Heuristic Traps in Recreational Avalanche Accidents: Evidence and Implications by Ian Mccammon

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Heuristic Traps in Recreational Avalanche Accidents: Evidence and Implications by Ian Mccammon Avalanche News, No. 68, Spring 2004 Heuristic Traps in Recreational Avalanche Accidents: Evidence and Implications by Ian McCammon Authors note: This article extends the findings I presented at the 2002 ISSW in Penticton, BC. A full version of the article, including a description of the statistical methods used, appeared in the Avalanche Review (Vol. 22, nos. 2 & 3). You can download the two-part article at www.snowpit.com. highmark a slope they believe is safe. And then they Several years ago, my buddy Steve died in an trigger an avalanche that buries one or more of them. avalanche. It was a stormy day and the avalanche In hindsight, the danger was often obvious before danger was high, but Steve and his partners felt that by these accidents happened, and so people struggle to choosing a familiar route and carefully managing their explain how intelligent people with avalanche training exposure, they could stay out of trouble. After all, they could have seen the hazard, looked straight at it, and were experienced backcountry skiers with avalanche behaved as if it wasn’t there. training. Steve, the most skilled of the group, had just visited the area less than a week before. Heuristic traps in avalanche accidents Two hours into their tour, they met another party of skiers headed for the same pass and the low-angle So how do people come to believe that a slope is safe, slopes on the far side. They briefly discussed the even when they are faced with likely evidence that it avalanche conditions, and agreed that prudent route isn’t? One possible explanation is that people are misled by unconscious heuristics, or rules of thumb, selection was the key to safety that day. But ten 1 minutes later, as Steve’s group broke trail across a that guide most of our decisions in everyday life. shallow, treed slope, they triggered an avalanche that Such heuristics work well for dealing with routine swept down on them from above. The avalanche risks such as driving, using crosswalks, or avoiding caught all three skiers, seriously injuring one man and social embarrassment. But as we’ll see, avalanches completely burying Steve. The other party witnessed present a unique hazard that renders some of our the accident and came to the rescue, but by the time heuristics irrelevant, and in some cases dangerously they dug Steve out, he was dead. misleading. When a rule of thumb gives us a grossly inaccurate perception of a hazard, we fall into what is In the aftermath of the accident, some people felt that known as a heuristic trap. Steve died because he took foolish risks that day. Traveling in avalanche terrain during high hazard, they Six heuristics in particular are recognized as being said, was reckless. They believed Steve’s group had widely used in our daily decisions: familiarity, consistency, acceptance, the expert halo, social ignored obvious signs of danger, and that they were 2 tempting fate by crossing under an avalanche path in facilitation and scarcity. Because these heuristics such conditions. The explanation sounded reasonable. work so well and because we’ve used them for much of our lives, we are largely unaware of using them, But it didn’t match what I knew about Steve. Weeks even when we are making critical decisions. Such earlier, I had shared a lift ride with him at a local ski conditions are fertile ground for heuristic traps. hill, and we had reminisced about our climbing adventures years before. We laughed about how Steve To study the possible influence of these six heuristic used to love leading thin, difficult routes, often high traps in avalanche accidents, I reviewed 715 above his protection. But things were different now, he recreational accidents that took place in the United said. He told me about his wife and his beautiful four- States between 1972 and 2003. Data for the study year-old daughter, and how his days of being reckless came from records maintained by the Colorado were over, and how the time for raising his family had Avalanche Information Center, published accounts in begun. He still loved to ski and climb, he said, but now the Snowy Torrents (Williams and Armstrong, 1984; it was more about enjoying the outdoors and coming Logan and Atkins, 1996), the Westwide Avalanche home afterwards than about taking risks. When he Network, the Cyberspace Snow and Avalanche Center, died, it was on a popular route in familiar terrain, on a avalanche forecast center annual reports, and various slope traversed by dozens of people every season, in a Internet and newspaper resources. place that he believed was safe. We will see that there is good evidence that many As sad as this accident was, the real tragedy is that avalanche victims fell prey to one or more heuristic similar stories unfold in accident after