Is Lethal Violence an Integral Part of Chimpanzee Society? Like It Or Not, Yes

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Is Lethal Violence an Integral Part of Chimpanzee Society? Like It Or Not, Yes Is lethal violence an integral part of chimpanzee society? Like it or not, yes. Kevin D. Hunt Indiana University In her March 29, 2011 blog post, “Male chimps and humans are genetically violent— NOT!” Darcia Narvaez asks “is violence in our genes? Do chimpanzees in the wild want to kill others?” She concludes that evidence that lethal violence plays a critical role in chimpanzee society is flimsy. Narvaez, who is a psychologist by training, relied on a 2009 book Donna Hart and Bob Sussman and a 20‐year‐old book by Margaret Power for her information. These authors maintain that deadly chimp‐on‐chimp violence is so rare we can conclude little from the few instances where it has been seen, and that in many cases the evidence is over‐interpreted and exaggerated. Full disclosure: Richard Wrangham, a coauthor of Demonic Males, a book many view as the quintessence of the chimps‐are‐murderers view, was my doctoral co‐supervisor. In responding to the rarity of our observations of chimpanzee murders, I can’t help returning to that old saw, the proverbial Martian anthropologist sent to earth disguised as a human to study human society. Let’s imagine she logs many hours studying people in my hometown, Bloomington. While her language and cognitive capacities are bizarrely different from ours, leaving it nearly impossible for her to understand human language or even to de‐code the written word, she is a keen observer. I see her around town often at public gatherings, watching people, making notes. Meanwhile, unbeknownst to me, there are other Martians studying humans, too. Like my Martian, they’ve found humans to be a relatively peaceful lot, the occasional murder notwithstanding. In a breakthrough, one day I manage to communicate with my Martian friend, teaching her just a few words of English but mostly communicating through pointing and gesturing. Relying on my information, she returns to Mars to offer the astonishing theory that human society is divided into large units she calls ‘nations.’ Nations exist, she hypothesizes, in part as coalitions to protect their members against aggression from other such groups. Humans invest enormous resources in building special killing tools, ‘weapons,’ and maintain thousands of their members in readiness to defend their ‘nation’ against other potentially hostile such groups. She suggests that violence occurs most often when members of one coalition cross an invisible line, a ‘border’ between two ‘nations,’ and such incursions can act like a spark in a gas‐filled room. Even when only a few individuals are killed, border crossings can escalate into an incredible mobilization of people and machinery, leading to mass killings and devastation of whole cities. 100s of thousands, even millions of humans may be arrayed against one another once the killing starts. Her informant has told her, my Martian relates, of many such border incursions and mass killings, including one less than 75 years ago in which 40 million humans died, a ‘war’ that resulted in large parts of ‘Europe’ and ‘Japan’ being leveled. Our Martian anthropologist finds her hypothesis greeted with great skepticism, even scorn. Other anthropologists have studied humans for thousands—tens of thousands— of Martian‐hours, at 100s of sites randomly distributed across the globe, and have seen no evidence of such mass killings or ‘border crossings.’ A quick examination of ‘Europe’ and ‘Japan’ shows no evidence of devastation. And where are these ‘weapons’? Are we to believe the incredible coincidence that they just happen all to be stored in those few places the humans seem to be very touchy about outsiders visiting, these so called ‘military bases’? How convenient! No, Martian scholars conclude, humans form nations only to pool resources to build highways, regulate trade and control the miniscule number of humans who truly are violent. This ‘war’ hypothesis is a just‐so story, cobbled together from over‐ extrapolating from paltry data. Lesson: a behavior may be extremely important even if it is rare and difficult to observe. Not surprisingly, to me at least, chimpanzee lethal violence is rare and similarly difficult to observe. Despite this difficulty, there is irrefutable evidence that the threat of lethal violence has exerted a strong evolutionary force on chimpanzee nature, and its effects are visible on a minute‐to‐minute basis in chimpanzee society. It is the origin of the very unusual social bonding among male chimpanzees—they must to hang together to protect one another against extra‐group murderers. Despite the rarity of deadly violence—it takes 15 years to grow a chimp; if every community lost a chimp a month, there would soon be no chimps—evidence of it is far from paltry, including one chimpanzee killing has been documented on film, from first wraa‐bark to the dead body lying on the ground. Those who disagree