The Use of Music in Learning Languages

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Use of Music in Learning Languages Music and Language 1 Running head: THE USE OF MUSIC FOR LEARNING LANGUAGES: A REVIEW The Use of Music for Learning Languages: A Review of the Literature Jon Weatherford Stansell University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Updated September 14, 2005 Music and Language 2 Abstract Throughout time, healers, philosophers, scientists, and teachers have recognized the place of music for therapeutic and developmental functions (Bancroft 3-7). Researchers over the last twenty years have made astounding advances in the theory of language acquisition. Many find the pedagogical conjoining of language and music compelling. The first part of this review focuses on the historical and developmental proofs of music's relationship with language learning. In part two, neurological theory on music and the mind are covered. Part three summarizes scholarly inquiry on the use of music for learning languages, especially those studies that could prove most instructive both for language teachers and for music therapists in the development of curricula. Music and Language 3 The Use of Music for Learning Languages: A Review of the Literature Described in the earliest cultural records, enacted throughout the development of infants, evidenced from cognitive scientists, and utilized by innovative teachers and therapists, the deep and profound relationship between music and language supports their discriminate, concurrent use to improve outcomes for language acquisition. Melodic recognition, contour processing, timbre discrimination, rhythm, tonality, prediction, and perception of the sight, sound, and form of symbols in context are required in both music and language. Like supportive sisters, they comprise "separate, though complimentary systems of structured communication... language primarily responsible for content and music evoking emotion” (Jourdain 293). Music positively affects language accent, memory, and grammar as well as mood, enjoyment, and motivation. Language teachers and music therapists alike should encourage the conjoined study of these natural partners, because communicating through a musical medium benefits everyone. Music Pervades Life: Therapy, Development, and Learning Throughout time and in all areas of the world, music‟s universal presence asserts its importance. W. Jane Bancroft presents an impressive litany of historical music therapists, including tribal shamans, Egyptian priest-physicians, the biblical David, Pythagoras, Aristotle, and Plato (Bancroft 4-5). She asserts, "In every part of the ancient world, music and musical instruments served magical or 'therapeutic' purposes rather than aesthetic ones” (Bancroft 4). Plato believed that "musical training is a more potent instrument than any other, because rhythm and harmony find their way into the inward places of the soul, on which they mightily fasten... making the soul of him who is rightly educated graceful” (Jowett 271). He took the concept of physical healing a step further, in the holistic manner common to the Greeks, advocating the Music and Language 4 learning outcome of correct education as a graceful soul. As if bringing grace to the soul was not enough, music also embraced language and movement. In The Greek View of Life, G. Lowes Dickinson defines mousikas, from which our word "music" derives, as an "intimate union of melody, verse, and dance” (217). The Greek concept of mousikas was much more inclusive than ours. Music implied language, Plato himself considering a tune without words a "sign of a want of true artistic taste." Language uniquely enabled the Greek listener "to distinguish the exact character of the mood which the rhythm and tune is supposed to represent” (Dickinson 217). Plato expected language in a musical context, but he did not write about the music inside language. For insight into this, one must look to the Greek myths. The word mousikas means "from the muses," and understanding the origins of the muses shows how they understood music's role in the development of linguistic genres. Thomas Bullfinch's Mythology describes the Muses' birth to Mnemosyne, one of the Titans, original rulers of the mythic universe (Bullfinch 22). Mnemosyne's main concern was the human memory, a primal dominion as important to the ancient Greeks as the sea and sky. Her daughters, the nine Muses, presided over song and prompted the memory. Seven of these divine beings used their "music" to inspire language, including the spoken genres of epic poetry, lyric poetry, sacred poetry, love poetry, comedy, tragedy, and history (Bullfinch 22). The eighth focused on, of all things, astronomy, and the last Muse did something different; through her "music," mortals became inspired not only to choral song but also to dance (Bullfinch 22). Each originating from these sisters of inspiration, the arts of poetry, tragedy, song, and dance comprised the classic Greek theatre. With music and language, the drama and dance of life can occur. The section on human development that follows will echo this powerful metaphor as it discusses the