John C. Eccles' Conversion and the Meaning of 'Authority'

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

John C. Eccles' Conversion and the Meaning of 'Authority' Chapter 7 John C. Eccles’ Conversion and the Meaning of ‘Authority’ Fabio De Sio, Nils Hansson and Ulrich Koppitz Robert K. Merton’s tentative taxonomy of the ‘instructively ambiguous’ catego- ries of ‘excellence’ and ‘recognition’ features the Nobel Prize as an example of the couple ‘excellence as performance/recognition as honorific’.1 In this con- nection, Merton raises the problem of what the performance to be recognised should look like. In the sciences, he concludes, the single achievement (as op- posed to ‘life-work’2) seems to be the standard, although what this means is far from self-evident. Alfred Nobel’s three famous criteria for a prize-worthy achievement (‘re- cency’, ‘benefit to mankind’ and ‘discovery’) have equally proven difficult to handle, requiring progressive adjustments (see the Introduction to this vol- ume). In situations of real-life complexity, Merton’s taxonomy of ‘recognition’ and ‘excellence as performance’ shows its analytical limit, as do Nobel’s crite- ria. Even in early, apparently simple, cases of undivided awards, the stumbling block of the ‘individual discovery’ had made itself perspicuous, as shown by the lengthy debate over Ivan Pavlov’s3 or Paul Ehrlich’s award,4 demonstrating how problematic the ‘snapshot’ conception of discovery can be. One is here reminded of Roland Barthes’ concept of ‘punctum’,5 ‘[the] element which rises from the scene, shoots out of it like an arrow, and pierces [us]’. The ‘punctum’ is what commands our attention and makes us notice an image. This event, however, can only be perceived as such within the less perspicuous framework of an educated and idiosyncratic approach, which he calls ‘studium’, and is the ‘application to a thing […] a kind of general, enthusiastic commitment, but without special acuity’.6 Transposed to the problem at hand, the ‘punctum’ can be abruptly translated as ‘(beneficial) discovery’, whereas ‘studium’ be- comes the set of conditions that makes the achievement recognised as a great 1 Merton 1973. 2 Merton 1973, 433. 3 Todes 2002, Chapter 10. 4 Hüntelmann 2018. 5 Barthes 1981, 26. 6 Barthes 1981, 26. © fabio de sio, nils hansson and ulrich koppitz, ���9 | doi:�0.��63/97890044064��_009 This is an open access chapter distributed under the terms ofFabio the cc-by-nc-ndDe Sio, Nils Hansson, 4.0 License. and Ulrich Koppitz - 9789004406421 Downloaded from Brill.com10/03/2021 01:54:05PM via free access <UN> 144 De Sio, Hansson and Koppitz contribution to a specific field. Since it provides the background for our punc- tual awareness of the event, the ‘studium’ requires some effort to be acknowl- edged at all. In what follows, we will attempt the analysis of one famous Nobel laureate from a perspective complementary to that of the binomial excellence/recogni- tion, namely, that of ‘authority’. Although no less ambiguous than the couple it is intended to replace, the concept we propose is just as productive: it allows to consider the co-evolution of discovery7 (individual level) and honorific recog- nition (community), their articulation, the process of maturation of both the knowledge claims and technical innovations that are considered ‘discoveries’, and the general and local criteria that sanction them as such. In other words, we will try in this case to show the inevitable interdependence of ‘punctum’ and ‘studium’. The case is the 1963 award given to the Australian physiologist John C. Eccles (1903–1997), with Alan L. Hodgkin and Andrew F. Huxley, ‘for their discoveries concerning the ionic mechanisms involved in excitation and inhibition in the peripheral and central portions of the nerve cell membrane’. The focus will be on Eccles, the first in the official list of the awardees, a towering figure in the history of neurophysiology and the neurosciences.8 Conversion