Sign Language: Morphology T Johnston, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia Face, and Body) Means That Each Sign Gesture Takes ß 2006 Elsevier Ltd

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Sign Language: Morphology T Johnston, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia Face, and Body) Means That Each Sign Gesture Takes ß 2006 Elsevier Ltd 324 Sign Language: Lexicography Sternberg M L A (1981). American Sign Language. A com- Van Herreweghe M, Slembrouck S & Vermeerbergen M prehensive dictionary. New York: Harper and Row. (eds.) (2004). ‘Digitaal Vlaamse Gebarentaal-Nederlands/ Stokoe W C, Casterline D & Cronenberg C (1965). Nederlands-Vlaamse Gebarentaal woordenboek.’ http:// A dictionary of American Sign Language. Silver Spring, gebaren.ugent.be/. MD: Linstok Press. Sign Language: Morphology T Johnston, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia face, and body) means that each sign gesture takes ß 2006 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. more time to execute than each spoken articulatory gesture (Bellugi and Fischer, 1972). If segments are added to a stem, producing a multisyllabic sign, pro- Morphology deals with the regular, minimal, meaning- cesses of assimilation and deletion tend to restructure bearing units in language – morphemes – which are the resulting sign into a bisyllabic or monosyllabic words or parts of words. Morphemes can effect one with simultaneously expressed morphemes. changes in meaning by signaling the creation of a This process is most clearly seen in the formation of new word or a change in word class (derivation), or new signs through compounding. In Auslan/BSL, for by signaling grammatical information such as case, example, the sign CHECK derives from SEE and MAYBE. number, person, aspect, tense, etc., (inflection) (see SEE has lost its outward movement with final hold Morpheme). and has incorporated the anticipated handshape of Individual signs in a signed language are the basic MAYBE, while MAYBE has lost its repeated twisting equivalent of words in a spoken language. Each movement. The compound has a single syllable signed language has a vocabulary of conventional (Sutton-Spence and Woll, 1999). See Figure 2. lexical signs which are often monomorphemic. In With the exception of prefixes in Israeli Sign Lan- the closely related Australian and British Sign Lan- guage (Aronoff et al., 2000), the few affixes that have guages (Auslan and BSL, respectively), for example, been identified in signed languages are all suffixes. none of the formational aspects of the sign SISTER has Indeed, many appear to have grammaticized from any separate meaning of its own (see Sign Language: one of the elements in erstwhile compounds. For Phonology and Sign Languages of the World). See example, a negative suffix –NEG can be attached to Figure 1. AGREE to derive the new sign DISAGREE, in Auslan/ The type of morphological processes commonly BSL. The affix appears to be a reduced form of an found in signed languages seems to be influenced by independent sign, which itself seems related to a ges- the fact that most lexical signs are monosyllabic or, at ture (meaning something like ‘not know’) shared with most, bisyllabic (Johnson and Liddell, 1986; Liddell, the hearing culture. A similar suffix (in both form and 1984; Sandler, 1995; Wilbur, 1993). Signed languages meaning) has been identified in a number of signed appear to favor simultaneous sign internal modifica- languages (Zeshan, forthcoming). See Figure 3. tion, rather than the concatenation of morphemes. Researchers have also identified a derivational pro- This may be related to the fact that the larger size of cess whereby stem signs for certain units that are the articulators in signed languages (the hands, arms, enumerable (e.g., TOMORROW, WEEK) may incorporate Figure 1 A monomorphemic sign. Reproduced from Johnston Figure 2 A compound and its components. Reproduced from T & Schembri A (eds.) (2003). The survival guide to Auslan: a begin- Johnston T & Schembri A (eds.) (2003). The survival guide to Auslan: ner’s pocket dictionary of Australian Sign Language. Sydney: North a beginner’s pocket dictionary of Australian Sign Language. Sydney: Rocks Press with permission. North Rocks Press with permission. Sign Language: Morphology 325 Figure 3 Derivation through negative affixation. Reproduced from Johnston T & Schembri A (eds.) (2003). The survival guide to Figure 5 Derivation through movement modification. Repro- Auslan: a beginner’s pocket dictionary of Australian Sign Language. duced from Johnston T & Schembri A (eds.) (2003). The survival Sydney: North Rocks Press with permission. guide to Auslan: a beginner’s pocket dictionary of Australian Sign Lan- guage. Sydney: North Rocks Press with permission. Figure 4 Number incorporation in Auslan/BSL. Reproduced from Johnston T & Schembri A (eds.) (2003). The survival guide to Figure 6 The derivation of a nominal from a verb. Reproduced Auslan: a beginner’s pocket dictionary of Australian Sign Language. from Johnston T & Schembri A (eds.) (2003). The survival guide to Sydney: North Rocks Press with permission. Auslan: a beginner’s pocket dictionary of Australian Sign Language. Sydney: North Rocks Press with permission. numeral handshapes to create specific signs for