SIG02 Bullen

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

SIG02 Bullen CLOTTES J. (dir.) 2012. — L’art pléistocène dans le monde / Pleistocene art of the world / Arte pleistoceno en el mundo Actes du Congrès IFRAO, Tarascon-sur-Ariège, septembre 2010 – Symposium « Signes, symboles, mythes et idéologie… » Finding the first intentional mark makers: Clues in brain development Margaret BULLEN* Keywords: Marks; Intentionality; cognition; synapse. “‘Lascaux Man’ created and created out of nothing, this world of art in which communication between individual minds begins.” (Bataille 1955: 11) Lascaux was, for Bataille, the “birth of art” despite the fact that when it was found by school boys in 1940, it was far from the first painted cave to be discovered. Altamira was almost as famous for its initial rejection by Cartailhac in the 1870s and his later acknowledgement of its being genuinely Palaeolithic, as for its bison curled into recesses in the ceiling; Altamira, La Mouthe, Les Combarelles, Font de Gaume and Niaux are just a few of the caves with paintings already, long before 1940, accepted as being from the Upper Palaeolithic (Bahn 1997 ; Grand 1967). Yet Bataille, a noted French philosopher could still perceive Lascaux as the “end to that timeless deadlock” and “the shift from the world of work to the world of play, from the rough hewn to the finished individual being.” (Bataille 1955: 27). But this is not where or when it all began Here I will endevour to reach back to a time before the creation of the bison, lionesses, horses and reindeer, which “stimulate multiple distributed cerebral processors in a novel and harmonious way” (Dehaene 2009: 309) and which we acknowledge as masterpieces. Our twenty first century eyes have no difficulty in reading into them a narrative which we search for clues to the message it surely contains. We can speculate but we do not know exactly what the lionesses of Chauvet are doing, however we sense their strength and know we too would be fearful of their power to kill. We are moved and awed by them and feel that in some way we share the emotions felt by the people of the past for whom those images were part of their present. It is possible to shift temporal reality in our own lives, to re-experience an earlier age that was our personal experience but we cannot move outside our own frame of reference into another time, place or person. We have tangible evidence for peoples’ capacity to communicate through art thirty to forty thousand years ago. We seek to ”listen in” on their artistic conversations but we cannot know how it was for the people of thirty thousand years ago by studying their pictures. It can tell us something of their external world and we can try to extrapolate from our inner world to theirs. To go * Melbourne Australia. Symposium Signes, symboles, mythes et idéologie… back further is even more difficult; to find the elusive point in time when the realisation occurred that a mark made by one individual could influence another. Not only is it a quest for a time when this happened but also a quest for what was needed. Was it a larger, more specialised brain, or was it more connectivity or more complexity at the fundamental level of synapses? Was it necessary to have language before pictures or are they one and the same? The physical traces we have of our early hominid ancestors are few; remnants of tool kits, faint evidence for habitation and fossilised skeletal material that has managed to survive despite the many taphonomic processes that militate against such survival. How do we get inside the heads of these distant ancestors? As will be discussed later, endocasts made from the skulls gives some information about the potential capacity of the individuals but may be misleading if taken literally at face value. We have to start with the brains that are available for investigation, those of our closest living relatives, the non-human primates. The separation of the line which would lead to Homo sapiens sapiens from that of the chimpanzee occurred about 10 million years ago and this paper will first consider how closely the behaviour of non-human primates reflects that of humans. 