Medial Prefrontal Cortex and Anterior Cingulate Cortex in the Generation of Alpha Activity Induced by Transcendental Meditation : a Magnetoencephalographic Study

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Medial Prefrontal Cortex and Anterior Cingulate Cortex in the Generation of Alpha Activity Induced by Transcendental Meditation : a Magnetoencephalographic Study Acta Med. Okayama, 2006 Vol. 60, No. 1, pp. 51ン58 CopyrightⒸ 2006 by Okayama University Medical School. Original Article http ://www.lib.okayama-u.ac.jp/www/acta/ Medial Prefrontal Cortex and Anterior Cingulate Cortex in the Generation of Alpha Activity Induced by Transcendental Meditation : A Magnetoencephalographic Study Shin Yamamotoa,c*, Yoshihiro Kitamurab,c, Norihito Yamadac, Yoshihiko Nakashimad, and Shigetoshi Kurodac aMakoto Clinic, Okayama 700ン0813, Japan, bOkayama Ryogo Center, Okayama 700ン0927, Japan, cDepartment of Neuropsychiatry, Okayama University Graduate School of Medicine, Dentistry and Pharmaceutical Sciences, Okayama 700ン8558, Japan, and dSanyo Hospital, Okayama 702ン8006, Japan Previous EEG studies have shown that transcendental meditation (TM) increases frontal and central alpha activity. The present study was aimed at identifying the source of this alpha activity using magnetoencephalography (MEG) and electroencephalography (EEG) simultaneously on eight TM practitioners before, during, and after TM. The magnetic fi eld potentials corresponding to TM-induced alpha activities on EEG recordings were extracted, and we attempted to localize the dipole sources using the multiple signal classifi cation (MUSIC) algorithm, equivalent current dipole source analysis, and the multiple spatio-temporal dipole model. Since the dipoles were mapped to both the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), it is suggested that the mPFC and ACC play an important role in brain activity induced by TM. Key words : transcendental meditation, magnetoencephalography (MEG), source analysis, medial prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex ranscendental meditation (TM) is a mental (MEG) has been reported analyzing the brain activity T stress reduction technique in which the practi- of a Yoga Master during meditation [3]. MEG has tioner closes his or her eyes and silently repeats a several theoretical advantages, including high spatial “mantra”, a meaningless sequence of sounds specifi c and temporal resolution, in identifying the localiza- to each individual, to promote a natural shift of tion of brain dipoles [4, 5]. Therefore, the use of awareness to a wakeful but deeply restful state [1]. MEG off ers a new approach to the recording of EEG studies have shown that TM increases frontal TM-related brain activity. and central alpha activity consistent with wakefulness In the present study, we used multichannel [2]. SQUID (superconducting quantum interference Recently, a study using magnetoencephalography device) magnetometers to extract the magnetic fi eld potentials that corresponded to TM-induced activities on EEG recordings, in an attempt to identify and Received August 10, 2005 ; accepted October 6, 2005. *Corresponding author. Phone : +81ン86ン231ン2500 ; Fax : +81ン86ン231ン2521 localize the sources of the brain activities during E-mail : [email protected] (S. Yamamoto) practice of TM. 52 Yamamoto et al. Acta Med. Okayama Vol. 60, No. 1 Materials and Methods EEG recorder (SYNAFIT 5000). Topographical maps during these tasks were made by fast Fourier Subjects. Sixteen healthy subjects (8 men and transformation (FFT) every 30 sec, which enabled 8 women ; mean age 38.8 years old, range 24ン61 us to confi rm the change in distribution of ク waves years old) participated in the study. We obtained and the state of consciousness of the subjects. The their written informed consent prior to performing EEG topographies consisted of 6 frequency the experiments. bands : δ(2ン4 Hz), θ(4ン8), ク1(8ン10), ク2(10ン13), ケ1(13 The study group consisted of 8 volunteers, aged ン20), and ケ2(20ン30). 