and the Creation of Authority Eccles’ scientific upbringing took place between 1927 and 1937, in a Mecca of physiological science, the Oxford laboratory of Charles Scott Sherrington, where he directly participated in the completion and refinement of his mas- ter’s life-work on spinal reflexes, rewarded with a Nobel Prize in 1932. In the eyes of his pupil, the old ‘figurehead’ of British science9 was an authority in every sense, a model he tried to follow and match, also beyond the labora- tory walls. Eccles borrowed the master’s experimental approach, integrative outlook,10 and focus on inhibition as a bona fide physiological mechanism.11 7 Throughout the present chapter, we will use ‘discovery’ as a synonym of ‘excellence as performance’ in Merton’s sense. 8 Curtis and Andersen 2001. 9 Smith 2000; see also Smith 1992; Smith 2001; Smith 2003. 10 on which see Sherrington 1906. 11 On the debate over the statute of inhibition (mechanism vs. simple physical consequence of the refractory period after discharge) see Granit 1961, Chapter iv; Jacobson 1993, Chap- ter 4; Smith 1992, Chapter 5. Fabio De Sio, Nils Hansson, and Ulrich Koppitz - 9789004406421 Downloaded from Brill.com10/03/2021 01:54:05PM via free access <UN> John C. Eccles’ Conversion and the Meaning of ‘Authority’ 145 Not least, he fashioned himself as an apostle of Sherrington’s Cartesian- dualistic view of man.12 As a consequence of his collaboration with Sherrington, in a series of studies on the summation of impulses at the synapse in the early 1930s, Eccles became involved13 in one of the harshest controversies in the history of neurophysiol- ogy: the dispute over the nature (chemical or electrical) of synaptic transmis- sion, aka ‘the war of the soups and the sparks’.14 The hypothesis, championed since the 1910s by some pharmacologists, that acetylcholine played a central role in the transmission of the nerve impulse at every level (neuromyal junc- tion and spinal neurones) had met fierce resistance from ‘proper’ neurophysi- ologists, who favoured a physical (electrical) interpretation in analogy with the mechanism of conduction along the axon. Eccles quickly rose to the leadership of the electrical side. His knowledge of the physiology of reflexes, experimental ingenuity and pugnacity made him, in the words of a witness, ‘the one to whom all the non-believers made reference when they had run out of arguments’.15 He approached the problem at the level of the neuromuscular junction, sensory ganglia and motor cells, piling a mountain of evidence against a hypothesis which he considered insufficient to account for the essential quantitative details (speed, summative character) of synaptic transmission. The main theatre of the dispute were the meetings of the Physiological Society, where he staged memorable fights with the lead- ing ‘chemagonist’16 and future Nobel laureate (1936) Henry H. Dale and associ- ates, often to the embarrassment of the session chairs. The personal relations among the fighters, however, and especially those between Eccles and Dale, were not in the least harmed, as is testified by the eyewitness Bernhard Katz (1996) and the correspondence among the protagonists (fraught with military metaphors).17 Dale and his group privately and publicly acknowledged the beneficial effect of Eccles’ criticism (forcing them to refine experimentation and theory to meet the standards of the neurophysiologists), as well as the balance and measure he displayed in the occasional role of the reviewer.18 12 Sherrington 1938; but see on this Smith 2000; De Sio 2018. 13 Curtis and Andersen 2001. 14 Valenstein 2002; Valenstein 2006. 15 Bacq 1974, 62; For a full history of the controversy see Valenstein 2006; Shepherd 2009; Bennett 2001; Borck 2017. 16 ‘Chemagonists’ was the collective name coined by Walter Cannon (1939) for the support- ers of chemical transmission. Their opponents he called ‘electragonists’. 17 Partly published in Eccles 1976; Eccles 1982; Girolami et al. 1994; two letters also in Freund et al. 2011. 