with my perspective attempt to throw doubt on individual data points by citing a few episodes where the cause of death truly is doubtful, as if these few cases were all the scientific community has to rely on in our attempt to understand lethal violence. Let’s consider the evidence from the four chimpanzee populations that are perhaps the best‐studied, Gombe, Mahale (both in Tanzania), Tai (Ivory Coast) and Kibale (Uganda). Some maintain that Jane Goodall failed to find violence among Gombe chimpanzees on her own, and it was only when other more bloody‐minded scholars arrived on the scene that killings became a focus of attention. A quick perusal of Goodall’s 1986 chapter on aggression in The Chimpanzees of Gombe will show that this is not true. Moreover, Goodall was a coauthor on a 2008 publication by Williams and colleagues that documented the cause of death in a staggering 130 cases at Gombe. Williams, Goodall and colleagues found that after illness (58% of deaths), murder by other chimpanzees was the most common cause of death (20% of deaths). At Mahale Nishida (1996) reported on circumstances that he felt were clear evidence that chimpanzee Ntologi had been lethally attacked by members of his own community. I mention this one incident because Nishida’s description of it is so compelling. In the 70s Nishida and his colleagues watched as every male member of Nishida’s original study group, the so‐called K‐group, disappeared; he and his colleagues believe Ntologi’s community, the M‐group, killed most of them. At Tai, Boesch and colleagues published an article in 2008 documenting intergroup killings in which they wrote, “in the past 3 years, two cases of fatal intercommunity attacks have been observed;” they recount one attack that lasted 39 minutes. When the victim was near death, an attacking male “bit his arm and the noise of the breaking bones could be heard” meters away, after which “the victim seemed dead.” At Kibale‐Ngogo Mitani, Watts and colleagues report that they “observed the Ngogo chimpanzees kill or fatally wound 18 individuals from other groups.” Mitani and Watt’s team taped from beginning to end one horrifically brutal attack in which the victim died. Richard Wrangham and Dale Peterson’s book Demonic Males is commonly cited as the epitome of the exaggerated chimpanzee violence genre, so I have avoided discussing Wrangham and colleagues’ comprehensive review of lethal violence among chimpanzees, “Comparative rates of violence in chimpanzees and humans.” I find their argument, based on data from 75 chimp‐on‐chimp killings, to be utterly convincing. It is to me a clinching observation that no scholar who specializes on chimpanzee social behavior, no scholar who has studied them in the wild, doubts that chimpanzees can be murderously violent, and that chimpanzee society cannot be understood without considering the role of violence plays in their social organization. I completely agree with Dr. Narvaez when she writes that when it comes to violence, “we have ourselves to blame, not selfish genes, not evolution,” and when she goes on to suggest that “we can change the practices and beliefs” to become nonviolent. However, so also would many of those who have documented violence among primates agree with her. Certainly Wrangham agrees with her on this point. In Demonic Males Wrangham and his co‐author Peterson wrote “we are blessed with an intelligence that can…draw us away” from violence, from a demonic evolutionary history. References Christophe Boesch, Catherine Crockford, Ilka Herbinger, Roman Wittig, Yasmin Moebius, and Emmanuelle Normand (2008). Intergroup conflicts among chimpanzees in Taï National Park: lethal violence and the female perspective. American Journal of Primatology. 70: 519‐532. Jane Goodall, (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Donna Hart and Robert W. Sussman (2009). Man the Hunted: Primates, Predators, and Human Evolution. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. John C. Mitani, David P. Watts, DP; Sylvia J. Amsler (2010) Lethal intergroup aggression leads to territorial expansion in wild chimpanzees. Current Biology 12: R507‐R508. Darcia Narvaez (2011). Male chimps and humans are genetically violent‐‐‐NOT! http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/moral‐landscapes/201103/male‐chimps‐and‐ humans‐are‐genetically‐violent‐not Toshisada Nishida (1996). The death of Ntologi, the unparalleled leader of M Group. Pan Africa News 2(2): 9‐11. Margaret Power (1991). The Egalitarians, Humans and Chimpanzees: An Anthropological View of Social Organization. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. J.M. Williams, E.V. Lonsdorf, M.L. Wilson, J. Schumacher‐Stankey, J. Goodall, and A.E. Pusey (2008). Causes of death in the Kasekela chimpanzees of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. American Journal of Primatology 70: 766‐777. Richard Wrangham & D. Peterson (1996). Demonic Males: Apes and Origins of Human Violence. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Richard W. Wrangham, Michael L. Wilson and Martin N.