spe- However, typical exemplars in many signed lan- cific numbers of these units (e.g., IN TWO DAYS, TWO- guages overlap considerably in both form and mean- WEEKS). See Figure 4. ing, and the productivity of the derivational process Modification of the quality of the movement in a appears influenced by underlying iconicity. Though it sign can also be used to derive new signs, such as BUSY is to be expected that derivational paradigms in any from WORK (Auslan/BSL). A similar process derives language will be restricted and morpheme productiv- NARROW-MINDED/PRUDISH from CHURCH in ASL, and ity limited, for some signed languages at least the this has been compared to templated morphology as degree of grammaticization of these modifications is found in Semitic languages (Fernald and Napoli, as yet uncertain. 2000). See Figure 5. Morphemes and morphological processes can also It is often difficult to clearly distinguish between signal the inflection of existing signs, adding gram- stem modification or suprasegmental modification in matical information (e.g., marking for number, per- signed languages. In many respects, modifying the son, aspect, etc.) while maintaining the essential movement parameter of a sign is akin to changing a lexical meaning of the stem. Inflections in signed vowel (a stem modification); however, in others, and spoken languages can be found on nominals or modifying for manner of movement is akin to tone predicates (verbs and adjectives). (a suprasegmental modification). The derivation of Inflection by concatenative affixation appears to nouns from verbs in some signed languages is a case be extremely rare in signed languages. One example in point. is a nominal genitive suffix in Auslan which is used Originally described in ASL (Supalla and Newport, to signal possessive relationships between two nouns. 1978), this morphological process, in which nouns This sign is not used as a free morpheme of any kind, are derived from verbs by a sign-internal modification but only as a suffix, as in MOTHERþGEN SISTERþGEN of movement, also has parallels in other signed lan- HUSBAND to mean ‘mother’s sister’s husband.’ guages. The continuous single movement found in the Data from a growing number of signed languages verb is modified to be restrained and tense, and often have shown that all of them exploit space and move- repeated, in the noun, as in the Auslan/BSL pair DOOR ment patterns inherent in sign formation to convey and OPEN-DOOR. See Figure 6. information regularly encoded in the inflectional 326 Sign Language: Morphology Figure 7 Inflection (‘person agreement’) through movement modification in the Auslan/BSL sign GIVE, and the Auslan sign HELP. Reproduced from Johnston T & Schembri A (eds.) (2003). The survival guide to Auslan: a beginner’s pocket dictionary of Australian Sign Language. Sydney: North Rocks Press with permission. systems of spoken languages. Indeed, the markings often appear to be in part phonologically conditioned. For example, space will be exploited when a sign is not anchored throughout its produc- tion at a particular location on the body, or if its movement parameter is not specified for repetition. These processes are exemplified in both nominal and verbal inflections. In nominal signs, inflection for plurality can be marked by repetition (often in differ- ent locations if the sign is not anchored). Spatial Figure 8 Inflection (‘distribution’) through movement modifica- tion in Auslan/BSL. Reproduced from Johnston T & Schembri A modifications can also signal topographical informa- (eds.) (2003). The survival guide to Auslan: a beginner’s pocket dictio- tion about referents (e.g., a noun may be placed in the nary of Australian Sign Language. Sydney: North Rocks Press with signing space to mean ‘thing-at-such-and-such-a- permission. place’). A basic tripartite division of ASL verb signs as plain, spatial, and agreeing (Padden, 1988) has been within a mental space representation of an event found to apply to signed languages to which the (Liddell, 2003). framework has been applied (with or without various A second related phenomenon in verb inflection modifications and reinterpretations). refers to distribution and involves a ‘plural sweep’ in For example, no sign language lacks the ability to which the end point is moved in an arc through loca- modify the direction of the movement parameter of tions associated with referents or relocated and redir- agreeing verbs so that the beginning and end points of ected at each in a series of repetitions, as in the these signs move between regions in the signing space modification of ASK to mean ‘ask all’ or ‘ask each.’ associated with the subject (or the agent) and the See Figure 8. object (or the patient) of the action. (Alternative inter- Similarly, modifications can be made to the move- pretations of this phenomenon include those that ment parameter of verb signs to express a number of assign source and goal as the underlying significance aspectual nuances. Researchers in many signed lan- of these locations (Johnston, 1991; Meier, 1982).) guages have identified similar patterns of movement The sign glossed as GIVE in many signed languages modification, with similar meanings, as first de- can have the meanings ‘I give you’ when moved scribed in ASL (Klima et al., 1979).