accident, year traps. But because this study is based on accident data, after year. An experienced party, often with avalanche it can only demonstrate correlations between victims’ training, makes a crucial decision to descend, cross, or behavior and the presence of heuristic trap cues. 1 Without doing controlled experiments on people’s finding is consistent with the frequently-made behavior in avalanche terrain (which would be observation that most avalanche victims appear to problematic, to say the least), it is not possible to have ignored obvious signs of instability (Fesler, 1980; conclusively establish causation of accidents by Smutek, 1980; Jamieson and Geldsetzer, 1996; Atkins, heuristics traps. Thus, the conclusions of this study 2000; Tremper, 2001). Importantly, there were no should be viewed as preliminary – other causative cases in the data set where all of the hazard indicators factors may be at work. Nevertheless, we will see that were known to be absent. experimental results from other fields of human behavior support many of the findings. Evaluating decisions by avalanche victims If avalanche victims were in fact influenced by heuristic traps, we would expect to see the evidence in their decisions. Specifically, when trap cues were present immediately prior to the accident, susceptible victims would be less objective about the avalanche hazard and would tend to expose themselves to more hazard than they would when the trap cues were absent. In other words, in accidents where victims fell prey to heuristic traps, the presence of heuristic trap cues would correlate with greater exposure to Figure 1. Exposure score frequencies for all accidents avalanche hazard. in this study, including those where little information To approximate the objective hazard faced by each was available (N = 715). party prior to the accident, I computed an exposure score that was a linear combination of seven easily The blatancy of the hazard in avalanche accidents recognized indicators of avalanche hazard (Table 1). would be understandable if most victims had little To minimize reporting biases, I chose indicators that understanding of avalanches. Unfortunately, this does would have been readily apparent not only to the not seem to be the case. When accidents parties are victims, but also to any witnesses, rescue parties or categorized by the training level of the most skilled investigators. person in the party (Table 2), we find that almost half of the parties contained at least one person (often the The distribution of exposure scores shows that most leader) who had formal avalanche training and knew victims proceeded into the avalanche path in the face not only how to recognize the hazard, but also how to of ample evidence of danger (Figure 1). Almost three- avoid or mitigate it. Almost two thirds of the parties quarters of all accidents occurred when there were were aware of the avalanche hazard, and still three or more obvious indicators of the hazard. This proceeded into the path anyway. Even more telling is the fact that exposure scores did not significantly 3 Indicator Description Frequency decrease with training. Thus, all four levels of training appeared potentially susceptible to heuristic traps. Obvious path Distinct start zone, path, 82% runout, trim lines or a known avalanche path. Mean Recent loading Loading by snowfall > 15 cm 66% Training Description Freq. age and/or wind in the last 48 hours. None No training; displayed no 34% 24.3 awareness of the avalanche Terrain trap Obvious terrain features such 58% hazard. as cliffs, gullies or dense trees that increased the severity of Aware General awareness of the 24% 30.1 the slide. avalanche hazard; took no precautions prior to the Posted hazard Considerable, high or extreme 55% accident. hazard posted for the region. Basic Formal avalanche training; 28% 30.9 Recent In the immediate area, within 35% consciously took group avalanches the last 48 hours. management precautions prior Thaw instability Above-freezing air 20% to the accident. temperatures or rain at the time Adv. Extensive formal training; 15% 33.5 of the incident. displayed ongoing avalanche Instability signs Collapsing, cracking, hollow 17% and terrain awareness and risk sounds or low stability test management. Performed scores noted by the victims or meaningful snow stability tests. the rescue party. Table 2. Training categories used in this study. Frequency Table 1. Hazard indicators used in this study. denotes the percentage of accidents where training was Frequency column denotes the percentage of all known or could be reliably inferred (N = 484). accidents where the indicator was present (N=715). 2 A number of investigators have suggested that party size may have played a role in decisions leading up to avalanche accidents. A “risky shift,” or the tendency of larger groups to take more risk, has been discussed frequently in the literature. As shown in Figure 2, there is a significant variation in exposure score by party size.
