dome-shaped ferent aspects of group life. Grooming data has been mesh enclosure approximately 35 feet high by 45 feet used to measure developmental processes in the in diameter. Starting from the enclosure floor six dif- mother-infant relationship (Hinde 1974, Kaufman ferent levels ofsitting bars and platforms, interspersed and Rosenblum 1969), to document the growing net- every three to five feet, allowed proper social and work of attachments and subgroup formations within spatial refuge.
    [Show full text]
  • How Mammals Stay Healthy in Nature: the Evolution of Behaviours to Avoid Rstb.Royalsocietypublishing.Org Parasites and Pathogens
    How mammals stay healthy in nature: the evolution of behaviours to avoid rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org parasites and pathogens Benjamin L. Hart and Lynette A. Hart Review School of Veterinary Medicine, University of California, Davis, CA 95616, USA BLH, 0000-0003-2342-6058; LAH, 0000-0002-7133-6535 Cite this article: Hart BL, Hart LA. 2018 How mammals stay healthy in nature: the evolution Mammals live and thrive in environments presenting ongoing threats from of behaviours to avoid parasites and parasites in the form of biting flies, ticks and intestinal worms and from pathogens as wound contaminants and agents of infectious disease. Several pathogens. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 373: strategies have evolved that enable animals to deal with parasites and patho- 20170205. gens, including eliminating away from the sleeping–resting areas, use of an http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0205 array of grooming techniques, use of saliva in licking, and consuming med- icinal plant-based compounds. These strategies all are species-specific and Accepted: 2 April 2018 reflect the particular environment that the animal inhabits. This article is part of the Theo Murphy meeting issue ‘Evolution of pathogen and parasite avoidance behaviours’. One contribution of 14 to a Theo Murphy meeting issue ‘Evolution of pathogen and parasite avoidance behaviours’. 1. Introduction Subject Areas: From biting flies and ticks, to intestinal worms, to wound contaminants and to behaviour, health and disease and infectious diseases, animals living in nature are in an environment that presents epidemiology, evolution an ongoing threat to health and even survival from the standpoint of parasites and pathogens.
    [Show full text]
  • Population-Specific Social Dynamics in Chimpanzees COLLOQUIUM
    PAPER Population-specific social dynamics in chimpanzees COLLOQUIUM Edwin J. C. van Leeuwena,b,1, Katherine A. Croninc,d, and Daniel B. M. Haune,f,g aSchool of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, KY16 9JP St Andrews, Scotland; bMax Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, 6500 AH Nijmegen, The Netherlands; cLester E. Fisher Center for the Study and Conservation of Apes, Lincoln Park Zoo, Chicago, IL 60614; dCommittee on Evolutionary Biology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL 60637; eLeipzig Research Centre for Early Child Development, Faculty of Education, Leipzig University, 04109 Leipzig, Germany; fDepartment for Early Child Development and Culture, Faculty of Education, Leipzig University, 04109 Leipzig, Germany; and gMax Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, 04103 Leipzig, Germany Edited by Douglas L. Medin, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, and approved February 27, 2018 (received for review January 2, 2018) Understanding intraspecific variation in sociality is essential for as a pressing question in the study of psychological and behavioral characterizing the flexibility and evolution of social systems, yet its diversity: Beyond isolated accounts of tradition formation in non- study in nonhuman animals is rare. Here, we investigated whether human animals, is there any indication that nonhuman animals chimpanzees exhibit population-level differences in sociality that exhibit intraspecific population-level variation in their everyday so- cannot be easily explained by differences in genetics or ecology. We cial interactions that might be instigated by cultural processes? compared social proximity and grooming tendencies across four One seminal case demonstrating the plausibility of learned, semiwild populations of chimpanzees living in the same ecological population-level differences in sociality was reported by Sapolsky Papio anubis environment over three consecutive years, using both linear mixed and Share (31) in their study of olive baboons ( ).
    [Show full text]
  • UNIVERSITY of CALIFORNIA RIVERSIDE Causes And
    UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA RIVERSIDE Causes and Consequences of Parasitism in the California Fiddler Crab, Uca Crenulata A Dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Evolution, Ecology, and Organismal Biology by Adrienne Brooke Mora December 2013 Dissertation Committee: Dr. Marlene Zuk, Chairperson Dr. Daphne Fairbairn Dr. Bradley Mullens Copyright by Adrienne Brooke Mora 2013 The Dissertation of Adrienne Brooke Mora is approved: _______________________________________________ _______________________________________________ _______________________________________________ Committee Chairperson University of California, Riverside ! ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I owe a large debt of gratitude to the many people who contributed to the completion of this dissertation. First and foremost, I would like to thank my advisor, Dr. Marlene Zuk. Marlene, you always encouraged me to push myself and taught me to develop my own voice and mind. You were also a strong advocate for my research, and with your support I was inspired to reach beyond my comfort zone and take on new challenges that greatly contributed to my intellectual and personal growth. I would also like to thank Dr. Daphne Fairbairn for her help and guidance throughout my graduate career. Daphne, you gave me great insights into the life of an academic, and helped me learn to think critically and set high standards for myself. When my working space was jeopardized, you jumped in and offered your lab space to me. I am forever grateful to you for your support during this time, as I would not have been able to complete my experiments were it not for you. I consider both you and Marlene to be great role models.