dual processes of language and music learning. Music and Language 5 W. Jane Bancroft references Apollo and Dionysus, both of whom used music. These two, one the wise physician to the gods, and other a sensual corruptor of mortals, represent the modern therapeutic uses of music either "to assuage and soothe" or "to arouse and energize” (Bancroft 4). Music therapists utilize both types of music in clinical situations to relieve many kinds of psychological and physical stressors. Linguistic inadequacies resulting from trauma or delay often benefit from music therapy. Likewise, language students that lack familiarity with a target culture and have trouble expressing themselves can connect through the freeing influence of music. In these cases, the goals of the teacher closely resonate with those of the music therapist. Meaningful communication is a multimodal construct, a large part of which is musical. Spanish music therapist Patxi Del Campo (1997) asserts, “In any oral interaction only 15% of the information corresponds to verbal language, while 70% of the message is performed through body language; the final 15% belongs to intonation, the musical character of language” (as cited in Mora, p.147). Although this ratio likely varies depending on the exact nature of the language task, interlocutors, and intentions, by drawing oral interaction in such a light, Del Campo evokes the three classical elements of mousikas, melody (intonation), verse (words), and dance (body language). This suggests that face-to-face interaction is as much a musical call-and-response as an exchange of words. Moreover, it could be more precise to classify it as a type of dance with musical and linguistic aspects that add expressive or concrete details. The elements of movement, language, and song are also developmentally connected. Dr. Alfred Tomatis asserted that the ear's integration of information from sound and motor movements is crucial to the early nervous system. From aural input, an infant develops not only sound perception, location, and discrimination, but also the physical movements of verticality Music and Language 6 and laterality, as well as language. Dr. Tomatis also described fetal and infant orientation to the melody contours of their native language, recognizing the mother's voice (as cited in Thompson and Andrews, 181-182). The pre-existing patterns of music in the early development of language prove that the two are already long acquainted. Through its mother's body, womb, and amniotic fluid, a fetus cannot hear consonants; it only hears the musical vowel sounds. Carmen F. Mora claims, Discourse intonation, the ordering of pitched sounds made by a human voice, is the first thing we learn when we are acquiring a language. Later on, it is through interaction that a child picks up not only the musicality of each language, but also the necessary communication skills. Mora 149 Mora asserts that a child can imitate the rhythm and musical contours of the language long before he can say the words, and caretakers of young children will agree. She says that musical aspects of language, tone, pauses, stress, and timbre are sonorous units into which phonemes, the consonant and vowel sounds of language, are later placed (Mora 149). Joanne Loewy proposes that language should be considered not in a cognitive context, but in a musical one, which she calls the Musical Stages of Speech (Loewy 48). It evolved from the work of Charles Van Riper, a founder of modern speech therapy. Infants begin with 1) crying and comfort utterances, proceed to 2) babbling, and eventually begin 3) acquiring/ comprehending words. All of these sounds developmentally prepare for the telegraphic speech that follows (Van Riper 87). Loewy's model specifies the mental, physical, and emotional developments at each level and offers specific techniques to encourage vocalizing (Loewy 49). Instead of thinking about language development from the first words, caretakers can follow a child's orientation to communication from the first utterances. Physicians can tell if an infant will have problems with speech by testing their production of cooing sounds, which are a precursor to Music and Language 7 and predictor of speech (Loewy 52). Prelinguistically, music serves as the carrier for communicative intent. The intonation contours within crying and babbling behavior have an emerging communicative purpose. These are the infant's "first audible expression of emotional need" (Loewy 51). Because there are no words involved, all of this communication comes through the musical elements of the cry. Loewy asserts that adults who wish to comfort children can sing in a child‟s tonality, modeling notes that resolve dissonant notes of distress (Loewy 53). Use of drums to encourage internal rhythm is also helpful (Loewy 55). An infant's preverbal communication through crying incorporates turn taking, pausing when her needs are met, and this builds a foundation for social interaction with peers (Loewy 67). With a solid background in crying, most infants soon move to babbling, which enables them to consciously experiment with prosodic elements of speech, such as tone, pauses, timbre, and stress.