24 to 61 years old (4 men and 4 women ; mean age Analysis of EEG variables. The ク power, 39.0 years old), who had been practicing TM regu- index (content percentage), and mean frequency were larly for an average period of 3.9 years. The control obtained before and during TM practice using a soft- group comprised 8 persons aged 25 to 61 years old (4 ware for EEG mapping, ‘Atalas’ (Kissei Comtec, men and 4 women ; mean age 38.6 years old) who had Nagano, Japan). Data were conditioned with a not practiced TM or any other type of meditation Hanning window. These parameters in the 8ン13 Hz before the study. window were averaged for a 60-s analysis period at Tasks and data acquisition. The subjects of frontal (Fp1, Fp2), parietal (P3, P4) and occipital the study group were required to practice TM for sites (O1, O2), respectively, although mean frequency 20 min. The control subjects were instructed to per- was obtained only at the occipital site. form a sham task ; a mock meditation involving a The index of each band is obtained using the fol- silent repetition of a simple non-mantra word (a sim- lowing formula : ple number) with their eyes closed. We employed repetition of a single number as the sham task since 21 Sn it is equally non-challenging and simple as a TM task. i=6 Cp = ×100(オ) The TM and the sham task were of equal length. δ 21 41 66 Sn + Sn+ Sn The recordings were made in a magnetically i=6 i=22 i=42 shielded room. The subject sat comfortably with his 41 or her head under a whole-scalp neuromagnetometer Sn i=22 that housed 148 SQUIDs (Magnes, Biomagnetic Cpθ= 21 41 66 ×100(オ) Technologies Inc., San Diego, CA, USA). Sn + Sn+ Sn Simultaneous EEG recordings were also performed i=6i=22 i=42 with electrodes over the scalp, according to the 66 Sn International 10ン20 method. Ag/AgCl electrodes i=42 Cp = ×100(オ) were referenced to the linked ears. The recording α 21 41 66 Sn + Sn+ Sn time was divided into 3 parts : fi rst, 5 min of rest i=6i=22 i=42 before TM or the sham task with the eyes closed ; second, TM or the sham task for 20 min ; and where Sn is the averaged spectrum. third, 10 min of rest after the task, also with the 12 eyes closed. The experimentalist verbally signaled Sn= 1 Sn 12 the subjects to begin and end each task. i=0 Both the MEGs and the EEGs were digitized at a Repeated-measures ANOVA was performed to sampling frequency of 678.17 Hz and fi ltered with a test condition-dependent diff erences in ク power and band-pass of 0.1 Hz ン 200 Hz. Digitized data were index. Mean frequency and laterality of the parame- stored and analyzed off -line, using further fi ltering ters were analyzed with Student’s t-test. through a 3ン50 Hz band-pass fi lter. The duration of MEG. Based on visual inspection, ク waves, each epoch was 30 sec at regular intervals of 60 sec which were active during TM or the control task and set by the internal trigger. Epochs were recorded 35 recorded simultaneously with the EEG, were times, once a minute, over the whole session. detected after the off set was performed. As stated in EEG. The data were sent to an NEC digital the Results section, increased frontal and central February 2006 MEG in Transcendental Meditation 53 alpha activities were observed during TM practice. from MUSIC analysis and the ECD model, another We extracted 300ン500 epochs of 100 ms duration (50 analysis was performed with the multiple spatio-tem- ms pre- and 50 ms post-ク wave peak) of MEG waves poral dipole model using the BESA Program. This in each subject. method allows spatio-temporal modeling of multiple Analysis of MEG data. We applied 3 diff er- simultaneous sources over defi ned intervals. Signal ent methods to analyze sources of the brain activities epochs for source analysis were defi ned on the basis obtained during the task : the multiple signal classifi - of the global fi eld power (GFP) [14]. We considered cation (MUSIC) algorithm, equivalent current dipole that when the residual variance (100-GOF(オ)) was (ECD) source analysis for both raw and averaged less than 5オ, the adaptation of the dipoles would be MEG data, and the multiple spatio-temporal dipole signifi cant. model method using the brain electric source analysis (BESA) Program [6]. MEG records were averaged Results for every cycle of ク waves using individual negative peaks of ク waves in the EEG as a trigger point. EEG and EEG topography. The EEG of Source analysis using the MUSIC algorithm and TM practitioners appeared to show increased frontal BESA program were also performed from the aver- and central ク activity (diff use high-amplitude ク aged MEG data. waves) during TM, which is in good agreement with A GE Signa 1.5 T system was used for magnetic the results of a previous report [2]. In contrast, resonance imaging (MRI). 3D spoiled gradient pulse control subjects showed ordinary occipital-dominant sequence (SPGR) images were used for overlays with ク waves during the sham task. EEG topography also MUSIC localization and ECD sources detected by indicated that TM practitioners showed substantially MEG. The nasion was identifi ed on MR images with high ク power over a large cortical area, but the con- the aid of high-contrast cod liver oil capsules. trol subjects showed little change during the sham Source analysis was performed with the MUSIC task. Typical recordings of EEGs and EEG topogra- algorithm using Advanced Source Analysis (A.N.T. phies during TM and the sham task are shown in Fig. Software) on a PC/AT compatible personal com- 1. puter. The principle of the MUSIC algorithm has Analysis of EEG variables. Table 1 pres- been explained in detail in previous papers [7ン13]. ents means and standard errors for the ク power and The advantage of using the MUSIC algorithm is that mean frequency of ク waves before and during the multiple asynchronous dipole sources can be drawn in task. Means of bilateral EEG parameters at each contour maps that describe the appropriateness of a site were used for the analysis since no signifi cant single dipole at each grid.
