Toward an Ethology of Human Conflict: a Review

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Toward an Ethology of Human Conflict: a Review Discussions and Reviews Toward an ethology of human conflict: a review Konrad Lorenz, On Aggression London: Methuen, 1966. Pp. 306. $5.75. Robert Ardrey, African Genesis New York: Atheneum, 1961. Pp. 380. $6.95. Robert Ardrey, The Territorial Imperative New York: Atheneum, 1966. Pp. 390. $6.95. Claire Russell and W. M. S. Russell, Human Behavior—A New Approach Boston: Little, Brown, 1961. Pp. 532. PETER M. DRIVER Mental Health Research Institute, University of Michigan The study of human conflict seems to siderable impact. Those of Ardrey and that labor under three major difficulties: the dis- of Lorenz are best-sellers, and the Russells’ like of the majority of people for viewing book had an enthusiastic reception among man in his evolutionary perspective; the the members of that small ingroup of biolo- tendency of any specialist to consider his gists and humanists forming a tenuous bridge own speciality as more relevant than any between the &dquo;two cultures.&dquo; When taken other applied to a problem of common in- together-though such a mixture is very terest ; and the universal stumbling-block of strong medicine-they indicate a significant man’s sheer lack of ability to deal success- (albeit controversial) development in the fully with his social problems at the com- study of man as an organism, that is, as an munity level. All of the four books under animal which has evolved, like other ani- review at least manage to avoid the first mals, by mechanisms of natural selection. difficulty, and one of them brings powerful Unfortunately, unlike other animals, man argument to play upon the other two. now seems to be developing characteristics These four books have already had con- of &dquo;unnatural&dquo; selection which give concern 362 to many people, including the authors of discipline of objective observation of be- these books and of this review. Certainly, havior is a much longer one. Charles Dar- no other animal exhibits anything like the win, of course, included an understanding degree of intraspecific killing and maiming of ethological principles in his battery of shown by our own species. Whether any of biological techniques. Nora Barlow, Dar- the extinct animals owed their extinction to win’s granddaughter and authority on his this kind of behavioral breakdown we shall writings, has pointed out in a personal com- probably never know, but what we must be munication that in his work on barnacles, clear about is that it is a behavioral break- which predated The Origin of Species, he down-a biological malfunction-and that was all the time &dquo;thinking of behavior, ex- the behavioral biologist-the ethologist-is actly how the animal lives, and its needs.&dquo; particularly fitted to study it. The same can be seen in his Journal off Ethology, as I have indicated, and as Researches (1839). More strictly ethological Lorenz himself (1965) has stated, may be were the studies on &dquo;instinct&dquo; carried out regarded as the biology of behavior. This by Douglas Spalding (Haldane, 1954), a description, however, needs clarification. contemporary of Darwin and, briefly, tutor Anyone studying the behavior of organisms to Bertrand Russell. But such studies-and is studying biological phenomena, yet many there were many others-were only a nat- students of behavior-psychologists, sociol- ural development of the British tradition of ogists, psychiatrists, anthropologists, and the scientific study of natural history, first so on-have little or only partial biological clearly revealed in Gilbert White’s classic, training. The ethologist has the distinction The Natural History of Selbourne ( 1789 ) . of being properly qualified, without obvious White was the first to make a clear dis- limits, for the study of his subject; he has tinction between the three common British training both in biological science and in species of leaf-warbler-and did it on the behavioral observation and analysis. The basis of their behavior. And his distinction ethologist tends to be a student of the whole is as valid today as it ever was. animal which, strictly speaking, is the nor- The work of Lorenz, of Ardrey, and of mal animal in its natural environment. To the Russells represents the natural develop- be a &dquo;pure&dquo; ethologist is obviously, there- ment of the earlier naturalists’ approach to fore, very difficult, and most ethologists animal behavior, only now it is scientific, compromise to some degree by putting with a rigorous methodology and standards some kind of restriction upon their animals. of quality equivalent to those of any other This need not, however, result in significant scientific discipline. At the same time it distortion of the animal’s behavior, and the must be noted that the work of Lorenz and ethological approach to