City University of New York (CUNY) CUNY Academic Works

All Dissertations, Theses, and Capstone Projects Dissertations, Theses, and Capstone Projects

9-2017

Cognitive and Affective Control Deficits in Adults with utismA Spectrum Disorder

Melissa-Ann Mackie The Graduate Center, City University of New York

How does access to this work benefit ou?y Let us know!

More information about this work at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu/gc_etds/2271 Discover additional works at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu

This work is made publicly available by the City University of New York (CUNY). Contact: [email protected]

COGNITIVE AND AFFECTIVE CONTROL DEFICITS IN ADULTS WITH

SPECTRUM DISORDER

by

MELISSA-ANN MACKIE, M.S., M.PHIL.

A dissertation submitted to the Graduate Faculty in Psychology

in partial fulfillment of the of the requirements of the degree of

Doctor of Philosophy, The City University of New York

2017

Ó 2017 MELISSA-ANN MACKIE All Rights Reserved ii

Cognitive and Affective Control Deficits in Adults with Disorder

by

Melissa-Ann Mackie, M.S., M.Phil.

This manuscript has been read and accepted for the Graduate Faculty in Psychology in

satisfaction of the dissertation requirement for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy.

______Date Jin Fan, Ph.D.

Chair of Examining Committee

______Date Richard Bodnar, Ph.D.

Executive Officer

Supervisory Committee:

Jin Fan, Ph.D.

Jeffrey Halperin, Ph.D.

Justin Storbeck, Ph.D.

Kurt Schulz, Ph.D.

A. Ting Wang, Ph.D.

THE CITY UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK

iii

ABSTRACT

Cognitive and Affective Control Deficits in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder

by

Melissa-Ann Mackie, M.S., M.Phil.

Advisor: Jin Fan, Ph.D.

Cognitive control constrains mental operations to prioritize information that reaches conscious awareness and is essential to flexible, adaptive behavior under conditions of uncertainty. However, cognitive control can be compromised by neurodevelopmental disorders such as autism spectrum disorder (ASD), which is characterized by the presence of social and communicative deficits, and restricted interests/repetitive behaviors. Although prior investigations have attempted to elucidate the nature of cognitive control deficits in ASD, whether there is an underlying deficit in cognitive and affective control associated with the symptom domains of ASD remains unclear. The present series of eight experiments presents an information theoretic framework for the study of cognitive control in high-functioning adults with ASD, and aims to investigate deficits in cognitive and affective control under conditions of uncertainty, and the relation of these deficits to ASD symptoms, to better understand the nature of symptoms and cognitive deficits in ASD.

iv

Table of Contents

Abstract iv

List of Tables ix

List of Figures x

Chapter I: Research Objective 1

Introduction 1

Rationale for Study 2

Research Questions and Hypotheses 3

Chapter II: Literature Review 5

Existing Explanations for ASD Symptoms and Associated Features 6

Theory of 6

Weak Central Coherence 8

Executive Dysfunction 10

Complex Information Processing 12

Predictive Coding Accounts 13

Summary 15

An Information Theory Account of Cognitive Control 16

Application of information theory to existing tasks examining 18

cognitive control in ASD

ASD Symptoms Can Be Explained By Inefficient Cognitive 21

Control

v

Social-Communication Deficits 22

Restricted Interests/Repetitive Behaviors 24

Brain Correlates of Cognitive Control 25

Advantages of The Information Theory Approach 26

Reconciling the Information Theory and Predictive Coding Accounts 27

of ASD

Context for the Proposed Study 28

Chapter III: Investigation of Cognitive Control Deficits in ASD 30

Method 30

Participants 30

Protection of Human Subjects 31

Recruitment 32

Screening Materials and Questionnaires 32

Autism Diagnostic Interview- Revised 32

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic 33

Autism Quotient 33

Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV 34

Social Responsiveness Scale – 2 34

Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence-2 34

Cognitive Control Tasks 35

Entropy Variation Task (EVT) 35

vi

Majority Function Task (MFT) 36

Dual Conflict Task (DCT) 38

Masked Majority Function Task (MFT-M) 40

Procedure 42

Data Analysis 43

Results 44

Cognitive control for sequential stimuli: Results of the EVT 44

Cognitive control efficiency for sequential stimuli 45

– entropy

Cognitive control efficiency for sequential stimuli 46

– surprise

Cognitive control for non-sequential stimuli: Results of the 46

MFT

Cognitive control capacity under time constraints: Results of 48

the DCT and MFT-M

DCT 48

MFT-M 50

Cognitive Control Capacity 50

Relationships between task performance and symptom domains 51

Discussion 52

Limitations and alternative explanations 56

Conclusion 57

vii

Chapter IV: Investigation of Cognitive Control of Affective Information 58

in ASD

Method 58

Participants 58

Tasks 59

Compound Emotion Recognition Task (CERT) 59

Multiple Emotion Processing Task (MEPT) 60

Recursive Theory of Mind Task (RToM) 63

Majority Function Task - Emotional (MFT-E) 64

Procedure 65

Data Analysis 66

Results 67

CERT 67

MEPT 68

R-ToM 69

MFT-E 71

Correlations between task performance and ASD symptoms 73

Discussion 74

Limitations and alternative explanations 76

Conclusion 79

Chapter V: General Discussion 81

General cognitive control deficits in ASD 81

viii

Affective cognitive control deficits 83

Conclusions 86

Appendix A: Recursive Theory of Mind Task Sample Stimuli 88

Appendix B: Supplemental Results (RT and accuracy) 91

References 103

viii

List of Tables

Table 1. Demographic and questionnaire data (range) of ASD and TD groups 30

Table 2. Experimental conditions and estimations of input information and 40

computational load under both SOAs of the DCT

Table 3. Mean performance on the EVT in terms of efficiency, RT, and accuracy 44

Table 4. Mean performance on the MFT in terms of efficiency, RT, and accuracy 47

Table 5. Mean performance on Task 2 of the DCT in terms of efficiency, RT, and 49

accuracy

Table 6. Mean performance on the MFT-M in RT and accuracy 51

Table 7. Kendall’s tau correlation coefficients for relationships between 52

efficiency and ADI-R and ADOS-G subscales

Table 8. Demographic and questionnaire data for ASD and TD groups 58

Table 9. Mean performance on the CERT in terms of efficiency, RT, and 68

accuracy

Table 10. Mean performance on the MEPT in terms of efficiency, RT, and 69

accuracy

Table 11. Mean performance on the RToM in terms of efficiency, RT, and 70

accuracy

Table 12. Mean performance on the MFT-E in terms of efficiency, RT, and 72

accuracy

Table 13. Kendall’s tau correlation coefficients for relationships between 73

efficiency and ASD symptoms ix

List of Figures

Figure 1. Schematic of Entropy Variation Task 35

Figure 2. Sample stimuli in the Majority Function Task 37

Figure 3. Schematic of Dual Conflict Task 39

Figure 4. Schematic of Masked Majority Function Task 42

Figure 5. EVT efficiency results for entropy and surprise 45

Figure 6. MFT efficiency results 47

Figure 7. DCT efficiency results of performance in Task 2 48

Figure 8. MFT-M accuracy and cognitive control capacity results 50

Figure 9. Hypothetical performance models of ASD relative to TD 53

Figure 10. Sample stimuli in the Complex Emotion Recognition Task 60

Figure 11. Sample stimuli in the Multiple Emotion Processing Task 62

Figure 12. Sample stimuli in the Emotional Majority Function Task 65

Figure 13. CERT efficiency results 67

Figure 14. MEPT efficiency results 68

Figure 15. R-ToM efficiency results 70

Figure 16. MFT-E efficiency results 73

Figure 17. EVT RT and accuracy results 93

Figure 18. MFT RT and accuracy results 95

Figure 19. DCT RT and accuracy results 96

Figure 20. MFT-M Accuracy, predicted accuracy, and RT results 97

Figure 21. CERT RT and accuracy results 98

x

Figure 22. MEPT RT and accuracy results 99

Figure 23. RToM RT and accuracy results 100

Figure 24. MFT-E RT and accuracy results 102

xi 1 Chapter I

Research Objective

Introduction

At any given moment, the human brain is tasked with selecting among immense amounts of mental representations for further processing by the conscious mind. Much of this information contains uncertainty, which must be processed in order to formulate and execute goal-appropriate actions. Efficient information processing allows us to engage in voluntary control over cognition, flexibly adapt to different contexts, and behave in a goal-directed manner. In the existing literature, this function is commonly referred to as

“cognitive control” and the ability to efficiently process incoming information and rapidly generate responses is largely dependent on its integrity (Badre, 2008; Fan, 2014;

Kouneiher, Charron, & Koechlin, 2009; Mackie, Van Dam, Fan, Dam, & Fan, 2013;

Miller & Cohen, 2001; Posner & Snyder, 1975).

Typically developing (TD) individuals are generally efficient in employing cognitive control, but in cases of neurodevelopmental disorders, cognitive control can be compromised, resulting in functional impairment (Burden et al., 2009; Durston et al.,

2003; Minshew & Goldstein, 1998; Minshew, Johnson, & Luna, 2000; Poljac &

Bekkering, 2012; Rowe, Lavender, & Turk, 2006; Shapiro, Wong, & Simon, 2013;

Solomon et al., 2013; Solomon, Ozonoff, Cummings, & Carter, 2008; Vaidya et al.,

2005; van Meel, Heslenfeld, Oosterlaan, & Sergeant, 2007). Autism spectrum disorder

(ASD) is one such disorder, diagnosed in 1 in 68 children (Baio, 2014), and characterized by the presence of symptoms in the domains of social and communicative deficits, and

2 restricted interests/repetitive behaviors (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). One clear behavioral marker of this disorder is the rigid, inflexible way in which these individuals interact with the world, often with extreme negative reactions to novel or uncertain events (Gomot & Wicker, 2012). This inflexible style of cognition and behavior has been framed in terms of cognitive control deficits (D. V. M. Bishop, 1993; Bogte,

Flamma, van der Meere, & van Engeland, 2008; Damasio & Maurer, 1978; García-

Villamisar & Della Sala, 2002; Geurts, Corbett, & Solomon, 2009; Hughes, Russell, &

Robbins, 1994; Lopez, Lincoln, Ozonoff, & Lai, 2005; Ozonoff, Pennington, & Rogers,

1991; Solomon et al., 2008; Turner, 1999, 1997; Yerys et al., 2009). However, studies aimed at examining cognitive control performance in ASD have provided vastly inconsistent results (Barnard-Brak, 2011; Geurts et al., 2009; Poljac & Bekkering, 2012;

Russo et al., 2007; Solomon et al., 2008), and many questions remain regarding the cognitive profile of this disorder.

Rationale for Study

Contexts requiring cognitive control typically have relatively high information processing demands, and the flexibility allowed by cognitive control facilitates interactions with complex environments in adaptive ways to attend to and process goal- relevant information, as well to switch and further processing to other salient aspects of the environment if necessary (Fan, 2014; Mackie et al., 2013). It is therefore informative to study disorders with impairments in flexible control (such as ASD) in order to understand how cognitive control deficits shape interactions with an uncertain world.

3 The goal of this study is to extend the information theory approach to parametrically manipulate information related to cognitive and affective control in ASD.

The significance of this study lies in theoretical contributions to the conceptualization of deficits in ASD, and in providing a framework to examine cognitive control processes with a computational approach.

Research Questions and Hypotheses

This study is designed to investigate whether reduced efficiency and capacity of cognitive control underlie the symptoms of ASD, particularly under uncertainty conditions associated with social and emotional contexts.

The aims of the present study are:

I. To further investigate the efficiency and capacity of cognitive control in ASD

relative to TD controls, in relation to symptom presentation.

II. To investigate the efficiency of affective information processing in ASD

relative to TD controls, in relation to symptom presentation.

The specific hypotheses to be tested are:

I. Individuals with ASD have reduced efficiency and capacity of cognitive

control relative to TD controls, and have greater difficulty than TD controls

with cognitive control as uncertainty increases.

a. Deficits in cognitive control are negatively correlated with ASD symptom

report (i.e., lower performance associated with higher symptom report).

II. Individuals with ASD have greater difficulty than TD controls with affective

information processing, as uncertainty increases.

4 a. Deficits in affective information processing are negatively correlated with

ASD symptom report.

5 Chapter II

Literature Review

Autism was first described by Kanner (1943) as a specific impairment in

“affective contact”. Wing & Gould (1979) initially described the classic ‘triad’ of deficits in social engagement, communication, and stereotyped behavior that commonly defined the disorder. The repetitive behavior symptom domain was later expanded to include well-documented extreme and intense interests, and as features of the disorder became better understood, so came the expansion of diagnostic features to incorporate them

(American Psychiatric Association, 2000). With the new DSM-5 diagnostic criteria, the social and communication symptoms are now subsumed into one symptom cluster, and restricted interests/repetitive behavior (RI/RB) into the other (American Psychiatric

Association, 2013), though the content validity of these clusters is not without debate (S.

L. Bishop, Richler, & Lord, 2006; Lam, Bodfish, & Piven, 2008; Leekam, Prior, &

Uljarevic, 2011; Szatmari, Tuff, Finlayson, & Bartolucci, 1990)

ASD symptoms arise within the first few years of life, and it is considered a lifelong disorder (Duncan & Bishop, 2013; Fein et al., 2013). Recently there has been increasing focus on how the profile of the disorder may change with development. For example, deficits that were severe in childhood, may resolve or improve by adulthood, but these individuals do often have difficulties that persist into adulthood, such as social deficits, executive dysfunction, odd thinking, attention problems, and the psychiatric symptoms of anxiety and depression (Anderson, Liang, & Lord, 2013; Billstedt, Gillberg,

& Gillberg, 2005; Farley et al., 2009; Fein et al., 2013; Helt et al., 2008; McGovern &

6 Sigman, 2005; Mundy, 1993). Even within the context of normal intellectual functioning, these deficits are associated with limits on functional independence (Anderson et al.,

2013; Bal, Kim, Cheong, & Lord, 2015; Billstedt et al., 2005; Howlin, Goode, Hutton, &

Rutter, 2004; Piven, Harper, Palmer, & Arndt, 1996). There also appears to be neural correlates to the evolution of symptoms across the lifespan, at least into early adulthood

(Courchesne, Campbell, & Solso, 2011; Dajani & Uddin, 2015). While there has been a preponderance of research on the cognitive profiles of children with ASD, the cognitive profile of adults with the disorder remains understudied.

Existing explanations for ASD symptoms and associated features

There have been several theoretical approaches to explain the symptom presentation of ASD, with focus on social (e.g., theory of mind) and/or non-social (e.g., executive function, central coherence, complex information processing) deficits. More recently, computational approaches based on Bayesian inference (i.e., predictive coding) have been proposed to explain the full range of deficits and features. The most influential and/or comprehensive theories to date are reviewed below.

Theory of Mind. The theory of mind (ToM) account of ASD (Baron-Cohen,

Leslie, & Frith, 1985) was one of the early frameworks to be proposed and tested. ToM refers to the ability to attribute mental states to others that are separate from one’s own, and to be able to infer those mental states, given contextual cues (Premack & Woodruff,

1978). Given that social deficits are so prominent in ASD, it has been described as a disorder of “mind-blindness”, (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985). Support for this theory comes from studies that show deficits relative to controls on tasks of false belief (Baron-Cohen

7 et al., 1985; Leekam & Perner, 1991; Phillips, Gómez, Baron-Cohen, Laá, & Rivière,

1995), tasks that require mental inferences (i.e., mentalizing) (Baron-cohen, 1989; Baron-

Cohen, Wheelwright, Spong, Scahill, & Lawson, 2001), tasks requiring non-literal interpretations of language (Happé, 1994; Jolliffe & Baron-Cohen, 1999; Wang, Lee,

Sigman, & Dapretto, 2006), inferring mental states from the eye region only (Baron-

Cohen, Jolliffe, Mortimore, & Robertson, 1997), and naturalistic, dynamic film clips

(Dziobek et al., 2006; Golan, Baron-Cohen, & Hill, 2006; Heavey, Phillips, Baron-

Cohen, & Rutter, 2000). ToM performance is negatively correlated with ASD symptoms

(Lerner, Hutchins, & Prelock, 2011), and individuals who show intact ToM performance in childhood tend to have good communication skills as adolescents (Bennett,

Ramasamy, & Honsberger, 2013).

In terms of specificity, ToM is one deficit that appears to differentiate ASD from other neurodevelopmental disorders, and mindblindness is a common and prominent deficit in this population (U. Frith & Happé, 1994), though it has been demonstrated to not be universal. For example, some adolescents and adults with ASD perform within norm on false belief (Happé, 1995), second-order false-belief tasks (Happé, 1994;

Ozonoff, Rogers, & Pennington, 1991) and mentalizing tasks involving facial stimuli

(Ponnet, Roeyers, Buysse, De Clercq, & Van Der Heyden, 2004; Roeyers, Buysse,

Ponnet, & Pichal, 2001) and geometric cartoons (Castelli, Frith, Happé, & Frith, 2002).

Children with ASD tend to perform poorly on mentalizing tasks, whereas adults usually show deficits on the more dynamic, naturalistic tasks (Roeyers & Demurie, 2010). Given

8 these inconsistent findings in mentalizing ability, the nature of and mechanisms underlying theory of mind deficits in this population remain unclear.

Overall, this theory suffers from a lack of precise definition of what is involved in mindblindness and the type of information that participants with ASD have difficulty processing. Evidence of intact functioning for some suggests that it is not perspective- taking per se that is impaired, but rather perhaps the unconstrained nature of some of these tasks and uncertainty related to task demands may impact performance beyond the ability to infer mental states (S. J. White, Burgess, & Hill, 2009). For example, existing second- and higher-order mentalizing tasks rely on the processing of recursive language structures (e.g. “Mary knows that John thinks…”), in which the amount of information to be processed increases with each level of mental inference. As uncertainty increases within each level, it may become more challenging for individuals with ASD to perform efficiently on these tasks. Consequently, the “pass”/ “fail” criterion often applied to these tasks does little to inform about differences in the processing of this type and amount of information, regardless of whether the result is a correct response.

Weak Central Coherence. The weak central coherence theory conceptualizes deficits associated with ASD in terms of a bias toward processing of local stimulus features, combined with diminished ability to integrate information into coherent concepts (U. Frith & Happé, 1994). The authors propose that individuals with ASD show diminished performance on tasks require taking gestalt or context into account, but enhanced performance on tasks that benefit from enhanced local processing. Support for this theory comes from evidence of impairment on perceptual and semantic tasks that rely

9 on context, for example using the context of a sentence to determine which pronunciation of a word to use when reading out loud (Booth & Happé, 2010; U. Frith & Snowling,

1983; Happé, 1997; Jolliffe & Baron-Cohen, 1999; López & Leekam, 2003; Snowling &

Frith, 1986), but superior performance in ASD on tasks such as the Embedded Figures

Test (Jarrold, Gilchrist, & Bender, 2005; Keehn et al., 2009; Shah & Frith, 1983) and

Block Design (Shah & Frith, 1993). This account explains not only the deficits observed in ASD, but also areas of superior performance (albeit achieved via abnormal processing;

U. Frith & Happé, 1994). However, the theory has been challenged by studies reporting intact global processing in ASD samples (Mottron, Burack, Iarocci, Belleville, & Enns,

2003; Ozonoff, Strayer, McMahon, & Filloux, 1994).

This framework has undergone several modifications since its inception. For example, in light of contradictory evidence, the theory has moved away from a “deficit” account, to describe weak central coherence as a “cognitive style”, with the implication that there is not necessarily a deficit in global processing, but rather a bias, which can be overridden by top-down control (Happé, 1999). Others argued that the deficit lies in reduced ability to conceptualize similarities among multiple pieces of information (i.e.,

“generalize” (Plaisted, 2001)). Others have recast deficits in terms of difficulty processing hierarchical stimuli (Mottron & Burack, 2001; Mottron, Dawson, Soulières,

Hubert, & Burack, 2006). It has been demonstrated that when specifically instructed to pay attention to global features, participants with ASD are able to do so. However, the processing difference appears to lie in the direction of interference: Whereas typically developing individuals experience interference when switching from global to local

10 processing, this effect is reversed in ASD (Rajendran & Mitchell, 2007). Overall, the weak central coherence theory is a compelling account to explain a limited number of features of this disorder. This theory has received less attention in recent years, but some newer, more computational approaches described below (i.e., predictive coding) incorporate this point of view into a larger, more comprehensive framework.

Executive Dysfunction. The origins of this theory are based on the observation that the clinical presentation of ASD shares characteristics with frontal lobe dysfunction

(Damasio & Maurer, 1978), and subsequently, the executive dysfunction theory of autism was posited to account for the range of behaviors observed in individuals with this disorder that could not be explained by the ToM account (Hughes et al., 1994; Ozonoff,

Pennington, et al., 1991). This account asserts that deficits in higher-level cognitive processes such as planning, working memory, inhibition, shifting, and flexibility underlie the symptom presentation of ASD, including ToM deficits (D. V. M. Bishop, 1993; Hill,

2004b; Hughes et al., 1994; Ozonoff, 1995; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996). In particular, the RI/RB domain has been explained in terms of executive dysfunction in generativity

(Turner, 1997). However, although many studies have aimed to clarify the nature of executive function deficits in ASD, results have been largely inconsistent and there is not yet a clear neuropsychological profile (Geurts et al., 2009; Van Eylen et al., 2011).

Initial evidence for the executive dysfunction account of ASD came from deficits on tasks such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) (Ambery, Russell, Perry,

Morris, & Murphy, 2006; Goldstein, Johnson, & Minshew, 2001; Hill, 2004a; Lopez et al., 2005; Minshew et al., 2000; Ozonoff, Pennington, et al., 1991; Pascualvaca, Fantie,

11 Papageorgiou, & Mirsky, 1998); Tower of London (ToL) (Hughes et al., 1994; Ozonoff

& McEvoy, 1994; Robinson, Goddard, Dritschel, Wisley, & Howlin, 2009; Rumsey &

Hamburger, 1990); Tower of Hanoi (Ozonoff & Jensen, 1999); Trail Making Test (TMT)

(Goldstein et al., 2001; Ozonoff & McEvoy, 1994; Prior & Hoffman, 1990; Rumsey &

Hamburger, 1990; Shu, Lung, Tien, & Chen, 2001; Szatmari et al., 1990); and

Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB)

Intradimensional/Extradimensional Shift (Yerys et al., 2009). Intact performance has generally (though not always) been found on the Stroop (Goldstein et al., 2001;

Kleinhans, Akshoomoff, & Delis, 2005) and Go/No-Go (Happé, Booth, Charlton, &

Hughes, 2006; Schmitz et al., 2006) tasks in ASD. Further complicating the story, different studies have reported inconsistent results for the same tasks, in child, adolescent, and adult samples (Geurts et al., 2009).