18 See Dale to Eccles 29/1/37, Eccles Archives, Medizingeschichte Düsseldorf (eamd) 2NZ-2023. Fabio De Sio, Nils Hansson, and Ulrich Koppitz - 9789004406421 Downloaded from Brill.com10/03/2021 01:54:05PM via free access <UN> 146 De Sio, Hansson and Koppitz Nevertheless, Eccles’ ‘super aggressive personality’19 did not fit the Oxonian style and raised more than an eyebrow. When Sherrington retired in 1936, his pupil’s aspirations to succession were thwarted, leaving him no choice but to accept a research position in his native Australia. The rest of the story has been recounted several times by the protagonist,20 echoed by pupils and ha- giographers, finally making its way into historiography. It is a story of endless meandering (from Sydney to Dunedin, New Zealand and back to Canberra), isolation, and frustration, but also of great achievement against all odds, of an astonishing inversion of the centre-periphery power relations in wartime, and of a mighty new school of neurophysiology being born down under.21 Most perspicuously, however, it is a story of conversions. At a most delicate juncture in his career, as the electrical theory of synaptic transmission was progressively buried under a snowfall of contrary evidence,22 Eccles came under the spell of the philosopher Karl Popper. According to his own, oft-repeated account23 Popper liberated him from the ‘inductive dogma’, persuading him of the necessity of clear-cut hypotheses, and that falsification of one’s own theories is not a shame. With his guidance, Eccles set to work to formulate a theoretical version of his electrical hypotheses of synaptic excita- tion24 and inhibition,25 with predictions and possible tests, and then tried to disprove them. The inhibition theory was the first to fall under the fire ‘of his own Popperian architect’,26 in a fateful summer night of 1951. The model was based on the competition between the sensory stimuli (excitatory), and the interfering action of some short-axon interneurones, the ‘Golgi cells’, which, once activated by collaterals of afferent axons, subliminally depolarized the motoneurone, making it inert to the following trains of impulses from the re- ceptors.
Recommended publications
  • Eric Kandel Form in Which This Information Is Stored
    RESEARCH I NEWS pendent, this system would be expected to [2] HEEsch and J E Bums. Distance estimation work quite well, since the new recruit bees by foraging honeybees, The Journal ofExperi­ mental Biology, Vo1.199, pp. 155-162, 1996. tend to take the same route as the experi­ [3] M V Srinivasan, S W Zhang, M Lehrer, and T enced scout bee. What dOJhe ants do when S Collett, Honeybee Navigation en route to they cover similarly several kilometres on the goal: Visual flight control and odometry, foot to look for food? Preliminary evidence The Journal ofExperimental Biology, Vol. 199, pp. 237-244, 1996. indicates that they don't use an odometer but [4] M V Srinivasan, S W Zhang, M Altwein, and instead might count steps! J Tautz, Honeybee Navigation: Nature and Calibration of the Odometer, Science, Vol. Suggested Reading 287, pp. 851-853, 1996. [1] Karl von Frisch, The dance language and OT­ Moushumi Sen Sarma, Centre for Ecological Sci­ ientation of bees, Harvard Univ. Press, Cam­ ences, Indian Institute of Science, Bangalore 560012, bridge, MA, USA, 1993. India. Email: [email protected] Learning from a Sea Snail: modify future behaviour, then memory is the Eric Kandel form in which this information is stored. Together, they represent one of the most valuable and powerful adaptations ever to Rohini Balakrishnan have evolved in nervous systems, for they In the year 2000, Eric Kandel shared the allow the future to access the past, conferring Nobel Prize in Physiology and Medicinel flexibility to behaviour and improving the with two other neurobiologists: Arvid chances of survival in unpredictable, rapidly Carlsson and Paul Greengard.