Recommended publications
  • What, If Anything, Is a Darwinian Anthropology?
    JONATHAN MARKS What, if anything, is a Darwinian anthropology? Not too many years ago, I was scanning the job advertisements in anthropology and stumbled upon one for a faculty post in a fairly distinguished department in California. The ad specified that they were looking for someone who ‘studied culture from an evolutionary perspective’. I was struck by that, because it seemed to me that the alternative would be a creationist perspective, and I had never heard of anyone in this century who did that. Obviously my initial reading was incorrect. That department specifically wanted someone with a particular methodological and ideo- logical orientation; ‘evolutionary perspective’ was there as a code for something else. It has fascinated me for a number of years that Darwin stands as a very powerful symbol in biology. On the one hand, he represents the progressive aspect of science in its perpetual struggle against the perceived oppressive forces of Christianity (Larson 1997); and on the other, he represents as well the prevailing stodgy and stultified scientific orthodoxy against which any new bold and original theory must cast itself (Gould 1980). Proponents of the neutral theory (King and Jukes 1969) or of punctuated equilibria (Eldredge 1985) represented themselves as Darwinists to the outside worlds, and as anti-Darwinists to the inside world. Thus, Darwinism can be both the new and improved ideology you should bring home today, and is also the superseded Brand X ideology. That is indeed a powerful metaphor, to represent something as well as its opposite. Curiously, nobody ever told me in my scientific training that scientific progress was somehow predicated on the development of powerful metaphors.
    [Show full text]
  • Bringing in Darwin Bradley A. Thayer
    Bringing in Darwin Bradley A. Thayer Evolutionary Theory, Realism, and International Politics Efforts to develop a foundation for scientiªc knowledge that would unite the natural and social sci- ences date to the classical Greeks. Given recent advances in genetics and evolu- tionary theory, this goal may be closer than ever.1 The human genome project has generated much media attention as scientists reveal genetic causes of dis- eases and some aspects of human behavior. And although advances in evolu- tionary theory may have received less attention, they are no less signiªcant. Edward O. Wilson, Roger Masters, and Albert Somit, among others, have led the way in using evolutionary theory and social science to produce a synthesis for understanding human behavior and social phenomena.2 This synthesis posits that human behavior is simultaneously and inextricably a result of evo- lutionary and environmental causes. The social sciences, including the study of international politics, may build upon this scholarship.3 In this article I argue that evolutionary theory can improve the realist theory of international politics. Traditional realist arguments rest principally on one of two discrete ultimate causes, or intellectual foundations. The ªrst is Reinhold Niebuhr’s argument that humans are evil. The second is grounded in the work Bradley A. Thayer is an Assistant Professor of Political Science at the University of Minnesota—Duluth. I am grateful to Mlada Bukovansky, Stephen Chilton, Christopher Layne, Michael Mastanduno, Roger Masters, Paul Sharp, Alexander Wendt, Mike Winnerstig, and Howard Wriggins for their helpful comments. I thank Nathaniel Fick, David Hawkins, Jeremy Joseph, Christopher Kwak, Craig Nerenberg, and Jordana Phillips for their able research assistance.
    [Show full text]
  • The Descent of Edward Wilson
    prospectmagazine.co.uk http://www.prospectmagazine.co.uk/magazine/edward-wilson-social-conquest- earth-evolutionary-errors-origin-species/ The descent of Edward Wilson A new book on evolution by a great biologist makes a slew of mistakes The Social Conquest of Earth By Edward O Wilson (WW Norton, £18.99, May) When he received the manuscript of The Origin of Species, John Murray, the publisher, sent it to a referee who suggested that Darwin should jettison all that evolution stuff and concentrate on pigeons. It’s funny in the same way as the spoof review of Lady Chatterley’s Lover, which praised its interesting “passages on pheasant raising, the apprehending of poachers, ways of controlling vermin, and other chores and duties of the professional gamekeeper” but added: “Unfortunately one is obliged to wade through many pages of extraneous material in order to discover and savour these sidelights on the management of a Midland shooting estate, and in this reviewer’s opinion this book can not take the place of JR Miller’s Practical Gamekeeping.” I am not being funny when I say of Edward Wilson’s latest book that there are interesting and informative chapters on human evolution, and on the ways of social insects (which he knows better than any man alive), and it was a good idea to write a book comparing these two pinnacles of social evolution, but unfortunately one is obliged to wade through many pages of erroneous and downright perverse misunderstandings of evolutionary theory. In particular, Wilson now rejects “kin selection” (I shall explain this below) and replaces it with a revival of “group selection”—the poorly defined and incoherent view that evolution is driven by the differential survival of whole groups of organisms.