Recommended publications
  • Sign Language Typology Series
    SIGN LANGUAGE TYPOLOGY SERIES The Sign Language Typology Series is dedicated to the comparative study of sign languages around the world. Individual or collective works that systematically explore typological variation across sign languages are the focus of this series, with particular emphasis on undocumented, underdescribed and endangered sign languages. The scope of the series primarily includes cross-linguistic studies of grammatical domains across a larger or smaller sample of sign languages, but also encompasses the study of individual sign languages from a typological perspective and comparison between signed and spoken languages in terms of language modality, as well as theoretical and methodological contributions to sign language typology. Interrogative and Negative Constructions in Sign Languages Edited by Ulrike Zeshan Sign Language Typology Series No. 1 / Interrogative and negative constructions in sign languages / Ulrike Zeshan (ed.) / Nijmegen: Ishara Press 2006. ISBN-10: 90-8656-001-6 ISBN-13: 978-90-8656-001-1 © Ishara Press Stichting DEF Wundtlaan 1 6525XD Nijmegen The Netherlands Fax: +31-24-3521213 email: [email protected] http://ishara.def-intl.org Cover design: Sibaji Panda Printed in the Netherlands First published 2006 Catalogue copy of this book available at Depot van Nederlandse Publicaties, Koninklijke Bibliotheek, Den Haag (www.kb.nl/depot) To the deaf pioneers in developing countries who have inspired all my work Contents Preface........................................................................................................10
    [Show full text]
  • Florida Department of Education Home Language Codes
    Florida Department of Education Home Language Codes Code Home Language OM (Afan) Oromo AB Abkhazian AC Abnaki AD Achumawi AA Afar AK Afrikaans AE Ahtena EF Akan EK Akateko AF Alabama AL Albanian, Shqip AG Aleut AH Algonquian WJ American Sign Language AM Amharic AI Apache AR Arabic AJ Arapaho AO Araucanian AP Arikara AN Armenian, Hayeren AS Assamese AQ Athapascan AT Atsina AU Atsugewi AV Aucanian WK Awadhi AW Aymara AZ Azerbaijani AX Aztec BA Bantu BC Bashkir BQ Basque, Euskera BS Bassa BJ Belarusian Code Home Language BE Bengali, Bangla BR Berber BP Bhojpuri DZ Bhutani BH Bihari BI Bislama BG Blackfoot BF Breton BL Bulgarian BU Burmese, Myanmasa BD Byelorussian CB Caddo CC Cahuilla CD Cakchiquel CA Cambodian, Khmer CN Cantonese EC Carolinian CT Catalan CE Cayuga ZA Cebuano ED Chamorro CF Chasta Costa CG Chemeheuvi CI Cherokee CJ Chetemacha CK Cheyenne ZB Chhattisgarhi ZC Chinese, Hakka ZD Chinese, Min Nau (Fukienese or Fujianese) CH Chinese, Zhongwen CL Chinook Jargon CM Chiricahua ZE Chittagonian CP Chiwere CQ Choctaw CS Chumash EE Chuukese/Trukese Code Home Language CU Clallam CV Coast Miwok CW Cocomaricopa CX Coeur D’Alene CY Columbia DF Comanche CO Corsican DG Cowlitz DJ Cree ZF Creole HR Croatian, Hrvatski DK Crow DH Cuna DI Cupeno CZ Czech DB Dakota DA Danish DL Deccan DC Delaware DD Delta River Yuman DE Diegueno DU Dutch, Netherlands DO Dzongkha EN English EA Eskimo EO Esperanto ES Estonian EB Eyak FO Faroese FA Farsi, Persian FJ Fijian FL Filipino FI Finnish, Suomi FB Foothill North Yokuts FC Fox FR French FD French Cree Code Home
    [Show full text]