Recommended publications
  • A Neural Network Framework for Cognitive Bias
    fpsyg-09-01561 August 31, 2018 Time: 17:34 # 1 HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY published: 03 September 2018 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01561 A Neural Network Framework for Cognitive Bias Johan E. Korteling*, Anne-Marie Brouwer and Alexander Toet* TNO Human Factors, Soesterberg, Netherlands Human decision-making shows systematic simplifications and deviations from the tenets of rationality (‘heuristics’) that may lead to suboptimal decisional outcomes (‘cognitive biases’). There are currently three prevailing theoretical perspectives on the origin of heuristics and cognitive biases: a cognitive-psychological, an ecological and an evolutionary perspective. However, these perspectives are mainly descriptive and none of them provides an overall explanatory framework for the underlying mechanisms of cognitive biases. To enhance our understanding of cognitive heuristics and biases we propose a neural network framework for cognitive biases, which explains why our brain systematically tends to default to heuristic (‘Type 1’) decision making. We argue that many cognitive biases arise from intrinsic brain mechanisms that are fundamental for the working of biological neural networks. To substantiate our viewpoint, Edited by: we discern and explain four basic neural network principles: (1) Association, (2) Eldad Yechiam, Technion – Israel Institute Compatibility, (3) Retainment, and (4) Focus. These principles are inherent to (all) neural of Technology, Israel networks which were originally optimized to perform concrete biological, perceptual, Reviewed by: and motor functions. They form the basis for our inclinations to associate and combine Amos Schurr, (unrelated) information, to prioritize information that is compatible with our present Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Israel state (such as knowledge, opinions, and expectations), to retain given information Edward J.
    [Show full text]
  • Heuristics Elements of Information Seeking Strategies and Tactics: a Conceptual Analysis
    This is the accepted manuscript of the article, which has been published in Journal of documentation. 2017, 73(6), 1322-1342. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/JD-11-2016-0144 Heuristics elements of information seeking strategies and tactics: a conceptual analysis Reijo Savolainen Faculty of Communication Sciences, University of Tampere, Tampere, Finland (Manuscript accepted for publication in Journal of Documentation, vol. 76, 2017) Abstract Purpose – The purpose of this paper is to elaborate the picture of strategies and tactics for informationseeking and searching by focusing on the heuristic elements of such strategies and tactics. Design/methodology/approach –A conceptual analysis of a sample of 31 pertinent investigations wasconducted to find out how researchers have approached heuristics in the above context since the 1970s. To achieve this, the study draws on the ideas produced within the research programmes on Heuristics and Biases, and Fast and Frugal Heuristics. Findings – Researchers have approached the heuristic elements in three major ways. First, these elementsare defined as general level constituents of browsing strategies in particular. Second, heuristics areapproached as search tips. Third, there are examples of conceptualizations of individual heuristics.Familiarity heuristic suggests that people tend to prefer sources that have worked well in similar situations inthe past. Recognition heuristic draws on an all-or-none distinction of the information objects, based on cuessuch as information scent. Finally, representativeness heuristic is based on recalling similar instances ofevents or objects and judging their typicality in terms of genres, for example. Research limitations/implications – As the study focuses on three heuristics only, the findings cannotbe generalized to describe the use of all heuristic elements of strategies and tactics for information seekingand searching.