behavior would of Ardrey attracts a good deal of fierce crit- seem to be the most objective and poten- icism from mathematically-oriented ethol- tially valuable of the behavioral sciences. ogists and psychologists. Such critics seem It should be mentioned that ethology is to feel that the Lorenzian type of ethology not just another &dquo;ism&dquo; among the plethora is unscientific, apparently because much of of recently-emerged subdisciplines of nat- the data it collects cannot be subjected to ural science. Although the term &dquo;ethology&dquo; the processes of mathematical analysis found -in its present sense-was first used in to be useful in other types of behavior English by W. M. Wheeler as recently as study. I believe that this criticism may be 1902, the history of ethology as a rigorous an unwise one. As Slobodkin (1965) has CONFLICT RESOLUTION VOLUME XI NUMBER 3 363 pointed out, every empirical science has to It may have been unwise for Lorenz to develop its own standards of quality, and allow this statement to be printed on the &dquo;to insist on the theory of biology conform- dust-jacket of the book, but even so it should ing to aesthetic standards derived from not prejudice the reader against everything extant mathematics is illegitimate and is, in he says. Anyone who has seen Lorenz at fact, an imposition of metaphysical criteria work will know that he is very rigorous in- on the empirical world.&dquo; While all students deed, and can predict more accurately than of behavior must beware of &dquo;intellectual the majority of people what an animal will exercise carried out in the dark void of con- do next. He clearly has a fingerspitzengefiihl temporary ignorance of cerebral functions&dquo; -one of his favorite words, meaning (Walshe, 1957), the ethologist working with roughly, &dquo;fingertip sensitivity&dquo;-for animal animals under relatively natural conditions behavior study, and a fertile and broadly- can be confident that his activities will in- integrated mind. The Russells’ book pro- troduce a minimum of undetectable vari- vides evidence to suggest that certain kinds ables into his experiments. It is in this light of regressive thought process, notably ra- that the books here under review should tionalization, in the sense used by Freud, be considered. common to all of us-though to varying On Aggression, written by one of the degrees-are antipathetic to the broad ap- leading biologists and the most celebrated proach, and may be operative in the nega- ethologist of our time, has been reviewed tive reactions to Lorenz’s work. by a number of eminent people from several On Aggression is a book which must be fields. The variety of response to the book taken very seriously, as it provides a power- is of interest in itself, and though it cannot ful thesis to explain the behavioral break- be considered here in detail, it is worth down which has resulted in the deaths of noting that those who approve are largely more than sixty million humans since 1820 experts in other fields. These include Alsop as a result of internecine strife. Basically (1966), Gorer (1966), Koestler (1966), this thesis is that aggression in the proper and Mead (1966). Those against-all work- sense of the word is intraspecific aggression, ing somewhere in the field of animal be- and normally fulfills a species-preserving havior-are, principally, Barnett (1967), function. The fantastic aggression we see Schneirla (1966), and Zuckerman (1966). in humans today is aggression gone wrong, I believe that this antagonism from other out of hand, and now resembling in its re- behavioral scientists is only partly explained sults the predator/prey combats in which by Lorenz’ tendency to make broad sweep- the predator’s behavior has been evolved for the of the Intra- ing assertions, e.g., &dquo;I am honestly con- purpose killing prey. is in vinced that in the near future very many specific aggression normally (that is, most or all animals other than men-indeed perhaps the majority of man- man) only similar to behavior, kind-will regard as obvious and banal superficially predator which is both business and truth all that I have written in this book pleasure- not war. &dquo;The buffalo which the about intra-specific aggression and the hunting, lion fells provokes his aggression as little dangers which its perversion entails for as the which I have humanity.&dquo; appetizing turkey just seen hanging in the larder provokes mine. The difference in these inner drives can 1 For further information see Letters, Scien- tific American, May 1967, and Driver, 1967. clearly be seen in the expression movements 364 of the animal: a dog about to catch a that of intraspecific aggression. If the latter hunted rabbit has the same kind of excitedly is a fundamental characteristic of animal happy expression as he has when he greets organization, then we must be able to pos- his master or awaits some longed-for treat.