Recommended publications
  • Cognitive Processes for Infering Tonic
    University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Student Research, Creative Activity, and Performance - School of Music Music, School of 8-2011 Cognitive Processes for Infering Tonic Steven J. Kaup University of Nebraska-Lincoln, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/musicstudent Part of the Cognition and Perception Commons, Music Practice Commons, Music Theory Commons, and the Other Music Commons Kaup, Steven J., "Cognitive Processes for Infering Tonic" (2011). Student Research, Creative Activity, and Performance - School of Music. 46. https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/musicstudent/46 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Music, School of at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Student Research, Creative Activity, and Performance - School of Music by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. COGNITIVE PROCESSES FOR INFERRING TONIC by Steven J. Kaup A THESIS Presented to the Faculty of The Graduate College at the University of Nebraska In Partial Fulfillment of Requirements For the Degree of Master of Music Major: Music Under the Supervision of Professor Stanley V. Kleppinger Lincoln, Nebraska August, 2011 COGNITIVE PROCESSES FOR INFERRING TONIC Steven J. Kaup, M. M. University of Nebraska, 2011 Advisor: Stanley V. Kleppinger Research concerning cognitive processes for tonic inference is diverse involving approaches from several different perspectives. Outwardly, the ability to infer tonic seems fundamentally simple; yet it cannot be attributed to any single cognitive process, but is multi-faceted, engaging complex elements of the brain. This study will examine past research concerning tonic inference in light of current findings.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effects of Diegetic and Nondiegetic Music on Viewers’ Interpretations of a Film Scene
    Loyola University Chicago Loyola eCommons Psychology: Faculty Publications and Other Works Faculty Publications 6-2017 The Effects of Diegetic and Nondiegetic Music on Viewers’ Interpretations of a Film Scene Elizabeth M. Wakefield Loyola University Chicago, [email protected] Siu-Lan Tan Kalamazoo College Matthew P. Spackman Brigham Young University Follow this and additional works at: https://ecommons.luc.edu/psychology_facpubs Part of the Musicology Commons, and the Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Wakefield, Elizabeth M.; an,T Siu-Lan; and Spackman, Matthew P.. The Effects of Diegetic and Nondiegetic Music on Viewers’ Interpretations of a Film Scene. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 34, 5: 605-623, 2017. Retrieved from Loyola eCommons, Psychology: Faculty Publications and Other Works, http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/mp.2017.34.5.605 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Faculty Publications at Loyola eCommons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Psychology: Faculty Publications and Other Works by an authorized administrator of Loyola eCommons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-Noncommercial-No Derivative Works 3.0 License. © The Regents of the University of California 2017 Effects of Diegetic and Nondiegetic Music 605 THE EFFECTS OF DIEGETIC AND NONDIEGETIC MUSIC ON VIEWERS’ INTERPRETATIONS OF A FILM SCENE SIU-LAN TAN supposed or proposed by the film’s fiction’’ (Souriau, Kalamazoo College as cited by Gorbman, 1987, p. 21). Film music is often described with respect to its relation to this fictional MATTHEW P. S PACKMAN universe. Diegetic music is ‘‘produced within the implied Brigham Young University world of the film’’ (Kassabian, 2001, p.
    [Show full text]
  • A Study Into Musical Preferences, Personality Traits and Memory
    Running head: MUSIC FOR THE MIND Music for the Mind: A study into musical preferences, personality traits and memory retention Rhiannon Rogers 17071491 Western Sydney University *Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Master of Research at Western Sydney University. MUSIC FOR THE MIND 2 MUSIC FOR THE MIND Contents Abstract ...................................................................................................................................... 5 Chapter one: Introduction .......................................................................................................... 6 Chapter two: Memory .............................................................................................................. 11 Forms of Memory................................................................................................................. 12 Methods of Improving Memory ........................................................................................... 15 Rote Learning and Gist Reasoning ................................................................................... 16 Mnemonics ....................................................................................................................... 19 Relative usefulness of types of memorisation .................................................................. 20 Chapter three: Music ................................................................................................................ 22 Music x Memory .................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Aubinet-The-Craft-Of-Yoiking-Revised