  • The 5 Parameters of ASL Before You Begin Sign Language Partner Activities, You Need to Learn the 5 Parameters of ASL
    The 5 Parameters of ASL Before you begin Sign Language Partner activities, you need to learn the 5 Parameters of ASL. You will use these parameters to describe new vocabulary words you will learn with your partner while completing Language Partner activities. Read and learn about the 5 Parameters below. DEFINITION In American Sign Language (ASL), we use the 5 Parameters of ASL to describe how a sign behaves within the signer’s space. The parameters are handshape, palm orientation, movement, location, and expression/non-manual signals. All five parameters must be performed correctly to sign the word accurately. Go to https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=FrkGrIiAoNE for a signed definition of the 5 Parameters of ASL. Don’t forget to turn the captions on if you are a beginning ASL student. Note: The signer’s space spans the width of your elbows when your hands are on your hips to the length four inches above your head to four inches below your belly button. Imagine a rectangle drawn around the top half of your body. TYPES OF HANDSHAPES Handshapes consist of the manual alphabet and other variations of handshapes. Refer to the picture below. TYPES OF ORIENTATIONS Orientation refers to which direction your palm is facing for a particular sign. The different directions are listed below. 1. Palm facing out 2. Palm facing in 3. Palm is horizontal 4. Palm faces left/right 5. Palm toward palm 6. Palm up/down TYPES OF MOVEMENT A sign can display different kinds of movement that are named below. 1. In a circle 2.
    [Show full text]
  • University of California Santa Cruz Minimal Reduplication
    UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA SANTA CRUZ MINIMAL REDUPLICATION A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY in LINGUISTICS by Jesse Saba Kirchner June 2010 The Dissertation of Jesse Saba Kirchner is approved: Professor Armin Mester, Chair Professor Jaye Padgett Professor Junko Ito Tyrus Miller Vice Provost and Dean of Graduate Studies Copyright © by Jesse Saba Kirchner 2010 Some rights reserved: see Appendix E. Contents Abstract vi Dedication viii Acknowledgments ix 1 Introduction 1 1.1 Structureofthethesis ...... ....... ....... ....... ........ 2 1.2 Overviewofthetheory...... ....... ....... ....... .. ....... 2 1.2.1 GoalsofMR ..................................... 3 1.2.2 Assumptionsandpredictions. ....... 7 1.3 MorphologicalReduplication . .......... 10 1.3.1 Fixedsize..................................... ... 11 1.3.2 Phonologicalopacity. ...... 17 1.3.3 Prominentmaterialpreferentiallycopied . ............ 22 1.3.4 Localityofreduplication. ........ 24 1.3.5 Iconicity ..................................... ... 24 1.4 Syntacticreduplication. .......... 26 2 Morphological reduplication 30 2.1 Casestudy:Kwak’wala ...... ....... ....... ....... .. ....... 31 2.2 Data............................................ ... 33 2.2.1 Phonology ..................................... .. 33 2.2.2 Morphophonology ............................... ... 40 2.2.3 -mut’ .......................................... 40 2.3 Analysis........................................ ..... 48 2.3.1 Lengtheningandreduplication.