Recommended publications
  • Connectivity of BA46 Involvement in the Executive Control of Language
    Alfredo Ardila, Byron Bernal and Monica Rosselli Psicothema 2016, Vol. 28, No. 1, 26-31 ISSN 0214 - 9915 CODEN PSOTEG Copyright © 2016 Psicothema doi: 10.7334/psicothema2015.174 www.psicothema.com Connectivity of BA46 involvement in the executive control of language Alfredo Ardila1, Byron Bernal2 and Monica Rosselli3 1 Florida International University, 2 Miami Children’s Hospital and 3 Florida Atlantic University Abstract Resumen Background: Understanding the functions of different brain areas has Estudio de la conectividad del AB46 en el control ejecutivo del lenguaje. represented a major endeavor of contemporary neurosciences. Modern Antecedentes: la comprensión de las funciones de diferentes áreas neuroimaging developments suggest cognitive functions are associated cerebrales representa una de las mayores empresas de las neurociencias with networks rather than with specifi c areas. Objectives. The purpose contemporáneas. Los estudios contemporáneos con neuroimágenes of this paper was to analyze the connectivity of Brodmann area (BA) 46 sugieren que las funciones cognitivas se asocian con redes más que con (anterior middle frontal gyrus) in relation to language. Methods: A meta- áreas específi cas. El propósito de este estudio fue analizar la conectividad analysis was conducted to assess the language network in which BA46 is del área de Brodmann 46 (BA46) (circunvolución frontal media anterior) involved. The DataBase of Brainmap was used; 19 papers corresponding con relación al lenguaje. Método: se llevó a cabo un meta-análisis para to 60 experimental conditions with a total of 245 subjects were included. determinar el circuito o red lingüística en la cual participa BA46. Se utilizó Results: Our results suggest the core network of BA46.
    [Show full text]
  • Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex-Based Control with an Implanted Brain–Computer Interface
    www.nature.com/scientificreports OPEN Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex‑based control with an implanted brain–computer interface Sacha Leinders 1, Mariska J. Vansteensel 1, Mariana P. Branco 1, Zac V. Freudenburg1, Elmar G. M. Pels1, Benny Van der Vijgh1, Martine J. E. Van Zandvoort2, Nicolas F. Ramsey1* & Erik J. Aarnoutse 1 The objective of this study was to test the feasibility of using the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex as a signal source for brain–computer interface control in people with severe motor impairment. We implanted two individuals with locked‑in syndrome with a chronic brain–computer interface designed to restore independent communication. The implanted system (Utrecht NeuroProsthesis) included electrode strips placed subdurally over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. In both participants, counting backwards activated the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex consistently over the course of 47 and 22months, respectively. Moreover, both participants were able to use this signal to control a cursor in one dimension, with average accuracy scores of 78 ± 9% (standard deviation) and 71 ± 11% (chance level: 50%), respectively. Brain– computer interface control based on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activity is feasible in people with locked‑in syndrome and may become of relevance for those unable to use sensorimotor signals for control. Locked-in syndrome (LIS) is characterized by intact cognition and (almost) complete loss of voluntary movement1. Brain–computer interface (BCI) systems allow computer control with brain activity and can there- fore provide an alternative communication channel to people with LIS. BCI systems depending on self-initiated modulation of brain activity generally measure from sensorimotor areas, e.g.2–9. However, not all people with LIS may be able to reliably modulate sensorimotor activity, due to for instance atrophy secondary to neurode- generative disease10,11, damage afer stroke or injury, or atypical neural activity 12.