    [Show full text]
  • Title MAHALE and GOMBE COMPARED: PATTERNS
    CORE Metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk Provided by Kyoto University Research Information Repository MAHALE AND GOMBE COMPARED: PATTERNS OF Title RESEARCH ON WILD CHIMPANZEES IN TANZANIA OVER FOUR DECADES Author(s) McGREW, W.C. Citation African Study Monographs (2007), 28(3): 143-153 Issue Date 2007-09 URL http://dx.doi.org/10.14989/68260 Right Type Departmental Bulletin Paper Textversion publisher Kyoto University African Study Monographs, 28(3): 143-153, September 2007 143 MAHALE AND GOMBE COMPARED: PATTERNS OF RESEARCH ON WILD CHIMPANZEES IN TANZANIA OVER FOUR DECADES W.C. McGREW Departments of Anthropology and Zoology, Miami University and Leverhulme Centre for Human Evolutionary Studies, University of Cambridge ABSTRACT Students of science have contrasted Japanese and Western primatology. This paper aims to test such claims by comparing two long-term African field projects, Mahale and Gombe, in terms of research productivity as measured by scientific publications. Gombe, directed by Jane Goodall since 1960, and Mahale, directed by Toshisada Nishida since 1965, have much in common, in addition to their main focus on the eastern chimpanzee, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii. They have produced similar total numbers of journal articles, books and chapters since the projects were founded. When these are categorized by subject matter, the main topics make up a similar proportion of publications, e.g. social relations, behavioural ecology, sex and reproduction, etc. Although most research output is on similar subjects, there are important differences between the sites, e.g. Mahale emphasizing medici- nal plant use, Gombe predominating in modelling human evolution. Both sites favour pub- lishing in Primates among the specialist primatological journals, but important differences exist in publishing elsewhere.
    [Show full text]
  • Pan Africa News 15(1) PDF(651Kb)
    Pan Africa News TheThe Newsletter Newsletter of the of theCommittee Committee for forthe theCare Care andand Conservation Conservation of ofChimpanzees, Chimpanzees, and and the the MahaleMahale Wildlife Wildlife Conservation Conservation Society Society JUNE 2008 VOL. 15, NO. 1 P. A . N . EDITORIAL STAFF Contents Chief Editor: <NEWS> Toshisada Nishida, Japan Monkey Centre, Japan Drs. Jane Goodall and Toshisada Nishida win 2008 Leakey Deputy Chief Editors: Prize! / Dr. Toshisada Nishida wins 2008 IPS Lifetime Kazuhiko Hosaka, Kamakura Women’s University, Japan Achievement Award! 1 Michio Nakamura, Kyoto University, Japan <ARTICLE> Associate Editors: Hunting with tools by Mahale chimpanzees Christophe Boesch, Max-Planck Institute, Germany Michio Nakamura & Noriko Itoh 3 Jane Goodall, Jane Goodall Institute, USA Takayoshi Kano, Kyoto University, Japan <NOTE> Tetsuro Matsuzawa, Kyoto University, Japan Snare removal by a chimpanzee of the Sonso community, William C. McGrew, University of Cambridge, UK Budongo Forest (Uganda) John C. Mitani, University of Michigan, USA Stephen Amati, Fred Babweteera & Roman M Wittig 6 Vernon Reynolds, Budongo Forest Project, UK <NOTE> Yukimaru Sugiyama, Kyoto University, Japan Use of wet hair to capture swarming termites by a Richard W. Wrangham, Harvard University, USA chimpanzee in Mahale, Tanzania Editorial Secretaries: Mieko Kiyono-Fuse 8 Noriko Itoh, Japan Monkey Centre, Japan <FORUM> Koichiro Zamma, GARI Hayashibara, Japan Why were guava trees cut down in Mahale Park? The Agumi Inaba, Japan Monkey Centre, Japan question of exterminating all introduced plants Toshisada Nishida 12 Instructions for Authors: Pan Africa News publishes articles, notes, reviews, forums, news, essays, book reviews, letters to <NEWS> editor, and classified ads (restricted to non-profit organizations) on any aspect of conservation and Drs.