    [Show full text]
  • 書 名 等 発行年 出版社 受賞年 備考 N1 Ueber Das Zustandekommen Der
    書 名 等 発行年 出版社 受賞年 備考 Ueber das Zustandekommen der Diphtherie-immunitat und der Tetanus-Immunitat bei thieren / Emil Adolf N1 1890 Georg thieme 1901 von Behring N2 Diphtherie und tetanus immunitaet / Emil Adolf von Behring und Kitasato 19-- [Akitomo Matsuki] 1901 Malarial fever its cause, prevention and treatment containing full details for the use of travellers, University press of N3 1902 1902 sportsmen, soldiers, and residents in malarious places / by Ronald Ross liverpool Ueber die Anwendung von concentrirten chemischen Lichtstrahlen in der Medicin / von Prof. Dr. Niels N4 1899 F.C.W.Vogel 1903 Ryberg Finsen Mit 4 Abbildungen und 2 Tafeln Twenty-five years of objective study of the higher nervous activity (behaviour) of animals / Ivan N5 Petrovitch Pavlov ; translated and edited by W. Horsley Gantt ; with the collaboration of G. Volborth ; and c1928 International Publishing 1904 an introduction by Walter B. Cannon Conditioned reflexes : an investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex / by Ivan Oxford University N6 1927 1904 Petrovitch Pavlov ; translated and edited by G.V. Anrep Press N7 Die Ätiologie und die Bekämpfung der Tuberkulose / Robert Koch ; eingeleitet von M. Kirchner 1912 J.A.Barth 1905 N8 Neue Darstellung vom histologischen Bau des Centralnervensystems / von Santiago Ramón y Cajal 1893 Veit 1906 Traité des fiévres palustres : avec la description des microbes du paludisme / par Charles Louis Alphonse N9 1884 Octave Doin 1907 Laveran N10 Embryologie des Scorpions / von Ilya Ilyich Mechnikov 1870 Wilhelm Engelmann 1908 Immunität bei Infektionskrankheiten / Ilya Ilyich Mechnikov ; einzig autorisierte übersetzung von Julius N11 1902 Gustav Fischer 1908 Meyer Die experimentelle Chemotherapie der Spirillosen : Syphilis, Rückfallfieber, Hühnerspirillose, Frambösie / N12 1910 J.Springer 1908 von Paul Ehrlich und S.
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction and Historical Perspective
    Chapter 1 Introduction and Historical Perspective “ Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution. ” modified by the developmental history of the organism, Theodosius Dobzhansky its physiology – from cellular to systems levels – and by the social and physical environment. Finally, behaviors are shaped through evolutionary forces of natural selection OVERVIEW that optimize survival and reproduction ( Figure 1.1 ). Truly, the study of behavior provides us with a window through Behavioral genetics aims to understand the genetic which we can view much of biology. mechanisms that enable the nervous system to direct Understanding behaviors requires a multidisciplinary appropriate interactions between organisms and their perspective, with regulation of gene expression at its core. social and physical environments. Early scientific The emerging field of behavioral genetics is still taking explorations of animal behavior defined the fields shape and its boundaries are still being defined. Behavioral of experimental psychology and classical ethology. genetics has evolved through the merger of experimental Behavioral genetics has emerged as an interdisciplin- psychology and classical ethology with evolutionary biol- ary science at the interface of experimental psychology, ogy and genetics, and also incorporates aspects of neuro- classical ethology, genetics, and neuroscience. This science ( Figure 1.2 ). To gain a perspective on the current chapter provides a brief overview of the emergence of definition of this field, it is helpful
    [Show full text]
  • English Summary
    English summary The Nobel Prize Career of Ragnar Granit. A Study of the Prizes of Science and the Science of the Prizes This study is concerned with two closely related themes: the reward system of science – i .e . the various means by which scientists express their admiration and esteem for their colleagues – and the role played by social networks within this broader framework . The study approa- ches its topic from the viewpoint of the Nobel Prize for Physiology or Medicine, often referred to as the Nobel Prize in Medicine . The focus of the study is on the lengthy process that led to the granting of the 1967 Nobel Prize to Ragnar Granit (1901–1991) for his discoveries concer- ning the primary physiological visual processes in the eye . His award was preceded by one of the most dramatic conflicts within the prize authorities during the post-war decades, and serves here to illustrate the dynamics and the various strategies employed in the Nobel Com- mittee of the Karolinska Institute . In addition, Granit’s career as a No- bel Prize candidate is used as a window through which it is possible to examine the various ways in which elite networks in the scientific field operate . In order to enable comparison, the Nobel careers of Charles Best, Hugo Theorell, and John Eccles are also discussed . On a more ge- neral level the Nobel careers of other scientists who received the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine in the period 1940–1960 are also dis- cussed, whereby, as an offshoot of the study, a general picture of the Nobel institution in the post-war decades emerges .