    [Show full text]
  • Deaf-Specific Jobsite Launched
    Get your daily news online for FREE Hardcopy only £5 per year Visit www.hearingtimes.co.uk email [email protected] or call 0845 2930688 HearingSummer 2014 Timeswww.hearingtimes.co.uk Deaf-specific jobsite launched Independent newspaper Hearing Times CIC has launched an accessible, interactive jobsite with the support of a Big Lottery Fund grant The new jobsite, which is now available on the Hearing Times website, will facilitate the search for jobs and include interesting roles matching the skills profiles of deaf and hard of hearing jobseekers as well as a range of jobs in the hearing industry. Another section will provide support and information, with invaluable career advice from industry professionals, which will be updated periodically. The unemployment rate in the Deaf Community lies at 13% – a staggering 9% higher than the national average of 4% About 3.5 million people of working age (16-65 years) are deaf or hard of hearing*. Of these, 160,000 are severely or profoundly deaf. According to statistics from the British Society for Mental Health and Deafness, deaf people are far more likely to be unemployed. Indeed, the unemployment rate in the Deaf Preston MP signs up to Deaf Rights Group’s concerns Community lies at 13% – a staggering 9% higher than the national average of 4%.** Preston MP Mark Hendrick has facilitated a meeting between members of the With the launch of the first jobsite of its local deaf community and leader of the Lancashire County Council, Councillor kind, Hearing Times will proactively help to Jennifer Mein, to discuss deaf rights in Lancashire tackle the high unemployment rate among Following on from a meeting with local the procurement process of deaf services people in the UK with hearing loss.
    [Show full text]
  • Assessing the Bimodal Bilingual Language Skills of Young Deaf Children
    ANZCED/APCD Conference CHRISTCHURCH, NZ 7-10 July 2016 Assessing the bimodal bilingual language skills of young deaf children Elizabeth Levesque PhD What we’ll talk about today Bilingual First Language Acquisition Bimodal bilingualism Bimodal bilingual assessment Measuring parental input Assessment tools Bilingual First Language Acquisition Bilingual literature generally refers to children’s acquisition of two languages as simultaneous or sequential bilingualism (McLaughlin, 1978) Simultaneous: occurring when a child is exposed to both languages within the first three years of life (not be confused with simultaneous communication: speaking and signing at the same time) Sequential: occurs when the second language is acquired after the child’s first three years of life Routes to bilingualism for young children One parent-one language Mixed language use by each person One language used at home, the other at school Designated times, e.g. signing at bath and bed time Language mixing, blending (Lanza, 1992; Vihman & McLaughlin, 1982) Bimodal bilingualism Refers to the use of two language modalities: Vocal: speech Visual-gestural: sign, gesture, non-manual features (Emmorey, Borinstein, & Thompson, 2005) Equal proficiency in both languages across a range of contexts is uncommon Balanced bilingualism: attainment of reasonable competence in both languages to support effective communication with a range of interlocutors (Genesee & Nicoladis, 2006; Grosjean, 2008; Hakuta, 1990) Dispelling the myths….. Infants’ first signs are acquired earlier than first words No significant difference in the emergence of first signs and words - developmental milestones are met within similar timeframes (Johnston & Schembri, 2007) Slight sign language advantage at the one-word stage, perhaps due to features being more visible and contrastive than speech (Meier & Newport,1990) Another myth….