    [Show full text]
  • Human Adaptation to the Control of Fire
    Human Adaptation to the Control of Fire The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters Citation Wrangham, Richard W., and Rachel Naomi Carmody. 2010. Human adaptation to the control of fire. Evolutionary Anthropology 19(5): 187–199. Published Version doi:10.1002/evan.20275 Citable link http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:8944723 Terms of Use This article was downloaded from Harvard University’s DASH repository, and is made available under the terms and conditions applicable to Open Access Policy Articles, as set forth at http:// nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:dash.current.terms-of- use#OAP Human adaptation to the control of fire Richard Wrangham* and Rachel Carmody Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University 11 Divinity Avenue, Cambridge, MA 02138 USA For Evolutionary Anthropology * Corresponding Author Telephone: +1-617-495-5948 Fax: +1-617-496-8041 email: [email protected] Text Pages: 23 (pp. 3-25) References: 95 (pp. 26-35) Figures and Legends: 4 (pp. 36-41) Text boxes: 1 (pp. 42-45) Words: 10,479 Key words: cooking, life history, anatomy, behavior, cognition 1 About the authors: Richard Wrangham is a professor in the Department of Human Evolutionary Biology at Harvard University. Since 1987 he has directed a study of chimpanzee behavioral ecology in Kibale National Park, Uganda (currently co-director with Martin Muller). He is the author of Catching Fire: How Cooking Made Us Human (2009, Basic Books). E-mail: [email protected]. Rachel Carmody is a Ph.D.
    [Show full text]
  • Primates, Plants, and Parasites: the Evolution of Animal Self-Medication and Ethnomedicine - Michael A
    ETHNOPHARMACOLOGY - Vol. II - Primates, Plants, and Parasites: The Evolution of Animal Self-Medication and Ethnomedicine - Michael A. Huffman, Sylvia K. Vitazkova PRIMATES, PLANTS, AND PARASITES: THE EVOLUTION OF ANIMAL SELF-MEDICATION AND ETHNOMEDICINE Michael A. Huffman, Section of Ecology, Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, JAPAN Sylvia K. Vitazkova, EPSCoR, University of Virgin Islands, 2 Brewer’s Bay, St. Thomas, USVI, USA. Keywords: African great apes, zoopharmacognosy, self-medication, diet, ethnomedicine, parasite control, evolution, phytochemistry. Contents 1. Introduction 2. Animal self-medication and ethnomedicine 3. The impact of parasites on the evolution of self-medicative behavior 4. Food as medicine in animals and humans 5. Use of plants as medicine by chimpanzees in the wild 5.1. Whole leaf swallowing and the physical expulsion of parasite 5.2. Vernonia amygdalina and bitter pith chewing behavior 5.3. The ethnomedicine and phytochemistry of bitter pith chewing 6. A link between animal self-medication and ethnomedicine 7. Tongwe ethnozoology and health care 8. Future studies and directions of research Acknowledgments Glossary Bibliography Biographical Sketches Summary Early in the co-evolution of plant–animal relationships, some arthropod species began to utilize the chemical defenses of plants to protect themselves from their own predators and parasites. It is likely thus that the origins of herbal medicine have their roots deep within the animalUNESCO kingdom. Humans have looked – to wildEOLSS and domestic animals for sources of herbal remedies since prehistoric times. Both folklore and living examples provide accounts of howSAMPLE medicinal plants were obtained CHAPTERS by observing the behavior of animals. Animals too learn about the details of self-medication by watching each other.