    [Show full text]
  • U. S. Von Euler (1905-1983) Uif S. Von Euler's Contributions to Science
    296 fermentation of sugar. His mother, Astrld Cleve, daughter of the notable chemist P. T. Cleve, was a scientist in her own right, known for her studic~ tff f~ssil Diatomaceae. His godththeT was Svantc U. S. von Euler Arrhenius. Speaking of the scientific atmosphere at (1905-1983) his home and the regular opportunities for meeting scientists, von Euler stated that this Ulf von Euler, who was Professor of and testing substances from body fluids and "no doubt had a great part in my growing Physiology at the Karolinska Institute from tissues was sustained throughout his life. interest in research'. To his own work his 1939 to 1972, died in March this year at the The work suited his gifts - with their basic attitude was objective and he readily ack- age of 78 years. After his major discovery elements of technical skill, great care,' nowledged the contributions to his own in 1946 that consisted of the isolation and perseverance and industry - beyond ordi- field of interest by forerunners and content- identification of noradrenaline in sympathe- nary measures. He was richly rewarded poraries. His matter-of-fact attitude and tic ganglia and nerve fibres, he achieved an (see Bengt Pemow's contribution to this courteous manner contained elements of international reputation as a pioneer in issue of TINS). guarded prudence, producing a barrier catecholamine physiology. Von Euler's life seemed from the begin- which had to be penetrated by those who In 1921 Otto Loewi had found adrenaline ning to be destined for a scientific career. were his co-workers or just his friends.
    [Show full text]
  • Consciousness Eclipsed: Jacques Loeb, Ivan P. Pavlov, and the Rise of Reductionistic Biology After 1900
    Consciousness and Cognition Consciousness and Cognition 14 (2005) 219–230 www.elsevier.com/locate/concog Consciousness eclipsed: Jacques Loeb, Ivan P. Pavlov, and the rise of reductionistic biology after 1900 Ralph J. Greenspan*, Bernard J. Baars The Neurosciences Institute, 10640 John Jay Hopkins Dr., San Diego, CA 92121, United States Received 17 May 2004 Available online 25 November 2004 Abstract The life sciences in the 20th century were guided to a large extent by a reductionist program seeking to explain biological phenomena in terms of physics and chemistry. Two scientists who figured prominently in the establishment and dissemination of this program were Jacques Loeb in biology and Ivan P. Pavlov in psychological behaviorism. While neither succeeded in accounting for higher mental functions in physical- chemical terms, both adopted positions that reduced the problem of consciousness to the level of reflexes and associations. The intellectual origins of this view and the impediment to the study of consciousness as an object of inquiry in its own right that it may have imposed on peers, students, and those who followed is explored. Ó 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: History of ideas; Reductionism; Tropism; Conditional reflex 1. Introduction The current acceptance of consciousness as a suitable object of study in the life sciences came late in the 20th century (Flanagan, 1984). By that time other biological processes—physiology, biochemistry, genetics, embryology, and even many aspects of brain function—had long since * Corresponding author. Fax: +1 858 626 2099. E-mail addresses: [email protected] (R.J. Greenspan), [email protected] (B.J.
    [Show full text]
  • Walter Rudolf Hess. Cirujano, Fisiólogo Y Premio Nobel
    Cir CirujMartínez-Mier 2000; 68: 132-137G y col. Walter Rudolf Hess. Cirujano, fisiólogo y premio Nobel Dr. Gustavo Martínez-Mier,* Acad. Dr. Luis Horacio Toledo-Pereyra* Resumen Summary Objetivo: narrar la vida y obra de Walter Rudolf Hess. Objective: To narrate the life and legacy of Walter Rudolf Diseño: ensayo histórico (13 referencias). Hess. Sede: Departamento de historia universitario. Design: Historic essay (13 References). Walter Rudolf Hess nació en Suiza el 17 de marzo de 1881. Setting: University Department of History. Realizó sus estudios de medicina en las universidades de Walter Rudolf Hess was born in Switzerland on March 17, Lausana, Berna, Berlín y Kiel, graduándose en la Universi- 1881. He did his medical school studies at the Universities dad de Zurich en 1905. Adquirió entrenamiento quirúrgico y of Lausenne, Bern, Berlin, and Kiel and graduated from Uni- oftalmológico en Münsterlingen y Zurich, para en 1908 ini- versity of Zurich in 1905. He was trained in surgery and oph- ciar una próspera práctica privada en Rapperswil, Suiza thalmology in Münsterlingen and Zurich and began a lucra- como cirujano oftalmólogo. En 1912, sacrificó la bonanza tive private practice in Repperswil, Switzerland as an oph- económica e ingresó al Instituto de Fisiología de la Univer- thalmologist in 1908. He sacrificed economic wealth in 1912 sidad de Zurich, donde lograría ser su director y jefe, per- and entered The Physiology Institute of the University of maneciendo en el puesto por 34 años. Fue un oftalmólogo Zurich, where he became its chairman holding the position talentoso y preciso, que diseñó un aparato para el examen for 34 years.