    [Show full text]
  • Neural Correlates Underlying Change in State Self-Esteem Hiroaki Kawamichi 1,2,3, Sho K
    www.nature.com/scientificreports OPEN Neural correlates underlying change in state self-esteem Hiroaki Kawamichi 1,2,3, Sho K. Sugawara2,4,5, Yuki H. Hamano2,5,6, Ryo Kitada 2,7, Eri Nakagawa2, Takanori Kochiyama8 & Norihiro Sadato 2,5 Received: 21 July 2017 State self-esteem, the momentary feeling of self-worth, functions as a sociometer involved in Accepted: 11 January 2018 maintenance of interpersonal relations. How others’ appraisal is subjectively interpreted to change Published: xx xx xxxx state self-esteem is unknown, and the neural underpinnings of this process remain to be elucidated. We hypothesized that changes in state self-esteem are represented by the mentalizing network, which is modulated by interactions with regions involved in the subjective interpretation of others’ appraisal. To test this hypothesis, we conducted task-based and resting-state fMRI. Participants were repeatedly presented with their reputations, and then rated their pleasantness and reported their state self- esteem. To evaluate the individual sensitivity of the change in state self-esteem based on pleasantness (i.e., the subjective interpretation of reputation), we calculated evaluation sensitivity as the rate of change in state self-esteem per unit pleasantness. Evaluation sensitivity varied across participants, and was positively correlated with precuneus activity evoked by reputation rating. Resting-state fMRI revealed that evaluation sensitivity was positively correlated with functional connectivity of the precuneus with areas activated by negative reputation, but negatively correlated with areas activated by positive reputation. Thus, the precuneus, as the part of the mentalizing system, serves as a gateway for translating the subjective interpretation of reputation into state self-esteem.
    [Show full text]
  • Healthy and Abnormal Development of the Prefrontal Cortex
    Developmental Neurorehabilitation, October 2009; 12(5): 279–297 Healthy and abnormal development of the prefrontal cortex MEGAN SPENCER-SMITH1,2 & VICKI ANDERSON1,2,3 1Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, Melbourne, Australia, 2University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia, and 3Royal Children’s Hospital, Melbourne, Australia (Received 8 October 2008; accepted 3 June 2009) Abstract Background: While many children with brain conditions present with cognitive, behavioural, emotional, academic and social impairments, other children recover with seemingly few impairments. Animal studies and preliminary child studies have identified timing of brain lesion as a key predictor in determining functional outcome following early brain lesions. Review: This research suggests that knowledge of healthy developmental processes in brain structure and function is essential for better understanding functional recovery and outcome in children with brain lesions. This review paper aims to equip researchers with current knowledge of key principles of developmental processes in brain structure and function. Timetables for development of the prefrontal cortex (PFC), a brain region particularly vulnerable to lesions due to its protracted developmental course, are examined. In addition, timetables for development of executive skills, which emerge in childhood and have a prolonged developmental course that parallels development of the PFC, are also discussed. Conclusions: Equipped with this knowledge, researchers are now in a better position to understand functional
    [Show full text]
  • Human Prefrontal Cortex: Processing and Representational Perspectives
    REVIEWS HUMAN PREFRONTAL CORTEX: PROCESSING AND REPRESENTATIONAL PERSPECTIVES Jacqueline N. Wood and Jordan Grafman Through evolution, humans have acquired ‘higher’ cognitive skills — such as language, reasoning and planning — and complex social behaviour. Evidence from neuropsychological and neuroimaging research indicates that the prefrontal cortex (PFC) underlies much of this higher cognition. A number of theories have been proposed for how the PFC might achieve this. Although many of these theories focus on the types of ‘process’ that the PFC carries out, we argue for the validity of a representational approach to understanding PFC function. Although it is clear that the PFC is important for higher The first of our five proposed criteria is that a theory cognitive skills, particularly in humans, how it achieves must be explicit about the information that is stored in these functions is unknown. The human PFC is not nec- the PFC. Does it store information akin to a memory essarily larger than that of other primate species1, but its function (representational approach)? Does it store neural architecture is probably more sophisticated or algorithms or computational procedures only for organized differently to accommodate higher cognitive manipulating information stored elsewhere in the brain functions that are superior to those of related species2,3. (processing approach)? Does it do a combination of Researchers have proposed a number of theories of PFC these things (hybrid approach)? Second, the theory function, many of which centre around the representa- must be consistent with our knowledge of stimulus rep- tions or processes that are mediated by the PFC. We resentation in the brain.