    [Show full text]
  • The Human Family—Its Evolutionary Context and Diversity
    social sciences $€ £ ¥ Review The Human Family—Its Evolutionary Context and Diversity Karen L. Kramer Department of Anthropology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT 84112, USA; [email protected] Abstract: The family defines many aspects of our daily lives, and expresses a wide array of forms across individuals, cultures, ecologies and time. While the nuclear family is the norm today in developed economies, it is the exception in most other historic and cultural contexts. Yet, many aspects of how humans form the economic and reproductive groups that we recognize as families are distinct to our species. This review pursues three goals: to overview the evolutionary context in which the human family developed, to expand the conventional view of the nuclear family as the ‘traditional family’, and to provide an alternative to patrifocal explanations for family formation. To do so, first those traits that distinguish the human family are reviewed with an emphasis on the key contributions that behavioral ecology has made toward understanding dynamics within and between families, including life history, kin selection, reciprocity and conflict theoretical frameworks. An overview is then given of several seminal debates about how the family took shape, with an eye toward a more nuanced view of male parental care as the basis for family formation, and what cooperative breeding has to offer as an alternative perspective. Keywords: behavioral ecology; family studies; cooperative breeding; patrilineal Citation: Kramer, Karen L. 2021. The 1. Introduction Human Family—Its Evolutionary Family formation shapes many aspects of our daily lives—who we work, eat and sleep Context and Diversity.
    [Show full text]
  • Title Tribute to Prof. Toshisada Nishida Author(S) Reynolds
    Title Tribute to Prof. Toshisada Nishida Author(s) Reynolds, Vernon Citation Pan Africa News (2011), 18(special issue): 2-2 Issue Date 2011-09 URL http://hdl.handle.net/2433/147294 Right Copyright © Pan Africa News. Type Article Textversion publisher Kyoto University 2 Pan Africa News, 18 special issue, September 2011 His passing cannot be mourned too much. He was a pio- neer in the study of wild chimpanzees. Since 1965, he Tribute to Prof. Toshisada maintained research at Mahale, Tanzania, and accumu- Nishida lated accomplishments unique to his team, rivaling an- other longtime chimpanzee researcher, Dr. Jane Goodall Vernon Reynolds whose work at Gombe is well known. He published many Oxford University, UK/Budongo Conservation Field Station excellent papers, for example, on patrilineal structure of I first met Prof. Nishida (“Toshi” as we came to know chimpanzee society, political strategy among males, and him) on a visit to the Mahale Mountains chimpanzee scientific documentation of newly discovered cultural project which I made in the late 1970s. I met up with behaviors. He was awarded the Leakey Prize and the In- my colleague Yuki Sugiyama in Dar es Salaam and we ternational Primatological Society Lifetime Achievement travelled across Tanzania to Kigoma. There we were sup- Award. In addition, he served as President of the Inter- posed to meet a boat from the chimp project which would national Primatological Society. These honors tell how take us down Lake Tanganyika to Mahale. But it turned highly he was esteemed internationally. out that the boat’s outboard motor was broken. We waited a few days during which we met Toshi and spent some time with him, talking about his work and the Mahale chimpanzees.
    [Show full text]
  • Pan Africa News 16(1)
    Pan Africa News TheThe Newsletter Newsletter of the of theCommittee Committee for forthe theCare Care andand Conservation Conservation of ofChimpanzees, Chimpanzees, and and the the MahaleMahale Wildlife Wildlife Conservation Conservation Society Society JUNE 2009 VOL. 16, NO. 1 P. A. N. EDITORIAL STAFF Contents Chief Editor: <NOTE> Toshisada Nishida, Japan Monkey Centre, Japan Flu-like epidemics in wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Wamba, the Luo Scientific Reserve, Democratic Republic of Deputy Chief Editors: Congo Kazuhiko Hosaka, Kamakura Women’s University, Japan Tetsuya Sakamaki, Mbangi Mulavwa & Takeshi Furuichi 1 Michio Nakamura, Kyoto University, Japan <NOTE> Grooming Hand-Clasp by Chimpanzees of the Mugiri Associate Editors: Community, Toro-Semliki Wildlife Reserve, Uganda Christophe Boesch, Max-Planck Institute, Germany Tim H. Webster, Phineas R. Hodson & Kevin D. Hunt 5 Jane Goodall, Jane Goodall Institute, USA <NOTE> Takayoshi Kano, Kyoto University, Japan Preliminary Report on Hand-Clasp Grooming in Tetsuro Matsuzawa, Kyoto University, Japan Sanctuary-Released Chimpanzees, Haut Niger National William C. McGrew, University of Cambridge, UK Park, Guinea John C. Mitani, University of Michigan, USA Tatyana Humle, Christelle Colin & Estelle Raballand 7 Vernon Reynolds, Budongo Forest Project, UK <Book Review> Yukimaru Sugiyama, Kyoto University, Japan Raymond Corbey, The Metaphysics of Apes. Negotiating the Richard W. Wrangham, Harvard University, USA Animal-Human Boundary, Cambridge University Press, Editorial Secretaries: Cambridge 2005 Noriko Itoh, Japan Monkey Center, Japan Paola Cavalieri 10 Koichiro Zamma, Hayashibara GARI, Japan Agumi Inaba, Japan Monkey Centre, Japan <NOTE> Instructions for Authors: Pan Africa News publishes articles, notes, reviews, Flu-like Epidemics in Wild forums, news, essays, book reviews, letters to editor, and classified ads (restricted to non-profit Bonobos (Pan paniscus) at organizations) on any aspect of conservation and research regarding chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Wamba, the Luo Scientific and bilias (Pan paniscus).