    [Show full text]
  • The Fessard's School of Neurophysiology After
    The fessard’s School of neurophysiology after the Second World War in france: globalisation and diversity in neurophysiological research (1938-1955) Jean-Gaël Barbara To cite this version: Jean-Gaël Barbara. The fessard’s School of neurophysiology after the Second World War in france: globalisation and diversity in neurophysiological research (1938-1955). Archives Italiennes de Biologie, Universita degli Studi di Pisa, 2011. halshs-03090650 HAL Id: halshs-03090650 https://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-03090650 Submitted on 11 Jan 2021 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. The Fessard’s School of neurophysiology after the Second World War in France: globalization and diversity in neurophysiological research (1938-1955) Jean- Gaël Barbara Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Paris, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, CNRS UMR 7102 Université Denis Diderot, Paris, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, CNRS UMR 7219 [email protected] Postal Address : JG Barbara, UPMC, case 14, 7 quai Saint Bernard, 75005,
    [Show full text]
  • Balcomk41251.Pdf (558.9Kb)
    Copyright by Karen Suzanne Balcom 2005 The Dissertation Committee for Karen Suzanne Balcom Certifies that this is the approved version of the following dissertation: Discovery and Information Use Patterns of Nobel Laureates in Physiology or Medicine Committee: E. Glynn Harmon, Supervisor Julie Hallmark Billie Grace Herring James D. Legler Brooke E. Sheldon Discovery and Information Use Patterns of Nobel Laureates in Physiology or Medicine by Karen Suzanne Balcom, B.A., M.L.S. Dissertation Presented to the Faculty of the Graduate School of The University of Texas at Austin in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy The University of Texas at Austin August, 2005 Dedication I dedicate this dissertation to my first teachers: my father, George Sheldon Balcom, who passed away before this task was begun, and to my mother, Marian Dyer Balcom, who passed away before it was completed. I also dedicate it to my dissertation committee members: Drs. Billie Grace Herring, Brooke Sheldon, Julie Hallmark and to my supervisor, Dr. Glynn Harmon. They were all teachers, mentors, and friends who lifted me up when I was down. Acknowledgements I would first like to thank my committee: Julie Hallmark, Billie Grace Herring, Jim Legler, M.D., Brooke E. Sheldon, and Glynn Harmon for their encouragement, patience and support during the nine years that this investigation was a work in progress. I could not have had a better committee. They are my enduring friends and I hope I prove worthy of the faith they have always showed in me. I am grateful to Dr.