1. Communication in non-human animals Humans communicate between each other and with non-human animals in a multiplicity of ways; touch, gesture, sounds and voice are all signs which people use to manipulate their way through society (Sebeok 1975: 11), but it is only with other humans that they use indirect communication, making marks with the intention of affecting some one else, in other words leaving messages. Non-human animals make marks that affect others; scats and tracks betray the presence and the passage of an animal, but does the cat covering up its faeces do so because it knows their presence could alert a predator or because it is replicating the behaviour of its mother? Non-human primates use a wide range of gestures in the wild and in captivity will adopt human gestures such as pointing. In contrast to the inflexibility of their vocal calls chimpanzees’ gestures are flexible and multi-referential making them more like human language than are their vocalisations (Tomasello & Call 2007). Assumptions about primate behaviour are frequently made on the basis of their interaction with humans rather than with their own species. For example non-human primates appear not to appreciate the significance of a human paying attention to an object unless there is an obvious source of gratification or threat. Infants aged fourteen months were able to follow the gaze of an experimenter who hid a toy under one of two opaque buckets and then stared at it (Tomasello & Carpenter 2005: 99), whereas chimpanzees gazing at two opaque cups one of which covered a food item and again was stared at by the experimenter, chose the correct cup no more frequently than by chance (ibid.: 105) They did not interpret the gaze as providing information. It is inferred that they are not motivated as humans are to share emotions, experiences and activities with others of their own kind (ibid.: 19). Chimpanzees could point to an object when the goal was that the chimpanzee received food if the human used a tool pointed out to him or her by the chimpanzee. In this situation there was an immediate goal for the chimpanzee, it obtained desired food. However, not unlike some humans, they have been observed to respond better to competition than to cooperation. For example chimpanzees did not find food when CD-1614 BULLEN M., Finding the first intentional mark makers: Clues in brain development its location was pointed out to them by a helpful human but they did when the human competitively reached for the food without looking at the chimpanzee (ibid.: 120). Non-human primates have not been observed to use symbolic forms of communication in the wild, but does this mean they do not have the potential to learn that something can stand for something else? Savage-Rumbaugh painstakingly trained pygmy chimpanzees to use tools and to request particular tools to obtain food using symbols, and was able to show that a trained chimpanzee could gain the cooperation of an untrained animal to get food which neither could reach without the cooperation of the other (Donald 1991: 133) Donald, commenting on this work in which the chimpanzees also learned to understand English sentences and acquire and use a large lexicon of visual symbols, says “in sum, after undergoing this radical process of enculturation pygmy chimpanzees do not act , think or communicate like the same species. They do things they could never achieve in the wild, obviously without changes to their genome… Savage-Rumbaugh’s chimps may be regarded as over-achievers in the sense that they did not create the culture that revealed their latent capacities” (Donald 1998: 8). 2. The brain in humans and non-human primates – what’s the difference? The cultural inheritance of humans is clearly different from that of non-human primates while, although there are differences in their brains, the biological inheritance of humans and other primates is very similar. The question that arises is whether the differences can explain why humans alone have developed the capacity to recognise others as intentional agents like themselves, who will respond, not only to direct vocalisations or touch but also to creative symbolising and metaphor. Of course we can never be absolutely sure to what extent chimpanzees know each other; we are not privy to their intimate communications. While separation from a common ancestor of the line that would lead to Homo sapiens sapiens from that of the chimpanzee happened only about 10 million years ago, the separation from monkeys such as the macaque goes back about 25 million years. However it is the brains of macaques that have been studied most closely. Newer non-invasive techniques of exploring brain activity have led to major breakthroughs in our understanding but there are still limitations on what can be done. Many humans cannot tolerate the claustrophobic environment of an MRI scanner and persuading chimpanzees to do so when not sedated is problematic. Direct experiments on the brains of humans are only carried out on people requiring brain surgery for conditions such as epilepsy and who are able to consent to the investigations. Post-mortem studies in humans can give information when correlated with clinical information but tissues rapidly become altered post-mortem and particularly the white tissue of connecting tracts. While there is a reluctance to put electrodes into the brains of chimpanzees this does not seem to have been so in the case of macaques and therefore they have been the subjects of most direct scanning and invasive activities.