    [Show full text]
  • Hawai'i Civil Rights Commission
    ,¢,..... € o F "‘‘“_ 1",,’ .. .4 Q,‘*- "0 /"I ,u' 4' ‘ .-‘$-@9359 \”,r 0 Q.-E - )‘\ ' 2‘_ ; . .J.(‘"1_.-' 1:" ._- '2-n44!‘ cull ? ‘ b mm.» HAWAI‘I CIVIL RIGHTS COMMISSION ,,.,,...,_ ‘ » $5‘. .._,,.. /\~ ,‘ ___\ ‘fife 830 PUNCHBOWL STREET, ROOM 411 HONOLULU, HI 96813 ·PHONE: 586-8636 FAX: 586-8655 TDD: 568-8692 $‘_,-"‘i£_'»*,, ,.‘-* A-_ ,a:¢j_‘f.§.......+9-.._ ~4~ -u--........~-‘Q; ,1'\n_.'9. February 24, 2021 Videoconference, 9:45 a.m. To: The Honorable Karl Rhoads, Chair The Honorable Jarrett Keohokalole, Vice Chair Members of the Senate Committee on Judiciary From: Liann Ebesugawa, Chair and Commissioners of the Hawai‘i Civil Rights Commission Re: S.B. No. 537 The Hawai‘i Civil Rights Commission (HCRC) has enforcement jurisdiction over Hawai‘i’s laws prohibiting discrimination in employment, housing, public accommodations, and access to state and state funded services. The HCRC carries out the Hawai‘i constitutional mandate that no person shall be discriminated against in the exercise of their civil rights. Art. I, Sec. 5. S.B. No. 537 would add a new section to Chapter 1 of the Hawai‘i Revised Statutes which would recognize American Sign Language (ASL) as a fully developed, autonomous, natural language with its own grammar, syntax, vocabulary and cultural heritage. Just as is the case with languages that are characteristic of ancestry or national origin, ASL is a language that is closely tied to culture and identity. Over 40 US states recognize ASL to varying degrees, from a foreign language for school credits to the official language of that state's deaf population, with several enacting legislation similar to S.B.
    [Show full text]
  • What Sign Language Creation Teaches Us About Language Diane Brentari1∗ and Marie Coppola2,3
    Focus Article What sign language creation teaches us about language Diane Brentari1∗ and Marie Coppola2,3 How do languages emerge? What are the necessary ingredients and circumstances that permit new languages to form? Various researchers within the disciplines of primatology, anthropology, psychology, and linguistics have offered different answers to this question depending on their perspective. Language acquisition, language evolution, primate communication, and the study of spoken varieties of pidgin and creoles address these issues, but in this article we describe a relatively new and important area that contributes to our understanding of language creation and emergence. Three types of communication systems that use the hands and body to communicate will be the focus of this article: gesture, homesign systems, and sign languages. The focus of this article is to explain why mapping the path from gesture to homesign to sign language has become an important research topic for understanding language emergence, not only for the field of sign languages, but also for language in general. © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. How to cite this article: WIREs Cogn Sci 2012. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1212 INTRODUCTION linguistic community, a language model, and a 21st century mind/brain that well-equip the child for this esearchers in a variety of disciplines offer task. When the very first languages were created different, mostly partial, answers to the question, R the social and physiological conditions were very ‘What are the stages of language creation?’ Language different. Spoken language pidgin varieties can also creation can refer to any number of phylogenic and shed some light on the question of language creation.
    [Show full text]
  • Page 1 E X T R a P O S I T I O N a N D P R O N O M I N a L
    EXTRAPOSITION AND PRONOMINAL AGREEMENT IN SEMITIC LANGUAGES By GEOFFREY ALLAN KHAN Submitted for the degree of PhD at the School of Oriental and African Studies, University of London. ProQuest Number: 10673220 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a com plete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. uest ProQuest 10673220 Published by ProQuest LLC(2017). Copyright of the Dissertation is held by the Author. All rights reserved. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States C ode Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. ProQuest LLC. 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106- 1346 - 2 - ABSTRACT This thesis is a study of extraposition and pronominal agreement in Semitic languages. By the term 'extraposition* I understand the syntactic construction in which a noun or nominal phrase stands isolated at the front of the clause without any formal connection to the predication. The grammatical relation of the nominal is usually indicated vicariously by means of a co-referential resumptive pronoun, e.g. (Arabic) Zaydun *abu-hu tajirun "Zayd - his father is a merchant”. 'Pronominal agreement' is a construction where a noun or nominal phrase whose grammatical rela.tion is indicated by its case inflection or by an adjoining relational particle is accompanied in the same clause by a co-referential pronoun agreeing with it in number, gender, person, and grammatical relation, e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • American Sign Language (ASL) 1
    American Sign Language (ASL) 1 American Sign Language (ASL) Courses ASL A101 Elementary American Sign Language I 4 Credits Introductory course for students with no previous knowledge of American Sign Language (ASL). Develops receptive and expressive signing skills in ASL for effective communication at the elementary level. Students gain understanding of basic cross-cultural perspectives. Special Note: Course conducted in American Sign Language. Attributes: UAA Humanities GER. ASL A102 Elementary American Sign Language II 4 Credits Continuation of introductory course. Further develops elementary receptive and expressive signing skills in American Sign Language for effective communication. Enhances appreciation of cross-cultural perspectives. Special Note: Course conducted in American Sign Language. Prerequisites: ASL A101 with a minimum grade of C. Attributes: UAA Humanities GER. ASL A201 Intermediate American Sign Language I 4 Credits Intermediate course for students with basic knowledge of American Sign Language. Enhances receptive and expressive signing skills for effective communication at the intermediate level. Students critically examine diverse cultural perspectives. Special Note: Course conducted in American Sign Language. Prerequisites: ASL A102 with a minimum grade of C. Attributes: UAA Humanities GER. ASL A202 Intermediate American Sign Language II 4 Credits Continuation of first semester in intermediate American Sign Language (ASL). Further develops receptive and expressive signing proficiency for effective communication and in preparation for advanced study of ASL. Students interpret diverse cultural perspectives. Special Note: Course conducted in American Sign Language. Prerequisites: ASL A201 with a minimum grade of C. Attributes: UAA Humanities GER..