    [Show full text]
  • Developmental Pathways to Functional Brain Networks: Emerging Principles
    TICS-1251; No. of Pages 14 Review Special Issue: The Connectome Developmental pathways to functional brain networks: emerging principles 1,2,3 V. Menon 1 Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA 94304, USA 2 Department of Neurology and Neurological Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA 94304, USA 3 Program in Neuroscience, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA 94304, USA The human brain undergoes protracted developmental disorders. The perspective advanced in this review is that changes during which it constructs functional networks a thorough understanding of the functional architecture of that engender complex cognitive abilities. Understand- the adult brain requires critical consideration of the devel- ing brain function ultimately depends on knowledge of opmental pathways by which plasticity and learning lead how dynamic interactions between distributed brain to the construction of dedicated large-scale brain systems. regions mature with age to produce sophisticated cog- Most, if not all, major psychopathologies, with the excep- nitive systems. This review summarizes recent progress tion of the dementias, have a prominent origin in childhood in our understanding of the ontogeny of functional brain or adolescence [4]. The onset and diagnosis of these psycho- networks. Here I describe how complementary methods pathologies vary greatly: some, like autism, are mainly for probing functional connectivity are providing unique diagnosed in early childhood, others such as attention deficit insights into the emergence and maturation of distinct hyperactivity disorder and anxiety disorders are mainly functional networks from childhood to adulthood. diagnosed in middle childhood, whereas bipolar disorder, I highlight six emerging principles governing the devel- depression, and schizophrenia are predominantly diag- opment of large-scale functional networks and discuss nosed in late adolescence.
    [Show full text]
  • A Brief Anatomical Sketch of Human Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Jamil P
    This article is a supplement referenced in Delgado, M. R., Beer, J. S., Fellows, L. K., Huettel, S. A., Platt, M. L., Quirk, G. J., & Schiller, D. (2016). Viewpoints: Dialogues on the functional role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 19(12), 1545-1552. Brain images used in this article (vmPFC mask) are available at https://identifiers.org/neurovault.collection:5631 A Brief Anatomical Sketch of Human Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Jamil P. Bhanji1, David V. Smith2, Mauricio R. Delgado1 1 Department of Psychology, Rutgers University - Newark 2 Department of Psychology, Temple University The ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) is a major focus of investigation in human neuroscience, particularly in studies of emotion, social cognition, and decision making. Although the term vmPFC is widely used, the zone is not precisely defined, and for varied reasons has proven a complicated region to study. A difficulty identifying precise boundaries for the vmPFC comes partly from varied use of the term, sometimes including and sometimes excluding ventral parts of anterior cingulate cortex and medial parts of orbitofrontal cortex. These discrepancies can arise both from the need to refer to distinct sub-regions within a larger area of prefrontal cortex, and from the spatially imprecise nature of research methods such as human neuroimaging and natural lesions. The inexactness of the term is not necessarily an impediment, although the heterogeneity of the region can impact functional interpretation. Here we briefly address research that has helped delineate sub-regions of the human vmPFC, we then discuss patterns of white matter connectivity with other regions of the brain and how they begin to inform functional roles within vmPFC.