Recommended publications
  • Assessing Relationships Between Human Adaptive Responses and Ecology Via Eco-Cultural Niche Modeling William E
    Assessing relationships between human adaptive responses and ecology via eco-cultural niche modeling William E. Banks To cite this version: William E. Banks. Assessing relationships between human adaptive responses and ecology via eco- cultural niche modeling. Archaeology and Prehistory. Universite Bordeaux 1, 2013. hal-01840898 HAL Id: hal-01840898 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01840898 Submitted on 11 Nov 2020 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Thèse d'Habilitation à Diriger des Recherches Université de Bordeaux 1 William E. BANKS UMR 5199 PACEA – De la Préhistoire à l'Actuel : Culture, Environnement et Anthropologie Assessing Relationships between Human Adaptive Responses and Ecology via Eco-Cultural Niche Modeling Soutenue le 14 novembre 2013 devant un jury composé de: Michel CRUCIFIX, Chargé de Cours à l'Université catholique de Louvain, Belgique Francesco D'ERRICO, Directeur de Recherche au CRNS, Talence Jacques JAUBERT, Professeur à l'Université de Bordeaux 1, Talence Rémy PETIT, Directeur de Recherche à l'INRA, Cestas Pierre SEPULCHRE, Chargé de Recherche au CNRS, Gif-sur-Yvette Jean-Denis VIGNE, Directeur de Recherche au CNRS, Paris Table of Contents Summary of Past Research Introduction ..................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • CURRICULUM VITAE Shara E
    CURRICULUM VITAE Shara E. Bailey July 2019 Home Address: 14 Lancaster Avenue Office Address: New York University Maplewood, NJ 07040 Department of Anthropology 25 Waverly Place Mobile Phone: 646.300.4508 New York, NY 10003 E-mail: [email protected] Office Phone: 212.998.8576 Education Arizona State University, Department of Anthropology, Tempe, AZ PhD in Anthropology Jan 2002 Dissertation Title: Neandertal Dental Morphology: Implications for Modern Human Origins Dissertation director: Prof. William H. Kimbel Master of Arts in Anthropology 1995 Thesis Title: Population distribution of the tuberculum dentale complex and anomalies of the anterior maxillary teeth. Thesis director: Regents’ Professor, Christy G. Turner, II Temple University, Philadelphia, PA Bachelor of Arts in Psychology and Anthropology 1992 Positions/Affiliations Associate Professor, New York University, Department of Anthropology, New York, NY 2011- Associated Scientist, Department of Human Evolution, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary 2006- Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany Assistant Professor, New York University, Department of Anthropology, New York, NY 2005-2011 Research Scientist, The Max Planck Institute, Department of Human Evolution, 2004-2006 Leipzig, Germany Postdoctoral Research Associate (Prof. Bernard Wood, Research Director) 2002-2004 The George Washington University, CASHP, Washington DC Appointments Associate Chair, Anthropology Department 2018- New York University, College of Arts and Sciences Director of Undergraduate Studies, Anthropology Department 2016-2018
    [Show full text]
  • L'ours Des Cavernes De Font-De-Gaume
    PALEO Revue d'archéologie préhistorique 15 | 2003 Varia L’ours des cavernes de Font-de-Gaume III The cave bear of Font-de-Gaume III Dominique Armand, Frédéric Plassard et François Prat Édition électronique URL : http://journals.openedition.org/paleo/1303 DOI : 10.4000/paleo.1303 ISSN : 2101-0420 Éditeur SAMRA Édition imprimée Date de publication : 1 décembre 2003 Pagination : 241-244 ISSN : 1145-3370 Référence électronique Dominique Armand, Frédéric Plassard et François Prat, « L’ours des cavernes de Font-de-Gaume III », PALEO [En ligne], 15 | 2003, mis en ligne le 03 août 2010, consulté le 07 juillet 2020. URL : http:// journals.openedition.org/paleo/1303 ; DOI : https://doi.org/10.4000/paleo.1303 Ce document a été généré automatiquement le 7 juillet 2020. PALEO est mis à disposition selon les termes de la licence Creative Commons Attribution - Pas d'Utilisation Commerciale - Pas de Modification 4.0 International. L’ours des cavernes de Font-de-Gaume III 1 L’ours des cavernes de Font-de- Gaume III The cave bear of Font-de-Gaume III Dominique Armand, Frédéric Plassard et François Prat 1 La grotte de Font-de-Gaume (Les Eyzies, Dordogne, France) est connue depuis sa découverte, en 1901, comme un site d’art pariétal paléolithique de premier ordre. Depuis cette date, plusieurs campagnes de fouille ont été conduites dans cette cavité. 2 Dans la monographie que publièrent les inventeurs (Capitan, Breuil et Peyrony 1910), un chapitre présente le matériel archéologique recueilli dans les différentes galeries. Quelques lignes sont également consacrées aux ossements d’ours découverts dans le remplissage argileux et qui furent étudiés par F.