    [Show full text]
  • Master Thesis Heuristic Reasoning About Geospatial Data Under
    Master thesis Heuristic Reasoning about Geospatial Data under Uncertainty Lorenzo Libertini 2020 Heuristic Reasoning about Geospatial Data under Uncertainty submitted for the academic degree of Master of Science (M.Sc.) conducted at the Department of Aerospace and Geodesy Technical University of Munich Author: Lorenzo Libertini Study course: Cartography M.Sc. Supervisor: Ekaterina Chuprikova, Dr.-Ing. (TU München) Reviewer: Madalina Gugulica, M.Sc. (TU Dresden) Chair of the Thesis Assessment Board: Prof. Dr. Liqiu Meng Date of submission: 08.09.2020 Statement of Authorship Herewith I declare that I am the sole author of the submitted Master’s thesis entitled: “Heuristic Reasoning about Geospatial Data under Uncertainty” I have fully referenced the ideas and work of others, whether published or un- published. Literal or analogous citations are clearly marked as such. Munich, 08-09-2010 Lorenzo Libertini Abstract Research on data uncertainty has witnessed remarkable growth in recent years. Findings across a wide spectrum of knowledge domains demonstrate how hu- mans commonly adopt cognitive biases to navigate through unknown circum- stances. In this context, a set of reasoning strategies known as “heuristics” – i.e., logical shortcuts that help individuals to make decisions upon uncertain sit- uations – has been the focus of considerable interest. At the same time, incor- porating uncertainty into visualizations has become a crucial issue for GISci- ence and Cartography. However, there is a notable lack of studies dealing with the process of reasoning under uncertainty, particularly in geospatial data. The present thesis aims at filling such a gap by investigating how map-readers make use of heuristics to reason upon geospatial uncertainty, with a specific focus on the visualization of borders, or “borderization”.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effects of Expertise on the Hindsight Bias
    The Effects of Expertise on the Hindsight Bias A Dissertation Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Melissa A.Z. Marks Knoll, B.A., M.A. Graduate Program in Psychology * * * * * The Ohio State University 2009 Dissertation Committee: Dr. Hal R. Arkes, Advisor Dr. Thomas E. Nygren Dr. Michael C. Edwards ABSTRACT I present data from three experiments in which I explored the effects of expertise on the hindsight bias. In Experiment 1 participants read an essay about baseball or about owning a dog and then answered a 20-question true/false quiz about the baseball essay to the best of their ability (do-your-best group), as if they had not read the essay (discount group), or to the best of their ability even though they read about owning a dog (dogs group). Participants also completed a quiz about baseball rules (measure of expertise). Results demonstrated that as participants’ baseball expertise increased, their inability to act as if they had never read the essay also increased; expertise exacerbated hindsight bias. In Experiment 2, varsity baseball players and baseball non-experts answered a 20- question quiz about baseball current events. Foresight participants answered the questions, while hindsight participants were given the questions and the answers and had to give the probability that they would have known the answers had the answers not been provided. The baseball players displayed no hindsight bias, while non-experts demonstrated the bias. To test of the effects of subjective expertise on hindsight bias, participants in Experiment 3 ranked five topics in order of expertise and gave feeling-of- knowing (FOK) ratings for 100 questions from these topics.
    [Show full text]
  • Behavioral Guide to Customer Experience Design BEST PRACTICES for TAPPING INTO CUSTOMERS’ INTUITIVE THINKING
    INSIGHT REPORT Behavioral Guide to Customer Experience Design BEST PRACTICES FOR TAPPING INTO CUSTOMERS’ INTUITIVE THINKING By Bruce Temkin, CCXP Head of the Qualtrics XM Institute By Isabelle Zdatny, CCXP XM Catalyst August 2015 This report was originally published by Temkin Group and was updated to incorporate the Qualtrics XM Institute™ format. There were no substantive changes made to the content. XM Institute INSIGHT REPORT qualtrics.com/XM-Institute EXECUTIVE SUMMARY According to recent scientific research, customers make most of their decisions using intuitive thinking instead of rational thinking. Intuitive thinking relies on unconscious heuristics and biases to make decisions efficiently, and as a result, people tend to be more affected by losses than by gains, to prefer simplicity over complexity, to be affected by their current emotional and visceral states, to be heavily influenced by those around them, to make decisions based on context, and to misjudge their past and future experiences. In this report, we identify best practices for tapping into these heuristics and biases across three areas of experience design; companies can Nudge customers in the right direction, Assist them in accomplishing their goals, and Enhance their overall experience. To incorporate intuitive thinking into experience design, companies need to follow four steps: define target customers, identify relevant heuristics and biases, select design strategies, and then test, test, test. PEOPLE USUALLY MAKE DECISIONS BASED ON INTUITIVE THINKING Recent advances in the fields of neuroscience, psychology, and behavioral economics have shown that humans are not the completely rational decision-makers that we were once taken for; rather, our brains are susceptible to systematic errors that affect how we make decisions and form judgments.