In these studies, cognitive control is often equated with , creating conceptual confusion. Various studies have attempted to isolate specific executive functions (e.g., response inhibition, task-switching, working memory) to demonstrate deficits in cognitive control. However, many tasks (most notably the WCST, on which much of the evidence for cognitive inflexibility lies) suffer from the problem of task impurity (Hill, 2004a; Van Eylen et al., 2011), and generally the executive functions are difficult to dissociate completely (Mackie et al., 2013; Miyake et al., 2000).

Conversely, conceptualizing cognitive control as a broader construct may be useful in terms of understanding group differences relevant to ASD.

12 Complex Information Processing. To explain findings that ASD participants are not impaired in all cognitive domains, the complex information processing theory was proposed (Minshew, Goldstein, & Siegel, 1997; Williams, Minshew, & Goldstein, 2015).

This account asserts that intact performance can be expected on tasks that require basic perception and low-level cognition, but that deficits would emerge on more “complex” tasks. This account refers to complexity in terms of the roughly estimated demand on the brain’s information processing resources by the nature and quantity of information to be processed, including constraints on processing time (Williams, Goldstein, & Minshew,

2006). Support for this account comes from studies using a variety of common neuropsychological tests with ASD groups showing impairment on complex tasks of memory, language, concept formation, reasoning, and skilled motor movements but intact performance on simple tasks of attention, memory, language, learning, and visuospatial skills (Minshew et al., 1997; Williams et al., 2006). However, within this framework, the distinction between “simple” and “complex” was made between different tasks assumed to have different information processing demands, rather than within-task complexity manipulations (Minshew et al., 1997), and it is unclear to what extent confounding factors were held constant for these comparisons.

Other support for the idea of an information-processing deficit comes from studies reporting deficits in ASD when information processing is challenged by the nature or parameters of the task. For example, whereas Go/No-Go performance has been shown to be generally intact in ASD, performance is impaired at a fast presentation rate

(Raymaekers, Van Der Meere, & Roeyers, 2004). Similarly, others have asserted that

13 individuals with ASD have a more limited capacity for information processing, and consequently develop a cognitive style that is avoidant of higher-level processing and over-dependent on processing of lower-level features in order to avoid information overload (Belmonte et al., 2004; Burack, 1994). This information processing bottleneck is proposed to arise due to heightened arousal to sensory stimuli in concert with reduced target specificity in ASD (Belmonte et al., 2004).

Overall, it is clear that there is a difference in information processing in ASD compared to typically developing controls. However, this approach is lacking a precise definition of complexity that would facilitate an understanding of what aspect of the information is difficult for this population to manage. The distinction between “simple” and “complex” tasks is too rough to be meaningful.

Predictive Coding Accounts. Within the predictive coding framework, perception consists of hypothesis-testing of expectations within the context of Bayesian inference about the causes of sensory information (Feldman & Friston, 2010; see also

Pellicano & Burr, 2012). Top-down predictions are compared to bottom-up sensory input, and a mismatch between these is known as a “prediction error”. Prediction errors are useful to learning, because they indicate to the organism that what has occurred is informative, and this information is fed back upstream to update existing representations to allow for better future predictions (Feldman & Friston, 2010). Prediction errors can lead to “precision”, by which sensory signals of interest are augmented in order to reduce uncertainty about the state of the world.

14 The first application of this framework to ASD (Friston, Lawson, & Frith, 2013;

Lawson, Rees, & Friston, 2014) described the disorder as associated with attenuation of high-level precision, and augmentation of low-level (sensory) precision, to account for hyperfocus on low-level stimulus features (Lawson et al., 2014). Similarly, the High

Inflexible Precision of Prediction Errors in Autism (HIPPEA) account (de Cruys et al.,

2014) asserts that whereas typically developing individuals can effectively and flexibly distinguish between prediction errors that matter (reducible uncertainty) and those that don’t (irreducible uncertainty), this mechanism of weighting prediction errors is abnormal in ASD, leading to too much weight being placed on all prediction errors, with over-allocation of attention to irrelevant information. This results in inflexibility and abnormal processing that interferes with the ability to generalize learning to other situations.

Also within realm of Bayesian inference, the new predictive impairment in autism account (PIA) asserts that deficits in estimating conditional probabilities of events can explain many features of ASD (Sinha et al., 2014). In this model, the strength of associations and the time that lapses between events determines whether a relationship can be detected. This model comprehensively accounts for RI/RB, sensory hypersensitivity, failures of ToM, and savant abilities. For example, RI/RB is proposed to arise under conditions unpredictability, which are anxiety-provoking, and the behaviors are engaged as a coping mechanism. In this context, ToM is especially vulnerable to failure, as it requires deduction about causes of behaviors and prediction about what someone will do next based on an estimation of conditional probability with weak

15 associations. Islets of ability are framed in terms of being strongly rule-based, low in uncertainty (e.g., mathematics, music), increasing the likelihood of successful performance in ASD.

Overall, these Bayesian accounts of ASD have advanced the conceptualization of the disorder to a level that is more computational, comprehensive, with specifically testable hypotheses and predictions. Each of these accounts casts ASD symptoms as related to uncertainty management, at the level of differences between predicted and observed sensory information. However, empirical data to support these theories have not yet been published.

Summary. While there has been progress in terms of conceptualizing ASD deficits within a comprehensive framework, the contribution of information processing deficits to the presentation is still not well understood. It is clear that individuals with

ASD interact with the world in a way that is markedly different from those who are typically developing, and empirical evidence to date suggests the presence of an inefficient processing style that contributes to cognitive and behavioral difficulties. The weak central coherence and theory of mind accounts are limited, and do not explain a sufficient range of deficits. The executive dysfunction theory is rife with inconsistent findings due to vast differences in tasks across studies, and the problem of task impurity.

The definition of “information” within the complex information processing account is imprecise, and between-task comparisons of complexity make it difficult to determine where the “complexity” lies. The newer theories based on predictive coding present precise, comprehensive frameworks, with specific predictions for a wide range of

16 deficits, but evidence to support these theories is still lacking. Existing studies have not employed quantitative definitions of information or parametric within-task manipulations of cognitive load to investigate the effects of the nature or “amount” of information on information processing deficits in ASD. Parametric manipulations of information, quantified in computational units such as ‘bits’ would result in clearer comparisons between conditions in terms of how much information the cognitive control system is able to efficiently manage.

An information theory account of cognitive control

Information theory is concerned with the communication of information under uncertainty, with signals being transmitted from a source to a receiver across a noisy channel with limited capacity (Shannon, 1948). This framework provides a new perspective to the study of cognitive control in ASD, and complements predictive coding accounts described above. Here, cognitive control is a limited-capacity integrative interface between input and response that dynamically facilitates the processing of information by prioritizing the transfer of relevant information and suppressing the transfer of irrelevant information to further processing stages (Fan, 2014).

In a recent publication we proposed an information theory model of cognitive control in which it is engaged for uncertainty processing and the prioritization of task- relevant information for adaptive behavior (Mackie et al., 2013). From this perspective, uncertainty is conceptualized in terms of Shannon surprise and entropy, which are tied to probability of the occurrence of events (Shannon, 1948). These metrics, in units of ‘bits’ quantify the minimum amount of information that must be stored or transferred, in order

17 to make a judgment about the nature of an event. A ‘bit’ is a binary unit that is the basic unit of information storage and transfer, and refers to the “amount” information under consideration. The information transfer rate (the average amount of information transmitted per unit time in bits per second) is related to the capacity of the channel.

Surprise refers to information contained in the occurrence of a certain type of

event, and is defined as: I(xi ) = −log2 p(xi ) , where p(xi) is the probability of the occurrence of event xi. The base 2 log transformation results in information quantified in units of bits. A low-probability event is associated with a high surprise value, and vice versa. For example, in a sequence set that predominantly requires ‘left’ responses (e.g.,

87.5% left, 12.5% right), a stimulus requiring a ‘right’ response, would carry a higher surprise value (3 bits) than the ‘left’ response (0.19 bits). Entropy, on the other hand, is the information contained in a sequence of events, and is defined as:

n

H(X ) = E(I(X )) = p(xi ) log2 p(xi ) . Entropy is therefore the weighted average of i=1 surprise over all types of events. For a predictable sequence, entropy is low. For example, in the sequence mentioned above, the entropy is 0.54 bits, with a very high probability of a ‘left’ response event. For this scenario, entropy would be highest (1 bit) when ‘left’ and

‘right’ responses are equally probable (i.e., 50% left, 50% right; see Fan, 2014 for a review) and events are less predictable. To summarize, the entropy expressed in bits is the minimum amount of information one would need to process in order to respond to this series of events with continuous probabilities.

18 Cognitive control is known to be a limited-capacity function (Posner & Snyder,

1975), and under circumstances in which the required rate of information transfer exceeds that of the channel capacity, there would be a breakdown in channel efficiency and increase in error, leading to diminished performance (Fan, 2014). Increased entropy is associated with a linear increase in RT, reflective of Hick’s law (Hick, 1952), and greater recruitment of the brain systems involved in uncertainty reduction (Fan, 2014).

This increased processing time is not only a function of the number of stimuli to be processed, but rather how much information needs to be processed in the brain using certain mental algorithms that manage uncertainty. This is illustrated in the majority function task (Fan, Guise, Liu, & Wang, 2008), wherein participants must respond to the majority direction of a group of arrows. When set size is constant, RT differences occur between conditions with varying levels of directional congruence, and consequently, the stimuli carry different amounts of information (Fan et al., 2008; Mackie et al., 2013). In other tasks, including a task switch condition, presenting different types of stimuli on each trial, increasing the number of response alternatives, and increasing the required processing rate all increase the amount of information uncertainty involved in the task.

Consequently, two tasks purporting to measure the same cognitive functions, may actually be manipulating entropy at different levels through differences in stimulus and response presentation.

Application of information theory to existing tasks examining cognitive control in ASD. Cognitive control is typically examined by way of tasks that manipulate conflict between stimulus dimensions or responses such as Stroop (Stroop, 1935) and

19 flanker (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974) tasks. In these cases, cognitive control is involved to facilitate the constraint of the prepotent response that is inconsistent with task demands.

From an information theory perspective, the conflict effect can be conceptualized as resulting from an increase in uncertainty from the congruent condition (only one possible stimulus-response representation) to the incongruent condition (two possible stimulus- response representations), corresponding to an entropy increase of 1 bit (base 2 log of 2 possibilities) between conditions. Increased RT (and decreased accuracy) is typically observed in the incongruent condition compared to the congruent condition, corresponding to an approximately 100 ms difference (Fan, 2014). In a recent study, a group of ASD adults performed significantly less accurately at a flanker task compared to typically developing adults, although there was a similar increase in RT for the incongruent condition (Fan et al., 2012).

Comparably, tasks that rely on response to infrequent occurrences, such as oddball and Go/No-Go tasks can also be understood in terms of uncertainty increases between conditions. In an oddball task, the low frequency oddball event, by definition has a high surprise value. In a Go/No-Go task, the No-Go trials (20%) are far less frequent than Go trials (80%), carrying a relatively high surprise value (~2.32 bits) compared to

Go trials (~0.32 bits). Additionally, when the processing rate is increased on this task, the performance of participants with ASD declines (Raymaekers et al., 2004).

Task switching paradigms, to which participants with ASD have been shown to be particularly susceptible (Bogte et al., 2008; Geurts et al., 2009; Van Eylen et al.,

2011), are associated with a performance cost due to a channel switch, and the relative

20 infrequency of ‘switch’ trials, relative to ‘stay’ trials. Some such tasks include random alternation of ‘switch’ and ‘stay’, which would represent the maximum entropy value, due to the equal likelihood of both trial types. Furthermore, channel switching (one task to the next) is associated with a performance cost (Fan, 2014). Task-switching paradigms often use unpredictable switches as a measure of cognitive flexibility (e.g., de Vries &

Geurts, 2012), and find larger switch costs in ASD groups (Maes, Eling, Wezenberg,

Vissers, & Kan, 2011; Yerys et al., 2009). However, it has also been noted that children with ASD do not show impairment when the switch is predictable (Stahl & Pry, 2002).

The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, commonly applied to the study of cognitive flexibility in ASD (Van Eylen et al., 2011), involves sorting cards to one of four piles

(log2(4) = 2 bits of information), according to the participant must infer based on feedback about whether a previous trial was correct or incorrect. There are three stimulus dimensions (log2(3) = 1.58 bits of information) on which the sort rule can be based, resulting in a minimum of 3.58 bits (uncertainty about piles + uncertainty about dimensions) of information on every trial until the participant infers the rule, at which point the information is reduced to 2 bits (uncertainty about piles only) for each trial on which the participant infers that the rule remains the same. After ten consecutive correct sorts, the rule changes to another one of the three stimulus dimensions, resulting in an increase of information to 3.58 bits plus the information contained in the channel switch

(at least 1 bit), for a minimum of 4.58 bits on switch trials. It is unsurprising that this high-entropy task results in deficient performance in participants with ASD (Geurts,

21 Verté, Oosterlaan, Roeyers, & Sergeant, 2004; Kaland, Smith, & Mortensen, 2008;

Minshew et al., 2000).

For many neuropsychology tasks used to investigate the cognitive profile of ASD, it is difficult to estimate the behavioral effects of entropy due to the nature of responses required. For example accumulated evidence of deficits for “complex” information

(Williams et al., 2015) includes tasks that are high in uncertainty, such as non-literal language and semantic judgments with multiple representations and tasks involving channel switching, such as Trails B. The use of a computational approach to quantify the amount of information to be processed would likely be a better indicator of complexity, and is possible within an information theory framework (discussed below).

ASD symptoms can be explained by inefficient cognitive control. Uncertainty is present by definition in variables, which take on more than one value. As the number of possible outcomes increases, so does the uncertainty associated with that variable. The presence of uncertainty indicates the need for cognitive control to facilitate information processing and prioritization of information for further computation (Fan, 2014; Mackie et al., 2013; Mushtaq, Bland, & Schaefer, 2011). Efficient functionality depends on the ability to process uncertainty in order to formulate appropriate thoughts or actions, based on the information available. Cognitive control processes may include either uncertainty reduction, to infer the value of a variable at a given time, or uncertainty inflation, to allow for generalization to other contexts. These processes work in concert to allow for adaptive behavior across various situations. If cognitive control is less efficient in ASD,

22 dynamically dealing with uncertainty should therefore be problematic, contributing to the clinical presentation of the disorder.

Social-communication deficits. The domain of social communication requires cognitive control for the efficient allocation of brain resources in constraining information to be processed, and to avoid information processing overload under incredibly uncertain conditions (Gomot & Wicker, 2012). According to uncertainty reduction theory (based upon information theory, Berger & Calabrese, 1975), people undertake several steps to reduce uncertainty in social situations. For example, theory of mind requires using information from the verbal and nonverbal communication of others in the context of situational cues to form an inference about the person’s mental state.

Variables related to the causes of mental states and the mental and emotional effects of situational causes notoriously have one-to-many (and vice versa) mappings that also vary depending on the individual. In communication, there is uncertainty inherent in semantic ambiguity associated with false belief tasks, irony, sarcasm, and other non-literal interpretations of language (e.g., Jolliffe & Baron-Cohen, 1999). The reduction of multiple pieces of (sometimes conflicting) information into a ‘gist’ concept bears heavily on our ability to interact with others according to the emotional states that they convey

(related to the idea of weak central coherence, Cashin, Gallagher, Newman, & Hughes,

2012). Reciprocal communication, which is clearly deficient in this population (C. D.

Frith & Frith, 2012), requires rapid information processing, idea generation and response, as well as ongoing processing of non-verbal information. Therefore, successful social behavior requires flexible adaptation to variable social contexts (Cañadas, Rodríguez-

23 Bailón, Milliken, & Lupiáñez, 2013; Cashin et al., 2012; Dichter & Belger, 2008; Happé

& Frith, 2006; Kenworthy, Case, Harms, Martin, & Wallace, 2010). Lower efficiency of cognitive control, combined with a reduced upper limit of information processing capacity, may negatively impact the cognitive flexibility required for smooth social interaction in dynamic contexts, and therefore set the stage for the emergence of social and communicative deficits (Mackie & Fan, 2015).

The processing of emotional information in social situations presents a special challenge to individuals with ASD, who are deficient in recognizing the emotional component of a social stimulus (Adolphs, Sears, & Piven, 2001; Boraston, Blakemore,

Chilvers, & Skuse, 2007; Castelli, 2005; Dalton et al., 2005; Dawson et al., 2004;

Hadjikhani, Joseph, Snyder, & Tager-Flusberg, 2007). Theory of mind deficits have been argued to be strongly related to emotion processing, and information about the thoughts and feelings of others come from social interactions (Cashin, 2005). In studies of emotional processing, there are often ceiling effects for emotion recognition tasks, or failure to find differences between ASD and typically developing groups (Adolphs et al.,

2001; Baron-Cohen et al., 1997; Cassidy, Ropar, Mitchell, & Chapman, 2013; Spezio,

Adolphs, Hurley, & Piven, 2007). However, group differences are found for more nuanced stimuli that are dynamic, or show emotions of lower intensity (Baron-Cohen et al., 1997; Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Spong, et al., 2001; Golan et al., 2006; Roeyers et al., 2001).

Inconsistencies on tests of emotion recognition suggest that the deficit is not in emotion recognition per se may be explained by differences in response structure. For

24 example, in forced-choice tasks, the number of alternatives available impacts the amount of uncertainty, consistent with the relationship between uncertainty and number of possible responses. In this sense, performance on a task that presents only two choices may be better than on a task with five choices, even though the stimulus is the same, due to increased uncertainty associated with the response. Furthermore, emotion recognition depends on use of information provided by combinations of facial action units that represent an emotion. However, some these individual action units are not emotion- specific and are shared across different emotional expressions resulting in increased uncertainty, particularly as it relates to compound and complex emotional expressions

(Du, Tao, & Martinez, 2014). In summary, difficulty with uncertainty processing can thus account for deficits in social communication, including emotion processing, theory of mind, and verbal and nonverbal communication.

Restricted Interests/Repetitive Behaviors. Uncertainty is a relatively aversive state (Hirsh, Mar, & Peterson, 2012) and this may be amplified in those with impaired cognitive control. “Intolerance of uncertainty” has been shown to be strongly related to anxiety disorders (Carleton, 2012; Carleton et al., 2012; Dugas, Gagnon, Ladouceur, &

Freeston, 1998), which are also highly comorbid with ASD (S. W. White, Oswald,

Ollendick, & Scahill, 2009; Wood & Gadow, 2010). The symptom domain of restricted interests/repetitive behaviors may thus be reflective of an attempt to avoid uncertainty by restricting interests and activities to a confined, predictable set (Baron-Cohen, Ashwin,

Ashwin, Tavassoli, & Chakrabarti, 2009). It has been also proposed that repetitive behaviors may be used as a calming coping mechanism in uncertain, anxiety-provoking

25 situations (Gillott, Furniss, & Walter, 2001; Happé & Ronald, 2008; Sinha et al., 2014;

Turner, 1997). Both may serve as a protective mechanism (conscious or unconscious) to avoid the subjective frustration associated with information overload (Hutt, Hutt, Lee, &

Ounsted, 1965; Markram, Rinaldi, & Markram, 2007; O’Connor & Kirk, 2008; Valla &

Belmonte, 2013). Furthermore, deficits in the domain of restricted interests and repetitive behaviors may serve to compensate for a diminished cognitive control capacity. By restricting interests to predictable sequences and familiar domains, individuals with ASD are able to avoid cognitive control overload.

Brain correlates of cognitive control

The frontoparietal network, with the anterior cingulate (ACC) and anterior insular

(AIC) cortices as core regions, in addition to activation in other regions such as the frontal eye fields and areas near or along the intraparietal sulcus, is typically associated with tasks of cognitive control (Fan, 2014). It has been specifically proposed that dysfunction in structures containing von economo neurons (such as ACC and AI) result in impaired cognitive control for uncertainty reduction in ASD, resulting in a diminished ability to quickly respond in dynamic situations, with reliance instead on a slower, rule- based method in social situations (Allman, Watson, Tetreault, & Hakeem, 2005). This deficit in cognitive control feeds into other high-level systems such as ToM, and social behavior producing further deficits, but is also observable in non-social tasks (Allman et al., 2005).

We have demonstrated an absence of activation of the ACC in ASD during conflict processing (Fan et al., 2012). The ACC is involved in baseline state uncertainty

26 monitoring, and abnormal recruitment of this crucial network hub could underlie the inefficient performance demonstrated in our experiments (Fan et al., 2014). In addition to region-specific neural differences, there is also evidence for differences in connectivity within the frontoparietal network in ASD, resulting in inefficient information transfer between anterior and posterior areas (Belmonte et al., 2004; Just, Cherkassky, Keller, &

Minshew, 2004; Just, Keller, Malave, Kana, & Varma, 2012; Kana, Keller, Cherkassky,

Minshew, & Just, 2006), related to symptom presentation (Uddin et al., 2014). In information theory terms, this disturbance in functional connectivity has been described as lower information “bandwidth” or data transfer rate within the cognitive control neural network in ASD relative to healthy controls (Just et al., 2004, 2012).

Advantages of the information theory approach

Applying information theory to the study of cognitive control under uncertainty in

ASD provides a new, explicitly computational approach to understanding the mechanisms underlying social and non-social deficits in ASD. It is widely accepted that a major function of the brain is “information processing”, but to date, this concept has remained vague and elusive. The inconclusive search for a neuropsychological profile of

ASD may be because this is not a ‘focal’ disorder, but rather a systems-level disorder in the brain networks that are involved in cognitive control. The information theory approach allows for the quantification of the amount of information that individuals need to (and can) efficiently process under various conditions. The development of tasks with systematic and parametric manipulations of information entropy within-task allows us to investigate the efficiency and capacity of cognitive control both in healthy controls and

27 various patient populations, as well as the brain systems involved. Application of this model to ASD has provided some insight into the nature of information processing deficits in this population, although to this point, only within the context of non-social information.

An explanatory theory must be able to provide a full causal account of the disorder (U. Frith & Happé, 1994). Thus far, approaches attributing ASD to deficits in a single domain have been largely unsuccessful at explaining all aspects of the disorder. It is also likely that the best account of ASD may be a multiple-deficit account (Rajendran

& Mitchell, 2007). Within the information theory approach, it remains unclear to what extent it accounts for information processing at lower levels (i.e., primary sensation) for which there are well-documented group differences. Currently, the more specific predictive coding accounts are better able to account for sensory-level abnormalities.

Furthermore, like executive dysfunction, cognitive control deficits are not specific to

ASD. However, this information theory approach makes it possible to quantify upper limits of information processing under time constraints in different cognitive domains, which may characterize the ASD from other disorders. This approach may also be able to explain savant skills beyond what has been previously proposed in predictive coding theories, by examining the uncertainty of the information. The idea that that the subject areas for which such skills are demonstrated comprise highly rule-based information with low uncertainty (and usually only one possible outcome) is consistent with the information theory approach.