Recommended publications
  • Man Meets Dog Konrad Lorenz 144 Pages Konrad Z
    a How, why and when did man meet dog and cat? How much are they in fact guided by instinct, and what sort of intelligence have they? What is the nature of their affection or attachment to the human race? Professor Lorenz says that some dogs are descended from wolves and some from jackals, with strinkingly different results in canine personality. These differences he explains in a book full of entertaining stories and reflections. For, during the course of a career which has brought him world fame as a scientist and as the author of the best popular book on animal behaviour, King Solomon's Ring, the author has always kept and bred dogs and cats. His descriptions of dogs 'with a conscience', dogs that 'lie', and the fallacy of the 'false cat' are as amusing as his more thoughtful descriptions of facial expressions in dogs and cats and their different sorts of loyalty are fascinating. "...this gifted and vastly experiences naturalist writes with the rational sympathy of the true animal lover. He deals, in an entertaining, anectdotal way, with serious problems of canine behaviour." The Times Educational Supplement "...an admirable combination of wisdom and wit." Sunday Observer Konrad Lorenz Man Meets Dog Konrad Lorenz 144 Pages Konrad Z. Lorenz, born in 1903 in Vienna, studied Medicine and Biology. Rights Sold: UK/USA, France, In 1949, he founded the Institute for Comparative Behaviourism in China (simplified characters), Italy, Altenberg (Austria) and changed to the Max-Planck-Institute in 1951. Hungary, Romania, Korea, From 1961 to 1973, he was director of Max-Planck-Institute for Ethology Slovakia, Spain, Georgia, Russia, in Seewiesen near Starnberg.
    [Show full text]
  • The Dublin Gate Theatre Archive, 1928 - 1979
    Charles Deering McCormick Library of Special Collections Northwestern University Libraries Dublin Gate Theatre Archive The Dublin Gate Theatre Archive, 1928 - 1979 History: The Dublin Gate Theatre was founded by Hilton Edwards (1903-1982) and Micheál MacLiammóir (1899-1978), two Englishmen who had met touring in Ireland with Anew McMaster's acting company. Edwards was a singer and established Shakespearian actor, and MacLiammóir, actually born Alfred Michael Willmore, had been a noted child actor, then a graphic artist, student of Gaelic, and enthusiast of Celtic culture. Taking their company’s name from Peter Godfrey’s Gate Theatre Studio in London, the young actors' goal was to produce and re-interpret world drama in Dublin, classic and contemporary, providing a new kind of theatre in addition to the established Abbey and its purely Irish plays. Beginning in 1928 in the Peacock Theatre for two seasons, and then in the theatre of the eighteenth century Rotunda Buildings, the two founders, with Edwards as actor, producer and lighting expert, and MacLiammóir as star, costume and scenery designer, along with their supporting board of directors, gave Dublin, and other cities when touring, a long and eclectic list of plays. The Dublin Gate Theatre produced, with their imaginative and innovative style, over 400 different works from Sophocles, Shakespeare, Congreve, Chekhov, Ibsen, O’Neill, Wilde, Shaw, Yeats and many others. They also introduced plays from younger Irish playwrights such as Denis Johnston, Mary Manning, Maura Laverty, Brian Friel, Fr. Desmond Forristal and Micheál MacLiammóir himself. Until his death early in 1978, the year of the Gate’s 50th Anniversary, MacLiammóir wrote, as well as acted and designed for the Gate, plays, revues and three one-man shows, and translated and adapted those of other authors.
    [Show full text]
  • Coping with CROWDING by Frans B
    POPULATION GROWTH has been thought, since the time of Thomas Malthus, to produce dire consequences such as disease, scarcity and social deviancy. This dark view seemed confirmed by rodent studies. Yet little evi- dence suggests that people are similarly affected: we seem to handle large crowds quite well for the most part. Coping with CROWDING by Frans B. M. de Waal, Filippo Aureli and Peter G. Judge 76 Scientific American May 2000 Coping with Crowding Copyright 2000 Scientific American, Inc. n 1962 this magazine published a seminal But, one could argue, perhaps such a re- paper by experimental psychologist John lation is obscured by variation in national IB. Calhoun entitled “Population Density income level, political organization or some and Social Pathology.” The article opened other variable. Apparently not, at least for dramatically with an observation by the late- income. We divided the nations into three 18th-century English demographer Thomas categories—free-market, former East Block Malthus that human population growth is and Third World—and did the analysis automatically followed by increased vice and again. This time we did find one significant misery. Calhoun went on to note that al- correlation, but it was in the other direc- though we know overpopulation causes dis- tion: it showed more violent crime in the ease and food shortage, we understand virtu- least crowded countries of the former East ally nothing about its behavioral impact. Block. A similar trend existed for free-mar- This reflection had inspired Calhoun to ket nations, among which the U.S. had by conduct a nightmarish experiment.