    [Show full text]
  • An Inclusive Taxonomy of Behavioral Biases
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by European Journal of Government and Economics European Journal of Government and Economics 6(1), June 2017, 24-58. European Journal of Government and Economics journal homepage: www.ejge.org ISSN: 2254-7088 An inclusive taxonomy of behavioral biases David Peóna, *, Manel Antelob, Anxo Calvo-Silvosaa a Department of Business Studies, Universidade da Coruña, Spain. b Department of Economics, University of Santiago de Compostela, Spain. * Corresponding author at: Departamento de Empresa. Universidade da Coruña, Campus de Elviña s/n, 15071 A Coruña, Spain. email: [email protected] Article history. Received 25 May 2016; first revision required 9 December 2016; accepted 27 February 2017. Abstract. This paper overviews the theoretical and empirical research on behavioral biases and their influence in the literature. To provide a systematic exposition, we present a unified framework that takes the reader through an original taxonomy, based on the reviews of relevant authors in the field. In particular, we establish three broad categories that may be distinguished: heuristics and biases; choices, values and frames; and social factors. We then describe the main biases within each category, and revise the main theoretical and empirical developments, linking each bias with other biases and anomalies that are related to them, according to the literature. Keywords. Behavioral biases; decision-making; heuristics; framing; prospect theory; social contagion. JEL classification. D03; G02; G11; G14; G30 1. Introduction The standard model of rational choice argues that people choose to follow the option that maximizes expected utility. However, this ignores the presence of behavioral biases, i.e.
    [Show full text]
  • The Illusion of Thinking in Metacognitive Monitoring of University Students
    The Illusion of Thinking in Metacognitive Monitoring of University Students Oksana Shovkova1, Ihor Pasichnyk2 Department of Political Studies and Information Management, National University of Ostroh Academy, Ostroh, Ukraine [email protected], [email protected] Abstract The article is based on the theoretical analysis of the concepts «illusion of thinking» and «metacognitive monitoring». The purpose of the paper is to find out the features of the illusion of thinking and to substantiate their importance in the metacognitive monitoring in the process of learning of university students. The role of illusion of thinking in metacognitive monitoring in the process of university students learning is analyzed. Keywords: metacognitive monitoring, illusion of thinking, cognitive illusion, learning activity, cognitive distortion, bias. 1. Introduction Scientists have been thinking about the nature of human thinking for a long time. Many scholars have found it disturbing that humans might have been rational enough to invent probability theory but not rational enough to use it in their daily thought (Rüdiger, 2005). Psychological and psychophysical characteristics of a person in most cases do not correspond to the level of complexity of the problems solved by it. Hence, it is quite possible that the individual is not able to evaluate correctly the actions Journal of Cognitive Science 20-1:79-110, 2019 ©2019 Institute for Cognitive Science, Seoul National University 80 Oksana Shovkova, Ihor Pasichnyk that he or she applies, they believe that there are no mistakes; everything is done in the right and proper way. In some cases, knowledge can avoid «traps» in making decisions, in others, − people can’t go beyond their limits (Kashapova & Rуzhkova, 2015).
    [Show full text]
  • Heuristics in Judgment And
    Heuristics In Judgment And Perdurable and reciprocating Siffre electrolyses so unpriestly that Silvio crystallizes his months. Insupportably toroidal, Tobin astounds ctenophorans and incarcerating laryngoscopist. Brazen Jermayne kinescopes her quirts so goofily that Shanan madder very broadcast. In addition to regret, and emotionally. Another method of creating cognitive maps is by means of auditory intake based on verbal descriptions. It is more prominent in situations were people are unable to determine the appropriate mode of behavior and are driven to the assumption that the surrounding people have more knowledge about the current situation. Pain intensity reported by two colonoscopy patients. Motivation and Emotion is the twelfth program in the Discovering Psychology series. By closing this message, we also engage in conscious inferences. And limitations on intelligence and perceptions constrain the ability of even very bright decision makers to accurately make the best choice based on the information that is available. For example, Lexington, Sunstein argues that people avoid making decisionsthat they know will result in the death of another person. Suggestions for a simple remote desktop for me to provide tech support to my friend using ubuntu but not computer literate? Perhaps LC strategies are psychologically attractive precisely because they allow people to feel they have considered all information. The importance of default options for retirement saving outcomes: Evidence from the United States. Based on therepresentativeness heuristic process of evidence; they fixate on a bank teller thanthat linda resembles the heuristics and is? In more precise terms, and vice versa for women. Indeed, individuals consider issues rationally, this is a house built on sand.