28 Reconciling the information theory and predictive coding accounts of ASD.

The information theory and predictive coding accounts of uncertainty are similar, with some overlapping predictions for both healthy controls and as applied to ASD. Both accounts consider the involvement of high-level brain systems (rather than structures) in uncertainty management. For example, both approaches have proposed that uncertainty reduction is achieved via brain structures underlying attentional functions, notably the frontoparietal network (Fan, 2014; Feldman & Friston, 2010; Mackie et al., 2013).

However, while the predictive coding account is concerned with what follows the detection of a prediction error (i.e., “precision”), the information theory account is more concerned with questions about information processing efficiency and capacity, and the precise quantification of information.

These accounts are reconciled in considering that precision is related to the probability of making a choice, and that high precision serves to reduce uncertainty by placing greater weight on task-relevant information, relative to competing noise. In this way, optimizing precision allows for brain systems to minimize “free energy” or the amount of prediction error (Friston, 2010), which bounds the limits of surprise in the system. It has been argued that individuals are not able to directly estimate surprise (as they do not know all the possible states of the world) but can avoid surprising situations by attempting to minimize free energy, which has a value that is always greater than surprise (Friston, 2010).

Context for the present study

29 We aimed to demonstrate that individuals with ASD have a generally lower efficiency of cognitive control under uncertainty, with a reduced capacity under time constraints, compared to controls. We further proposed that these cognitive deficits also contribute to deficits in affective and social contexts that require processing of complex emotional states and recursive theory of mind inferences, which map more representatively onto the established symptom domains. To improve upon previous investigations of emotion processing deficits in ASD, an information theory approach will allow for a parametric manipulation of uncertainty about emotional states, and the quantification of uncertainty inherent in higher-order recursive theory of mind and intentionality. A finding that individuals with ASD are less efficient than typically developing controls in emotion processing and mentalizing under increasingly uncertain conditions would support this account. Furthermore, a significant correlation between cognitive control capacity and efficiency and performance on social-emotional tasks would support the idea that cognitive control deficits contribute to deficits in social and emotional processing in ASD.

30 Chapter III

Investigation of Cognitive Control Deficits in ASD

We propose that individuals with ASD have a reduced efficiency and capacity for cognitive control under conditions of uncertainty, and that these deficits are related to

ASD symptom presentation. To test this hypothesis, we developed a series of cognitive control tasks that parametrically manipulate uncertainty and information processing rate.

Method

Participants

Fifteen adult participants diagnosed with ASD and fifteen typically developing

(TD), right-handed, control participants between the ages of 18-43 were matched with

ASD participants on average full scale intelligence quotient (IQ), age, and gender.

Table 1

Demographic and questionnaire data (range) of ASD and TD groups.

Participant characteristics ASD TD t p (n = 15) (n = 15) Age (years) 26.0 (18 - 42) 27.6 (19 - 43) -.59 .56 Full Scale IQ 110 (87- 143) 115 (96 - 132) -.87 .39 ASD diagnosis 15

ADI-R Social 18.8 (1 - 28) Verbal communication 15.7 (8 - 22) Repetitive behavior 5.9 (1 - 12) Development 2.9 (0 - 5) ADOS-G (n = 14) Communication + Social 9.7 (5 - 16) Restricted and repetitive behaviors 1.4 (0 - 4) Total 12.1 (8 - 18)

Note: 1n = 14 ASD, 13 TD;

31 Participants with ASD were diagnosed by trained clinicians at the Seaver Autism

Center of the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai (ISMMS), according to the

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fifth edition (DSM-5). Diagnoses were confirmed by the Autism Diagnostic Interview – Revised (Lord, Rutter, & Couteur,

1994) and Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule – Generic (Lord et al., 2000). IQ scores were obtained using the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, second edition (WASI-II; Wechsler & Hsiao-pin, 2011). All participants had full scale IQ scores

> 70, i.e., no . Because all participants were high-functioning and verbal, it was not necessary to match on nonverbal IQ. Independent samples t-tests were used to confirm that the groups did not differ on age or IQ.

Exclusion criteria include history of , a lifetime history of substance/alcohol abuse or dependence, or schizophrenia. Additional exclusion criteria include history of encephalitis, phenylketonuria, tuberous sclerosis, , anoxia during birth, neurofibromatosis, hypomelanosis of Ito, hypothyroidism, Duchenne muscular dystrophy, and maternal rubella. Participants were also excluded for history of head injury with loss of or other neurological disorders. TD participants were screened for developmental disorders and psychiatric disorders and were also excluded based on systemic medical or neurological disorders.

Protection of Human Subjects. All participants provided written informed consent, approved by the Institutional Review Boards of QC and ISMMS. All data were participant coded and identifying information was kept separately in a locked cabinet in a secure location.

32 Recruitment. ASD participants were recruited from the Seaver Autism Center at

ISMMS, Interactive Autism Network (IAN), and from the greater New York City area through advertisements placed in community publications. Control participants were recruited from the Seaver Center’s matched control pool, or via flyers posted at Queens

College and ISMMS and ISMMS Department of email listserv advertisements.

Screening Materials and Questionnaires

Autism Diagnostic Interview – Revised (ADI-R). The ADI-R is a semi- structured diagnostic interview for caregivers of children and adults for whom autism may be a possible diagnosis (Lord, Rutter, & Le Couteur, 1994) and is a gold standard diagnostic instrument for ASD. Items cover the domains of communication, social development and play, repetitive and restrictive behaviors, and general behavioral problems, and are meant to capture caregiver descriptions of the actual behavior of the subject as it has occurred in daily life. The instrument is scored on an ordinal scale from 0

(“no definite behavior of the type specified”) to 2 (“definite abnormal behavior of the type described in the definition and coding”), with an occasional code of 3 to indicate extreme severity. Each item is scored for current behavior, except for behaviors that are restricted to particular developmental periods (e.g., “imaginative play”). A diagnosis of

ASD is determined when scores exceed cutoff scores in each domain. The cutoff is 8 for verbal individuals in the communication domain, 10 for social items, and 3 for restricted interests and repetitive behaviors. This instrument demonstrates adequate reliability (κ =

.62 – .89) and internal consistency (α = .69 – .95 for domain subscales) (Lord et al.,

33 1994). The measure also has good sensitivity and specificity with receiver operating characteristics curve discriminability values (area under the curve) of .77 - .84 for the clinical classification of ASD vs. non-ASD (De Bildt et al., 2013)

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule – Generic (ADOS-G). The ADOS-G is a semi-structured, standardized assessment of the domains of social interaction, communication, and for individuals who may have ASD (Lord et al., 2000).

ADOS-G items are scored on a 3-point ordinal scale from 0 (“no evidence of abnormality related to autism) to 2 (“definite evidence”). Some items may be coded as 3 to indicate severe abnormality that interfered with observation. ADOS-G classifications of ASD are given when each of the respective three thresholds are met or exceeded (social = 4; communication = 2; and social + communication = 7). Because the ADOS-G is limited to a small window of time, restricted and repetitive behaviors are not assessed, although recorded if present. This measure shows good internal consistency (α = .91 – .94 for social + communication), and good inter-rater reliability (r = .82 – 93), but test-retest reliability is questionable (r = .59 - .82), though generally higher for the social and communication subscales relative to the restricted and repetitive behavior subscale.

Receiver operating characteristic curves show sensitivity of .90 and specificity of .93 for

ASD vs. non-ASD comparisons (Lord et al., 2000).

Autism Quotient (AQ). The AQ is a 50-item self-report questionnaire assessing social skills, attention switching, attention to detail, communication, and imagination

(Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Skinner, Martin, & Clubley, 2001). This measure is scored on a 4-point Likert scale from “definitely agree” to “definitely disagree”, and scores can

34 be collapsed into two categories (“agree” and “disagree”). Test re-test reliability is adequate (r = .70), inter-item consistency is marginally adequate (α = .63 – .77).

Recommended cutoff of > 32 has sensitivity of .95 and specificity of 0.52 for ASD vs. non-ASD comparisons (Woodbury-Smith, Robinson, Wheelwright & Baron-Cohen,

2005).

Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-IV). The SCID-IV is a semi- structured clinician-administered interview for making DSM-IV Axis I diagnoses, including modules on mood, anxiety, psychotic, substance use, somatoform, eating, and adjustment disorders (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1998). Inter-rater reliability is adequate (κ = .61 – .83). Information about sensitivity and specificity is not available.

Social Responsiveness Scale - 2 (SRS-2). The SRS-2 (Constantino & Gruber

2012) is a 65-item, self-report, ordinal-scale measure (1 = “not true” to 4 = “almost always true”) that measures the severity of autism traits in the domains of Social

Awareness, Social Cognition, Social Communication, Social Motivation, and Autistic

Mannerisms. Internal consistency is good (α = .95), strong test-retest reliability (r = .88 -

.95) and inter-rater reliability (r = .61 - .92)(Bruni, 2014). Responses are converted to normative T scores. Total Scores of T > 75 are indicative of potential ASD, with specificity of .60 and sensitivity of .86 for ASD vs. non-ASD comparisons

(Mandell et al., 2012).

Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence-2 (WASI-2). The WASI-II

(Wechsler & Hsiao-pin, 2011) is a brief measure of cognitive ability used to estimate full scale intelligence quotient (FSIQ), comprised of a verbal comprehension index score

35 (crystallized abilities), and a perceptual reasoning index score (fluid abilities). This measure has good test-retest reliability (r = .90 - .97), and subtests and index scores can distinguish between mild intellectual disability and matched controls.

Cognitive Control Tasks

Entropy Variation Task (EVT). The EVT examines the baseline cognitive control performance effect of both entropy (H) and surprise (I) in a single task for sequential information processing. Left- or right-pointing arrows appear randomly at one of eight possible locations arranged around a central fixation cross (Figure 1). Following a 0 to 500 milliseconds (ms) randomly varied fixation interval on each trial, the target arrow appears for 1500 ms, followed by a variable post-target fixation period, with a total trial time of 3000 ms. Participants must indicate the direction of the target arrow. This is a single-trial and block mixed design. For each block type, entropy has different values,

Figure 1. Schematic of Entropy Variation Task

36 with manipulation of the probability of left-pointing arrows (p), or right-pointing arrows

(q), and therefore surprise for each trial type has different values. There are four block types: 1) arrows point in a single direction throughout the entire sequence (H = 0 bits; p/q

= 0 or 1; I = 0 bits); 2) alternating sequence of left- and right-pointing arrows, e.g.

“LRLRLR…”, and vice versa (H = 0 bits; p = 1; q=1; I = 0 bits); 3) arrows point in one direction more frequently than the other (H = 0.54 bits; p/q = 0.125, I = 3 bits or p/q =

0.825, I = 0.19 bits); and 4) randomly presented left- and right-pointing arrows with equal probability (H = 1 bits; p = q = 0.5; I = 1 bits). Therefore, entropy is manipulated on three sequence levels (0, 0.54, and 1 bit) and surprise on four event type levels (0, 0.19, 1, and

3 bits). There are 8 runs, with 4 blocks each (Latin square counterbalanced), each block has 32 trials, for a total of 1024 trials. Each run lasts approximately 6 minutes, beginning and ending with a 30 s fixation period, with 5 s fixation periods between each block.

Total task time is approximately 50 minutes.

Majority Function Task (MFT). The MFT systematically manipulates uncertainty with computational load (estimated as information entropy) over a wide range to capture the effects of cognitive control for each target event, independent of the sequence (Fan et al., 2014; Fan, Guise, Liu, & Wang, 2008; Wang, Liu, & Fan, 2011). In this task, groups of arrows (set sizes 1, 3 or 5, corresponding to 3 types of blocks) are randomly presented at 8 possible locations arranged around a central fixation cross

(Figure 2). The arrows are presented simultaneously, pointing either left or right, and participants must indicate the direction in which the majority of arrows point. There are six conditions, indicating the ratios of arrows pointing in the same direction to arrows

37 pointing in the opposite direction: 1:0 for set size 1; 3:0 and 2:1 for set size 3; and 5:0,

4:1, and 3:2 for set size 5. Trials begin with a variable fixation period of 0 to 1000 ms.

Stimuli are then presented for 2500 ms, followed by a variable 1500 to 2500 ms post- stimulus fixation period. Each trial lasts 5 seconds. There are six runs with six blocks each (two for each set size), each block has 12 trials with the same set size, and each run has 72 trials, lasting 395 s. There are 5 s fixation periods at the beginning and end of each run, as well as 10 s between blocks in each run. The order of the blocks is counterbalanced with reversed repetition for each run. Total trial number is 432, with a total time of approximately 40 minutes. Previous algorithmic and computational modeling analyses of MFT performance revealed estimated computational loads for the six conditions are 1.00, 2.00, 3.58, 2.58, 3.91, and 5.91 bits, respectively, including an

Figure 2. Sample stimuli in the Majority Function Task

38 additive 1 bit for the response (Fan et al., 2008; Wang et al., 2011).

Dual Conflict Task (DCT). This task examines the impact of the bottleneck of cognitive control capacity by manipulating both conflict processing and time constraints.

On each trial, following a 0 to 500 ms randomly varied fixation interval, two tasks (Task

1 and Task 2) are presented sequentially for 750 ms each with a variable Task 1 to Task 2 stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) of 100 and 1000 ms (Figure 3). The 750 ms task duration is used to avoid the attentional blink effect, which would interfere with detection of the second target if a shorter (e.g., 500 ms) duration were used. For Task 1, the stimulus is presented in one of two locations, aligned vertically, either above or below the central fixation cross, and consists of a central target arrow, flanked by 4 direction- congruent or incongruent arrows (2 on each side), pointing either up or down. For Task 2, the stimulus is presented either to the left or right of the central fixation cross and similarly includes a central target arrow flanked by 4 direction-congruent or incongruent arrows, pointing either left or right. Task 2 is followed by a variable post-target fixation

(2000 – 2500 ms), with total trial time of 5000 ms. Participants must make an up/down response to the central arrows for Task 1 using the left hand buttons, and a left/right response for Task 2 using the right hand buttons, sequentially. There are 8 blocks, with

64 trials per block. Each block lasts approximately 6 minutes, and the total task time is approximately 50 minutes.

39

Computational loads for Tasks 1 and 2 are approximately 1 bit (which is log22 for

2 possible response directions) respectively under the congruent condition, and greater than 1 but less than 2 under the incongruent condition. The conflict resulting from task- irrelevant flankers can be estimated as less than or equal to a difference of 1 bit between conflict and no-conflict conditions (Fan, 2014; Wang et al., 2011). The two possible locations of the target for each task contribute to a computational load of 1 bit. Therefore, for Task 1 and Task 2, the minimum and maximum computational loads are 2 and 3, respectively. In a previous pilot study it was demonstrated that under the 1000 ms SOA the computational load of Task 2 is not significantly affected by Task 1, indicating that the information processing involved in each task does not overlap, resulting in sequential task processing. However, under the 100 ms SOA, the tasks occur in much quicker succession, resulting in task processing overlap, with the computational load during Task

2 processing approaching the sum of the computational load of Tasks 1 and 2, an additive

Figure 3. Schematic of Dual Conflict Task

40 effect based on RT pattern. Therefore, under the 100 ms SOA, the minimum (Task 1 congruent, Task 2 congruent, CC) and maximum (Task 1 incongruent, Task 2 incongruent, II) computational loads for Task 2 are 4 and 6 bits, respectively. Therefore, for the whole task (including both 1000 and 100 ms SOA conditions), the minimum and maximum computation loads are 2 bits for the 1000 SOA CC condition and 6 bits for the

100 SOA II condition, respectively. The estimated loads for Task 2 under different conditions are shown in Table 2.

Table 2

Experimental conditions and estimations of input information and computational load under both SOAs of the DCT

Stimulus condition and load in bits Load during Task 2 Task 1 load Task 2 load 1000 ms 100 ms C 2 C 2 2 4 C 2 I 3 3 5

I 3 C 2 2 5 I 3 I 3 3 6 Note: C: congruent; I: incongruent.

Masked Majority Function Task (MFT-M). This task was designed to investigate the capacity of cognitive control under time constraints aimed at computation of cognitive control bandwidth, with methods that have been previously published (Wu,

Dufford, Mackie, Egan, & Fan, 2016). In this task, groups of arrows with set sizes of 1, 3, and 5 are simultaneously randomly presented at 8 possible locations arranged as an octagon centered on a fixation cross (see Figure 4). Each arrow points either to the left or right, and participants must indicate by button press the majority direction of the arrows.

The ratios of majority and minority directions are as described for the MFT above. There

41 is a variable fixation period of 0 to 500 ms at the start of each trial, followed by the target arrow set. Exposure time (ET) to the target arrows is varied by manipulating stimulus onset duration and then applying backward masking. Exposure time can be 250, 500,

1000, or 2000 ms, followed by the mask for 500 ms. The mask consists of 8 diamond shapes presented at the same 8 possible target locations. The length of the arrows and diameter of diamond shape are kept constant at 6.5 mm. The offset of the mask initiates a varied post-stimulus fixation period, such that the total duration of the arrow set, mask, and post-stimulus fixation periods is 2500 ms. Participants are instructed to make a response as quickly and accurately as possible to indicate in which direction the majority of the arrows point by pressing one of two mouse buttons with the index or middle finger of the right hand. Responses must be made within a 2500 ms response window from the onset of the target arrow set. Participants are encouraged to guess when they fail to find the majority direction. Feedback is presented on each trial for 750 ms, with a variable post-feedback fixation period. Each trial is 5750 ms in duration.

The task consists of 12 blocks of trials in which each block is comprised of one of the set sizes and ETs. The presentation of the blocks is in random order and each block consists of 36 trials, also presented in random order. Within each block, there are the same number of trials in each congruence condition, with equal numbers of trials requiring left and right responses: 36 trials for 1:0 in a 1-arrow block; 18 trials for 3:0 and

2:1 in a 3-arrow block; and 12 trials each for 5:0, 4:1, and 3:2 in a 5-arrow block. There is a 3 s fixation period at the beginning and end of each block, and each block has a total duration of 213 s. The total number of trials in this task is 432, with a total task duration

42 of approximately 43 minutes. In a previous study investigating the capacity of cognitive control in healthy control participants using performance on the MFT-M, we tested various mental algorithms for performance and estimated the capacity of cognitive control to be approximately 3-4 bps, and detailed methods related to the estimation of capacity can be found therein (Wu et al., 2016). In the present study, we predicted that the estimated capacity of cognitive control would be significantly lower for the ASD group relative to the TD group.

a b 3:2 +

+ + Fixation 0~ 0.5 s 2:1 4:1 Arrow Set +

ET + + Congruency Mask + 0.5 s 1:0 3:0 5:0

Fixation Correct! 0~1.75 s + + +

Feedback + 0.75 s Set size 1 Set size 3 Set size 5 c d Fixation ...... 1~1.5 s 36 trials ...... 36 trials 0.25 s ...... 36 trials 0.5 s 1s

2s 2 2 2 2.5 s (Response window) 1 1 1 0.5 0.5 0.5 5.75 s 0.25 0.25 0.25

Figure 4. Schematic of Masked Majority Function Task

Procedure

For the first three tasks, all participants completed: 1) EVT; 2) MFT; and 3) DCT, in the same order. The MFT-M was completed in Phase II of this study with a different sample meeting the criteria above. Participants were instructed to respond as quickly and

43 accurately as possible and took self-initiated and self-terminated breaks as needed between runs within each task, as well as between each task, to control for fatigue.

Data Analysis

For all four tasks, the primary independent variable is Entropy (cognitive load) in bits, manipulated across various levels within each task. The MFT-M also manipulates

Exposure Time at four levels. The primary dependent variable is efficiency

(Accuracy/RT, reflecting the probability of a correct response per unit time in seconds), taking both speed and accuracy into account. Average efficiency >1 typically indicates high accuracy and/or RT <1 s. Efficiency <1 typically reflects lower accuracy and/or RT

>1s. For group comparisons, a higher efficiency score indicates better performance. In the interest of readability, only ANOVA results for efficiency are reported below. All RT and accuracy ANOVA results are reported in Appendix B.

Data were tested for normal distributions and homogeneity of variance (Levene’s test). For within-subjects factors where Mauchley’s test indicated a violation of the assumption of sphericity, univariate analyses of variance with Greenhouse-Geisser correction were reported.

We also estimated the best-fit regression line for all tasks (where applicable) to obtain the slope and intercept of performance as a function of cognitive load for each participant, and use independent t-tests to examine group differences in both baseline performance and rates of change in performance. A lower efficiency intercept indicates a lower level of cognitive control efficiency at baseline. With efficiency scores plotted against information entropy in bits, efficiency scores decrease as information increases,

44 resulting in a negative slope. A more negative number is indicative of a faster rate of decline in performance with increasing information. For each task mixed factorial analyses of variance (ANOVA) were conducted with Group as the between-subjects factor.

Non-parametric correlation analyses (Kendall’s tau) were performed to assess the relationship between efficiency on the tasks where applicable and symptom report on

ADI-R, ADOS-G, AQ, and SRS. False discovery rate corrections were applied to control for Type I error. One-tailed statistical tests were utilized to test our directional predictions.

Results

Cognitive control for sequential stimuli: Results of the EVT

For this analysis, we excluded one TD participant whose overall accuracy on this task was 62%, drastically below the group accuracy mean of 96% (final n = 15, ASD; n =

14 TD). This participant’s performance was within normal limits relative to his group on both the MFT and DCT. Efficiency performance on the EVT for both entropy and surprise is presented in Table 3 and Figure 5. Means of performance across all conditions are reported in Table 3.

Table 3

Mean (SD) performance on the EVT in terms of efficiency, RT, and accuracy.

Efficiency RT (ms) Accuracy (%) TD ASD TD ASD TD ASD Entropy

0 2.50 (0.30) 2.24 (0.41) 402 (45) 453 (82) 99 (1) 98 (1) 0.54 2.20 (0.21) 2.04 (0.30) 420 (111) 484 (73) 97 (2) 96 (2)

45 1 2.05 (0.19) 1.87 (0.24) 475 (46) 517 (73) 97 (3) 95 (3)

Surprise

0 2.50 (0.30) 2.24 (0.41) 402 (45) 453 (82) 99 (1) 98 (1) 0.19 2.27 (0.23) 2.11 (0.32) 439 (41) 474 (72) 99 (1) 98 (2) 1 2.05 (0.19) 1.87 (0.24) 475 (46) 517 (73) 97 (3) 95 (3) 3 1.73 (0.20) 1.55 (0.27) 503 (42) 559 (77) 87 (11) 85 (11)

a 2.5 b 2.5

2 2

1.5 1.5 Effciency Effciency 1 1

0.5 0.5 TD TD ASD ASD 0 0 0 0.54 1 00.19 1 3 Entropy (bits) Surprise (bits)

Figure 5. EVT efficiency results for both (a) entropy and (b) surprise

Cognitive control efficiency for sequential stimuli – entropy. Results are presented in Figure 5a. The main effect of Group was marginally significant (F(1,27) =

2 4.18, p = .05, ηp = .13), reflecting less efficient performance in the ASD group (M = 1.97

± 0.28) than the TD group (M = 2.17±0.22). For entropy, sphericity had been violated, c2(2) = 19.07, p < .001, e = .66, and with correction the main effect was significant

2 (F(1.32, 35.53) = 81.88, p < .001, ηp = .75). Pairwise comparisons (Bonferroni- corrected) revealed significant decreases in efficiency for each entropy value point, all ps

< .001). There was no significant Group by entropy interaction (F < 1). Intercept was significantly lower for the ASD group (M = 2.24±0.41, R2= .92) than for the TD group

46 (M = 2.48 ± 0.29, R2= .95), t(27) = -1.85, p < .05. There was no significant difference in slope between groups (t(27) = 0.981, p = .34).