    [Show full text]
  • The Use of Ochre and Painting During the Upper Paleolithic of the Swabian Jura in the Context of the Development of Ochre Use in Africa and Europe
    Open Archaeology 2018; 4: 185–205 Original Study Sibylle Wolf*, Rimtautas Dapschauskas, Elizabeth Velliky, Harald Floss, Andrew W. Kandel, Nicholas J. Conard The Use of Ochre and Painting During the Upper Paleolithic of the Swabian Jura in the Context of the Development of Ochre Use in Africa and Europe https://doi.org/10.1515/opar-2018-0012 Received June 8, 2017; accepted December 13, 2017 Abstract: While the earliest evidence for ochre use is very sparse, the habitual use of ochre by hominins appeared about 140,000 years ago and accompanied them ever since. Here, we present an overview of archaeological sites in southwestern Germany, which yielded remains of ochre. We focus on the artifacts belonging exclusively to anatomically modern humans who were the inhabitants of the cave sites in the Swabian Jura during the Upper Paleolithic. The painted limestones from the Magdalenian layers of Hohle Fels Cave are a particular focus. We present these artifacts in detail and argue that they represent the beginning of a tradition of painting in Central Europe. Keywords: ochre use, Middle Stone Age, Swabian Jura, Upper Paleolithic, Magdalenian painting 1 The Earliest Use of Ochre in the Homo Lineage Modern humans have three types of cone cells in the retina of the eye. These cells are a requirement for trichromatic vision and hence, a requirement for the perception of the color red. The capacity for trichromatic vision dates back about 35 million years, within our shared evolutionary lineage in the Catarrhini subdivision of the higher primates (Jacobs, 2013, 2015). Trichromatic vision may have evolved as a result of the benefits for recognizing ripe yellow, orange, and red fruits in front of a background of green foliage (Regan et al., Article note: This article is a part of Topical Issue on From Line to Colour: Social Context and Visual Communication of Prehistoric Art edited by Liliana Janik and Simon Kaner.
    [Show full text]
  • The Châtelperronian of Germolles (Grotte De La Verpillière I, Mellecey, Saône-Et-Loire, France)
    PALEO Revue d'archéologie préhistorique 27 | 2016 Varia The Châtelperronian of Germolles (Grotte de la Verpillière I, Mellecey, Saône-et-Loire, France) Harald Floss, Christian Hoyer and Heike Würschem Electronic version URL: http://journals.openedition.org/paleo/3242 DOI: 10.4000/paleo.3242 ISSN: 2101-0420 Publisher SAMRA Printed version Date of publication: 30 December 2016 Number of pages: 149-176 ISSN: 1145-3370 Electronic reference Harald Floss, Christian Hoyer and Heike Würschem, « The Châtelperronian of Germolles (Grotte de la Verpillière I, Mellecey, Saône-et-Loire, France) », PALEO [Online], 27 | 2016, Online since 01 June 2018, connection on 07 July 2020. URL : http://journals.openedition.org/paleo/3242 ; DOI : https://doi.org/ 10.4000/paleo.3242 This text was automatically generated on 7 July 2020. PALEO est mis à disposition selon les termes de la licence Creative Commons Attribution - Pas d'Utilisation Commerciale - Pas de Modification 4.0 International. The Châtelperronian of Germolles (Grotte de la Verpillière I, Mellecey, Saône... 1 The Châtelperronian of Germolles (Grotte de la Verpillière I, Mellecey, Saône-et-Loire, France) Harald Floss, Christian Hoyer and Heike Würschem We thank the regional direction of cultural affairs, the regional service of archaeology of Burgundy, UMR 6298 ARTeHIS of the University of Burgundy, the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, the Saône-et-Loire department, the University of Tübingen, the town hall of Mellecey, the Verpillière excavation teams, the members of the PCR “Paléolithique en Bourgogne Méridional” and in particular Pierre Andriot, Laetitia Bassereau-Vauthier, Sabine Boos, Didier Cailhol, Michel Cessot, Jean Combier, René Desbrosse, Victor Donguy, Denis Dubois, Annie Dumont, Ewa Dutkiewicz, Jean-Claude Gress, André-Charles Gros, Nadine Huber, Olivier Lemercier, Stephanie Lismann, Bernard Macioszczyk, Gwenaëlle Marchet-Legendre, Catherine Michel, René Parise, Yves Pautrat, Matthieu Pinette, Michel Prestreau, Benjamin Schürch, François Sikner, Pierre Voarick and Isabelle Vernus.