    [Show full text]
  • Grooming As an Agonistic Behavior in Garnett’S Small-Eared Bushbaby (Otolemur Garnettii)
    The University of Southern Mississippi The Aquila Digital Community Master's Theses Spring 5-2017 Grooming as an Agonistic Behavior in Garnett’s Small-Eared Bushbaby (Otolemur garnettii) Jennie L. Christopher University of Southern Mississippi Follow this and additional works at: https://aquila.usm.edu/masters_theses Part of the Animal Studies Commons, Comparative Psychology Commons, Experimental Analysis of Behavior Commons, and the Social Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Christopher, Jennie L., "Grooming as an Agonistic Behavior in Garnett’s Small-Eared Bushbaby (Otolemur garnettii)" (2017). Master's Theses. 289. https://aquila.usm.edu/masters_theses/289 This Masters Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by The Aquila Digital Community. It has been accepted for inclusion in Master's Theses by an authorized administrator of The Aquila Digital Community. For more information, please contact [email protected]. GROOMING AS AN AGONISTIC BEHAVIOR IN GARNETT’S SMALL-EARED BUSHBABY (OTOLEMUR GARNETTII) by Jennie L. Christopher A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate School and the Department of Psychology at The University of Southern Mississippi in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Science Approved: ________________________________________________ Dr. Heidi Lyn, Committee Chair Assistant Professor, Psychology ________________________________________________ Dr. Alen Hajnal, Committee Member Associate Professor, Psychology ________________________________________________ Dr. Richard Mohn,
    [Show full text]
  • Pan Troglodytes)
    University of Tennessee, Knoxville TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange Masters Theses Graduate School 12-2005 Exploring Behavior and Social Relationships of a Captive Group of Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Crystal E. Anderson University of Tennessee - Knoxville Follow this and additional works at: https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_gradthes Part of the Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Anderson, Crystal E., "Exploring Behavior and Social Relationships of a Captive Group of Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). " Master's Thesis, University of Tennessee, 2005. https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_gradthes/579 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. It has been accepted for inclusion in Masters Theses by an authorized administrator of TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. For more information, please contact [email protected]. To the Graduate Council: I am submitting herewith a thesis written by Crystal E. Anderson entitled "Exploring Behavior and Social Relationships of a Captive Group of Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)." I have examined the final electronic copy of this thesis for form and content and recommend that it be accepted in partial fulfillment of the equirr ements for the degree of Master of Arts, with a major in Psychology. Richard Saudargas, Major Professor We have read this thesis and recommend its acceptance: Gordon Burghardt, Debora Baldwin Accepted for the Council: Carolyn R. Hodges Vice Provost and Dean of the Graduate School (Original signatures are on file with official studentecor r ds.) To the Graduate Council: I am submitting herewith a thesis written by Crystal Elizabeth Anderson entitled "Exploring Behavior and Social Relationships ofa Captive Group ofChimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)." I have examined the final paper copy ofthis thesis for form and content and recommend that it be accepted in partial fulfillment ofthe requirements for the degree ofMaster ofArts, with a major in Psychology.
    [Show full text]
  • Females, Food, Family, and Friendship
    Evolutionary Anthropology 85 NEWS Females, Food, Family, and Friendship or much of the last twenty ances, female philopatry, well-differ- student in the late 1970s, I was ad- years, females have occupied entiated female relationships, and vised by a well-respected primatolo- Fcenter stage in theoretical and male dispersal. On the other hand, gist not to study the sources of varia- empirical analyses of primate social when within-group contest competi- tion in reproductive success among organization. Richard Wrangham1 tion is weak and between-group con- female bonnet macaques because I was one of the first to give females top test competition is strong, we expect wouldn’t find any. But I did, and so billing, when he suggested that social- to see egalitarian dominance relation- have many primatologists since ity has evolved in primates because it ships among females, group-level co- then.10–13 enhances females’ access to resources. alitions against other groups, female If female reproductive success var- Female nutritional needs drive the philopatry, and poorly differentiated ies, then we can ask how the form and plot because female fitness is largely female relationships.4 features of females’ social relation- determined by their access to re- Socioecological models shine the ships contribute to variability in their sources, while male fitness is mainly spotlight on females, and primatolo- fitness.14 Widespread (but not univer- affected by their access to receptive gists have spent much of the last sal) evidence that female dominance females. This means that females dis- twenty years collecting data on female rank influences reproductive perfor- tribute themselves in space according life histories, feeding behavior, and mance provides the first clue that so- to how their food is distributed, and social relationships.