Cognitive control efficiency for sequential stimuli – surprise. Results are presented in Figure 5b. The main effect of Group was significant (F(1,27) = 5.32, p < .05,

2 ηp = .16), with less efficient performance in the ASD group (M = 1.94 ± 0.41) than the

TD group (M = 2.14 ± 0.37). For surprise, sphericity was violated, c2(5) = 71.80, p <

.001, e = .41, and with correction the main effect was significant (F(1.23, 33.19) = 81.06,

2 p < .001, ηp = .75). Pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni corrections revealed significant decreases in efficiency for each surprise value point (all ps < .001). The

Group by surprise interaction for efficiency was not significant (F < 1). Intercept was significantly lower for the ASD group (M = 2.17 ± 0.36, R2= .89) than for the TD group

(M = 2.38 ± 0.26, R2= .87), t(27) = -1.77, p < .05. There were no significant group differences in slope (t(27) = 0.26, p = .80).

Cognitive control for non-sequential stimuli: Results of the MFT

Results are presented in Table 4 and Figure 6. The main effect of Group was

2 significant (F(1,28) = 12.70, p < 0.001, ηp = .31), such that the ASD group (M = 0.91 ±

0.12) demonstrated decreased overall performance efficiency compared to the TD group

(M = 1.06 ± 0.11). Sphericity was violated for entropy, c2(14) = 103.46, p < 0.001, e =

.41, and with correction, the main effect was significant, F(2.1,57.5) = 509.25, p < 0.001,

2 ηp = .95. The Group by entropy interaction was significant, F(2.1,57.5) = 3.68, p < 0.05,

2 ηp = .12. Pairwise comparisons (Bonferroni-corrected) indicated the TD group had significantly higher performance on each MFT condition compared to the ASD group.

47 For the six conditions of 1:0, 3:0, 2:1, 5:0, 4:1, and 3:2, ts(28) = 2.38, 2.73, 4.61, 3.18,

2.97, and 2.17, respectively, all ps < .05. We also found a significant difference in slope between the two groups t(28) = 2.37, p < .05, such that TD group (M = -0.28 ± 0.04, R2 =

.92) had a higher rate of decreasing performance efficiency than ASD group (M = -0.24 ±

0.05, R2 = .91). The ASD group had a significantly lower intercept (M = 1.42 ± 0.20) than the TD group (M = 1.66 ± 0.20), t(28) = -3.18, p < .01.

Table 4

Mean (SD) performance on the MFT in terms of efficiency, RT, and accuracy.

Efficiency RT (ms) Accuracy (%) Condition H TD ASD TD ASD TD ASD

1:00 1 1.76 (0.24) 1.54 (0.27) 570 (81) 649 (132) 99 (2) 97 (3)

3:00 2 1.38 (0.19) 1.19 (0.18) 735 (100) 841 (116) 99 (1) 99 (2) 2:01 3.58 0.78 (0.10) 0.67 (0.09) 1244 (131) 1348 (160) 96 (4) 89 (6)

5:00 2.58 1.26 (0.16) 1.01 (0.13) 804 (104) 985 (113) 99 (1) 98 (3) 4:01 3.91 0.73 (0.07) 0.65 (0.08) 1352 (129) 1441 (149) 97 (3) 92 (7) 3:02 5.91 0.44 (0.06) 0.38 (0.09) 1689 (123) 1767 (159) 74 (10) 66 (13) Note: H = entropy

2 TD 1.8 ASD 1.6 1.4 1.2 1

Effciency 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 Entropy (bits)

Figure 6. MFT efficiency results 48 Cognitive control capacity under time constraints: Results of the DCT and MFT-M

DCT. DCT performance efficiency as a function of computational load of Task 2 is shown in Table 5 and Figure 7. Additional analyses are also presented in Mackie &

2 Fan (2015). The main effect of Group was not significant (F(1,28) = 2.72, p = .11, ηp =

.09). Sphericity was violated for entropy c2(9) = 44.65, p < .001, e = .67, and with

2 correction there was a significant main effect (F(2.70,75.52) = 376.16, p < .001, ηp =

.93). Pairwise comparisons (Bonferroni-corrected) indicated significant decreases in efficiency for each increasing value of entropy, all ps > .001. The interaction between

2 entropy and Group was significant (F(2.70,75.52) = 4.78, p < .01, ηp = .15), with greater efficiency decrease in the ASD group compared to the TD group as a function of cognitive load. There were no significant group differences for intercept (t(28) = -0.64, p

= 0.26) or slope (t(28) = -1.08, p = 0.14). The groups did not differ significantly in

2 1.8 1.6 1.4 1.2 1

Effciency 0.8 0.6 0.4 TD 0.2 ASD 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 Task 2 entropy (bits)

Figure 7. DCT efficiency results for Task 2

49 performance at baseline (Task 1, CC 1000 ms SOA, 2 bits) (F < 1), but the ASD group performed significantly less efficiently than the TD group at high cognitive load (Task 2,

II 100 ms SOA, 6 bits), F(1,28) = 7.48, p < .05.

Table 5

Mean (SD) performance on Task 2 of the DCT in terms of efficiency, RT, and accuracy.

Efficiency SOA (ms)

100 1000

TD ASD TD ASD

Task 1 Task 2 congruent congruent 0.91 (0.17) 0.89 (0.19) 1.71 (0.23) 1.65 (0.30) congruent incongruent 0.77 (0.17) 0.63 (0.22) 1.30 (0.17) 1.05 (0.37) incongruent congruent 0.80 (0.16) 0.75 (0.24) 1.65 (0.22) 1.56 (0.35) incongruent incongruent 0.72 (0.18) 0.50 (0.25) 1.27 (0.20) 1.05 (0.38) RT (ms) SOA (ms)

100 1000

TD ASD TD ASD

Task 1 Task 2 congruent congruent 1099 (177) 1052 (146) 596 (88) 606 (135) congruent incongruent 1221 (209) 1152 (163) 763 (99) 802 (169) incongruent congruent 1224 (184) 1156 (158) 614 (88) 643 (145) incongruent incongruent 1266 (194) 1313 (312) 782 (126) 814 (191)

Accuracy (%)

SOA (ms)

100 1000

TD ASD TD ASD

Task 1 Task 2 congruent congruent 97 (4) 91 (10) 100 (1) 97 (5) congruent incongruent 91 (7) 71 (23) 97 (3) 79 (22) incongruent congruent 95 (7) 84 (21) 100 (1) 95 (7) incongruent incongruent 88 (9) 62 (28) 97 (2) 81 (20)

50 MFT-M. In the MFT-M, the primary dependent variable is accuracy as a function of cognitive load and exposure time (ET). Results are presented in Table 6 and Figure 8.

In terms of accuracy, the main effect of ET was significant (F(3, 72) = 36.09, p < .001,

2 ηp = .60), such that accuracy increased with increasing ET. Pairwise comparisons revealed that while there was no significant increase in accuracy between the 250 and 500 ms conditions, accuracy increased significantly between the 500, 1000, and 2000 ms conditions (ps < .01). Sphericity was violated for Entropy (c2(2) = 7.54, p = .02, e = .78), and the main effect was significant with correction (F(1.56, 37.51) = 499.50, p < .001,

2 ηp = .95), such that there was a significant decrease in accuracy with each increase in entropy (ps < .001). The ET by Entropy interaction was significant (F(6, 144) = 9.61, p <

2 .001, ηp = .29), such that the extent to which accuracy decreased with entropy became lesser as ET increased. The main effect of Group trended toward significance (F(1,244) =

2 2.81, p = .06, ηp = .11). The ET by Group (F < 1), Entropy by Group (p = .13), and ET by

Entropy by Group (F < 1) interactions were not significant.

Cognitive control capacity. For the capacity estimation, one participant from each group was excluded for inadequate accuracy performance. The estimated capacity of the

TD group was 3.98 bps on average (range = 3.04 to 4.92), and 3.38 bps for the ASD

Figure 8. Results of MFT-M for accuracy and cognitive control capacity. 51 group (range = 2.31 to 4.89). An independent samples t-test revealed that the capacity of the ASD group was significantly lower than the TD group (t(26) = -1.95, p = .04, one- tailed; Figure 8c). Please refer to Appendix B for RT results as a function of entropy and exposure time.

Table 6

Means (and SE) of RT and accuracy for all conditions of the MFT-M

ET (ms) Ratios 5:0 4:1 3:2 TD ASD TD ASD TD ASD RT (ms) 250 502 (24) 543 (43) 618 (48) 654 (61) 679 (59) 718 (68) 500 540 (29) 566 (29) 724 (43) 727 (47) 829 (60) 807 (54) 1000 560 (21) 625 (35) 820 (33) 885 (64) 1050 (36) 1046 (76) 2000 620 (27) 701 (44) 952 (56) 1119 (89) 1273 (79) 1371 (81) Accuracy (%) 250 99 (1) 96 (2) 82 (2) 81 (3) 64 (3) 60 (2) 500 99 (0) 98 (1) 88 (2) 85 (2) 64 (3) 62 (3) 1000 99 (0) 99 (0) 94 (1) 91 (2) 71 (3) 67 (3) 2000 99 (0) 99 (0) 96 (1) 93 (2) 80 (2) 73 (3)

Relationships between task performance and symptom domains

Correlation coefficients are presented in Table 7. Because the value for efficiency slope is negative, a negative correlation suggests that a more negative (steeper) efficiency slope, or faster rate of decrease in efficiency as a function of cognitive load (entropy), was associated with greater ASD symptom report. Due in part to small sample size and restricted variance of symptom scores, none of these correlations survived false discovery rate correction.

Table 7

52 Kendall’s tau correlation coefficients for relationships between behavioral efficiency and

ADI-R and ADOS-G subscales.

ADI-R ADOS-G

Efficiency S C B/I C S B/I

EVT1 entropy slope -.27 -.41* .15 -.21 -.44* .15 entropy intercept -.12 .16 -.17 .23 .00 -.37* surprise slope -.10 -.33* -.03 -.14 -.27 .12 surprise intercept -.29 .06 -.09 .14 -.12 -.42* Overall -.36* -.07 -.12 .35 -.18 -.24

MFT Slope .53** .20 .07 .11 .12 .57** Intercept -.53** -.06 -.19 -.04 -.17 -.49* Overall -.43* -.06 -.26 .04 -.10 -.30

DCT 2 Slope .32* .32* .08 .23 .01 .11 Intercept -.31 -.31 -.19 -.09 -.07 -.12 Overall -.23 -.22 -.15 .11 -.02 -.12 * p < .05, ** p < .01 (one-tailed) Note: S = social; C = communication; B/I = Behavior/Interests; : 1n = 14, except for overall efficiency analysis; 2n = 14 for ADOS-G. None of the correlations survived false discovery rate correction Discussion

In this study we tested our hypothesis that individuals with ASD implement cognitive control less efficiently than TD adults with a lower capacity for cognitive control, and that these deficits possibly contribute to the clinical presentation of the disorder. On tasks designed to systematically manipulate uncertainty and test cognitive control efficiency, the ASD group showed a lower baseline efficiency of information processing for sequential events, a general reduction in efficiency for non-sequential

53 events over a wide range of uncertainty, and a lower capacity for cognitive control under time constraints.

The performance of ASD relative to TD can be considered in terms of three hypothetical efficiency performance models (Figure 9): (a) The ASD group has a lower baseline than controls, both groups decrease in efficiency as uncertainty increases, and the ASD group has a lower upper limit than TD (Figure 9a, Model A); (b) The ASD group has a lower baseline than controls, the performance difference remains somewhat constant across increasing uncertainty values, with performance finally converging with

TD at the highest levels of uncertainty (Figure 9b, Model B); and (c) The ASD group and controls have similar baseline performance and then diverge as uncertainty increases, with ASD efficiency beginning to decrease at a lower uncertainty level compared to TD

(Figure 9c, Model C).

a TD b TD c TD ASD ASD ASD Effciency Effciency Effciency

Uncertainty Uncertainty Uncertainty

Figure 9. Hypothetical performance models of ASD relative to TD The EVT results fit best with Model A, with relatively lower efficiency in the

ASD group across the full range of uncertainty values when the task involved sequential information. The MFT results fit Model B, for non-sequential information processing,

54 performance was less efficient for the ASD group at low uncertainty levels, converging toward a similar level as TD at high uncertainty levels. At first glance, the EVT and MFT results might appear to be at odds with existing information processing theories that predict equivalent group performance at low load levels (e.g., (Minshew & Goldstein,

1998; Minshew et al., 2001). However, we speculate that differences at baseline may be explained by additional uncertainty due to spatial location (1/8 possible locations for ~3 bits) in these tasks. This possibility is highlighted in the DCT results, which best fit

Model C, and show that the ASD group performed less efficiently than controls at the high capacity condition rather than at the low capacity condition. In the EVT and MFT, participants are required to shift attention from fixation to one of eight possible target locations, whereas in the DCT, target stimuli appear in fewer possible locations (4 possible locations for Tasks 1 and 2 combined). It is possible then, that the additional attentional orienting requirement contributed to group differences at baseline for the EVT and MFT, but not for the DCT. These results are consistent with theories that propose that deficits in ASD arise at more demanding levels of information processing, and provide further support for the idea of a greater limitation on information processing capacity in ASD compared to TD (Belmonte et al., 2004; Just et al., 2012). Taken together, it appears that Model C is the most plausible, and can best explain performance in these experiments.

The observed group differences in cognitive control under uncertainty suggest that deficits in control of information processing may contribute to ASD symptoms, as social and communicative processing involves dealing with uncertainty at various levels,

55 such as decoding a linguistic message or inferring the mental states/intentions of others.

According to uncertainty reduction theory (based upon information theory (Berger &

Calabrese, 1975)), people undertake several steps to reduce uncertainty in social situations. In ASD, lower efficiency in sequential and non-sequential information processing in concert with a reduced upper limit of information processing capacity, may negatively impact the ability to effectively engage in this uncertainty reduction, resulting in social and communication deficits. Furthermore, deficits in the domain of restricted interests and repetitive behaviors may serve to compensate for a diminished uncertainty- reduction capacity. This is supported by strong correlation with a large effect size between MFT performance and the restricted interests repetitive behaviors domain in this study (though this did not survive false discovery rate multiple comparison correction).

By restricting interests to predictable sequences and familiar domains, individuals with

ASD are able to avoid cognitive control overload.

Cognitive control of information processing is supported by the frontoparietal network, with the anterior cingulate (ACC) and anterior insular (AIC) cortices as core regions, in addition to other regions such as the frontal eye fields and near/along the intraparietal sulcus (Fan, 2014; Fan et al., 2014). Reduced cognitive control efficiency in

ASD may be related to a deficiency in this network. We have previously shown that there is a lack of activation of the ACC in ASD during conflict processing (Fan et al., 2012).

ACC is involved in baseline state uncertainty monitoring, and abnormal recruitment of this crucial network hub could underlie the inefficient performance demonstrated in our experiments. In addition to region-specific neural differences, there is also evidence for

56 differences in connectivity within the frontoparietal network in ASD, resulting in inefficient information transfer between frontal and parietal areas (Matthew K. Belmonte et al., 2004; Just et al., 2004; Just et al., 2012; Kana, Keller, Cherkassky, Minshew, &

Just, 2006), contributing to symptom presentation (Uddin et al., 2014).

Limitations and alternative explanations

A primary limitation in this study is the relatively small size of the sample.

Further, given that participants in the ASD group were relatively high-functioning and all male, there are limits to the generalizability of these findings. However, one might expect that lower-functioning individuals would show even greater relative impairment on the tasks described in this study. ASD is often comorbid with other conditions, and two of our participants were previously diagnosed with ADHD. We do not believe that this significantly affected the results due to the small proportion of participants involved.

Ideally, these findings should be replicated in a larger sample, free of comorbidity, to address these limitations.

One could argue that the differences we observed in performance might be attributed to motor slowing previously documented in ASD (Kenworthy, Yerys,

Weinblatt, Abrams, & Wallace, 2013; Williams, Goldstein, & Minshew, 2013). While we did not directly examine motor speed, it is notable that there were no significant group differences in overall RT for the DCT; EVT overall RT difference trended toward significance; and MFT overall RT was significantly faster for the ASD group. This unclear pattern of RT differences makes it difficult to draw conclusions about generalized slowing in the ASD group. Previous work has suggested that slowness to respond in ASD

57 may result from use of psychoactive medications (Bogte et al., 2008). However, our sample was unmedicated at the time of participation in the study. Additionally, we presented a large number of trials across tasks, which could potentially result in differential fatigue effects between groups. However, failure to find group differences in

RT on the third task, the DCT, rules out this possibility.

Conclusion

This study represents a preliminary step in the investigation of cognitive control in ASD and the relationship between cognitive control deficits and the symptom presentation of ASD. We found participants with ASD had a generally lower efficiency of cognitive control, with a reduced capacity under time constraints, compared to controls. Of three possible explanations of group differences in performance, the model in which the ASD group is less efficient than controls as cognitive control load increases best fit the present data. While this study has targeted the processing of stimuli not typically encountered in daily life, it would be beneficial for future studies to investigate the role of cognitive control deficits as they arise in more ecologically valid tasks that require higher-level cognition, such as language and theory of mind, which map more representatively onto the established symptom domains.

58

Chapter IV

Investigation of Affective Cognitive Control in ASD

We proposed that individuals with ASD have greater difficulty than TD controls with affective cognitive control as uncertainty increases, and that these deficits are related to ASD symptom presentation. To test this hypothesis, we developed a series of social and affective information processing tasks that parametrically manipulate uncertainty and information processing rate.

Method

Participants

The affective information processing tasks were implemented with a sample of participants meeting the same inclusion criteria as described in Chapter III. Some were those included in the previous study (roughly half) and some were new recruits through the Seaver Center and the greater New York City area.

Table 8

Means of demographic data (range) of ASD and TD groups.

Participant characteristics ASD TD t p (n = 15) (n = 15) Age (years) 32.6 (21-49) 30.4 (21-47) 0.73 .47 Full Scale IQ 100.5 (82-123) 100.1 (83-119) 0.09 .93 ASD diagnosis 15 -- ADOS-G (n = 15) Communication + Social 9.6 (7-12) -- Restricted and repetitive behaviors 2.4 (1-3) -- Total 20.9 (9-38) SRS Total 62.5 (50-89) 49 (41-64) 3.58 .001 AQ Total 27.43 (15-36) 13.4 (6-23) 5.00 .000 EQ Total 30.0 (16-47) 47.3 (27-62) -3.92 .001

59

Note: SRS = Social Responsiveness Scale; AQ = Autism Quotient; EQ = Emotion

Quotient

Tasks

Complex Emotion Recognition Task (CERT). This task was designed to parametrically manipulate uncertainty in emotion recognition by displaying facial emotion expressions that vary in complexity (basic or compound; Figure 10). The complexity factor manipulates how readily one might be able to identify the emotion displayed. For basic emotions, happiness, sadness, fear, disgust, surprise, and anger are represented (6 total). In the compound condition, the emotions represented reflect combinations of basic emotions, that are presumed to occur naturally along the spectrum of human emotional expression (e.g., angrily disgusted, happily surprised) as described in a recent publication (Du et al., 2014). While the labels ascribed to these emotions may not be typically used by individuals to describe their emotional state, the presence of such combinations of emotions as part of the human experience is intuitive. The task utilizes a two-alternative forced-choice) response structure in which an emotional face is presented along with the label of a basic emotion, and the participant must indicate (Yes or No) whether they identify that emotion in the facial expression presented. That is, all emotion types are presented with labels of basic emotions, and participants must determine whether the prompted emotion can be found in the emotional display. The purpose of this design was to induce a component of response inhibition in the emotional domain. For instance, if the prompt reads “happy?”, and the emotion displayed is happily surprised, the participant must inhibit detection of task-irrelevant features related to the emotion of

60 surprise, and focus attention on the task-relevant features related to happiness, and likewise for all other combinations of basic and compound emotions.

BASIC COMPOUND Basic (True) Basic (False) Compound (True) Compound (False) (happy) (surprised) (sadly fearful) (angrily disgusted)

happy? angry? fearful? sad?

Figure 10. Sample stimuli in the Complex Emotion Recognition Task

The emotional stimuli are static photographs chosen from a database developed using facial action unit coding to represent 6 basic emotion and 12 compound emotions

(Du et al., 2014). Each trial begins with a variable fixation period of 0 – 1000 ms, followed by the target stimulus, which remains present for the duration of the response window (2500 ms). Following the response, there is a variable post-stimulus fixation period such that the total trial time is 4500 ms. There are 3 blocks of 72 trials, with equal numbers of basic and compound stimuli, equal numbers of trials for which the response is

True and False, presented in random order. There is a 3 second fixation period at the beginning of each block, and a 20 s fixation period between blocks. The total task duration is approximately 20 minutes.

Multiple Emotion Processing Task (MEPT). This task was designed to investigate behavioral effects related to emotional complexity manipulated by the number of individuals for whom emotional states must be inferred. This task parametrically

61 manipulates complexity of emotional information for three set sizes of actors (1, 2, and 3; see Figure 11). Within each set size, the congruence of emotion is also manipulated, resulting in six conditions. For set size 1, there is only one face portraying 1 emotion (1 face: 1 emotion condition), and congruence is not relevant. This condition can be considered a measure of basic emotion recognition that is necessary for adequate performance in the other conditions. For set size 2, the faces either show the same emotion (2 faces: 1 emotion condition) or different emotions (2 faces: 2 emotions condition). Presumably, individuals are more likely to respond more quickly and accurately when both emotions are the same; considering different emotional states for different individuals is assumed to be more challenging. For set size 3, the faces either all show the same emotion (3 faces: 1 emotion condition), two show the same emotion and one shows a different emotion (3 faces: 2 emotions), or all three show different emotions

(3 faces : 3 emotions). For each condition we can consider the amount of information under consideration to be a product of these two factors (without considering the uncertainty in the response). Consequently, for the 1:1 condition, participants must consider 1 piece of information, for the 2:1 condition, 2 pieces of information, and so on.