    [Show full text]
  • On the Aggression Diffusion Modeling and Minimization in Online Social
    On the Aggression Diffusion Modeling and Minimization in Twitter MARINOS POIITIS, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece ATHENA VAKALI, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece NICOLAS KOURTELLIS, Telefonica Research, Spain Aggression in online social networks has been studied mostly from the perspective of machine learning which detects such behavior in a static context. However, the way aggression diffuses in the network has received little attention as it embeds modeling challenges. In fact, modeling how aggression propagates from one user to another, is an important research topic since it can enable effective aggression monitoring, especially in media platforms which up to now apply simplistic user blocking techniques. In this paper, we address aggression propagation modeling and minimization in Twitter, since it is a popular microblogging platform at which aggression had several onsets. We propose various methods building on two well-known diffusion models, Independent Cascade (퐼퐶) and Linear Threshold (!) ), to study the aggression evolution in the social network. We experimentally investigate how well each method can model aggression propagation using real Twitter data, while varying parameters, such as seed users selection, graph edge weighting, users’ activation timing, etc. It is found that the best performing strategies are the ones to select seed users with a degree-based approach, weigh user edges based on their social circles’ overlaps, and activate users according to their aggression levels. We further employ the best performing models to predict which ordinary real users could become aggressive (and vice versa) in the future, and achieve up to 퐴*퐶=0.89 in this prediction task. Finally, we investigate aggression minimization by launching competitive cascades to “inform” and “heal” aggressors.
    [Show full text]
  • The Ethological Approach to Aggression1
    Psychological Medicine, 1980,10, 607-609 Printed in Great Britain EDITORIAL The ethological approach to aggression1 There are several ways in which ethology, the biological study of behaviour, can contribute to our understanding of aggression. First of all, by studying animals we can get some idea of what aggression is and how it relates to other, similar patterns of behaviour. We can also begin to grasp the reasons why natural selection has led to the widespread appearance of apparently destructive behaviour. To come to grips with this question requires the study of animals in their natural environment, for this is the environment to which they are adapted and only in it can the survival value of their behaviour be appreciated. By contrast, a final contribution of ethology, that of understanding the causal mechanisms underlying aggression, usually comes from work in the laboratory, for such research requires carefully controlled experiments. Ethology has shed light on all these topics, perhaps parti- cularly in the 15 years since Konrad Lorenz, one of the founding fathers of the discipline, discussed the subject in his book On Aggression. Lorenz's views were widely publicized and had great popular appeal: the fact that few ethologists agreed with what he said may explain why so many have since devoted time to studying aggression. The result, as I shall argue here, is that ethologists now have a much better insight into what aggression is, how it is caused and what functions it serves, and it is an insight sharply at odds with the ideas put forward by Lorenz.
    [Show full text]
  • Human Origins Studies: a Historical Perspective
    Evo Edu Outreach (2010) 3:314–321 DOI 10.1007/s12052-010-0248-7 ORIGINAL SCIENTIFIC ARTICLE Human Origins Studies: A Historical Perspective Tom Gundling Published online: 29 July 2010 # Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010 Abstract Research into the deep history of the human within the field of paleoanthropology, but rather to identify species is a relatively young science which can be divided broad patterns and highlight a collection of “events” that into two broad periods. The first spans the century between are most germane in shaping current understanding of our the publication of Darwin’s Origin and the end of World evolutionary origin. These events naturally include the War II. This period is characterized by the recovery of the accretion of fossil material, the raw data which is the direct, first non-modern human fossils and subsequent attempts at if mute, testimony of the past. These fossil discoveries are reconstructing family trees as visual representations of the situated among technological breakthroughs, theoretical transition from ape to human. The second period, from shifts, and changes in the sociocultural context in which 1945 to the present, is marked by a dramatic upsurge in the human origins studies were conducted. It is only through quantity of research, with a concomitant increase in such a contextualized historical approach that we can truly specialization. During this time, emphasis shifted from grasp our current understanding of human origins. Foibles classification of fossil humans to paleoecology in which of the past remind us to be critical in assessing newly hominids were seen as parts of complex evolving ecosys- produced knowledge, yet simultaneously we can genuinely tems.