    Title page 1 The Craft of Yoiking Title page 2 The Craft of Yoiking Philosophical Variations on Sámi Chants Stéphane Aubinet PhD thesis Department of Musicology University of Oslo 2020 Table of contents Abstract vii Sammendrag ix Acknowledgements xi Abbreviations xv Introduction 1 The Sámi 2 | The yoik 11 [Sonic pictures 17; Creation and apprenticeship 22; Musical structure 25; Vocal technique 29; Modern yoiks 34 ] | Theoretical landscape 39 [Social anthropology 46; Musicology 52; Philosophy 59 ] | Strategies of attention 64 [Getting acquainted 68; Conversations 71; Yoik courses 76; Consultations 81; Authority 88 ] | Variations 94 1st variation: Horizon 101 On the risks of metamorphosis in various practices Along the horizon 103 | Beyond the horizon 114 | Modern horizons 121 | Antlered ideas 125 2nd variation: Enchantment 129 On how animals and the wind (might) engage in yoiking Yoiks to non-humans 131 | The bear and the elk 136 | Enchantment and belief 141 | Yoiks from non-humans 147 | The blowing of the wind 152 | A thousand colours in the land 160 3rd variation: Creature 169 On the yoik’s creative and semiotic processes Painting with sounds 171 | The creation of new yoiks 180 | Listening as an outsider 193 | Creaturely semiosis 200 | The apostle and the genius 207 vi The Craft of Yoiking 4th variation: Depth 213 On the world inside humans and its animation Animal depths 214 | Modal depths 222 | Spiritual depths 227 | Breathed depths 231 | Appetition 236 | Modern depths 241 | Literate depths 248 5th variation: Echo 251 On temporality
    [Show full text]
  • Viewed the Thesis/Dissertation in Its Final Electronic Format and Certify That It Is an Accurate Copy of the Document Reviewed and Approved by the Committee
    U UNIVERSITY OF CINCINNATI Date: I, , hereby submit this original work as part of the requirements for the degree of: in It is entitled: Student Signature: This work and its defense approved by: Committee Chair: Approval of the electronic document: I have reviewed the Thesis/Dissertation in its final electronic format and certify that it is an accurate copy of the document reviewed and approved by the committee. Committee Chair signature: The Nature and Value of Accessibility in Western Art-Music, 1950–1970 A Thesis Submitted to the Division of Graduate Studies and Research of the University of Cincinnati in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Music in the Division of Composition, Musicology, and Theory of the College-Conservatory of Music 2009 by Rachel Hands B. M., University of Massachusetts, 2006 Committee Chair: Dr. Edward Nowacki ABSTRACT It often happens that a composer or performer of contemporary music, in preparing for a performance, asks the question: “Will my audience get it?” When the answer is “probably not,” a second question may arise: “Should I care?” This study takes up the latter question in detail. Put more specifically, the question at hand is: Should we take accessibility into consideration when we compose, perform, and criticize music? This, in turn, raises at least two other broad questions, both of which will be explored in this thesis. The first is “What is the nature of accessibility in music?” The second is “Should we consider accessibility a desirable quality of music?” To answer these questions, I use music cognition research and philosophical studies on musical understanding to characterize accessibility, and draw from that characterization to arrive at a way of determining its value.
    [Show full text]
  • The Learning Trajectory of Musical Memory: from Schematic Processing of Novel Melodies to Robust Musical Memory Representations
    THE LEARNING TRAJECTORY OF MUSICAL MEMORY: FROM SCHEMATIC PROCESSING OF NOVEL MELODIES TO ROBUST MUSICAL MEMORY REPRESENTATIONS A Dissertation Presented to the Faculty of the Graduate School of Cornell University in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy by Kathleen Rose Agres January 2013 © 2013 Kathleen Rose Agres ALL RIGHTS RESERVED THE LEARNING TRAJECTORY OF MUSICAL MEMORY: FROM SCHEMATIC PROCESSING OF NOVEL MELODIES TO ROBUST MUSICAL MEMORY REPRESENTATIONS Kathleen Rose Agres, Ph.D. Cornell University 2013 This dissertation utilizes a multi-method approach to investigate the processes underlying musical learning and memory. Particular emphasis is placed on schematic processing, musical structure, temporal aspects of learning, statistics-based predictive models, efficiency, and the role of musical expertise. We employed a set of behavioral change detection studies with musician and non- musician participants to test what is encoded into gist memory upon hearing unfamiliar melodies varying in musical structure. These studies demonstrate that listeners abstract a schematic representation of the melody that includes tonally and metrically salient tones. In well-structured music, change detection performance improves when a musical event does not conform to the listener’s schematic expectations. Musical expertise is also shown to benefit change detection, especially when the melodies conform to the conventions of Western tonal music. In a study examining learning over a period of increasing musical exposure, we used an information theoretic approach to capture how the statistical properties of music influence listeners’ musical memory. This work highlights how patterns and predictability can facilitate musical learning over time. In further investigation of what underlies this learning process, a series of neural network studies revealed that a compressed representation arose in the internal structure of a computational network as tonal and stylistic information were learned over time.