Notably, the 3:1 condition carries less information (3 pieces) than the 2:2 condition (4 pieces), despite being of a larger set size. The resultant levels of information corresponding to the 1:1, 2:1, 2:2, 3:1, 3:2, and 3:3 conditions are 1, 2, 4, 3, 6, and 9, respectively.

62

Set size 1:

A Aisfearful (1 face : 1 emotion)

Set size 2:

A B A B AissadbecauseBissad AissurprisedbecauseBisangry (2 faces : 1 emotion) (2 faces : 2 emotions) Set size 3:

A B C A B C AisdisgustedbecauseBisdisgusted AisfearfulbecauseBisfearful because C is disgusted because C is surprised (3 faces : 1 emotion) (3 faces : 2 emotions)

A B C AissadbecauseBisdisgustedbecauseCissurprised (3 faces : 3 emotions)

Figure 11. Sample stimuli in the Multiple Emotion Processing Task

At the beginning of each trial, participants are shown a statement describing either 63 the emotional state of an actor (1:1 condition) or the emotional states of multiple actors, dependent on conditions as described above. After 5000 ms, the photo depictions described above appear, and participants must judge whether the facial expressions shown match the statement displayed (True or False). There are 3 blocks of 108 trials each. Within each block, there are the same number of trials in each congruence condition, with equal numbers requiring True and False responses: 36 for 1:1, 18 each for

2:1 and 2:2, and 12 each for 3:1, 3:2, and 3:3. Total task duration is approximately 30 minutes.

Recursive Theory of Mind Task (R-ToM). This task is based upon the perspective-taking task developed by Stiller (2010, personal communication). In this task participants read three short stories of approximately 200 words in length (see Appendix

A for a sample story). Participants are instructed to read each story carefully as they will be asked questions about the content. There are three blocks, one for each story. At the beginning of each run, the story is displayed, along with instructions to read it carefully and remember it. Participants are instructed to push the spacebar when they are ready to proceed to the questions. Each story is followed by 20 statements, to which the participant must respond “True” or “False”. The 20 types statements correspond to the

Levels of embedding (1-5), the Information Type (Fact, Intention), and the correct response (True, False), with one of each type within a block, presented in random order.

The fact questions control for comprehension of complex sentences and short-term memory capacity. The total task duration is approximately 15 minutes.

64 Majority Function Task – Emotional (MFT-E). The emotional majority function task was designed to examine the effect of emotional information with different information values upon cognitive processing. In this task, stimuli consist of a group a three faces that appear randomly in three of eight possible locations centered around a fixation cross (Figure 12). Twenty fearful and neutral faces were selected from the

Pictures of Facial Affect (Ekman & Friesen, 1975), where 10 of the faces were of males, and 10 of females with 5 neutral, and 5 fearful faces for each gender respectively. On each trial, participants must indicate the majority gender of the faces as quickly and accurately as possible via mouse click. When participants detected a majority of male faces they clicked the left mouse button, and when the majority was female, they clicked the right mouse button.

Three independent variables are manipulated in this experiment: 1) Cognitive load

(low, high); 2) Emotional Valence (fearful, neutral); and 3) Frequency of emotional information (0%, 16%, 50%, 84%, 100%). The cognitive load manipulation was implemented in the ratio of same: other gendered faces presented on each trial. The 3:0 condition in which all faces were of the same gender was the low load condition, and the

2:1 condition with two faces of one gender and one of the other was the high load condition. Fear was selected as the emotional stimulus as it is considered to be an informative emotion which has been demonstrated to elicit a strong amygdala response

(Melcher, Born, & Gruber, 2011; Melcher, Obst, Mann, Paulus, & Gruber, 2012;

Shafritz, Bregman, Ikuta, & Szeszko, 2015). The frequency manipulation was intended to manipulate the entropy associated with the occurrence of fearful faces in a given

65 sequence (i.e., a block of trials in this case). Consistent with the discussion of entropy in

Chapter II, the 50% fearful condition with random presentation would have the highest emotional entropy in a sequence.

The task begins with fixation on a central fixation cross presented for 3000 ms.

There is a variable jittered response window between 2000-3000 ms. Each trial lasts approximately 5 seconds. Altogether, there are 2 runs of 5 blocks, corresponding to the 5 frequencies. Each block contains 24 trials presented in random order, except for the 0%, and 100% blocks, which have 12 trials each. The order of the blocks was counterbalanced with reversed repetition between each run. The total trial number in this task is 240, and the task takes approximately 22 minutes to complete.

Figure 12. Emotional MFT Stimuli

Procedure

This study was conducted at the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai

(ISMMS). Because we are interested in between-group comparisons for each task and not

66 within-subject comparisons across tasks, all participants completed the tasks in the same order. All tasks were compiled, and run using E-Prime 2.0 TM software (Psychology

Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA) with a 17” display. Participants were encouraged to take breaks between runs in each task, as well as between tasks, to control for fatigue.

Data Analysis

RT and accuracy data were collected for all tasks, and efficiency was computed as described in Chapter III. In the interest of readability, only results for analyses on efficiency will be presented in the following sections. RT and accuracy results are available for review in Appendix B. For each task, response times were recorded for accurate trials, and trials with RTs ± 3 standard deviations of the mean were excluded from further analysis. Means (±SEM) of behavioral performance were calculated and analyzed. Data were tested for normal distributions and homogeneity of variance

(Levene’s test). For within-subjects factors where Mauchley’s test indicated a violation of the assumption of sphericity, univariate analyses of variance with Greenhouse-Geisser correction were reported. For each task, mixed factorial analyses of variance (ANOVA) were conducted with the respective within-subjects factors and Group as the between- subjects factor. To investigate the potential influence of IQ on task performance, exploratory analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) were also conducted with IQ as a covariate. A significance level of .05 was selected. For statistical tests of specific, directional hypotheses, one-tailed p-values are reported, and will be indicated as applicable.

67 Non-parametric correlation analyses (Kendall’s tau) were performed to assess the relationship between efficiency on the three tasks and symptom report on, ADOS-G,

SRS, and AQ (non-continuous variables). False discovery rate correction was applied to control for Type I error. One-tailed statistical tests were utilized to test our directional predictions.

Results

CERT

Results of a 2 (Complexity) x 2 (Group) ANOVA revealed that (see Figure 13), the main effect of Complexity on efficiency was significant (F(1,28) = 139.86, p < .001,

2 ηp = .83), with more efficient performance for basic emotions (M = .60 ± .02) compared to compound emotions (M = .48 ± .02). The main effect of Group (F<1) was not significant. The Complexity by Group interaction was significant (F(1,28) = 4.45, p =

2 .04, ηp = .14), such that the extent to which efficiency decreased for compound emotions

1 Basic Compound 0.8

0.6

0.4 Effciency

0.2

0 TD ASD Group

Figure 13. CERT efficiency results

68 was greater for the TD group compared to the ASD group (Table 8). ANCOVA with IQ as a covariate did not improve the model.

Table 9

CERT efficiency, RT and accuracy (SEM) results

TD ASD Basic Compound Basic Compound Efficiency 0.61 (0.03) 0.48 (0.03) 0.58 (0.03) 0.49 (0.03) RT (ms) 1365 (59) 1453 (60) 1366 (59) 1428 (60) Accuracy (%) 80 (2) 67 (2) 78 (2) 67 (2)

MEPT

Results of a 2 (Group) x 6 (Level) ANOVA (see Figure 14) revealed that assumption of sphericity was violated for Level, (c2(14) = 59.91, p < .001, e = .46); with

2 correction the main effect was significant (F(2.3,64.9) = 56.75, p < .001, ηp = .67), such that efficiency decreased as level increased (Table 10). The main effect of Group was not

1 0.9 0.8 0.7 0.6 0.5

Effciency 0.4 0.3 0.2 TD 0.1 ASD 0 1 2 3 4 6 9 Level

Figure 14. MEPT efficiency

69 2 significant (F(1,28) = 1.26, p =.27, ηp = .04). The Level by Group interaction trended

2 toward significance (F(1,28) = 1.93, p = .09, ηp = .07). ANCOVA with IQ as a covariate did not improve the model. There were no significant group differences in efficiency slope (t(1,28) = 1.62, p = .17) or intercept (t(1,28) = -1.28, p = .21).

Table 10

MEPT efficiency, RT, accuracy (SEM) results

Information 1 2 3 4 6 9 Efficiency TD 0.51 (0.06) 0.44 (0.05) 0.41 (0.05) 0.34 (0.04) 0.31 (0.04) 0.28 (0.03) ASD 0.41 (0.06) 0.34 (0.05) 0.34 (0.05) 0.29 (0.04) 0.27 (0.04) 0.24 (0.03)

RT (ms) TD 2081 (307) 2471 (390) 3272 (662) 3249 (431) 4085 (701) 4237 (614) ASD 2393 (307) 2874 (390) 3755 (662) 3188 (431) 3886 (702) 4276 (614)

Accuracy (%) TD 82 (1) 88 (2) 95 (2) 82 (2) 87 (2) 87 (2) ASD 80 (1) 84 (2) 90 (2) 80 (2) 84 (2) 87 (2)

R-ToM

For this task the results for level 5 were not consistent with the trend across the other levels and were not considered to be valid. Consequently, only levels 1 through 4 were included in the analysis for a 2 (Group) x 4 (Level) ANOVA (see Figure 15). The assumption of sphericity was violated for Level (c2(5) = 18.25, p < .01, e = .65), and with

2 correction the main effect was significant (F(2.1, 57.4) = 91.61, p < .001, ηp = .77).

Pairwise comparisons revealed significant decreases in efficiency with each increasing

70 level of information (all ps < .001, see Table 11). The main effect of Type was significant

2 (F(1, 28) = 72.00, p < .001, ηp = .72), with less efficient performance overall for theory of mind questions (M = .14 ± .01) compared to factual questions (M = .21 ± .02). The

2 main effect of Group was not significant (F(1, 28) = 1.36, p = .25, ηp = .04). The Level

2 by Group interaction was significant (F(3, 84) = 3.30 p = .02, ηp = .11) such that the extent to which efficiency decreased as a function of Level was greater in the TD group compared to the ASD group. The Type by Group (F < 1), Level by Type (p = .28), and three-way (F < 1) interactions were not significant. ANCOVA with IQ as a covariate did not improve the model.

0.5 TD Fact ASD Fact 0.4 TD ToM ASD ToM

0.3

Effciency 0.2

0.1

0 1 2 3 4 Level

Figure 15. RToM results

Table 11

RToM efficiency, RT, and accuracy (SEM) results

Level Type of Information Fact ToM TD ASD TD ASD Efficiency 1 0.35 (0.04) 0.27 (0.04) 0.26 (0.03) 0.21 (0.03)

71 2 0.24 (0.03) 0.20 (0.03 0.18 (0.02) 0.15 (0.02) 3 0.19 (0.02) 0.18 (0.02) 0.11 (0.01) 0.09 (0.01) 4 0.13 (0.02) 0.14 (0.02) 0.05 (0.01) 0.05 (0.01)

RT (ms) 1 2959 (354) 3635 (354) 3761 (436) 4686 (436) 2 4176 (448) 4793 (448) 6139 (685) 6463 (685) 3 4748(558) 5631 (558) 8433(1024) 9281 (1024) 4 5556 (725) 6189 (725) 13198 (2112) 15111 (2112)

Accuracy (%) 1 90 (4) 82(4) 91 (4) 83 (4) 2 92 (4) 76 (4) 91 (3) 82 (3) 3 77 (5) 87 (5) 77 (5) 66 (5) 4 67 (5) 68 (5) 59 (6) 59 (6)

MFT-E

Results of a 2 (Group) x 2 (Uncertainty) x 6 (Frequency) ANOVA (see Figure 16) revealed that the main effect of Uncertainty was significant (F(1,28) = 118.81, p < .001,

2 ηp = .82), with lower efficiency in the high uncertainty condition (M = .70 ± .03) compared to the low uncertainty condition (M = .83 ± .04). The main effects of

2 2 Frequency (F(1,28) = 1.59, p = .18, ηp = .06) and Group (F(1,28) = 1.98, p = .17, ηp =

2 .07) were not significant. The Frequency by Group (F(1,28) = 1.30, p = .56, ηp = .05),

2 Frequency by Uncertainty (F<1), and Uncertainty by Group (F(1,28) = 1.92, p = .18, ηp

= .07) interactions were not significant. The Frequency by Uncertainty by Group

2 interaction was significant (F(4,104) = 2.34, p = .03, ηp = .08), such that the effect of fearful face frequency differed between the two groups (see Table 12). Specifically, within the 50% condition, which is estimated to have the highest overall entropy, the

72 effect of cognitive load on efficiency was larger in the TD group (low – high M = .18) compared to the ASD group (low – high M = 0.05). ANCOVA with IQ as a covariate did not improve the model.

Table 12

MFT-E efficiency, RT, and accuracy results

Uncertainty Frequency of Low High fearful faces (%) TD ASD TD ASD Efficiency 0 0.87 (0.06) 0.77 (0.06) 0.75 (0.04) 0.67 (0.04) 16 0.88 (0.06) 0.75 (0.06) 0.76 (0.04) 0.64 (0.04) 50 0.93 (0.06) 0.77 (0.06) 0.75 (0.07) 0.72 (0.07) 84 0.88 (0.05) 0.75 (0.05) 0.74 (0.05) 0.62 (0.05) 100 0.84 (0.05) 0.8 (0.05) 0.69 (0.03) 0.65 (0.05)

RT (ms) 0 1100 (61) 1219 (61) 1251 (63) 1347 (63) 16 1116 (71) 1234 (71) 1277 (66) 1432 (66) 50 1111 (78) 1249 (78) 1293 (82) 1365 (82) 84 1187 (67) 1265 (67) 1323 (69) 1415 (69) 100 1187 (61) 1233 (61) 1307 (64) 1393 (64)

Accuracy (%) 0 91 (2) 90 (2) 90 (3) 86 (3) 16 91 (2) 90(2) 92 (2) 89 (2) 50 95 (2) 92 (2) 91 (2) 91 (2) 84 97 (2) 94 (2) 91 (3) 87 (3) 100 95 (2) 96 (2) 95 (2) 88 (4)

73

1 0.9 0.8 0.7 0.6 0.5

Effciency 0.4

0.3 ASD Low 0.2 ASD High TD Low 0.1 TD High 0 0 16 50 84 100 Frequency of fearful faces

Figure 16. MFT-E efficiency results

Correlations between task performance and ASD symptoms

Correlation coefficients are presented in Table 13. Rate of change in efficiency across levels of the MEPT was positively associated with AQ scores such that as rate of change in performance became less negative, AQ scores increased, and as slope became steeper (more negative) EQ scores increased (higher EQ scores reflect better performance). Rate of change in efficiency across levels of mentalizing information on the RToM was positively associated with ADOS social + communication scores. RToM intercept for mentalizing information was negatively associated with ADOS social + communication and total scores. Due to the small sample size, none of these correlations survived false discovery rate correction.

Table 13

Kendall’s tau correlation coefficients between performance efficiency across tasks and measures of ASD symptoms.

74

ADOS Social + SRS AQ EQ Efficiency Total Communication Total Total Total

CERT overall -.23 -.26 -.02 -.15 .22

MEPT slope .15 .18 .16 .25* -.32* MEPT intercept -.31 -.34 -.05 -.16 .22

RToM mentalizing slope .42* .34 -.11 -.05 .08 RToM mentalizing intercept -.45* -.36* .08 .02 -.04

MFT-E 50% fearful faces -.12 -.21 .02 -.07 .17

Note: * = p < .05. None of the correlations survived false discovery rate correction

Discussion

We aimed to demonstrate that individuals with ASD would show deficits in affective cognitive control under uncertainty requiring processing of complex emotional information and recursive theory of mind inferences, and that the ASD group would be more susceptible to cognitive control interference from emotional information. Using an information theory approach, we designed several novel tasks to allow for systematic manipulations of uncertainty in emotional and social contexts. We predicted that individuals with ASD would be less efficient than typically developing controls in emotion processing and mentalizing under increasingly uncertain conditions, and that these deficits would be correlated with ASD symptom report.

Across the various tasks in this study, we did not find the expected interaction effect between group performance and uncertainty. Instead, we found that it was the TD group’s performance that decreased to a greater extent under conditions of increasing

75 uncertainty. Notably, the ASD group efficiency performance tracked the TD group performance very closely but slightly (not always significantly) lower across levels of uncertainty before converging with TD performance at the most demanding levels.

Furthermore, performance on these tasks was associated with social and communication deficits in the ASD group. While this pattern of results did not support our hypothesis, it closely resembled the pattern seen in the earlier cognitive control results (see General

Discussion for elaboration on this), where there were slight differences at baseline that decreased as complexity increased (Mackie & Fan, 2015).

We found the cognitive control performance of the ASD group was less susceptible to interference by emotional information compared to the TD group. This is contrary to previous reports of increased interference in cognitive control performance by emotional information in ASD (Dichter & Belger, 2007). One possible explanation is that emotional stimuli do not hold the same information value for individuals with ASD as it does for neurotypical controls. Further supporting this idea, there appeared to be no difference in performance dependent on valence of information in the ASD group. For example, under the high load condition on the affective MFT, while TD performance decreased as frequency of fearful faces increased, ASD group performance was similar when the stimuli were all neutral compared to all fearful, and similar performance was also found when fear was low frequency compared to when neutral was low frequency.

Nonetheless, the ASD group still used the emotional information to their advantage, as a facilitation effect of emotion was found in the 100% fearful condition. Compared to the

76 corresponding decrease in performance found in the TD group, this suggests differential effects of the presence of fearful information on cognitive control performance.

The ASD group performance relative to the TD group can again be considered in terms of the hypothetical efficiency performance models described in Chapter III (Figure

8). The results support Model B, in which there is a slight (but not always significant) group difference at baseline, and this difference decreases as a function of complexity.

While unexpected, these patterns of results reliably demonstrate at the very least that cognitive control in the ASD group was not more susceptible to interference from emotional information than the TD group on these tasks.

These results are consistent with other studies that failed to find group differences in performance on tasks of emotion processing or emotion-cognition interactions

(Adolphs et al., 2001; de Vries & Geurts, 2012; Fridenson-Hayo et al., 2016; Gedek,

Pantelis, & Kennedy, 2016; Golan et al., 2006; Hubert, Wicker, Monfardini, & Deruelle,

2009; Louwerse et al., 2014). Overall, it appears that there is no clear evidence of a frank

ASD deficit in affective cognitive control across a wider range of uncertainty values than is typically used to investigate this phenomenon.

Limitations and alternative explanations

The major limitation of this study was the limited sample size and associated power limitations. While within-subjects manipulations and associated power were quite strong, between-subjects effects were difficult to detect. In some cases it was clear that group differences would not be significant even with increased power (e.g., CERT), but on other tasks (e.g., MFT-E, RToM), it is likely that increased power would have resulted

77 in significant group differences and interactions. Due to limitations in our access to this population of high functioning adults with ASD with minimal comorbidities, increasing the sample size was not feasible.

Given that our sample was relatively high-functioning, these results may not generalize to the broader ASD population as a whole, particularly those with intellectual disability (Brown, Chouinard, & Crewther, 2017) and other neurodevelopmental disorders. It remains an open research question whether we would have found the expected group by uncertainty interaction effect with lower-functioning samples.

Although we attempted to increase ecological validity by incorporating facial picture stimuli that represented a wide range of actors displaying a wider range of emotions than is typically displayed in tasks that use emotional stimuli, the ecological validity of our tasks remained sub-optimal. The primary concern is that the stimuli were presented only to one sensory modality, whereas it has been suggested that difficulties with integration of information coming from multiple modalities may be responsible for social interaction deficits in naturalistic settings (Beaumont & Newcombe, 2006;

Collignon et al., 2013), although there is other evidence that sensory modality does not impact group differences (Gedek et al., 2016). It has been argued that the social impairment in ASD may not be detectable on tasks that are not ecologically valid in the way that they simulate the experience of an autistic individual in a highly stimulating environment with need for rapid information processing (Barak & Feng, 2016; Wallace et al., 2016; Williams, Mazefsky, Walker, Minshew, & Goldstein, 2014). For the purpose of this study we chose to prioritize internal over ecological validity. Given that we did not

78 find frank deficits in processing of emotional information, using more ecologically valid tasks requiring multisensory integration remains a future direction for study.

An alternative explanation to the present results is that although group differences in performance were unclear, qualitative differences in the ways that the information was processed might still exist. That is, the groups processed the information differently, but arrived at comparable behavioral performance. Support for this possibility comes from previous studies that failed to show group differences on behavioral measures, but found qualitative (Adolphs et al., 2001; Dalton et al., 2005), physiological (Gu et al., 2015;

Hubert et al., 2009), or neural processing (Duerden et al., 2013; Fan et al., 2012; Gu et al., 2015) differences between groups. These types of qualitative differences may be aided in part by the development of compensatory mechanisms in the course of development. Our sample was recruited in large part through the Seaver Autism Center, where participants had received an evaluation and/or services at some point in their development, such as social skills training. It is possible that exposure to these services may have contributed to a normalization of performance in adulthood (Adolphs et al.,

2001). Consequently, it is unclear whether these results would generalize to others without exposure to such services.

As mentioned in Chapter II, successful social interaction relies on a series of several information processing steps that facilitate the selection of a suitable response, including encoding and interpretation of cues, setting goals for the situation, response construction and selection, and enactment of chosen response (Crick & Dodge, 1994;

79 Ziv, Hadad, & Khateeb, 2013). Each of these steps is guided by a mental database for social behavior that includes memories of past social situations, rules for behavior, etc.

We did not capture explicit difficulty in the ASD group in encoding of social cues or interpretation of cues, and our data suggest that behavioral responses are overall generated just as efficiently as typically developing controls. However, examining the full range of processes involved in social interaction was beyond the scope of this study, and there is still the possibility of differential difficulty among the various steps even as development of some stages (i.e., encoding and interpretation) begin to approximate an age-appropriate level (Mazza et al., 2017). The presence of social deficits in ASD is well- documented, and a previous study has demonstrated deficits at every stage of these processes in preschoolers with ASD (Ziv et al., 2013), but differences in older children with ASD were less clear (Flood, Julian Hare, & Wallis, 2011), supporting the idea that social information processing development occurs at a slower rate in the ASD population.

Conclusion

This study aimed to investigate efficiency of cognitive control for emotional information and the relationship between emotional control deficits and the symptom presentation of ASD. We found that participants with ASD were not significantly less efficient than typically developing controls in employing cognitive control in the context of emotional information. Of three possible explanations of group differences in performance, the model in which the ASD group is less efficient than controls at baseline and group performances converge as they decline as a function of complexity. These findings suggest that there is not a frank deficit in affective cognitive control in adults

80 with ASD, especially when processing demands are high (DiCriscio et al., 2016). While this may appear counterintuitive, it is not at odds with an information theory approach to cognitive control, as outlined in Chapter V (General Discussion).