    [Show full text]
  • What Does the Prefrontal Cortex “Do” in Affect: Perspectives on Frontal EEG Asymmetry Research
    Biological Psychology 67 (2004) 219–233 Commentary What does the prefrontal cortex “do” in affect: perspectives on frontal EEG asymmetry research Richard J. Davidson∗ Laboratory for Affective Neuroscience, W.M. Keck Laboratory for Functional Neuroimaging and Behavior, University of Wisconsin-Madison, 1202 West Johnson Street, Madison, WI 53706, USA Abstract This commentary provides reflections on the current state of affairs in research on EEG frontal asymmetries associated with affect. Although considerable progress has occurred since the first report on this topic 25 years ago, research on frontal EEG asymmetries associated with affect has largely evolved in the absence of any serious connection with neuroscience research on the structure and function of the primate prefrontal cortex (PFC). Such integration is important as this work progresses since the neuroscience literature can help to understand what the prefrontal cortex is “doing” in affective processing. Data from the neuroscience literature on the heterogeneity of different sectors of the PFC are introduced and more specific hypotheses are offered about what different sectors of the PFC might be doing in affect. A number of methodological issues associated with EEG measures of functional prefrontal asymmetries are also considered. © 2004 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Frontal EEG asymmetry; Emotion; Prefrontal cortex; Neuroimaging 1. Introduction It is very gratifying for me personally to be writing this commentary since the publi- cation of this Special Issue marks the fact that there now exists a substantial corpus of scientific literature investigating methodological and conceptual issues surrounding the use of measures of asymmetric prefrontal electrical signals recorded from the scalp surface to make inferences about emotional processes.
    [Show full text]
  • A Practical Review of Functional MRI Anatomy of the Language and Motor Systems
    REVIEW ARTICLE FUNCTIONAL A Practical Review of Functional MRI Anatomy of the Language and Motor Systems X V.B. Hill, X C.Z. Cankurtaran, X B.P. Liu, X T.A. Hijaz, X M. Naidich, X A.J. Nemeth, X J. Gastala, X C. Krumpelman, X E.N. McComb, and X A.W. Korutz ABSTRACT SUMMARY: Functional MR imaging is being performed with increasing frequency in the typical neuroradiology practice; however, many readers of these studies have only a limited knowledge of the functional anatomy of the brain. This text will delineate the locations, anatomic boundaries, and functions of the cortical regions of the brain most commonly encountered in clinical practice—specifically, the regions involved in movement and language. ABBREVIATIONS: FFA ϭ fusiform face area; IPL ϭ inferior parietal lobule; PPC ϭ posterior parietal cortex; SMA ϭ supplementary motor area; VOTC ϭ ventral occipitotemporal cortex his article serves as a review of the functional areas of the brain serving to analyze spatial position and the ventral stream working Tmost commonly mapped during presurgical fMRI studies, to identify what an object is. Influenced by the dorsal and ventral specifically targeting movement and language. We have compiled stream model of vision, Hickok and Poeppel2 hypothesized a sim- what we hope is a useful, easily portable, and concise resource that ilar framework for language. In this model, the ventral stream, or can be accessible to radiologists everywhere. We begin with a re- lexical-semantic system, is involved in sound-to-meaning map- view of the language-processing system. Then we describe the pings associated with language comprehension and semantic ac- gross anatomic boundaries, organization, and function of each cess.