    [Show full text]
  • Kansas City, Missouri
    Forty-Fourth Annual Conference Hosted by University of Missouri-Kansas City InterContinental Kansas City at the Plaza 28 February–4 March 2018 Kansas City, Missouri Mission of the Society for American Music he mission of the Society for American Music Tis to stimulate the appreciation, performance, creation, and study of American musics of all eras and in all their diversity, including the full range of activities and institutions associated with these musics throughout the world. ounded and first named in honor of Oscar Sonneck (1873–1928), the early Chief of the Library of Congress Music Division and the F pioneer scholar of American music, the Society for American Music is a constituent member of the American Council of Learned Societies. It is designated as a tax-exempt organization, 501(c)(3), by the Internal Revenue Service. Conferences held each year in the early spring give members the opportunity to share information and ideas, to hear performances, and to enjoy the company of others with similar interests. The Society publishes three periodicals. The Journal of the Society for American Music, a quarterly journal, is published for the Society by Cambridge University Press. Contents are chosen through review by a distinguished editorial advisory board representing the many subjects and professions within the field of American music.The Society for American Music Bulletin is published three times yearly and provides a timely and informal means by which members communicate with each other. The annual Directory provides a list of members, their postal and email addresses, and telephone and fax numbers. Each member lists current topics or projects that are then indexed, providing a useful means of contact for those with shared interests.
    [Show full text]
  • Our Varying Histories and Future Potential: Models and Maps In
    University of Pennsylvania ScholarlyCommons Departmental Papers (Music) Department of Music 9-2011 Our Varying Histories and Future Potential: Models and Maps in Science, the Humanities, and in Music Theory Eugene Narmour University of Pennsylvania, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: http://repository.upenn.edu/music_papers Part of the Music Theory Commons Recommended Citation Narmour, E. (2011). Our Varying Histories and Future Potential: Models and Maps in Science, the Humanities, and in Music Theory. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 29 (1), 1-21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/mp.2011.29.1.1 This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. http://repository.upenn.edu/music_papers/2 For more information, please contact [email protected]. Our Varying Histories and Future Potential: Models and Maps in Science, the Humanities, and in Music Theory Abstract Part 1 briefly er counts the influence of social unrest and the explosion of knowledge in both psychology and the humanities circa 1970-1990. As the sciences rely on explicit top-down theories connected to bottom-up maps and models, and whereas the humanities build on bottom-up differences within malleable top-down “theories” (approaches, themes, theses, programs, methods, etc.), the changes in the sciences during this period contrasted sharply with the changes in the humanities. Part 2 discusses in detail how these two social transformations affected the histories of music theory and cognitive music theory. The former fractiously withdrew from its parent organization (AMS), whereas the latter was welcomed into SMPC. Inasmuch as both music theory and cognitive music theory rely on maps and models, Part 3 examines the metatheoretical importance of these terms for music cognition, music theory, and cognitive music theory.
    [Show full text]
  • Springer Handbook of Systematic Musicology
    Springer Handbook of Systematic Musicology Springer Handbooks provide a concise compilation of approved key information on methods of research, general principles, and functional relationships in physical and applied sciences. The world’s leading experts in the fields of physics and engineering will be as- signed by one or several renowned editors to write the chapters com- prising each volume. The content is selected by these experts from Springer sources (books, journals, online content) and other systematic and approved recent publications of scientific and technical information. The volumes are designed to be useful as readable desk reference book to give a fast and comprehen- sive overview and easy retrieval of essential reliable key information, including tables, graphs, and bibli- ographies. References to extensive sources are provided. HandbookSpringer of Systematic Musicology Rolf Bader (Ed.) With952Figuresand61Tables HK Editor Rolf Bader University of Hamburg Institute of Systematic Musicology Neue Rabenstr. 13 20354 Hamburg [email protected] ISBN: 978-3-662-55002-1 e-ISBN: 978-3-662-55004-5 https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-55004-5 Library of Congress Control Number: 2018930527 © Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany 2018 This work is subject to copyright. All rights are reserved by the Publisher, whether the whole or part of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of translation, reprinting, reuse of illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfilms or in any other physical way, and transmission or information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed. The use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, service marks, etc.