81 Chapter V

General Discussion

In this study, we aimed to investigate the efficiency and capacity of cognitive control in ASD relative to TD controls. We employed tasks that manipulated uncertainty in cognitive and affective domains, and examined the relationships between behavioral performance and ASD symptom report. We hypothesized that the ASD group would show reduced efficiency and capacity of cognitive control relative to TD controls, and have greater difficulty than TD controls with cognitive control as uncertainty increased.

We expected that these deficits would be negatively correlated with ASD symptom report

(i.e., lower performance associated with higher symptom report).

General cognitive control deficits in ASD

The present results support the hypothesis that high-functioning adults with ASD demonstrate lower efficiency and capacity for cognitive control under uncertainty. That is, the ASD group showed less efficient performance at baseline and had a lower capacity for rapid implementation of cognitive control. However, we did not find evidence that this deficit in cognitive control increased with increasing uncertainty. Rather, ASD performance either converged toward or stayed relatively parallel to the performance of the TD group. Performance on various tasks of cognitive control was negatively associated with ASD symptoms of social/communication deficits and restricted interest/repetitive behaviors.

82 Although counterintuitive, these results are not inconsistent with an information theory approach to cognitive control. It has been previously demonstrated that there is a linear increase in activation of the cognitive control network (notably in ACC and AI) and corresponding deactivation of the default mode network as a function of computational load measured in entropy (Fan et al., 2014). This increase in CCN activation is thought to reflect an increase in neural computation, while the deactivation in other regions represents the suppression of task-unrelated regions (e.g., DMN regions).

Previous studies have shown deactivation of ACC and AI under lower entropy conditions, which suggests that activation of these regions is relative to state uncertainty

(Fan et al., 2014). That is, if the uncertainty associated with the anticipation of the stimulus (e.g., during the fixation period) is higher than the task-related uncertainty, this will be reflected in a deactivation of these regions during task-related periods. Higher state uncertainty relative to task demands can occur when uncertainty is manipulated across a wide range of values, and participants learn to anticipate difficult trials.

Activation of ACC and AI is considered to be involved in dynamic switching between state uncertainty and task-related responses (Goulden et al., 2014). Participants with ASD have previously demonstrated an absence/hypoactivation of ACC under conditions of uncertainty that was associated with behavioral performance (Fan et al.,

2012). Together with the present results, it appears that the initial separation of performance between the groups reflects higher state uncertainty at baseline in the ASD group and/or a higher threshold for activation of the CCN relative to state uncertainty.

The activation threshold in healthy controls was previously estimated at 3.3 bits for

83 entropy. In our tasks, group performance converged on average at approximately 4 bits.

Furthermore, while efficient switching between the cognitive control and default mode networks has been found to be related to efficient behavioral performance, failure to deactivate DMN under uncertainty has also been demonstrated in this population (Fan et al., 2012; Spencer et al., 2012). Converging evidence therefore supports the idea that the efficiency of implementation of cognitive control under conditions of uncertainty lags behind that of TD controls. However, once the difference between state- and task-related uncertainty becomes positive, implementation of cognitive control under uncertainty resembles that of TD, presumably via appropriate engagement of CCN and deactivation of DMN. A similar argument can be applied to our findings in the affective domain, as described below.

Affective cognitive control deficits

In the affective domain, we did not find support for the hypothesis that the ASD group would show greater deficits in affective cognitive control as uncertainty increased.

Rather, the ASD group appeared to be less affected than the TD group by the presence/complexity of affective information, and emotional valence was non- contributory to performance in the ASD group. The pattern of results was remarkably similar to that in the cognitive domain, and therefore cannot be considered to be random

“noise”.

Given the observed similar patterns of performance in the cognitive and affective domains, speculation about the possible neural mechanisms is warranted. In addition to their role in rapid information processing under uncertainty, the ACC and AI also have a

84 role in rapid processing and control of emotional information (Bush, Luu, & Posner,

2000; Carter et al., 1998; Craig, 2009; Critchley, Wiens, Rotshtein, Öhman, & Dolan,

2004; Fan et al., 2011). Furthermore, the ACC and AI have been demonstrated to have reciprocal connections with amygdala to regulate responses to emotional information

(Bush et al., 2000; Ochsner & Gross, 2005; Wager, Davidson, Hughes, Lindquist, &

Ochsner, 2008). Given these overlaps in neural circuitry for processing of cognitive and affective information, when both types of information are simultaneously present, they should compete for neural representation (Fan et al., 2011; Gu, Liu, Van Dam, Hof, &

Fan, 2012; Padmala, Bauer, & Pessoa, 2011; L Pessoa, McKenna, Gutierrez, &

Ungerleider, 2002; Luiz Pessoa, 2008). It has been demonstrated that stimulus valence interferes with cognitive control, and this may vary based on cognitive load with interference under high load and facilitation under low uncertainty (Gu et al., 2012).

These results were replicated in our TD group, but the ASD group showed a different pattern of results, with no significant effect of emotional valence on cognitive control performance.

Two of our tasks of affective cognitive control (CERT and MFT-E) included distracting affective information that needed to be effectively filtered out for efficient task performance, while the other two tasks (MEPT and RToM) systematically increased the amount of affective to information to be processed, and placed increasing demands on the “cognitive” system of working memory. CERT performance confirmed that there is no explicit behavioral deficit in emotion recognition in ASD, consistent with other studies that failed to find group differences, even for other manipulations of complex

85 emotions (Fridenson-Hayo et al., 2016). However, on the tasks that manipulated amount of information to be processed a pattern similar to that on the cognitive tasks with a wide range of uncertainty was seen. Particularly for tasks with the widest range of affective uncertainty, the level of anticipatory state uncertainty is greatest, and performance efficiency looks closest to the TD group at the highest levels of uncertainty.

While previous studies have demonstrated a negative effect of emotional/social information on cognitive control in ASD (de Vries & Geurts, 2012; Dichter & Belger,

2007), we saw on the affective MFT that the ASD group was actually less susceptible to interference from emotional stimuli. Furthermore, the valence of the distracting emotional stimuli did not seem to matter to the ASD group, suggesting that they did not attend to negatively valenced stimuli (i.e., fear) in the same way as the TD group. In TD individuals, susceptibility to interference from distracting emotional information depends on the efficiency of cognitive control to prioritize task-relevant information over distractors (Mackie & Fan, 2015; Mackie et al., 2013; Mitchell et al., 2007; Mitchell,

Richell, Leonard, & Blair, 2006). However, emotionally salient stimuli can capture attention resulting in stronger competition for representation (Davis, 2005; Fan et al.,

2011; Gasquoine, 2014; Mitchell et al., 2007, 2006). This was not the case for the ASD group.

Many studies have described cognitive control, emotional processing, and mentalizing deficits in children and adolescents with ASD (review in Chapter II).

However, the present results suggest that these deficits are not present in high- functioning adults with ASD. Their performance approaches normal, particularly as task

86 demands increase and cognitive control is engaged to a greater extent. This suggests either that individuals with ASD learn compensatory strategies over time that help to normalize their behavioral performance (even if achieved by alternate processing routes)

(Hastings, 2003) or that there is simply a lag in the development of social and communication skills in ASD that begins to close in adolescence/early adulthood. Even in low-functioning groups, social behavior increases with social contact/imitation of others (Field, Sanders, & Nadel, 2001)

Conclusions

The present results support the idea that individuals with ASD have a lower capacity for cognitive control compared to TD individuals, but this does not necessarily mean impaired cognitive control performance across the board. There appears to be a lag in the implementation of cognitive control in the ASD group, such that a higher threshold of uncertainty is needed to trigger an adequate degree of control over information processing. The absence of frank deficits in affective cognitive control in this group stands in contrast with findings of clear patterns of deficits in children, suggesting that these processes are delayed in ASD, but come online later in development, at least in high functioning cohorts.

Overall, our data support a model of performance in which individuals with ASD are slightly less efficient in cognitive control at baseline, presumably due to inefficiency in engaging the CCN for processing of task-related uncertainty. However, as uncertainty increases, the CCN becomes more engaged and performance converges with that of TD controls. This information processing style is related (and likely contributes) to ASD

87 social/communication and restricted interests/repetitive behavior symptoms. Future studies may examine the processes that mediate the normalization of cognitive and affective control in high functioning adults with ASD.

88 Appendix A

Recursive Theory of Mind Task Sample Stimuli

MARK and LUKE

Mark and Luke are non-identical 13-year-old twins. Their mother has given them their allowance for the week. Every week they each receive a $5 note. However, this week their mother only has a $10 note and has left it up to them to split the money. Mark wants a CD that costs $9, and Luke needs to buy a book for his school project that costs $5.

Mark wants to go into town and spend his half of the money. Luke wants to stay at home and work but is afraid that Mark will spend the money on the CD. Luke suggests that

Mark should go into town to the bookshop first and buy the book for him, and then keep the change to spend on himself. Mark leaves for the town and Luke worries that Mark might not buy the book at all. After 20 minutes, Luke decides to call Mark on his cell phone to remind him. Mark tells Luke not to worry, he wrote the name of the book down so he won’t buy the wrong one.

Statements: Mark and Luke (T and F after statements indicate correct response.

Number in parentheses indicates level of information; 'm' indicates 'memory' a.k.a. 'fact' statement, all others are theory of mind statements).

1) Luke wants to spend $5 on a book. T (2m) T/F

2) Mark believes that Luke thinks he has bought a CD. F (2) T/F

3) Luke needs a CD. F (1m) T/F

89 4) Luke thinks that Mark understands that Luke believes that Mark thinks that Luke knows he has spent the money. F (5) T/F

5) Mark goes to town to buy a book that costs $5. T (3m) T/F

6) Luke phones Mark to see if he has spent the $5 note on a book. F (3m) T/F

7) They have a $10 note to take to town to buy a book for $5. T (4m) T/F

8) Mark thinks Luke might keep the money. F (1) T/F

9) Luke phones to see if the $5 note has been spent, in town, in a bookshop, on a book for his school project. F (5m) T/F

10) Mark has gone to town, to go to a bookshop, to buy a book for Luke and his School

Project. T (5m) T/F

11) Luke thinks Mark might not buy the book. T (1) T/F

12) Mark has a $5 note and wants to go to town. F (2m) T/F

13) Mark believes that Luke thinks that Mark does not know that Luke believes that they should share the money. F (4) T/F

14) Luke thinks that Mark wants to keep the money. T (2) T/F

15) They have a $10 note to buy a CD, in town that costs $5. F (4m) T/F

16) Luke thinks that Mark understands that Luke wants a book for a project. T (3) T/F

17) Mark understands that Luke believes that Mark thinks the money should be spent on a CD. F (3) T/F

18) Mark believes that Luke thinks that Mark knows that Luke believes that he cannot remember the name of the book. T (4) T/F

90 19) Mark thinks that Luke believes that Mark knows that Luke thinks that Mark does not know the name of the book. T (5) T/F

20) Mark goes to town. T (1m) T/F

91

Appendix B

Response Time and Accuracy Results

Sequential information processing: Results of EVT

RT

Entropy. For RT, the main effect of Group was significant (F(1,27) = 4.58, p <

2 .05, ηp = .15), and revealed that the ASD group (M = 485±75 ms) was slower to respond than the TD group (M = 442±43 ms) (Figure 17a). The assumption of sphericity was violated for entropy, c2(2) = 32.43, p < .05, e = .58, and with Greenhouse-Geisser correction there was a significant main effect of entropy (F(1.17, 31.53) = 17.69, p <

2 0.001, ηp = .40). Pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni correction revealed that RTs for

H = 1 (M = 497±64 ms) were significantly longer than for the H = 0.54 (M = 466±62 ms) and H = 0 (M = 429±70 ms) conditions (ps < .01, respectively). There was no significant difference between the H = 0 and H = 0.54 conditions (p = .29). The Group by entropy interaction was not significant for RT (F < 1). RT intercept was significantly higher ASD group (M= 452±80 ms, R2= .87) than for the TD group (M = 404±43 ms, R2= .96, t(27) =

1.99, p < .05, one-tailed). There was no significant difference in RT slope between groups (t(27) = -1.08, p = .29).

Surprise. The main effect of Group was trending toward significance (F(1,27) =

2 3.91, p = .06, ηp = .13), with slower performance in the ASD group (M = 501±88 ms) than the TD group (M = 455±57 ms) (Figure 17b). For surprise, sphericity had been violated, c2(5) = 32.23, p < .001, e = .56, and with correction there was a significant main

92 2 effect (F(1.67, 45.17) = 119.35, p < .001, ηp = .82). Pairwise comparisons with

Bonferroni corrections revealed significant increases in RT for each increasing value of surprise (429±70 ms, 457±61 ms, 497 ±64 ms, and 532±77 ms for I = 0, 0.19, 1, and 3, respectively, all ps < .001). The Group by surprise interaction was not significant for RT

(F<1). There was a higher accuracy intercept for the TD group (M = 100±1%, R2= .88) than the ASD group (M = 99±2%, R2= .83, t(27) = -1.79, p < .05, one- tailed). There were no significant group differences in RT slope (t(27) = 0.84, p = .41).

Accuracy

2 Entropy. The main effect of Group was significant (F(1,27) = 4.10, p = .05, ηp =

.13), with TD group (M = 96±3%) performing more accurately overall than those in the

ASD group (M = 95±3%) (Figure 17c). For entropy, sphericity was not violated (e = .93),

2 and there was a significant main effect (F(2,54) = 24.32, p < .001, ηp = .47). Pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni correction demonstrated that accuracy was significantly higher for H = 0 (M = 99±1%) compared to H = 0.54 (M = 97±2%, p < .01), and H = 1,

(M = 96±3%, p < .01). There was no significant difference between the H = 0.54 and H =

1 conditions (p = .13). The Group by entropy interaction was not significant (F < 1).

Accuracy intercept was significantly lower for the ASD group (M= 98±1%, R2= .71) than for the TD group (M = 99±1%, R2= .67, t(27) = -2.47, p < .01 one-tailed). There was no significant difference in accuracy slope between groups (t(27) = -0.85, p = .40).

93

a 600 b 600 550 550 500 500 450 450 400 400 350 350 300 300 RT(ms) RT(ms) 250 250 200 200 150 150 100 100 TD TD 50 ASD 50 ASD 0 0 0 0.54 1 00.19 1 3 Entropy (bits) Surprise (bits)

c 100 d 100 90 90 80 80 70 70 60 60 50 50 40 40 Accuracy (%) 30 Accuracy (%) 30 20 20 TD TD 10 10 ASD ASD 0 0 0 0.54 1 00.19 1 3 Entropy (bits) Surprise (bits)

Figure 17. EVT RT (a, b) and accuracy (c, d) results. Surprise. There was no significant group difference for overall accuracy (F(1,27)

= 1.30, p = .26; Figure 17d). For surprise, the assumption of sphericity was violated, c2(5) = 112.05, p < .001, e = .37, and with correction there was a significant main effect

2 (F(1.17, 30.12) = 32.53, p < .001, ηp = .55). Pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni corrections revealed significant decreases in accuracy for each increasing value of surprise from I = 0.19 up (Means: 98±2%, 96±3%, and 86±11% for I = 0.19, 1, and 3 respectively, ps < .05, respectively) but not from I = 0 to I = 0.19 (p = .87). The Group by surprise interaction was not significant (F < 1). The TD group had a significantly

94 higher accuracy intercept (M = 100±1%, R2= .88) than the ASD group (M = 99±2%, R2=

.83, t(27) = -1.85, p < .05, one-tailed). There were no significant group differences in accuracy slope (t(27) = -0.15, p = .88).

Cognitive control for non-sequential stimuli: Results of the MFT

RT. There was a significant main effect of Group (see Figure 18a), F(1,28) =

2 8.87, p < .01, ηp = .24, indicating that the ASD group (M = 1172±106 ms) had longer

RTs than the TD group (M = 1066±88 ms). For entropy, the assumption of sphericity had been violated, c2(14) = 54.53, p < .001, e = .56, and with correction, there was a significant main effect of entropy, F(2.8, 78.9) = 683.88 , p < .001, such that RT increased with increasing information. There was no significant Group by entropy interaction (F(2.8, 78.9) = 1.40, p = .25). There was no significant difference between the groups in RT slope (t(28) = -0.41, p = .68), but RT intercept was significantly higher for the ASD group (M = 650±126 ms, R2 = .95) than for the TD group (M = 533±98 ms, R2 =

.94, t(28) = 2.85, p < .01, one-tailed).

Accuracy. There was a significant main effect of Group (see Figure 18b), F(1,28)

2 = 8.11, p < 0.01, ηp = .23, indicating that the ASD group (M = 90±5%) was significantly less accurate than the TD group (M = 94±3%). The assumption of sphericity had been violated for the entropy factor, c2(14) = 148.60, p < 0.001, therefore univariate ANOVA with Greenhouse-Geisser corrections are reported (e = .32). There was a significant main

2 effect of the entropy on accuracy, (F(1.6, 45.1) = 173.90 , p < 0.001, ηp = .86), such that accuracy decreased with increasing information. The Group by entropy interaction was

2 marginally significant, F(1.6, 45.1) = 3.32 , p = .06, ηp = .11. There was a significant

95 difference in accuracy slope between the groups (t(28) = -2.01, p < .05, one-tailed), indicating that accuracy decreased to a greater extent for the ASD group (M = -.06±.02,

R2 = .76) than for the TD group (M = -.05±.02, R2 = .65), with increasing information load. There was no significant group difference for accuracy intercept (t(28) = -0.971, p =

.34).

a 1800 b 100 1600 90 1400 80 70 1200 60 1000 50 800 RT(ms) 40

600 Accuracy (%) 30 400 20 TD TD 200 10 ASD ASD 0 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 1 2 3 4 5 6 Entropy (bits) Entropy (bits)

Figure 18. MFT (a) RT and (b) accuracy results. Cognitive control capacity under time constraints: Results of the DCT and MFT-M

DCT

RT. Figure 19a shows RT performance as a function of computational load. The main effect of Group was not significant (F < 1). Sphericity was violated for entropy c2(9) = 81.47, p < .001, e = .50, and with Greenhouse-Geisser correction there was a

2 significant main effect (F(2.01,56.22) = 233.17, p < .001, ηp = .89). Pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni correction indicated significant increases in RT with each increasing value of entropy (all ps < .001). The interaction between Group and Load was

96 2 not significant (F(2.01,56.22) = 1.96, p = .15, ηp = .07). There were no significant group differences for slope (t(28) = -0.29, p = 0.39) or intercept t(28) = 0.37, p = 0.36).

Accuracy. Figure 19b shows accuracy performance as a function of computational load (entropy). The main effect of Group was significant (F(1,28) = 11.81,

2 p < .01, ηp = .30), with less accurate performance from the ASD group (M = 81±11%) than the TD group (M = 95±11%). Sphericity was violated for entropy c2(9) = 79.44, p <

.001, e = .52, and with Greenhouse-Geisser correction there was a significant main effect

2 (F(2.07,57.84) = 26.32, p < .001, ηp = .49). Pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni correction indicated that there were significant decreases in accuracy from the 2-bit to 3- bit condition (p < .01), the 4-bit to 5-bit condition (p < .01), and the 5-bit to 6-bit conditions (p < .001), but not from 3 bits to 4 bits (p = .37). The interaction between

2 Group and entropy was significant (F(2.07,57.84) = 7.01, p < .01, ηp = .20), with a greater decrease in accuracy in the ASD group compared to the TD group, as

b a 1400 100 90 1200 80 1000 70

800 60 50

RT(ms) 600 40 Accuracy (%) 400 30 20 200 TD TD 10 ASD ASD 0 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 1 2 3 4 5 6 Task 2 entropy (bits) Task 2 entropy (bits)

Figure 19. DCT RT and ACC on Task 2

97 computational load increased. The ASD group (M = -0.07±0.06) had a significantly greater accuracy slope than the TD group (M = -0.03±0.02), t(28) = -2.75, p < .01. There were no significant group differences in intercept (t(28) = 0.93, p = .18).

MFT-M. RT. Results are presented in Figure 20. For ET, the assumption of sphericity was violated (c2(5) = 16.68, p < .01, e = .67), and with correction the main

2 effect on RT was significant (F(2.01, 48.12) = 67.12, p < .001, ηp = .74). Pairwise comparisons revealed significant increases in RT with each increase in ET (all ps < .001).

Sphericity was violated for Entropy (c2(2) = 18.41, p < .001, e = .65), and with correction

2 the main effect on RT was significant (F(1.29, 30.95) = 184.36, p < .001, ηp = .88).

a b 1400 1400 3:2 1200 4:1 1200 5:0

1000 1000 RT (ms) 800 RT (ms) 800

600 600

400 400 250 500 1000 2000 250 500 1000 2000 SOA (ms) SOA (ms) TD ASD

Figure 20. MFT-M RT Results Pairwise comparisons revealed significant increases in RT with each increase in entropy

(all ps < .001). Sphericity was violated for the ET by Entropy interaction (c2(20) =

63.20, p < .001, e = .55), and it was significant with correction (F(3.32, 79.96) = 47.06, p

2 < .001, ηp = .66), such that the extent to which RTs increased with entropy was greater with each increase in ET. The main effect of Group was not significant (F < 1). The ET

98 2 by Group (F(3,72) = 1.32, p = .14, ηp = .05), Entropy by Group (F < 1), and ET by

Entropy by Group (F < 1) interactions were not significant.

CERT

For RT (See Figure 21 a & b), there was a main effect of Complexity (F(1,28) =

2 24.15, p < .001, ηp = .46), such that response times were longer on average in the compound emotion condition (M ± SEM =1441 ± 42 ms) compared to the basic emotion condition (M = 1365 ± 42 ms). The main effect of Group was not significant (F < 1).