    [Show full text]
  • Neural Correlates of Strategic Memory Retrieval: Differentiating Between Spatial-Associative and Temporal-Associative Strategies
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by MPG.PuRe r Human Brain Mapping 29:1068–1079 (2008) r Neural Correlates of Strategic Memory Retrieval: Differentiating Between Spatial-Associative and Temporal-Associative Strategies Mischa de Rover,1* Karl Magnus Petersson,1 Sieberen P. van der Werf,2 Alexander R. Cools,3 Hans J. Berger,2 and Guille´n Ferna´ndez1,4 1F.C. Donders Center for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Radboud University Nijmegen, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands 2Department of Medical Psychology, University Medical Centre Nijmegen, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands 3Department of Psychoneuropharmacology, University Medical Centre Nijmegen, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands 4Department of Neurology, University Medical Centre Nijmegen, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands Abstract: Remembering complex, multidimensional information typically requires strategic memory re- trieval, during which information is structured, for instance by spatial- or temporal associations. Although brain regions involved in strategic memory retrieval in general have been identified, differences in re- trieval operations related to distinct retrieval strategies are not well-understood. Thus, our aim was to identify brain regions whose activity is differentially involved in spatial-associative and temporal-associa- tive retrieval. First, we showed that our behavioral paradigm probing memory for a set of object-location associations promoted the use of a spatial-associative structure following an encoding condition
    [Show full text]
  • Functional Connectivity of the Precuneus in Unmedicated Patients with Depression
    Biological Psychiatry: CNNI Archival Report Functional Connectivity of the Precuneus in Unmedicated Patients With Depression Wei Cheng, Edmund T. Rolls, Jiang Qiu, Deyu Yang, Hongtao Ruan, Dongtao Wei, Libo Zhao, Jie Meng, Peng Xie, and Jianfeng Feng ABSTRACT BACKGROUND: The precuneus has connectivity with brain systems implicated in depression. METHODS: We performed the first fully voxel-level resting-state functional connectivity (FC) neuroimaging analysis of depression of the precuneus, with 282 patients with major depressive disorder and 254 control subjects. RESULTS: In 125 unmedicated patients, voxels in the precuneus had significantly increased FC with the lateral orbitofrontal cortex, a region implicated in nonreward that is thereby implicated in depression. FC was also increased in depression between the precuneus and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, temporal cortex, and angular and supramarginal areas. In patients receiving medication, the FC between the lateral orbitofrontal cortex and precuneus was decreased back toward that in the control subjects. In the 254 control subjects, parcellation revealed superior anterior, superior posterior, and inferior subdivisions, with the inferior subdivision having high connectivity with the posterior cingulate cortex, parahippocampal gyrus, angular gyrus, and prefrontal cortex. It was the ventral subdivision of the precuneus that had increased connectivity in depression with the lateral orbitofrontal cortex and adjoining inferior frontal gyrus. CONCLUSIONS: The findings support the theory that the system in the lateral orbitofrontal cortex implicated in the response to nonreceipt of expected rewards has increased effects on areas in which the self is represented, such as the precuneus. This may result in low self-esteem in depression. The increased connectivity of the precuneus with the prefrontal cortex short-term memory system may contribute to the rumination about low self-esteem in depression.
    [Show full text]
  • Frontal Lobe and Cognitive Development
    Journal of Neurocytology 31, 373–385 (2002) Frontal lobe and cognitive development JOAQUIN´ M. FUSTER Neuropsychiatric Institute and Brain Research Institute, UCLA School of Medicine Los Angeles, California [email protected] Received December 1, 2002; accepted December 12, 2002 Abstract In phylogeny as in ontogeny, the association cortex of the frontal lobe, also known as the prefrontal cortex, is a late-developing region of the neocortex. It is also one of the cortical regions to undergo the greatest expansion in the course of both evolution and individual maturation. In the human adult, the prefrontal cortex constitutes as much as nearly one-third of the totality of the neocortex. The protracted, relatively large, development of the prefrontal cortex is manifest in gross morphology as well as fine structure. In the developing individual, its late maturation is made most apparent by the late myelination of its axonal connections. This and other indices of morphological development of the prefrontal cortex correlate with the development of cognitive functions that neuropsychological studies in animals and humans have ascribed to this cortex. In broad outline, the ventromedial areas of the prefrontal cortex, which with respect to other prefrontal areas develop relatively early, are involved in the expression and control of emotional and instinctual behaviors. On the other hand, the late maturing areas of the lateral prefrontal convexity are principally involved in higher executive functions. The most general executive function of the lateral prefrontal cortex is the temporal organization of goal-directed actions in the domains of behavior, cognition, and language. In all three domains, that global function is supported by a fundamental role of the lateral prefrontal cortex in temporal integration, that is, the integration of temporally discontinuous percepts and neural inputs into coherent structures of action.
    [Show full text]