    [Show full text]
  • MICROFILMED 1989 INFORMATION to USERS the Most Advanced Technology Has Been Used to Photo­ Graph and Reproduce This Manuscript from the Microfilm Master
    UMI MICROFILMED 1989 INFORMATION TO USERS The most advanced technology has been used to photo­ graph and reproduce this manuscript from the microfilm master. UMI films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type of computer printer. The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are re­ produced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand corner and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each original is also photographed in one exposure and is included in reduced form at the back of the book. These are also available as one exposure on a standard 35mm slide or as a 17" x 23" black and white photographic print for an additional charge. Photographs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced xerographically in this copy. H igher quality 6" x 9" black and white photographic prints are available for any photographs or illustrations appearing in this copy for an additional charge. Contact UMI directly to order. University Microfilms International A Bell & Howell Information Com pany 300 North Zeeb Road, Ann Arbor.
    [Show full text]
  • (Meams) in Alzheimer Disease: Evidence for a Positivity Effect Lola L
    Cuddy et al., Cogent Psychology (2017), 4: 1277578 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/23311908.2016.1277578 COGNITIVE SCIENCE & NEUROSCIENCE | RESEARCH ARTICLE Music-evoked autobiographical memories (MEAMs) in Alzheimer disease: Evidence for a positivity effect Lola L. Cuddy1, Ritu Sikka1*, Kristen Silveira1, Sean Bai1 and Ashley Vanstone1 Received: 20 September 2016 Abstract: We present a study designed to assess the presence of the positivity ef- Accepted: 21 December 2016 fect in music-evoked autobiographical memories or MEAMs. In particular, we sought Published: 13 January 2017 to determine whether the positivity effect was evident in cases of Alzheimer’s *Corresponding author: Ritu Sikka, Department of Psychology, Queen’s disease (AD), where a preserved memory for music in AD might also be reflected University, 62 Arch Street, Humphrey in an ability to retrieve memories associated with music. The positivity effect re- Hall, Room 232, Kingston, Ontario, Canada fers to instances where, compared to younger adults, older adults reveal a rela- E-mail: [email protected] tive preference in attention and memory for positive over negative information. It Reviewing editor: is considered to be a hallmark of healthy aging. Three groups of participants—20 Peter Walla, University of Newcastle, Australia younger adults, 20 older adults, and 20 adults with mild-to-moderate AD—were asked to listen to familiar musical excerpts and describe any memories evoked by Additional information is available at the end of the article the excerpts. Word usage and topics in the memory transcripts were analyzed, as well as ratings of the memories provided by the participants in order to detect any positive bias or absence-of-negativity bias in older as compared to younger adults.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotions, Aesthetic
    MUSIC IN THE Social and Behavioral Sciences An Encyclopedia Volume 1 William Forde Thompson Macquarie University EDITOR Alexandra Lamont Keele University Richard Parncutt University of Graz Frank A. Russo Ryerson University ASSOCIATE EDITORS ($)SAGE reference Los Angeles ILondon INew Delhi Singapore IWashington DC Contents Volume 1 List of Articles vii Reader's Guide xiit About the Editor xviii List of Contributors xix Introduction xxv Chronology xxix Articles A 1 B 119 C 157 D 311 E 353 F 447 G 495 H 525 I 573 Volume 2 List of Articles Vtt Articles L 643 M 667 N 799 0 821 P 835 R 925 S 975 T 1097 U 1155 V 1165 W 1179 Y 1205 Glossary 1209 Resource Guide 1217 Appendix 1223 Index 1241 Photo Credits 1310 List of Contributors Rita Aiello Emmanuel Bigand New York University Universite de Bourgogne Paolo Ammirante Sarah Boak Ryerson University University of South hampton Christina Anagnostopoulou Sarah E. Boslaugh University of Athens Kennesaw State University J ames Andean Janet Bourne University of the Arts Helsinki Northwestern University Artemis Apostolaki Bernd Brabec de Mori University of Hull University ofMusic and Performing Arts Graz Richard Ashley Jillian L. Bracken Northwestern University University of Western Ontario Michael Austin Evan David Bradley Howard University Pennsylvania State University Amee Baird Alessandro Bratus Macquarie University Universita di Pavia, Cremona Felicity Baker Jacob Braun University of Melbourne Bowling Green State University Daniel Bangert Roberto Bresin University of New South Wales KTH Royal Institute
    [Show full text]