For accuracy (Figure 21c & d) the main effect of Complexity was significant

2 (F(1,28) = 151.88, p < .001, ηp = .84), with significantly lower accuracy in the compound emotion condition (M ± SEM = 67 ± 1%) compared to the basic emotion condition (M = 79 ± 1%). The main effect of Group (F<1) was not significant. The

Complexity by Group interaction trended toward significance (F(1,28) = 2.42, p = .09,

2 ηp = .10).

a b 2000 Basic 100 Basic Compound Compound

80 1500

60 1000

RT (ms) 40 Accuracy (%) 500 20

0 0 TD ASD TD ASD Group Group

Figure 21. CERT RT and accuracy results

99 MEPT

In terms of RT (Figure 22a), for the main effect of Level, the assumption of sphericity was violated (c2(14) = 97.81, p < .001 e = .48), and Greenhouse-Geisser

2 correction was applied (F(2.4,68.1) = 18.06, p < .001, ηp = .39), with longer RTs at higher levels of emotional information processing. The main effect of Group was not significant (F<1). The Level by Group interaction was not significant (F<1).

a 5000 b 100 4500 90 4000 80 3500 70 3000 60 2500 50

RT(ms) 2000 40 1500 Accuracy (%) 30 1000 20 TD TD 500 10 ASD ASD 0 0 1 2 3 4 6 9 1 2 3 4 6 9 Level Level

Figure 22. MEPT RT and accuracy For accuracy (Figure 22b), the assumption of sphericity was violated for Level

(c2(14) = 31.83, p < .01, e = .69); with correction the main effect was significant

2 (F(3.4,96.2) = 18.01, p < .001, ηp = .39), with lower accuracy at higher levels of emotional information processing. The main effect of Group trended toward significance

2 (F(1,28) = 2.51, p = .06, ηp = .08, one-tailed). The Level by Group interaction was not significant (F<1).

RToM

100 In terms of RT, sphericity was violated for Level (c2(5) = 47.29, p < .001, e =

.49), and with correction the main effect was significant (F(1.5, 41.4) = 46.36, p < .001,

2 ηp = .63). Pairwise comparisons revealed significant increases in RT with each increase in Level (all ps < .001, see Figure 23a). The main effect of Type of information was

2 significant (F(1, 28) = 74.80, p < .001, ηp = .72), with significantly longer RT for theory of mind questions (M = 8244 ± 648 ms) compared to fact questions (M = 4669 ± 321 ms). The main effect of Group was not significant (F<1). Sphericity was also violated for

Level by Type of information (c2(5) = 48.77, p < .001, e = .51), and with correction the

2 interaction effect was significant (F(1.5, 41.4) = 21.12, p < .001, ηp = .44), such that as

Level increased, the RT difference between the two information types also increased. The

Level by Group, Type by Group, and the three-way interaction were not significant (all

Fs < 1).

a b 18000 100 TD Fact 16000 ASD Fact 90 14000 TD ToM 80 ASD ToM 12000 70 60 10000

RT 50 8000 40

6000 Accuracy(%) 30 TD Fact 4000 20 ASD Fact TD ToM 2000 10 ASD ToM 0 0 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 Level Level

Figure 23. RToM RT and accuracy results

101 For accuracy, the main effect of Level was significant (F(3, 84) = 29.30, p < .001,

2 ηp = .50), such that accuracy decreased as information increased (Figure 23b). Accuracy decreased significantly for each increasing level of information (all ps < .05). The main

2 effect of Type was significant (F(1, 28) = 3.40, p = .04, ηp = .26, one-tailed), with lower accuracy for theory of mind questions (M = 75 ± 2 %) compared to factual questions (M

= 81 ± 2 %). The main effect of Group trended toward significance (F(1, 28) = 2.50, p =

2 .07, ηp = .08), with lower accuracy in the ASD group (M = 75 ± 2 %) compared to the

TD group (M = 81 ± 2 %). The Level by Type interaction was significant (F(1, 28) =

2 3.33, p = .02, ηp = .10), with lower accuracy for theory of mind questions as the amount

2 of information increased. The Level by Group (F(3,84) = 2.38, p = .08, ηp = .08) and

2 Level by Type by Group (F(3,84) = 2.77, p = .06, ηp = .09) interactions trended toward significance. The Type x Group (F<1) interaction was not significant.

MFT-E

For RT (Figure 24), The main effect of Frequency was significant (F(4,112) =

2 3.46, p = .01, ηp = .11), with longer response times associated with higher frequency of fearful faces. The main effect of Uncertainty was also significant (F(4,28) = 155.45, p <

2 .001, ηp = .85) with longer response times with higher uncertainty. The main effect of

2 Group was not significant (F(1,28) = 1.15, p = .29, ηp = .04). The Group by Frequency

(F<1), Group by Uncertainty (F<1), Frequency by Uncertainty (F<1), and the Frequency

2 by Uncertainty by Group (F(4,112) = 1.15, p = .26, ηp = .05) interactions were not significant.

102 In terms of accuracy, the main effect of Frequency was not significant (F(4,112) =

2 1.44, p = .23, ηp = .05). The main effect of Uncertainty was significant (F(1,28) = 16.71,

2 p < .001, ηp = .39), with lower accuracy in the high uncertainty compared to the low uncertainty condition. The main effect of Group was not significant (F<1). The

2 Frequency by Uncertainty interaction was significant (F(4,104) = 4.48, p = .001, ηp =

.15), such that the effect of uncertainty increased as frequency of fearful faces increased.

The Frequency by Group (F<1), Uncertainty by Group (F<1), and three-way (F<1) interactions were not significant.

a 1600 b 100 1400 90 80 1200 70 1000 60 800 50

RT (ms) 600 40 Accuracy(%) 30 400 ASD Low ASD Low ASD High 20 ASD High TD Low TD Low 200 10 TD High TD High 0 0 0 16 50 84 100 0 16 50 84 100 Frequency of fearful faces Frequency of fearful faces

Figure 24. MFT-E results

103 References

Adolphs, R., Sears, L., & Piven, J. (2001). Abnormal processing of social information

from faces in autism. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(2), 232–240.

Allman, J. M., Watson, K. K., Tetreault, N. a, & Hakeem, A. Y. (2005). Intuition and

autism: a possible role for Von Economo neurons. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,

9(8), 367–73. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.06.008

Ambery, F. Z., Russell, A. J., Perry, K., Morris, R., & Murphy, D. G. M. (2006).

Neuropsychological functioning in adults with . Autism : The

International Journal of Research and Practice, 10(6), 551–564.

http://doi.org/10.1177/1362361306068507

Anderson, D. K., Liang, J. W., & Lord, C. (2013). Predicting young adult outcome

among more and less cognitively able individuals with autism spectrum disorders.

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 55(5), 485–

494. http://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12178

Association, A. P. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-

IV-TR. Washington, DC: Author.

Association, A. P. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th

ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Badre, D. (2008). Cognitive control, hierarchy, and the rostro-caudal organization of the

frontal lobes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(5), 193–200.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.004

104 Baio, J. (2014). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years -

Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 sites, United States,

2010. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Morbidity and Mortality Weekly

Report, 63(2), 1–21. Retrieved from

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24670961

Bal, V. H., Kim, S.-H., Cheong, D., & Lord, C. (2015). Daily living skills in individuals

with autism spectrum disorder from 2 to 21 years of age. Autism,

1362361315575840.

Barak, B., & Feng, G. (2016). Neurobiology of social behavior abnormalities in autism

and Williams syndrome. Nature Neuroscience, 19(6), 647–655.

http://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4276

Barnard-Brak, L. (2011). The difference between autism and ADHD is in the eye of the

cognitive task? Personality and Individual Differences, 50(8), 1305–1308.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.02.007

Baron-cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child ’ s theory of mind : A case of specific

developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied

Disciplines, 30(2), 285–297. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1989.tb00241.x

Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., Ashwin, C., Tavassoli, T., & Chakrabarti, B. (2009).

Talent in autism: hyper-systemizing, hyper-attention to detail and sensory

hypersensitivity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series

B, Biological Sciences, 364(1522), 1377–1383.

105 http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0337

Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced

test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or

asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied

Disciplines, 38(7), 813–822. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01599.x

Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory

of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46.

Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The

Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger Syndrome/High-

Functioning Autism, Males and Females, Scientists and Mathematicians. Journal of

Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 5–17.

http://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005653411471

Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Spong, A., Scahill, V., & Lawson, J. (2001). Studies

of theory of mind: Are intuitive physics and intuitive psychology independent? The

Journal of Developmental and Learning Disorders.

Beaumont, R., & Newcombe, P. (2006). Theory of mind and central coherence in adults

with high-functioning autism or Asperger syndrome. Autism : The International

Journal of Research and Practice, 10(4), 365–382.

http://doi.org/10.1177/1362361306064416

Belmonte, M. K., Cook, E. H., Anderson, G. M., Rubenstein, J. L. R., Greenough, W. T.,

Beckel-Mitchener, A., … Tierney, E. (2004). Autism as a disorder of neural

106 information processing: directions for research and targets for therapy. Molecular

Psychiatry, 9(7), 646–63. http://doi.org/10.1038/sj.mp.4001499

Bennett, K. D., Ramasamy, R., & Honsberger, T. (2013). The effects of covert audio

coaching on teaching clerical skills to adolescents with autism spectrum disorder.

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(3), 585–593.

Berger, C. R., & Calabrese, R. J. (1975). Some explorations in initial interaction and

beyond: Toward a developmental theory of interpersonal communication. Human

Communication Research, 1(2), 99–112.

Billstedt, E., Gillberg, C., & Gillberg, C. (2005). Autism after adolescence: population-

based 13-to 22-year follow-up study of 120 individuals with autism diagnosed in

childhood. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(3), 351–360.

Bishop, D. V. M. (1993). Annotation: Autism, executive functions and theory of mind: A

neuropsychological perspective. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34(3),

279–293. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1993.tb00992.x

Bishop, S. L., Richler, J., & Lord, C. (2006). Association between restricted and

repetitive behaviors and nonverbal IQ in children with autism spectrum disorders.

Child Neuropsychology, 12(4–5), 247–267.

Bogte, H., Flamma, B., van der Meere, J., & van Engeland, H. (2008). Cognitive

flexibility in adults with high functioning autism. Journal of Clinical and

Experimental Neuropsychology, 30(1), 33–41.

http://doi.org/10.1080/13803390601186668

107 Booth, R., & Happé, F. (2010). “Hunting with a knife and… fork”: Examining central

coherence in autism, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and typical

development with a linguistic task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,

107(4), 377–393.

Boraston, Z., Blakemore, S.-J., Chilvers, R., & Skuse, D. (2007). Impaired sadness

recognition is linked to social interaction deficit in autism. Neuropsychologia, 45(7),

1501–1510.

Brown, A. C., Chouinard, P. A., & Crewther, S. G. (2017). Vision research literature may

not represent the full intellectual range of Autism Spectrum Disorder. Frontiers in

Human Neuroscience, 11, 57. http://doi.org/10.3389/FNHUM.2017.00057

Bruni, T. P. (2014). Test Review: Social Responsiveness Scale-Second Edition (SRS-2).

Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 32(4), 365–369.

http://doi.org/10.1177/0734282913517525

Burack, J. A. (1994). Selective attention deficits in persons with autism: preliminary

evidence of an inefficient attentional lens. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103(3),

535–43.

Burden, M. J., Andrew, C., Saint-Amour, D., Meintjes, E. M., Molteno, C. D., Hoyme, H.

E., … Jacobson, S. W. (2009). The effects of fetal alcohol syndrome on response

execution and inhibition: an event-related potential study. Alcoholism, Clinical and

Experimental Research, 33(11), 1994–2004. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-

0277.2009.01038.x

108 Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior

cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(6), 215–222.

http://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01483-2

Cañadas, E., Rodríguez-Bailón, R., Milliken, B., & Lupiáñez, J. (2013). Social categories

as a context for the allocation of attentional control. Journal of Experimental

Psychology. General, 142(3), 934–43. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0029794

Carleton, R. N. (2012). The intolerance of uncertainty construct in the context of anxiety

disorders: theoretical and practical perspectives. Expert Review of

Neurotherapeutics, 12(8), 937–947. http://doi.org/10.1586/ern.12.82

Carleton, R. N., Mulvogue, M. K., Thibodeau, M. a., McCabe, R. E., Antony, M. M., &

Asmundson, G. J. G. (2012). Increasingly certain about uncertainty: Intolerance of

uncertainty across anxiety and depression. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 26(3), 468–

479. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.01.011

Carter, C. S., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Botvinick, M. M., Noll, D., & Cohen, J. D.

(1998). Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of

performance. Science, 280(5364), 747. Retrieved from

http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=a9h&AN=579532&site=eh

ost-live

Cashin, A. J. (2005). Autism: understanding conceptual processing deficits. Journal of

Psychosocial Nursing and Mental Health Services, 43(4), 22–30.

Cashin, A. J., Gallagher, H., Newman, C., & Hughes, M. (2012). Autism and the

109 cognitive processing triad: a case for revising the criteria in the diagnostic and

statistical manual. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Nursing : Official

Publication of the Association of Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Nurses, Inc,

25(3), 141–8. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6171.2012.00337.x

Cassidy, S., Ropar, D., Mitchell, P., & Chapman, P. (2013). Can Adults With Autism

Spectrum Disorders Infer What Happened to Someone From Their Emotional

Response? Autism Research : Official Journal of the International Society for

Autism Research, (December 2013), 112–123. http://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1351

Castelli, F. (2005). Understanding emotions from standardized facial expressions in

autism and normal development. Autism, 9(4), 428–449.

Castelli, F., Frith, C. D., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and

brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain,

125(8), 1839–1849.

Collignon, O., Charbonneau, G., Peters, F., Nassim, M., Lassonde, M., Lepore, F., …

Bertone, A. (2013). Reduced multisensory facilitation in persons with autism.

Cortex, 49(6), 1704–1710. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2012.06.001

Courchesne, E., Campbell, K., & Solso, S. (2011). Brain growth across the life span in

autism: age-specific changes in anatomical pathology. Brain Research, 1380, 138–

145.

Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel — now? The anterior insula and human awareness.

Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(1), 59–70. http://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2555

110 Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). Review and Reformulation of Social Information-

Processing Mechanisms in Chidrens Social Adjustment. Psychological Bulletin,

115(1), 74–101. http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74

Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P., Öhman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Neural

systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 7(2), 189–195.

http://doi.org/10.1038/nn1176

Cruys, S. Van De, Evers, K., Hallen, R. Van Der, Eylen, L. Van, Boets, B., &

Wagemans, J. (2014). Precise in uncertain worlds : Predictive coding in

autism. Psychological Review, In press, 1–36. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0037665

Dajani, D. R., & Uddin, L. Q. (2015). Local brain connectivity across development in

autism spectrum disorder: A cross-sectional investigation. Autism Research, n/a-n/a.

http://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1494

Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., Johnstone, T., Schaefer, H. S., Gernsbacher, M. A.,

Goldsmith, H. H., … Davidson, R. J. (2005). Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry

of face processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience, 8(4), 519–526.

Damasio, A. R., & Maurer, R. G. (1978). A neurological model for childhood autism.

Archives of Neurology, 35(12), 777–786. Retrieved from

http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archneur.1978.00500360001001

Davis, K. D. (2005). Human anterior cingulate cortex neurons encode cognitive and

emotional demands. J. Neurosci., 25(37), 8402–8406.

http://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2315-05.2005

111 Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., & Liaw, J. (2004).

Early social attention impairments in autism: social orienting, joint attention, and

attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 271.

De Bildt, A., Oosterling, I. J., Van Lang, N. D. J., Kuijper, S., Dekker, V., Sytema, S., …

De Jonge, M. V. (2013). How to use the ADI-R for classifying autism spectrum

disorders? Psychometric properties of criteria from the literature in 1,204 Dutch

children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(10), 2280–2294.

http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1783-1 de Vries, M., & Geurts, H. M. (2012). Cognitive flexibility in ASD; task switching with

emotional faces. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(12), 2558–68.

http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1512-1

Dichter, G. S., & Belger, A. (2007). Social stimuli interfere with cognitive control in

autism. NeuroImage, 35(3), 1219–1230.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.12.038

Dichter, G. S., & Belger, A. (2008). Atypical modulation of cognitive control by arousal

in autism. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 164(3), 185–197.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.12.005

DiCriscio, A. S., Miller, S. J., Hanna, E. K., Kovac, M., Turner-Brown, L., Sasson, N. J.,

… Dichter, G. S. (2016). Brief Report: Cognitive Control of Social and Nonsocial

Visual Attention in Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2804-7

112 Du, S., Tao, Y., & Martinez, A. M. (2014). Compound facial expressions of emotion.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,

111(15), E1454-62. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1322355111

Duerden, E. G., Taylor, M. J., Soorya, L. V., Wang, T., Fan, J., & Anagnostou, E. (2013).

Neural correlates of inhibition of socially relevant stimuli in adults with autism

spectrum disorder. Brain Research, 1533, 1–11.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2013.08.021

Dugas, M., Gagnon, F., Ladouceur, R., & Freeston, M. H. (1998). Generalized anxiety

disorder: a preliminary test of a conceptual model. Behaviour Research and

Therapy, 36(2), 215–226. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00070-3

Duncan, A. W., & Bishop, S. L. (2013). Understanding the gap between cognitive

abilities and daily living skills in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders with

average intelligence. Autism : The International Journal of Research and Practice.

http://doi.org/10.1177/1362361313510068

Durston, S., Tottenham, N. T., Thomas, K. M., Davidson, M. C., Eigsti, I.-M., Yang, Y.,

… Casey, B. . (2003). Differential patterns of striatal activation in young children

with and without ADHD. Biological Psychiatry, 53(10), 871–878.

http://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01904-2

Dziobek, I., Fleck, S., Kalbe, E., Rogers, K., Hassenstab, J., Brand, M., … Convit, A.

(2006). Introducing MASC: a movie for the assessment of social cognition. Journal

of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(5), 623–636.

113 Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Pictures of facial affect. consulting psychologists

press.

Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of

a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16(1), 143–149.

Fan, J. (2014). An information theory account of cognitive control. Frontiers in Human

Neuroscience, 8(680). http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00680

Fan, J., Bernardi, S., Dam, N. T., Anagnostou, E., Gu, X., Martin, L., … Hof, P. R.

(2012). Functional deficits of the attentional networks in autism. Brain and

Behavior, 2(5), 647–60. http://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.90

Fan, J., Dam, N. T. Van, Gu, X., Liu, X., Wang, H., Tang, C. Y., & Hof, P. R. (2014).

Quantitative characterization of functional anatomical contributions to cognitive

control under uncertainty. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

http://doi.org/10.1162/jocn

Fan, J., Gu, X., Liu, X., Guise, K. G., Park, Y., Martin, L., … Hof, P. R. (2011).

Involvement of the anterior cingulate and frontoinsular cortices in rapid processing

of salient facial emotional information. NeuroImage, 54(3), 2539–2546.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.007

Fan, J., Guise, K. G., Liu, X., & Wang, H. (2008). Searching for the Majority:

Algorithms of Voluntary Control. PLoS One, 3(10).

http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0003522

Farley, M. A., McMahon, W. M., Fombonne, E., Jenson, W. R., Miller, J., Gardner, M.,

114 … Ritvo, R. A. (2009). Twenty-year outcome for individuals with autism and

average or near-average cognitive abilities. Autism Research, 2(2), 109–118.

Fein, D., Barton, M., Eigsti, I. M., Kelley, E., Naigles, L., Schultz, R. T., … Tyson, K.

(2013). Optimal outcome in individuals with a history of autism. Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 54(2), 195–205.

http://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12037

Feldman, H., & Friston, K. J. (2010). Attention, uncertainty, and free-energy. Frontiers in

Human Neuroscience, 4(December), 215. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2010.00215

Field, T., Sanders, C., & Nadel. (2001). Children with autism display more social

behaviors. Autism, (2000), 317–23.

First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1998). Structured Clinical

Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders: Patient Edition (February 1996 Final),

SCID-I/P. Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute.

Flood, A. M., Julian Hare, D., & Wallis, P. (2011). An investigation into social

information processing in young people with Asperger syndrome. Autism : The

International Journal of Research and Practice, 15(5), 601–24.

http://doi.org/10.1177/1362361310387803

Fridenson-Hayo, S., Berggren, S., Lassalle, A., Tal, S., Pigat, D., Bölte, S., … Golan, O.

(2016). Basic and Complex Emotion Recognition in Children with Autism : Cross

Cultural Findings. Molecular Autism, submited. http://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-016-

0113-9

115 Friston, K. J. (2010). The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? Nature Reviews.

Neuroscience, 11(2), 127–38. http://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2787

Friston, K. J., Lawson, R., & Frith, C. D. (2013). On hyperpriors and hypopriors:

comment on Pellicano and Burr. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(1), 1.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.11.003

Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2012). Mechanisms of social cognition. Annual Review of

Psychology, 63, 287–313. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100449

Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Autism: beyond “theory of mind”. Cognition, 50(1–3),

115–32.

Frith, U., & Snowling, M. (1983). Reading for meaning and reading for sound in autistic

and dyslexic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1(4), 329–342.

García-Villamisar, D., & Della Sala, S. (2002). Dual-task performance in adults with

autism. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 7(1), 63–74.

http://doi.org/10.1080/13546800143000140

Gasquoine, P. G. (2014). Contributions of the Insula to Cognition and Emotion.

Neuropsychology Review. http://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-014-9246-9

Gedek, H. M., Pantelis, P. C., & Kennedy, D. P. (2016). The influence of presentation

modality on the social comprehension of naturalistic scenes in adults with autism

spectrum disorder. Autism. http://doi.org/10.1177/1362361316671011

Geurts, H. M., Corbett, B., & Solomon, M. (2009). The paradox of cognitive flexibility in

autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(2), 74–82.

116 http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.006

Geurts, H. M., Verté, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., & Sergeant, J. a. (2004). How

specific are executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

and autism? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines,

45(4), 836–54. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00276.x

Gillott, A., Furniss, F., & Walter, A. (2001). Anxiety in High-Functioning Children with

Autism. Autism, 5(3), 277–286. http://doi.org/10.1177/1362361301005003005

Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., & Hill, J. (2006). The Cambridge Mindreading (CAM)

Face-Voice Battery: Testing complex emotion recognition in adults with and

without Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,

36(2), 169–83. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-005-0057-y

Goldstein, G., Johnson, C. R., & Minshew, N. J. (2001). Attentional Processes in Autism.

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(4), 433–440.

Gomot, M., & Wicker, B. (2012). A challenging, unpredictable world for people with

Autism Spectrum Disorder. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 83(2), 240–

247. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.09.017

Goulden, N., Khusnulina, A., Davis, N. J., Bracewell, R. M., Bokde, A. L., McNulty, J.

P., & Mullins, P. G. (2014). The Salience Network is responsible for switching

between the Default Mode Network and the Central Executive Network: Replication

from DCM. NeuroImage. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.052

Gu, X., Eilam-Stock, T., Zhou, T., Anagnostou, E., Kolevzon, A., Soorya, L., … Fan, J.

117 (2015). Autonomic and brain responses associated with empathy deficits in autism

spectrum disorder. Human Brain Mapping, 0(April), n/a-n/a.

http://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22840

Gu, X., Liu, X., Van Dam, N. T., Hof, P. R., & Fan, J. (2012). Cognition-emotion

integration in the anterior insular cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 23(1), 20–27.

http://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr367

Hadjikhani, N., Joseph, R. M., Snyder, J., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2007). Abnormal

activation of the social brain during face perception in autism. Human Brain

Mapping, 28(5), 441–449.

Happé, F. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’

thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children

and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129–154.

Happé, F. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task

performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66(3), 843–855.