    [Show full text]
  • Dietary and Behavioral Strategies Of
    University of Arkansas, Fayetteville ScholarWorks@UARK Theses and Dissertations 5-2011 Dietary and Behavioral Strategies of Neandertals and Anatomically Modern Humans: Evidence from Anterior Dental Microwear Texture Analysis Kristin Lynn Krueger University of Arkansas, Fayetteville Follow this and additional works at: http://scholarworks.uark.edu/etd Part of the Biological and Physical Anthropology Commons Recommended Citation Krueger, Kristin Lynn, "Dietary and Behavioral Strategies of Neandertals and Anatomically Modern Humans: Evidence from Anterior Dental Microwear Texture Analysis" (2011). Theses and Dissertations. 92. http://scholarworks.uark.edu/etd/92 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by ScholarWorks@UARK. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@UARK. For more information, please contact [email protected], [email protected]. 1 DIETARY AND BEHAVIORAL STRATEGIES OF NEANDERTALS AND ANATOMICALLY MODERN HUMANS: EVIDENCE FROM ANTERIOR DENTAL MICROWEAR TEXTURE ANALYSIS DIETARY AND BEHAVIORAL STRATEGIES OF NEANDERTALS AND ANATOMICALLY MODERN HUMANS: EVIDENCE FROM ANTERIOR DENTAL MICROWEAR TEXTURE ANALYSIS A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Anthropology By Kristin L. Krueger University of Wisconsin-Madison Bachelor of Science in Anthropology, 2003 University of Wisconsin-Madison Bachelor of Science in Spanish, 2003 Western Michigan University Master of Arts in Anthropology, 2006 May 2011 University of Arkansas ABSTRACT The extreme gross wear of Neandertal anterior teeth has been a topic of debate for decades. Several ideas have been proposed, including the excessive mastication of grit- laden foods and non-dietary anterior tooth use, or using the anterior dentition as a clamp or tool.
    [Show full text]
  • Kurzfassung Der Vorträge Und Poster
    Kurzfassung der Vorträge und Poster Eline N. van Asperen People, prey and predators: how faunal dynamics inform our models of hominin dispersal The climate and largely forested environment of the Last Interglacial (marine isotope stage 5e) are thought to have been similar over large stretches of north-west and central Europe. Though the British Isles are climatically comparable to the European mainland at this time, certain key species, notably hominins, horses and forest rhino, are missing from Last Interglacial faunas of the British Isles. Various explanations have been offered for the enigmatic absence of evidence for hominin occupation of the British Isles during this period, including taphonomic factors, sea level rise, population dynamics and adaptive limitations which prevented hominins from surviving in densely forested environments. The detailed faunal and environmental records for the Last Interglacial allow for an investigation of the impact of large mammal community composition and dynamics on the role of hominins in the faunal com- munity. Using GIS approaches and statistical methods, the influence of environmental variables and the location of refugia on hominin dispersal and distribution are explored. The carnivorous Last Interglacial hominin diet brought the hominins in direct competition with large carnivores that focus on similar-sized prey. The complex relationships between hominins, their prey and their carnivore competitors are examined, using ecological theories of food webs and species interactions. Approaching hominins as an integral part of the large mammal community thus opens new avenues of investigating hominin behaviour. Acknowledgements: This research is supported by the European Commission under the Marie Curie Actions of the Seventh Framework Programme (PERG07-GA-2010-268242).