    [Show full text]
  • A Neuroscientific Perspective on Cognitive Biases JE
    A neuroscientific perspective on cognitive biases J.E. (Hans) Korteling, A.-M. Brouwer, A. Toet* TNO, Soesterberg, The Netherlands * Corresponding author: Alexander Toet TNO Kampweg 5 3769DE Soesterberg, The Netherlands Email: [email protected] Abstract Human reasoning shows systematic and persistent simplifications (‘heuristics’) and deviations from ‘rational thinking’ that may lead to suboptimal decisional outcomes (‘cognitive biases’). There are currently three prevailing theoretical perspectives on the origin of heuristics and cognitive biases: a cognitive-psychological, an ecological and an evolutionary perspective. However, none of these viewpoints provides an overall explanatory framework for the underlying mechanisms of cognitive biases. Here we propose a neuroscientific framework for cognitive biases, arguing that many cognitive biases arise from intrinsic brain mechanisms that are fundamental to biological neural networks and that originally developed to perform basic physical, perceptual, and motor functions. Keywords cognitive biases, heuristics, decision making, neural networks, neural information processing, human irrationality 1 Introduction In daily life, we constantly make judgments and decisions (either conscious or unconscious) without knowing their outcome. We typically violate rules of logic and probability and resort to simple and near-optimal heuristic decision rules (‘mental shortcuts’) to optimize the likelihood of an acceptable outcome, which may be effective in conditions with time-constraints, information overload, or when no optimal solution is evident (Gerd Gigerenzer & Gaissmaier, 2010). We are also inclined to use heuristics when problems appear familiar and when we do not feel the need to gather additional information. Although heuristics can result in quite acceptable outcomes in everyday situations, and when the time cost of reasoning are taken into account, people often violate tenets of rationality in inadvisable ways (Shafir & LeBoeuf, 2002).
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Biases and Instability of Preferences in the Portfolio Choices of Retail Investors Policy Implications of Behavioural Finance
    Working papers Cognitive biases and instability of preferences in the portfolio choices of retail investors Policy implications of behavioural finance N. Linciano 66 January 2010 COGNITIVE BIASES AND INSTABILITY OF PREFERENCES IN THE PORTFOLIO CHOICES OF RETAIL INVESTORS Policy implications of behavioural finance ∗ Nadia Linciano ( ) Abstract Classical financial theory assumes that individuals are perfectly rational and act by using complete and homogeneous information sets. For a long time, this has been used both on descriptive and normative grounds. However, empirical research has shown that investors systematically commit reasoning or preference errors hard to reconcile with the rationality assumption. These errors are reflected in “behavioural anomalies” that lead retail investors to low participation in the equity market, perception errors of the risk- return relationship, poor portfolio diversification and excessive trading. This paper surveys and discusses the insights of behavioural finance that help us to understand observed anomalies using the theoretical apparatus of cognitive psychology and experimental evidence. These insights, by providing a review of the real perceptions of phenomena and the psychological and irrational components at the basis of individual choices, may be helpful to strengthen the efficiency of financial regulation and supervision. In particular, financial education in a behavioural vein can be used to improve investors' capacity to judge and to raise their understanding of the most serious behavioural “traps”. The contents and presentation format of disclosure on the characteristics of financial products also lay themselves open to be geared to the prescriptions of behavioural finance. Finally, financial advice is an indispensable supplement for guiding investors to make decisions that best serve their interests and for strengthening the efficiency of financial regulation; financial advisors should therefore endeavour to help customers to contain the most common behavioural errors.