Happé, F. (1997). Central coherence and theory of mind in autism: Reading homographs

in context. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15(1), 1–12.

Happé, F. (1999). Autism: cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in Cognitive

Sciences, 3(6), 216–222. http://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01318-2

Happé, F., Booth, R., Charlton, R., & Hughes, C. (2006). Executive function deficits in

autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: examining

profiles across domains and ages. Brain and Cognition, 61(1), 25–39.

118 http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2006.03.004

Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: detail-focused cognitive

style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,

36(1), 5–25. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0

Happé, F., & Ronald, A. (2008). The “fractionable autism triad”: a review of evidence

from behavioural, genetic, cognitive and neural research. Neuropsychology Review,

18(4), 287–304. http://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-008-9076-8

Hastings, R. P. (2003). Behavioral adjustment of siblings of children with autism engaged

in applied behavior analysis early intervention programs: the moderating role of

social support. J Autism Dev Disord, 33(2), 141–150. Retrieved from

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=

Citation&list_uids=12757353

Heavey, L., Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (2000). The Awkward Moments

Test: a naturalistic measure of social understanding in autism. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 225–36.

Helt, M., Kelley, E., Kinsbourne, M., Pandey, J., Boorstein, H., Herbert, M., & Fein, D.

(2008). Can children with autism recover? If so, how? Neuropsychology Review,

18(4), 339–366.

Hick, W. E. (1952). On the rate of gain of information. Quarterly Journal of

Experimental Psychology, 4(1), 11–26.

Hill, E. L. (2004a). Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism.

119 Developmental Review, 24(2), 189–233. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2004.01.001

Hill, E. L. (2004b). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1),

26–32. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.003

Hirsh, J. B., Mar, R. A., & Peterson, J. B. (2012). Psychological entropy: a framework for

understanding uncertainty-related anxiety. Psychol Rev, 119(2), 304–320.

http://doi.org/10.1037/a0026767

Howlin, P., Goode, S., Hutton, J., & Rutter, M. (2004). Adult outcome for children with

autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(2), 212–229.

Hubert, B. E., Wicker, B., Monfardini, E., & Deruelle, C. (2009). Electrodermal

reactivity to emotion processing in adults with autistic spectrum disorders. Autism,

13(1), 9–19. http://doi.org/10.1177/1362361308091649

Hughes, C., Russell, J., & Robbins, T. W. (1994). Evidence for executive dysfunction in

autism. Neuropsychologia, 32(4), 477–492. http://doi.org/10.1016/0028-

3932(94)90092-2

Hutt, S. J., Hutt, C., Lee, D., & Ounsted, C. (1965). A behavioural and

electroencephalographic study of autistic children. Journal of Psychiatric Research,

3(3), 181–197.

Jarrold, C., Gilchrist, I. D., & Bender, A. (2005). Embedded figures detection in autism

and typical development: Preliminary evidence of a double dissociation in

relationships with visual search. Developmental Science, 8(4), 344–351.

Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). A test of central coherence theory: linguistic

120 processing in high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome: is local

coherence impaired? Cognition, 71(2), 149–85.

Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. a, & Minshew, N. J. (2004). Cortical activation

and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism:

evidence of underconnectivity. Brain : A Journal of Neurology, 127(Pt 8), 1811–21.

http://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh199

Just, M. A., Keller, T. A., Malave, V. L., Kana, R. K., & Varma, S. (2012). Autism as a

neural systems disorder: A theory of frontal-posterior underconnectivity.

Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(4), 1292–1313.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.02.007

Kaland, N., Smith, L., & Mortensen, E. L. (2008). Brief report: cognitive flexibility and

focused attention in children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome or high-

functioning autism as measured on the computerized version of the Wisconsin Card

Sorting Test. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(6), 1161–5.

http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-007-0474-1

Kana, R. K., Keller, T. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Minshew, N. J., & Just, M. A. (2006).

Sentence comprehension in autism: thinking in pictures with decreased functional

connectivity. Brain : A Journal of Neurology, 129(Pt 9), 2484–93.

http://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl164

Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child.

Keehn, B., Brenner, L. A., Ramos, A. I., Lincoln, A. J., Marshall, S. P., & Müller, R.-A.

121 (2009). Brief report: eye-movement patterns during an embedded figures test in

children with ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(2), 383–

387.

Kenworthy, L., Case, L., Harms, M. B., Martin, A., & Wallace, G. L. (2010). Adaptive

behavior ratings correlate with symptomatology and IQ among individuals with

high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental

Disorders, 40(4), 416–23. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-009-0911-4

Kleinhans, N., Akshoomoff, N., & Delis, D. C. (2005). Executive Functions in Autism

and Asperger’s Disorder: Flexibility, Fluency, and Inhibition. Developmental

Neuropsychology, 27(3), 379–401. http://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2703_5

Kouneiher, F., Charron, S., & Koechlin, E. (2009). Motivation and cognitive control in

the human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 12(7), 939–947.

Lam, K. S. L., Bodfish, J. W., & Piven, J. (2008). Evidence for three subtypes of

repetitive behavior in autism that differ in familiality and association with other

symptoms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines,

49(11), 1193–200. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01944.x

Lawson, R. P., Rees, G., & Friston, K. J. (2014). An aberrant precision account of autism

. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00302

Leekam, S. R., & Perner, J. (1991). Does the autistic child have a metarepresentational

deficit? Cognition, 40(3), 203–218.

Leekam, S. R., Prior, M. R., & Uljarevic, M. (2011). Restricted and repetitive behaviors

122 in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A review of research in the last decade.

Psychological Bulletin.

Lerner, M. D., Hutchins, T. L., & Prelock, P. A. (2011). Brief report: Preliminary

evaluation of the theory of mind inventory and its relationship to measures of social

skills. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(4), 512–517.

López, B., & Leekam, S. R. (2003). Do children with autism fail to process information

in context? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(2), 285–300.

Lopez, B. R., Lincoln, A. J., Ozonoff, S., & Lai, Z. (2005). Examining the Relationship

between Executive Functions and Restricted, Repetitive Symptoms of Autistic

Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(4), 445–460.

http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-005-5035-x

Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H. J., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., …

Rutter, M. (2000). The Autism Diagnostic Schedule – Generic: A standard measures

of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism.

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.

Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A

Revised Version of a Diagnostic Interview for Caregivers of Individuals with

Possible Pervasive Developmental Disorders. Journal of Autism & Developmental

Disorders, 24(5), 659–685.

Louwerse, A., Tulen, J. H. M., van der Geest, J. N., van der Ende, J., Verhulst, F. C., &

Greaves-Lord, K. (2014). Autonomic responses to social and nonsocial pictures in

123 adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research : Official Journal of the

International Society for Autism Research, 7(1), 17–27.

http://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1327

Mackie, M.-A., & Fan, J. (2015). Reduced Efficiency and Capacity of Cognitive Control

in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Autism Research, n/a-n/a.

http://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1517

Mackie, M.-A., Van Dam, N. T., Fan, J., Dam, N. T. Van, & Fan, J. (2013). Cognitive

control and attentional functions. Brain and Cognition, 82(3), 301–312.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2013.05.004

Maes, J. H. R., Eling, P. A. T. M., Wezenberg, E., Vissers, C. T. W. M., & Kan, C. C.

(2011). Attentional set shifting in autism spectrum disorder: Differentiating between

the role of , learned irrelevance, and novelty processing. Journal of

Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33(2), 210–217.

Markram, H., Rinaldi, T., & Markram, K. (2007). The intense world syndrome--an

alternative hypothesis for autism. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 1(1), 77–96.

http://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.01.1.1.006.2007

Mazza, M., Mariano, M., Peretti, S., Masedu, F., Pino, M. C., & Valenti, M. (2017). The

Role of Theory of Mind on Social Information Processing in Children With Autism

Spectrum Disorders: A Mediation Analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental

Disorders, 0(0), 0. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3069-5

McGovern, C. W., & Sigman, M. (2005). Continuity and change from early childhood to

124 adolescence in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(4), 401–408.

Melcher, T., Born, C., & Gruber, O. (2011). How negative affect influences neural

control processes underlying the resolution of cognitive interference: An event-

related fMRI study. Neuroscience Research, 70(4), 415–427.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2011.05.007

Melcher, T., Obst, K., Mann, A., Paulus, C., & Gruber, O. (2012). Antagonistic

modulatory influences of negative affect on cognitive control: Reduced and

enhanced interference resolution capability after the induction of fear and sadness.

Acta Psychologica, 139(3), 507–514. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.01.012

Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function.

Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24(1), 167–202. Retrieved from

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11283309

Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (1998). Autism as a disorder of complex information

processing. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews,

4, 129–136.

Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., & Siegel, D. J. (1997). Neuropsychologic functioning in

autism: profile of a complex information processing disorder. Journal of the

International Neuropsychological Society, 3(4), 303–316. http://doi.org/doi:null

Minshew, N. J., Johnson, C., & Luna, B. (2000). The cognitive and neural basis of

autism: A disorder of complex information processing and dysfunction of

neocortical systems. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 23,

125 111–138.

Mitchell, D. G. V, Nakic, M., Fridberg, D., Kamel, N., Pine, D. S., & Blair, R. J. R.

(2007). The impact of processing load on emotion. NeuroImage, 34(3), 1299–1309.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.012

Mitchell, D. G. V, Richell, R. a, Leonard, A., & Blair, R. J. R. (2006). Emotion at the

expense of cognition: psychopathic individuals outperform controls on an operant

response task. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115(3), 559–566.

http://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.115.3.559

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T.

D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to

complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology,

41(1), 49–100. http://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

Mottron, L., & Burack, J. A. (2001). Enhanced perceptual functioning in the development

of autism. In J. A. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmaya, & P. R. Zalazo (Eds.), The

development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp. 131–148).

Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Mottron, L., Burack, J. A., Iarocci, G., Belleville, S., & Enns, J. T. (2003). Locally

oriented perception with intact global processing among adolescents with high-

functioning autism: evidence from multiple paradigms. Journal of Child Psychology

and Psychiatry, 44(6), 904–913.

Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulières, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced

126 perceptual functioning in autism: an update, and eight principles of autistic

perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 27–43.

http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-005-0040-7

Mundy, P. (1993). Normal versus high-functioning status in children with autism.

American Journal of Mental Retardation, 97(4), 381–384.

Mushtaq, F., Bland, A. R., & Schaefer, A. (2011). Uncertainty and cognitive control.

Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 249. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00249

O’Connor, K., & Kirk, I. (2008). Brief report: atypical social cognition and social

behaviours in autism spectrum disorder: a different way of processing rather than an

impairment. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(10), 1989–97.

http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-008-0559-5

Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in

Cognitive Sciences, 9(5), 242–249. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010

Ozonoff, S. (1995). Executive functions in autism. In Learning and cognition in autism

(pp. 199–219). Springer.

Ozonoff, S., & Jensen, J. (1999). Brief Report: Specific Executive Function Profiles in

Three Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Journal of Autism & Developmental

Disorders, 29(2), 171–177.

Ozonoff, S., & McEvoy, R. E. (1994). A longitudinal study of executive function and

theory of mind development in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 415–

431.

127 Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in

high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind. Journal Of

Child Psychology And Psychiatry, And Allied Disciplines, 32(7), 1081–1105.

Retrieved from

http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=1787138&site=e

host-live

Ozonoff, S., Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). Asperger’s Syndrome: Evidence

of an Empirical Distinction from High-Functioning Autism. Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry, 32(7), 1107–1122. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-

7610.1991.tb00352.x

Ozonoff, S., Strayer, D. L., McMahon, W. M., & Filloux, F. (1994). Executive function

abilities in autism and Tourette syndrome: an information processing approach.

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 35(6), 1015–

32. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01807.x

Padmala, S., Bauer, A., & Pessoa, L. (2011). Negative emotion impairs conflict-driven

executive control. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(AUG), 1–5.

http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00192

Pascualvaca, D. M., Fantie, B. D., Papageorgiou, M., & Mirsky, a F. (1998). Attentional

capacities in children with autism: is there a general deficit in shifting focus?

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(6), 467–78.

Pellicano, E., & Burr, D. (2012). When the world becomes “too real”: a Bayesian

128 explanation of autistic perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(10), 504–510.

http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.08.009

Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental

psychopathology. Journal Of Child Psychology And Psychiatry, And Allied

Disciplines, 37(1), 51–87.

Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature Reviews

Neuroscience, 9(2), 148–158. http://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2317

Pessoa, L., McKenna, M., Gutierrez, E., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2002). Neural processing

of emotional faces requires attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of

Sciences of the USA, 99, 11458–11463.

Phillips, W., Gómez, J. C., Baron-Cohen, S., Laá, V., & Rivière, A. (1995). Treating

people as objects, agents, or “subjects”: how young children with and without

autism make requests. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied

Disciplines, 36(8), 1383–1398.

Piven, J., Harper, J., Palmer, P. A. T., & Arndt, S. (1996). Course of behavioral change in

autism: A retrospective study of high-IQ adolescents and adults. Journal of the

American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 35(4), 523–529.

Plaisted, K. C. (2001). Reduced generalization in autism: An alternative to weak central

coherence. In J. A. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P. D. Zelazo (Eds.), The

development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp. 149–169).

Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

129 Poljac, E., & Bekkering, H. (2012). A review of intentional and cognitive control in

autism. Frontiers in Psychology, 3. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00436

Ponnet, K. S., Roeyers, H., Buysse, A., De Clercq, A., & Van Der Heyden, E. (2004).

Advanced mind-reading in adults with Asperger syndrome. Autism, 8(3), 249–266.

Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso

(Ed.), Information Processing and Cognition: The Loyola Symposium. Lawrence

Erlbaum.

Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind?

Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515–526.

Prior, M., & Hoffman, W. (1990). Brief report: Neuropsychological testing of autistic

children through an exploration with frontal lobe tests. Journal of Autism &

Developmental Disorders, 20(581–590).

Rajendran, G., & Mitchell, P. (2007). Cognitive theories of autism. Developmental

Review, 27(2), 224–260. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.02.001

Raymaekers, R., Van Der Meere, J., & Roeyers, H. (2004). Event-rate manipulation and

its effect on arousal modulation and response inhibition in adults with high

functioning autism. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26(1),

74–82.

Robinson, S., Goddard, L., Dritschel, B., Wisley, M., & Howlin, P. (2009). Executive

functions in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Brain and Cognition, 71(3),

362–368. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2009.06.007

130 Roeyers, H., Buysse, A., Ponnet, K., & Pichal, B. (2001). Advancing advanced mind-

reading tests: Empathic accuracy in adults with a pervasive developmental disorder.

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 271–278.

Roeyers, H., & Demurie, E. (2010). How impaired is mind-reading in high-functioning

adolescents and adults with autism? European Journal of Developmental

Psychology, 7(1), 123–134. http://doi.org/10.1080/17405620903425924

Rowe, J., Lavender, A., & Turk, V. (2006). Cognitive executive function in Down’s

syndrome. The British Journal of Clinical Psychology / the British Psychological

Society, 45(Pt 1), 5–17. http://doi.org/10.1348/014466505X29594

Rumsey, J. M., & Hamburger, S. D. (1990). Neuropsychological divergence of high-level

autism and severe dyslexia. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 20(155–

168).

Russo, N., Flanagan, T., Iarocci, G., Berringer, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Burack, J. A. (2007).

Deconstructing executive deficits among persons with autism: Implications for

cognitive neuroscience. Brain and Cognition, 65(1), 77–86.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2006.04.007

Schmitz, N., Rubia, K., Daly, E., Smith, A., Williams, S., & Murphy, D. G. M. (2006).

Neural correlates of executive function in autistic spectrum disorders. Biological

Psychiatry, 59(1), 7–16. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.06.007

Shafritz, K. M., Bregman, J. D., Ikuta, T., & Szeszko, P. R. (2015). Neural systems

mediating decision-making and response inhibition for social and nonsocial stimuli

131 in autism. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 60,

112–120. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2015.03.001

Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in autistic children: A research note.

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24(4), 613–620.

Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1993). Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on

the block design task? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34(8), 1351–

1364.

Shannon, C. E. (1948). A Mathematical Theory of Communication. Bell System

Technical Journal, 27(3), 379–423.

Shapiro, H. M., Wong, L. M., & Simon, T. J. (2013). A cross-sectional analysis of the

development of response inhibition in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion

syndrome. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4(August), 81.

http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00081

Shu, B.-C. C., Lung, F.-W. W., Tien, a. Y., & Chen, B.-C. C. (2001). Executive Function

Deficits in Non-Retarded Autistic Children. Autism, 5(2), 165–174.

http://doi.org/10.1177/1362361301005002006

Sinha, P., Kjelgaard, M. M., Gandhi, T. K., Tsourides, K., Cardinaux, a. L., Pantazis, D.,

… Held, R. M. (2014). Autism as a disorder of prediction. Proceedings of the

National Academy of Sciences. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1416797111

Snowling, M., & Frith, U. (1986). Comprehension in “hyperlexic” readers. Journal of

Experimental Child Psychology, 42(3), 392–415.

132 Solomon, M., Ozonoff, S. J., Cummings, N., & Carter, C. S. (2008). Cognitive control in

autism spectrum disorders. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience,

26(2), 239–247. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2007.11.001

Solomon, M., Yoon, J. H., Ragland, J. D., Niendam, T. A., Lesh, T. A., Fairbrother, W.,

& Carter, C. S. (2013). The Development of the Neural Substrates of Cognitive

Control in Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Biological Psychiatry,

76(5), 1–10. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.08.036

Spencer, M. D., Chura, L. R., Holt, R. J., Suckling, J., Calder, A. J., Bullmore, E. T., &

Baron-Cohen, S. (2012). Failure to deactivate the default mode network indicates a

possible endophenotype of autism. Molecular Autism, 3(1), 15.

http://doi.org/10.1186/2040-2392-3-15

Spezio, M. L., Adolphs, R., Hurley, R. S. E., & Piven, J. (2007). Abnormal use of facial

information in high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental

Disorders, 37(5), 929–939. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0232-9

Stahl, L., & Pry, R. (2002). Joint attention and set-shifting in young children with autism.

Autism, 6(4), 383–396.

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of

Experimental Psychology, 18(643–662), 643–662.

Szatmari, P., Tuff, L., Finlayson, M. A., & Bartolucci, G. (1990). Asperger’s syndrome

and autism: Neurocognitive aspects. Journal of the American Academy of Child and

Adolescent Psychiatry, 29(130–136).

133 Tabachnik, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using Multivariate Statistics (6th ed.). Boston:

Pearson/Allyn & Bacon.

Turner, M. A. (1997). Towards and executive dysfunction account of repetitive behavior

in autism. In Autism as an executive disorder (pp. 55–100). New York, NY: Oxford

University Press.

Turner, M. A. (1999). Generating novel ideas: fluency performance in high-functioning

and learning disabled individuals with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and

Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 40(2), 189–201.

Vaidya, C. J., Bunge, S. A., Dudukovic, N. M., Zalecki, C. A., Elliott, G. R., & Gabrieli,

J. D. E. (2005). Altered neural substrates of cognitive control in childhood ADHD:

evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. The American Journal of

Psychiatry, 162(9), 1605–13. http://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.162.9.1605

Valla, J. M., & Belmonte, M. K. (2013). Detail-oriented cognitive style and social

communicative deficits, within and beyond the autism spectrum: Independent traits

that grow into developmental interdependence. Developmental Review.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2013.08.004

Van Eylen, L., Boets, B., Steyaert, J., Evers, K., Wagemans, J., & Noens, I. (2011).

Cognitive flexibility in autism spectrum disorder: Explaining the inconsistencies?

Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(4), 1390–1401.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2011.01.025 van Meel, C. S., Heslenfeld, D. J., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2007). Adaptive

134 control deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): the role of error

processing. Psychiatry Research, 151(3), 211–20.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2006.05.011

Wager, T. D., Davidson, M. L., Hughes, B. L., Lindquist, M. A., & Ochsner, K. N.

(2008). Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation.

Neuron, 59(6), 1037–50. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.006

Wallace, G. L., Yerys, B. E., Peng, C., Dlugi, E., Anthony, L. G., & Kenworthy, L.

(2016). Chapter Three – Assessment and Treatment of Executive Function

Impairments in Autism Spectrum Disorder: An Update. International Review of

Research in Developmental Disabilities, 51, 85–122.

http://doi.org/10.1016/bs.irrdd.2016.07.004

Wang, a T., Lee, S. S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). Neural basis of irony

comprehension in children with autism: the role of prosody and context. Brain : A

Journal of Neurology, 129(Pt 4), 932–43. http://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl032

Wechsler, D., & Hsiao-pin, C. (2011). WASI-II: Wechsler abbreviated scale of

intelligence. Pearson.

White, S. J., Burgess, P. W., & Hill, E. L. (2009). Impairments on “open-ended”

executive function tests in autism. Autism Research : Official Journal of the

International Society for Autism Research, 2(3), 138–47.

http://doi.org/10.1002/aur.78

White, S. W., Oswald, D., Ollendick, T., & Scahill, L. (2009). Anxiety in children and

135 adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(3),

216–229. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.01.003

Williams, D. L., Goldstein, G., & Minshew, N. J. (2006). Neuropsychologic functioning

in children with autism: further evidence for disordered complex information-

processing. Child Neuropsychology : A Journal on Normal and Abnormal

Development in Childhood and Adolescence, 12(4–5), 279–98.

http://doi.org/10.1080/09297040600681190

Williams, D. L., Mazefsky, C. A., Walker, J. D., Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (2014).

Associations Between Conceptual Reasoning, Problem Solving, and Adaptive

Ability in High-functioning Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental

Disorders. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2190-y

Williams, D. L., Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (2015). Further understanding of

complex information processing in verbal adolescents and adults with autism

spectrum disorders. Autism. http://doi.org/10.1177/1362361315586171

Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments of social interaction and associated

abnormalities in children: Epidemiology and classification. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 9(1), 11–29.

Wood, J. J., & Gadow, K. D. (2010). Exploring the nature and function of anxiety in

youth with autism spectrum disorders. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice,

17(4), 281–292.

Wu, T., Dufford, A. J., Mackie, M.-A., Egan, L. J., & Fan, J. (2016). The Capacity of

136 Cognitive Control. Scientific Reports, 6(34025), 1–11.

http://doi.org/10.1038/srep34025

Yerys, B. E., Wallace, G. L., Harrison, B., Celano, M. J., Giedd, J. N., E, K. L., &

Kenworthy, L. E. (2009). Set-shifting in children with autism spectrum disorders:

reversal shifting deficits on the Intradimensional/Extradimensional Shift Test

correlate with repetitive behaviors. Autism, 13(5), 523–538.

http://doi.org/10.1177/1362361309335716

Ziv, Y., Hadad, B. S., & Khateeb, Y. (2013). Social Information Processing in Preschool

Children Diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1935-3