    [Show full text]
  • Michelle C. Langley Editor
    Vertebrate Paleobiology and Paleoanthropology Series Michelle C. Langley Editor Osseous Projectile Weaponry Towards an Understanding of Pleistocene Cultural Variability Osseous Projectile Weaponry Vertebrate Paleobiology and Paleoanthropology Series Edited by Eric Delson Vertebrate Paleontology, American Museum of Natural History New York, NY 10024,USA [email protected] Eric J. Sargis Anthropology, Yale University New Haven, CT 06520,USA [email protected] Focal topics for volumes in the series will include systematic paleontology of all vertebrates (from agnathans to humans), phylogeny reconstruction, functional morphology, Paleolithic archaeology, taphonomy, geochronology, historical biogeography, and biostratigraphy. Other fields (e.g., paleoclimatology, paleoecology, ancient DNA, total organismal community structure) may be considered if the volume theme emphasizes paleobiology (or archaeology). Fields such as modeling of physical processes, genetic methodology, nonvertebrates or neontology are out of our scope. Volumes in the series may either be monographic treatments (including unpublished but fully revised dissertations) or edited col- lections, especially those focusing on problem-oriented issues, with multidisciplinary coverage where possible. Editorial Advisory Board Ross D. E. MacPhee (American Museum of Natural History), Peter Makovicky (The Field Museum), Sally McBrearty (University of Connecticut), Jin Meng (American Museum of Natural History), Tom Plummer (Queens College/CUNY). More information about this series at http://www.springer.com/series/6978
    [Show full text]
  • (Neanderthal) Men: the Small Mammal Record of the Middle to Upper Paleolithic Transition in the Swabian Jura, Germany
    Of Mice and (Neanderthal) Men: The small mammal record of the Middle to Upper Paleolithic transition in the Swabian Jura, Germany Dissertation der Mathematisch-Naturwissenschaftlichen Fakultät der Eberhard Karls Universität Tübingen zur Erlangung des Grades eines Doktors der Naturwissenschaften (Dr. rer. nat.) vorgelegt von Sara E. Rhodes, M.Sc. aus Toronto (Kanada) Tübingen 2019 Gedruckt mit Genehmigung der Mathematisch-Naturwissenschaftlichen Fakultät der Eberhard Karls Universität Tübingen. Tag der mündlichen Qualifikation: 8. Juli 2019 Dekan: Prof. Dr. Wolfgang Rosenstiel 1. Berichterstatter: Prof. Nicholas J. Conard, PhD 2. Berichterstatter: PD Dr. Britt M. Starkovich Table of Contents I ABBREVIATIONS ................................................................................................................ III II ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS .................................................................................................. IV III SUMMARY ......................................................................................................................... VI IV LIST OF PUBLICATIONS ............................................................................................... XIV V PERSONAL CONTRIBUTION .......................................................................................... XIV 1.0 INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................................... 1 1.1 NEANDERTHALS IN THE EUROPEAN CONTEXT ....................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Under the Skin of a Lion: Unique Evidence of Upper Paleolithic Exploitation and Use of Cave Lion (Panthera Spelaea) from the Lower Gallery of La Garma (Spain)
    RESEARCH ARTICLE Under the Skin of a Lion: Unique Evidence of Upper Paleolithic Exploitation and Use of Cave Lion (Panthera spelaea) from the Lower Gallery of La Garma (Spain) MariaÂn Cueto1*, Edgard Camaro s2, Pedro Castaños3, Roberto Ontaño n1,4, Pablo Arias1 1 Instituto Internacional de Investigaciones PrehistoÂricas de Cantabria, Universidad de Cantabria, Santander, Spain, 2 Institut Català de Paleoecologia Humana i Evolucio Social, Universitat Rovira i Virgili, a11111 Tarragona, Spain, 3 Sociedad de Ciencias Aranzadi, San SebastiaÂn, Spain, 4 Museo de Prehistoria y ArqueologÂõa de Cantabria, Santander, Spain * [email protected] Abstract OPEN ACCESS Pleistocene skinning and exploitation of carnivore furs have been previously inferred from Citation: Cueto M, CamaroÂs E, Castaños P, archaeological evidence. Nevertheless, the evidence of skinning and fur processing tends Onta oÂn R, Arias P (2016) Under the Skin of a ñ to be weak and the interpretations are not strongly sustained by the archaeological record. Lion: Unique Evidence of Upper Paleolithic Exploitation and Use of Cave Lion (Panthera In the present paper, we analyze unique evidence of patterned anthropic modification and spelaea) from the Lower Gallery of La Garma skeletal representation of fossil remains of cave lion (Panthera spelaea) from the Lower (Spain). PLoS ONE 11(10): e0163591. Gallery of La Garma (Cantabria, Spain). This site is one of the few that provides Pleisto- doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0163591 cene examples of lion exploitation by humans. Our archaeozoological study suggests that Editor: Roberto Macchiarelli, Universite de Poitiers, lion-specialized pelt exploitation and use might have been related to ritual activities during FRANCE the Middle Magdalenian period (ca.