    [Show full text]
  • Chimpanzees Use of Sign Language
    Chimpanzees’ Use of Sign Language* ROGER S. FOUTS & DEBORAH H. FOUTS Washoe was cross-fostered by humans.1 She was raised as if she were a deaf human child and so acquired the signs of American Sign Language. Her surrogate human family had been the only people she had really known. She had met other humans who occasionally visited and often seen unfamiliar people over the garden fence or going by in cars on the busy residential street that ran next to her home. She never had a pet but she had seen dogs at a distance and did not appear to like them. While on car journeys she would hang out of the window and bang on the car door if she saw one. Dogs were obviously not part of 'our group'; they were different and therefore not to be trusted. Cats fared no better. The occasional cat that might dare to use her back garden as a shortcut was summarily chased out. Bugs were not favourites either. They were to be avoided or, if that was impossible, quickly flicked away. Washoe had accepted the notion of human superiority very readily - almost too readily. Being superior has a very heady quality about it. When Washoe was five she left most of her human companions behind and moved to a primate institute in Oklahoma. The facility housed about twenty-five chimpanzees, and this was where Washoe was to meet her first chimpanzee: imagine never meeting a member of your own species until you were five. After a plane flight Washoe arrived in a sedated state at her new home.
    [Show full text]
  • Ethnologue: Languages of Honduras Twentieth Edition Data
    Ethnologue: Languages of Honduras Twentieth edition data Gary F. Simons and Charles D. Fennig, Editors Based on information from the Ethnologue, 20th edition: Simons, Gary F. and Charles D. Fennig (eds.). 2017. Ethnologue: Languages of the World, Twentieth edition. Dallas, Texas: SIL International. Online: http://www.ethnologue.com. For personal use only Permission to distribute or reuse this work (in whole or in part) may be obtained through the Copyright Clearance Center at http://www.copyright.com. SIL International, 7500 West Camp Wisdom Road, Dallas, Texas 75236-5699 USA Web: www.sil.org, Phone: +1 972 708 7404, Email: [email protected] Ethnologue: Languages of Honduras 2 Contents List of Abbreviations 3 How to Use This Digest 4 Country Overview 6 Language Status Profile 7 Statistical Summaries 8 Alphabetical Listing of Languages 11 Language Map 14 Languages by Population 15 Languages by Status 16 Languages by Department 18 Languages by Family 19 Language Code Index 20 Language Name Index 21 Bibliography 22 Copyright © 2017 by SIL International All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, redistributed, or transmitted in any form or by any means—electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise—without the prior written permission of SIL International, with the exception of brief excerpts in articles or reviews. Ethnologue: Languages of Honduras 3 List of Abbreviations A Agent in constituent word order alt. alternate name for alt. dial. alternate dialect name for C Consonant in canonical syllable patterns CDE Convention against Discrimination in Education (1960) Class Language classification CPPDCE Convention on the Protection and Promotion of the Diversity of Cultural Expressions (2005) CSICH Convention for the Safeguarding of Intangible Cultural Heritage (2003) dial.
    [Show full text]