    [Show full text]
  • Primate Aggression and Evolution: an Overview of Sociobiological and Anthropological Perspectives JAMES J
    Primate Aggression and Evolution: An Overview of Sociobiological and Anthropological Perspectives JAMES J. McKENNA Attempts to explain the nature and causes of human aggression are hand­ icapped primarily because aggression is anything but a unitary concept. Aggression has no single etiology, no matter which mammalian species we consider or what kind of causation (developmental or evolutionary) we stress. Nevertheless, forensic psychiatrists are asked to evaluate instances of human aggression in ways that would send shivers up the spines of researchers who have been wrestling with the issue for over fifty years. This is not to say forensic psychiatry should be abolished nor to suggest be­ havioral scientists have not made progress in discovering causes of species aggression in genera}l and human violence in particular.2 But especially when predictive models are considered it does mean we are far from achiev­ ing highly reliable results.:l Particularly when one person is asked to assess the motivational state of another who has committed a serious aggressive act it becomes more evident just how much more data we need. Strangely, if a forensic psychia­ trist were asked to testify in a case in which, let us say, one monkey attacked another, the testimony would be based on more complete information than a case involving a human. This is because a plethora of context-specific data on nonhuman primates are available. These data illuminate a wide range of social, ecological, and endocrinological circumstances under which animals will be expected to act aggressively. Data on humans are much more complex, and sometimes they are absent altogether.
    [Show full text]
  • Mccarthy Era and the American Theatre Author(S): Albert Wertheim Source: Theatre Journal, Vol
    The McCarthy Era and the American Theatre Author(s): Albert Wertheim Source: Theatre Journal, Vol. 34, No. 2, Insurgency in American Theatre, (May, 1982), pp. 211 -222 Published by: The Johns Hopkins University Press Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/3207451 Accessed: 16/04/2008 16:42 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=jhup. Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. JSTOR is a not-for-profit organization founded in 1995 to build trusted digital archives for scholarship. We enable the scholarly community to preserve their work and the materials they rely upon, and to build a common research platform that promotes the discovery and use of these resources. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. http://www.jstor.org The McCarthy Era and the American Theatre Albert Wertheim Eric Bentley's Thirty Years of Treason, Lillian Hellman's Scoundrel Time, Lately Thomas's When Even Angels Wept, and Robert Goldston's The American Nightmare are only a few of the many studies that have been written about that unsettling and aberrant period of recent American history frequently known as the McCarthy era.1 The very titles of the books tell us immediately with what loathing and shame most Americans now look back to that time of political paranoia.
    [Show full text]
  • Warfare in an Evolutionary Perspective
    Received: 26 November 2018 Revised: 7 May 2019 Accepted: 18 September 2019 DOI: 10.1002/evan.21806 REVIEW ARTICLE Warfare in an evolutionary perspective Bonaventura Majolo School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Sarah Swift Building, Lincoln, UK Abstract The importance of warfare for human evolution is hotly debated in anthropology. Correspondence Bonaventura Majolo, School of Psychology, Some authors hypothesize that warfare emerged at least 200,000–100,000 years BP, University of Lincoln, Sarah Swift Building, was frequent, and significantly shaped human social evolution. Other authors claim Brayford Wharf East, Lincoln LN5 7AT, UK. Email: [email protected] that warfare is a recent phenomenon, linked to the emergence of agriculture, and mostly explained by cultural rather than evolutionary forces. Here I highlight and crit- ically evaluate six controversial points on the evolutionary bases of warfare. I argue that cultural and evolutionary explanations on the emergence of warfare are not alternative but analyze biological diversity at two distinct levels. An evolved propen- sity to act aggressively toward outgroup individuals may emerge irrespective of whether warfare appeared early/late during human evolution. Finally, I argue that lethal violence and aggression toward outgroup individuals are two linked but distinct phenomena, and that war and peace are complementary and should not always be treated as two mutually exclusive behavioral responses. KEYWORDS aggression, competition, conflict, cooperation, peace, social evolution, violence, war 1 | INTRODUCTION and others on the importance of organized/cooperative actions among members of one social group against members of the opposing The question of whether humans are innately peaceful or aggressive group.5 Clearly, how we define warfare affects how deep we can go has fascinated scientists and philosophers for centuries.1,2 Wars, eth- back in time in human evolution to investigate its emergence and evo- nic or religious contests, and intra-group or intra-family violence are lutionary bases.