    [Show full text]
  • ESRS 40Th Anniversary Book
    European Sleep Research Society 1972 – 2012 40th Anniversary of the ESRS Editor: Claudio L. Bassetti Co-Editors: Brigitte Knobl, Hartmut Schulz European Sleep Research Society 1972 – 2012 40th Anniversary of the ESRS Editor: Claudio L. Bassetti Co-Editors: Brigitte Knobl, Hartmut Schulz Imprint Editor Publisher and Layout Claudio L. Bassetti Wecom Gesellschaft für Kommunikation mbH & Co. KG Co-Editors Hildesheim / Germany Brigitte Knobl, Hartmut Schulz www.wecom.org © European Sleep Research Society (ESRS), Regensburg, Bern, 2012 For amendments there can be given no limit or warranty by editor and publisher. Table of Contents Presidential Foreword . 5 Future Perspectives The Future of Sleep Research and Sleep Medicine in Europe: A Need for Academic Multidisciplinary Sleep Centres C. L. Bassetti, D.-J. Dijk, Z. Dogas, P. Levy, L. L. Nobili, P. Peigneux, T. Pollmächer, D. Riemann and D. J. Skene . 7 Historical Review of the ESRS General History of the ESRS H. Schulz, P. Salzarulo . 9 The Presidents of the ESRS (1972 – 2012) T. Pollmächer . 13 ESRS Congresses M. Billiard . 15 History of the Journal of Sleep Research (JSR) J. Horne, P. Lavie, D.-J. Dijk . 17 Pictures of the Past and Present of Sleep Research and Sleep Medicine in Europe J. Horne, H. Schulz . 19 Past – Present – Future Sleep and Neuroscience R. Amici, A. Borbély, P. L. Parmeggiani, P. Peigneux . 23 Sleep and Neurology C. L. Bassetti, L. Ferini-Strambi, J. Santamaria . 27 Psychiatric Sleep Research T. Pollmächer . 31 Sleep and Psychology D. Riemann, C. Espie . 33 Sleep and Sleep Disordered Breathing P. Levy, J. Hedner . 35 Sleep and Chronobiology A.
    [Show full text]
  • Cells of the Nervous System
    3/23/2015 Nervous Systems | Principles of Biology from Nature Education contents Principles of Biology 126 Nervous Systems A flock of Canada geese use auditory and visual cues to maintain a V­ formation in flight. How are these animals able to respond so quickly to environmental cues? All animals possess neurons, cells that form a complex network capable of transmitting and receiving signals. This neural network forms the nervous system. The nervous system coordinates the movement and internal physiology of an organism, as well as its decision­making and behavior. In all but the simplest animals, neurons are bundled into nerves that facilitate signal transmission. More complex animals have a central nervous system (CNS) that includes the brain and nerve cords. Vertebrates also have a peripheral nervous system (PNS) that transmits signals between the body and the CNS. Cells of the Nervous System Structure of the neuron. Figure 1 shows the general structure of a neuron. The organelles and nucleus of a neuron are contained in a large central structure called the cell body, or soma. Most nerve cells also have multiple dendrites in addition to the cell body. Dendrites are short, branched extensions that receive signals from other neurons. Each neuron also has a single axon, a long extension that transmits signals to other cells. The point of attachment of the axon to the cell body is called the axon hillock. The other end of the axon is usually branched, and each branch ends in a synaptic terminal. The synaptic terminal forms a synapse, or junction, with another cell.
    [Show full text]
  • Kandel and Hawkins
    © 1992 SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN, INC The Biological Basis of Learnin and Individuality Recent discoveries suggestg that learning engages a simple set of rules that modify the strength of connections between neurons in the brain. These changes play an important role in making each individual unique by Eric R. Kandel and Robert D. Hawkins ver the past several decades, there surgeon at the Montreal Neurological Institute. has been a gradual merger of two In the 1940s Penfield began to use electri­ originally separate fields of science: cal stimulation to map motor, sensory and neurobiology,O the science of the brain, and language functions in the cortex of patients cognitive psychology, the science of the mind. undergoing neurosurgery for the relief of Recently the pace of unification has quick­ epilepsy. Because the brain itself does not ened, with the result that a new intellectual have pain receptors, brain surgery can be car­ framework has emerged for examining percep­ ried out under local anesthesia in fully con­ tion, language, memory and conscious aware­ scious patients, who can describe what they ness. This new framework is based on the abil­ experience in response to electric stimuli ap­ ity to study the biological substrates of these plied to different cortical areas. Penfield ex­ mental functions. A particularly faSCinating plored the cortical surface in more than 1,000 example can be seen in the study of learning. Elementary as­ patients. Occasionally he found that electrical stimulation pects of the neuronal mechanisms important for several dif­ produced an experiential response, or flashback, in which ferent types of learning can now be studied on the cellular the patients described a coherent recollection of an earlier and even on the molecular level.
    [Show full text]