    [Show full text]
  • Coping with Acute Crowding by Cebus Apella
    International Journal of Primatology, Vol. 27, No. 5, October 2006 (C 2006) DOI: 10.1007/s10764-006-9070-z Coping with Acute Crowding by Cebus apella Megan L. van Wolkenten,1,2,3 Jason M. Davis,1 May Lee Gong,1 and Frans B. M. de Waal1,2 Received September 29, 2005; accepted November 16, 2005 Published Online October 18, 2006 Traditionally, scientists believed that crowded housing conditions increased aggression in animals. Recent research on captive primates fails to support a linear relationship between population density and aggressive behavior. Our study is the first to investigate the effects of acute crowding on a New World primate. We observed brown capuchins (Cebus apella) under 2 different spatial conditions: a control that offered the full indoor space in the enclosure and a short-term experimental condition that restricted the capuchins to ap- proximately half the amount of space of the control condition. We compared rates of self-grooming, aggression, contact sitting, social grooming, and play between both conditions. We conducted a mixed factorial ANOVA at the individual level while taking subject sex and age class into account. Intense aggression, play, and social grooming decreased significantly in crowded conditions, suggesting that capuchins avoid social encounters if spatially confined. Their strategy for coping with acute crowding via a decrease in all forms of social behavior is intermediate between chimpanzee and macaque strategies in similar experiments. The only behavior to increase under crowding was a nonsocial, self-directed behavior with the potential of reducing arousal: self-grooming. KEY WORDS: aggression; Cebus apella; coping model; crowding; husbandry.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effects of Parasites on Human Behaviour: an Evolutionary Perspective
    ISSN 2029-8587 PROBLEMS OF PSYCHOLOGY IN THE 21st CENTURY Volume 5, 2013 46 THE EFFECTS OF PARASITES ON HUMAN BEHAVIOUR: AN EVOLUTIONARY PERSPECTIVE Pavol Prokop Trnava University, Trnava, Slovakia; Institute of Zoology, Slovak Academy of Sciences, Bratislava, Slovakia E-mail: [email protected] Peter Fedor Comenius University, Bratislava, Slovakia E-mail: [email protected] Abstract Parasites and predators possess serious threats to humans throughout our evolutionary history. Although the impact of predators in modern world is lower than previously, parasites still infl uence morbidity and mortality of contemporary humans. From the evolutionary perspective, selective pressures caused by parasites should favour individuals that successfully avoid objects/subjects which transmit parasitic diseases and/or those who are successful in combats with them. There is growing evidence for the existence of evolved anti-parasite behaviours in animals, beginning with invertebrates and ending with primates. Avoidance of infected individu- als, grooming and self-medication are most common behavioural strategies that are believed to reduce the transmission of parasites. Here we review recent evidence suggesting that several behavioural and emotional traits in humans evolved as a response to selective pressure caused by parasites. Disgust sensitivity and patho- gen threat may be a mechanism that generates both a cross-cultural variation in preferences for certain facial traits, as well as drivers of religious diversity, political stability of countries, parenting cognitive abilities and prevalence of extraversion and openness to new ideas. Finally, we declare areas for a future research based on questions guided by the evolutionary perspective. Key words: evolution, human, parasites, psychology. Introduction Archaeological evidence had revealed that organisms in the world change over time.
    [Show full text]
  • Co-Evolution of Neocortex Size, Group Size and Language in Humans
    Co-evolution of neocortex size, group size and language in humans http://www.bbsonline.org/documents/a/00/00/05/65/bbs000005... Below is the unedited preprint (not a quotable final draft) of: Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4): 681-735. The final published draft of the target article, commentaries and Author's Response are currently available only in paper. € For information about subscribing or purchasing offprints of the published version, with commentaries and author's response, write to: [email protected] (North America) or [email protected] (All other countries). € CO-EVOLUTION OF NEOCORTEX SIZE, GROUP SIZE AND LANGUAGE IN HUMANS R.I.M. Dunbar Human Evolutionary Biology Research Group Department of Anthropology University College London London WC1E 6BT Keywords Neocortical size, group size, humans, language, Macchiavellian Intelligence Abstract Group size is a function of relative neocortical volume in nonhuman primates. Extrapolation from this regression equation yields a predicted group size for modern humans very similar to that of certain hunter-gatherer and traditional horticulturalist societies. Groups of similar size are also found in other large-scale forms of contemporary and historical society. Among primates, the cohesion of groups is maintained by social grooming; the time devoted to social grooming is linearly related to group size among the Old World monkeys and apes. To maintain the stability of the large groups characteristic of humans by grooming alone would place intolerable demands on time budgets. It is suggested that (1) the evolution of large groups in the human lineage depended on the development of a more efficient method for time-sharing the processes of social bonding and that (2) language uniquely fulfills this requirement.
    [Show full text]