    [Show full text]
  • 6 Karavanic Format Kolektivni Monografie
    See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/287643498 Research on Underwater Mousterian: The Site of Resnik - Kaštel Štafilić, Dalmatia, Croatia Chapter · January 2015 DOI: 10.5817/CZ.MUNI.M210-7781-2015-6 CITATIONS READS 2 289 1 author: Ivor Karavanić University of Zagreb 98 PUBLICATIONS 2,099 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects: Experimental approach to the coevolution of language/cognition and Palaeolithic stone tool production View project PALAEOARTEAST: "Defining cultural boundaries in the European Upper PALAEOlithic: Archaeology and Rock arT in EASTern Europe" View project All content following this page was uploaded by Ivor Karavanić on 21 December 2015. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. Bibliographic Citation of the book: SÁZELOVÁ, Sandra, Martin NOVÁK and Alena MIZEROVÁ (eds.). Forgotten times and spaces: New perspectives in paleoanthropological, paleoetnological and archeological studies. 1st Edition. Brno: Institute of Archeology of the Czech Academy of Sciences; Masaryk University, 2015, 618 p. ISBN 978‐80‐7524‐000‐2; ISBN 978‐80‐210‐7781‐2. DOI: 10.5817/CZ.MUNI.M210‐ 7781‐2015. Bibliographic Citation of the article: KARAVANIĆ, Ivor. Research on underwater Mousterian: The site of Resnik – Kaštel Štafilić, Dalmatia, Croatia. In: Sandra SÁZELOVÁ, Martin NOVÁK and Alena MIZEROVÁ (eds.). Forgotten times and spaces: New perspectives in paleoanthropological, paleoetnological and archeological studies. 1st Edition. Brno: Institute of Archeology of the Czech Academy of Sciences; Masaryk University, 2015, pp. 73–79. ISBN 978‐80‐7524‐000‐2; ISBN 978‐80‐210‐7781‐2.
    [Show full text]
  • Radiocarbon Dates from the Grotte Du Renne and Saint-Césaire Support a Neandertal Origin for the Châtelperronian
    Radiocarbon dates from the Grotte du Renne and Saint-Césaire support a Neandertal origin for the Châtelperronian Jean-Jacques Hublina,1, Sahra Talamoa, Michèle Julienb, Francine Davidb, Nelly Connetb,c, Pierre Bodub, Bernard Vandermeerschd, and Michael P. Richardsa,e aDepartment of Human Evolution, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, 04103 Leipzig, Germany; bUnité Mixte de Recherche 7041 du Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Archéologies et Sciences de l’Antiquité, 92023 Nanterre, France; cInstitut national de recherches archéologiques préventives (INRAP), 86000 Poitiers, France; dUnité Mixte de Recherche 5199 du Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique—Préhistoire à l’Actuel: Culture, Environnement et Anthropologie, Université Bordeaux 1, 33405 Talence cedex, France; and eDepartment of Anthropology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6T 1Z1 Edited by Richard G. Klein, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, and approved September 24, 2012 (received for review July 27, 2012) The transition from the Middle Paleolithic (MP) to Upper Paleo- Despite this fossil evidence, the question of whether Nean- lithic (UP) is marked by the replacement of late Neandertals by dertals manufactured the CP remains the topic of intense debate. modern humans in Europe between 50,000 and 40,000 y ago. This results in part from the fact that the CP is the so-called Châtelperronian (CP) artifact assemblages found in central France “transitional” assemblage that documents the broadest spectrum and northern Spain date to this time period. So far, it is the only of behavioral features reminiscent of the subsequent local UP, such assemblage type that has yielded Neandertal remains directly which was undisputedly created by modern humans.
    [Show full text]