    [Show full text]
  • Law in Sociobiological Perspective
    Florida State University Law Review Volume 5 Issue 2 Article 2 Spring 1977 Law in Sociobiological Perspective Dr. Margaret Gruter Stanford University, Program in Human Biology Follow this and additional works at: https://ir.law.fsu.edu/lr Part of the Law and Society Commons Recommended Citation Dr. Margaret Gruter, Law in Sociobiological Perspective, 5 Fla. St. U. L. Rev. 181 (1977) . https://ir.law.fsu.edu/lr/vol5/iss2/2 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by Scholarship Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Florida State University Law Review by an authorized editor of Scholarship Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. LAW IN SOCIOBIOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVE DR. MARGARET GRUTER* I. INTRODUCTION This article is intended to acquaint legal scholars with recent findings in the biologically based behavioral sciences which may be relevant to an understanding of legal phenomena. The ideas expressed herein may stimulate further inquiries and research into the inter- action of law and behavior and may help bridge the gap between the natural sciences and the empirical studies of law. Legal research will keep pace more effectively with rapid changes in human society if the findings of the basic sciences are known and accepted by legal scholars. In view of the danger to the very continua- tion of the human species, due in part to the advancement in the natural sciences, legal research is of the utmost importance. Knowledge is neutral: what we do with it is crucial. Scientific research, with the help of law, could better our social order, but only if law builds on scientific research.
    [Show full text]
  • SCRAPBOOK of MOVIE STARS from the SILENT FILM and Early TALKIES Era
    CINEMA Sanctuary Books 790 - Madison Ave - Suite 604 212 -861- 1055 New York, NY 10065 [email protected] Open by appointment www.sanctuaryrarebooks.com Featured Items THE FIRST 75 ISSUES OF FILM CULTURE Mekas, Jonas (ed.). Film Culture. [The First 75 Issues, A Near Complete Run of "Film Culture" Magazine, 1955-1985.] Mekas has been called “the Godfather of American avant-garde cinema.” He founded Film Culture with his brother, Adolfas Mekas, and covered therein a bastion of avant-garde and experimental cinema. The much acclaimed, and justly famous, journal features contributions from Rudolf Arnheim, Peter Bogdanovich, Stan Brakhage, Arlene Croce, Manny Farber, David Ehrenstein, John Fles, DeeDee Halleck, Gerard Malanga, Gregory Markopoulos, Annette Michelson, Hans Richter, Andrew Sarris, Parker Tyler, Andy Warhol, Orson Welles, and many more. The first 75 issues are collected here. Published from 1955-1985 in a range of sizes and designs, our volumes are all in very good to fine condition. Many notable issues, among them, those designed by Lithuanian Fluxus artist, George Macunias. $6,000 SCRAPBOOK of MOVIE STARS from the SILENT FILM and early TALKIES era. Staple-bound heavy cardstock wraps with tipped on photo- illustration of Mae McAvoy, with her name handwritten beneath; pp. 28, each with tipped-on and hand-labeled film stills and photographic images of celebrities, most with tissue guards. Front cover a bit sunned, lightly chipped along the edges; internally bright and clean, remarkably tidy in its layout and preservation. A collection of 110 images of actors from the silent film and early talkies era, including Inga Tidblad, Mona Martensson, Corinne Griffith, Milton Sills, Norma Talmadge, Colleen Moore, Charlie Chaplin, Lillian Gish, and many more.
    [Show full text]
  • The Territorial Imperative
    Old School Ethological Model The blood-bespattered, slaughter-gutted archives of human history from the earliest Egyptian and Sumerian records to the most recent atrocities of the Second World War accord with early universal cannibalism, with animal and human sacrificial practices or their substitutes in formalized religions and with the world wide scalping, head-hunting, body-mutilating and necrophiliac practices of mankind in proclaiming this common blood lust differentiator, this mark of Cain that separates man dietetically from his anthropoidal relatives and allies him rather with the deadliest of Carnivora. Raymond Dart (1953) "The predatory transition from ape to man". International Anthropological and Linguistic Review I. Warfare and Evolution Old ethological & modern evolutionary models A strategic model of territoriality Chimpanzee coalitionary aggression Influential ethology books On Aggression, Konrad Lorenz (1966) African Genesis (1961), Robert Ardrey The Territorial Imperative, Robert Ardrey (1966) Lorenz's Hydraulic or Drive Discharge Theory: ¾ aggressive energy builds in the organism ¾ if the energy is not released the organism will be harmed ¾ the organism seeks a releasing mechanism (e.g., territorial intruder) ¾ aggression is expressed and the organism reestablishes equilibrium Reasoning behind the Phylogenetic Model: Humans are primates. Non-human primates are aggressive and territorial. Humans are aggressive and territorial. Non-human primate behavior is genetically determined. Therefore, human aggression, warfare, and territoriality are genetically determined. Strategic or conceptual models Under what conditions do we expect the behavior to manifest itself? What are the costs and benefits to the behavior? Does not require phylogenetic connections. Further, even if we find the behavior in closely related species or subspecies we would still have to explain the evolutionary logic behind the behavior.
    [Show full text]