    [Show full text]
  • The Role of Hunting in Human Origins
    anthropogeny tracksa CARTA newsletter vol. 6/1 - feb. 2018 Seen as a key human adaptation, hunting is thought to have influenced our anatomy, physiology and behavior. The Role While humans have been hunter-gatherers for most of our existence as a species, there is considerable uncertainty about where, when, why, and how our of Hunting early ancestors came to consume vertebrate meat on a regular basis. The goal of CARTA’s March 2, 2018 symposium, in Human The Role of Hunting in Anthropogeny, is to explore evidence pertaining to understanding the origins of hominin hunting in an attempt to focus research Origins agendas for the future. Turn the page for more details! Inside this Issue... Evolution 2017 CARTA- CARTA Member CARTA Endowment of Nervous Anthropogeny Inspired Awards & Symposium Progress Systems Field Course Publications Honors CARTA Symposium: Friday, March 2 1:00 - 5:30 pm (Pacific) The Role of Hunting Conrad T. Prebys Auditorium, Salk Institute in Anthropogeny FREE ADMISSION! LIVE WEBCAST! Each CARTA symposium Co-Chaired by features experts who James Moore, UC San Diego present on topics & Richard Wrangham, Harvard University addressing the origins of the human phenomenon. Featuring the following talks and speakers CARTA symposia are free and open to the public. Nutritional Significance of Meat If you’re curious about Alyssa Crittenden, University of Nevada, Las Vegas where we came from and how we got here, consider attending one of our events. Why Foragers Hunt Rebecca Bliege Bird, Pennsylvania State University For more information on this symposium, Pan the Hunter: Ecological Explanations for to register, to watch the live webcast, or Chimpanzee Predation for information on past and future events, Ian Gilby, Arizona State University please visit: Social Explanations for Chimpanzee Hunting https://carta.anthropogeny.
    [Show full text]
  • Richard Wranghani and Dale Peterson, from Demonic
    strength and instinct for aggression would gradually come to predominate in the population. Most cultural anthropologists from the 1950s onward rejected the expla- nation of human aggression in terms of instincts, emphasizing instead the social and cultural causes of violence. They pointed out that the amount of aggression tolerated varies widely from one society to another and that individ- uals can become aggressive or peaceful depending on how they are raised. They held up such groups as the Semai of Malaysia (see Robert K. Dentan, The Semai: A Nonviolent People of Malaya [Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 196811) as proof YES: Richard Wranghani and Dale Peterson, from Demonic that culture could create a people who abhorred all forms of aggression and Males: Apes and the Origins or Human Violence (Houghton Mifflin coercion. 'Whatever instinct for aggression humans might have, they argued, Company, 1996) must be very weak indeed. The question behind this issue concerns the nature of human nature. NO: Robert W. Sussman, from "Exploring Our Basic Human Have we a set of innate behavioral predispositions that, when set off by certain Nature," Anthro Notes (Fall 1997) stimuli, are very likely to be expressed? Or, are any such predispositions at best weak tendencies that can be shaped or even negated by cultural conditioning? Many apparently innate behaviors in humans turn out to be highly variable in ISSUE SUMMARY their strength and form. While the sucking instinct of babies operates predict- ably in all newborns, a hypothesized "mothering instinct" seems quite diverse YES: Biological anthropologist Richard Wrangham and science in expression and variable in strength from one woman to another.
    [Show full text]