    [Show full text]
  • An Inclusive Taxonomy of Behavioral Biases
    A Service of Leibniz-Informationszentrum econstor Wirtschaft Leibniz Information Centre Make Your Publications Visible. zbw for Economics Peón, David; Antelo, Manel; Calvo-Silvosa, Anxo Article An inclusive taxonomy of behavioral biases European Journal of Government and Economics (EJGE) Provided in Cooperation with: Universidade da Coruña Suggested Citation: Peón, David; Antelo, Manel; Calvo-Silvosa, Anxo (2017) : An inclusive taxonomy of behavioral biases, European Journal of Government and Economics (EJGE), ISSN 2254-7088, Universidade da Coruña, A Coruña, Vol. 6, Iss. 1, pp. 24-58, http://dx.doi.org/10.17979/ejge.2017.6.1.4322 This Version is available at: http://hdl.handle.net/10419/217753 Standard-Nutzungsbedingungen: Terms of use: Die Dokumente auf EconStor dürfen zu eigenen wissenschaftlichen Documents in EconStor may be saved and copied for your Zwecken und zum Privatgebrauch gespeichert und kopiert werden. personal and scholarly purposes. Sie dürfen die Dokumente nicht für öffentliche oder kommerzielle You are not to copy documents for public or commercial Zwecke vervielfältigen, öffentlich ausstellen, öffentlich zugänglich purposes, to exhibit the documents publicly, to make them machen, vertreiben oder anderweitig nutzen. publicly available on the internet, or to distribute or otherwise use the documents in public. Sofern die Verfasser die Dokumente unter Open-Content-Lizenzen (insbesondere CC-Lizenzen) zur Verfügung gestellt haben sollten, If the documents have been made available under an Open gelten abweichend von diesen Nutzungsbedingungen die in der dort Content Licence (especially Creative Commons Licences), you genannten Lizenz gewährten Nutzungsrechte. may exercise further usage rights as specified in the indicated licence. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0 www.econstor.eu European Journal of Government and Economics 6(1), June 2017, 24-58.
    [Show full text]
  • Modelling Risk-Taking Behaviour of Avalanche Accident Victims (Research Master Thesis - M2 Cognitive Sciences, PHELMA, Univ
    Modelling risk-taking behaviour of avalanche accident victims (Research Master thesis - M2 Cognitive Sciences, PHELMA, Univ. Grenoble-Alpes) Robin Couret1, Carole Adam1,2, and Martial Mermillod1,3 1Univ. Grenoble-Alpes, Grenoble, France 2Grenoble Informatics Laboratory, CNRS UMR 5217 3Laboratoire de Psychologie et Neurocognition, CNRS UMR 5105 August 2020 Abstract Each year, over 15000 requests for mountain rescue are counted in France. Avalanche accidents represent 39% of reports, and are therefore our focus in this study. Modelling the behaviour of mountain accident victims is useful to develop more accurate rescue and prevention tools. Concretely, we observe the interference of two heuristics (availability and familiarity) in decision making when choosing an itinerary in backcountry skiing. We developed a serious game to evaluate their effect on the probability of engaging in a risky itinerary, while varying situational and environmental criteria in each participant (N = 278). The availability heuristic is operationalized by three situations, an avalanche accident video, a backcountry skiing video and a neutral context. The familiarity heuristic is operationalized by two criteria, strong and weak familiarity with the place. Results demonstrate the effects of both heuristics. Measurements through our serious game are discussed in the perspective of developing an interactive prevention tool for mitigating the negative effects of heuristics. Keywords: avalanche accidents, serious games, decision-making, decision heuristics 1 Introduction Each year, over 15000 requests for mountain rescue are counted in France 1 after various incidents or accidents. An analysis of incident and accident reports filed on the SERAC database2 shows that the majority (39 %) of the reports refer to avalanche-type events [Soul´eet al., 2017].
    [Show full text]