Richard Wranghani and Dale Peterson, from Demonic

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Richard Wranghani and Dale Peterson, from Demonic strength and instinct for aggression would gradually come to predominate in the population. Most cultural anthropologists from the 1950s onward rejected the expla- nation of human aggression in terms of instincts, emphasizing instead the social and cultural causes of violence. They pointed out that the amount of aggression tolerated varies widely from one society to another and that individ- uals can become aggressive or peaceful depending on how they are raised. They held up such groups as the Semai of Malaysia (see Robert K. Dentan, The Semai: A Nonviolent People of Malaya [Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 196811) as proof YES: Richard Wranghani and Dale Peterson, from Demonic that culture could create a people who abhorred all forms of aggression and Males: Apes and the Origins or Human Violence (Houghton Mifflin coercion. 'Whatever instinct for aggression humans might have, they argued, Company, 1996) must be very weak indeed. The question behind this issue concerns the nature of human nature. NO: Robert W. Sussman, from "Exploring Our Basic Human Have we a set of innate behavioral predispositions that, when set off by certain Nature," Anthro Notes (Fall 1997) stimuli, are very likely to be expressed? Or, are any such predispositions at best weak tendencies that can be shaped or even negated by cultural conditioning? Many apparently innate behaviors in humans turn out to be highly variable in ISSUE SUMMARY their strength and form. While the sucking instinct of babies operates predict- ably in all newborns, a hypothesized "mothering instinct" seems quite diverse YES: Biological anthropologist Richard Wrangham and science in expression and variable in strength from one woman to another. The ques- writer Dale Peterson maintain that male humans and chimpanzees, tion, then, is whether the hypothesized aggression instinct is a powerful drive our closest nonhuman relatives, have an innate tendency to be that all human males must express in one way or another or a weak and pliable aggressive and to defend their territory by violence. They state that tendency that some males may never act upon. sexual selection, a type of natural selection, has fostered an instinct Richard Wrangham and Dale Peterson argue that human wars and for male aggression because males who are good fighters mate interpersonal violence are driven by inherited behavioral tendencies that have more frequently and sire more offspring than weaker and less evolved under pressure of sexual selection—natural selection of traits that aggressive ones. enhance the reproductive success of one sex. Among both humans and chim- panzees, males *ho are aggressive use their fighting ability to dominate other NO: Biological anthropologist Robert W. Sussman rejects the theory males and to prevent them from mating with the available females. Therefore that human aggression is an inherited propensity, arguing instead the genes causing aggression are passed on to succeeding generations in greater that violence is a product of culture and upbringing. He also rejects the contention that male chimpanzees routinely commit numbers than the genes causing nonaggressive behavior. violent acts against other male chimps. Sussman regards the Robert W. Sussman rejects Wrangham and Peterson's assertion that notion that human males are inherently violent as a Western natural selection favors aggressive human males and that aggressiveness is an cultural tradition, not a scientifically demonstrated fact. inherited tendency rather than a result of environment and upbringing. He contends that human hunter-gatherers and most apes are remarkably non- aggressive, as were our earliest human ancestors, the australopithecines. He h uman history is rife with wars between groups of all types, from clans to sees the evidence that chimpanzee males routinely attack other males as weak, nation-states, and interpersonal violence is common enough that all societies and he questions the relevance of chimpanzee behavior for humans. attempt to control it. But is violence a part of human nature, or is violence These selections raise a number of questions with regard to the nature merely one of many human capabilities that may be either encouraged or dis- and causes of human aggression. How can we determine whether a person's couraged by cultures? aggressive act is due to instinct, upbringing, or both? If male aggression in The question of whether or not humans have an instinct for aggres- humans is based on instinct, how can scholars explain variations in the sion has been around since the formative years of anthropology, and it is amount and type of aggression found in different individuals and cultures? still being debated today. Some nineteenth-century scholars interpreted If male aggression is not based on instinct, why is violence between men so Darwin's concepts of "natural selection" and "survival of the fittest" to widespread? What role did women have in the evolution of an instinct for mean that the strong would kill off the weak. The strongest and most male violence? To what extent can studies of animal behavior be applied aggressive individuals, therefore, would have the most offspring, and their to humans? 16 17 YES / Wrangham and Peterson 19 And yet science has still not grappled closely with the ultimate questions Richard Wrangham raised by the Kahama killings: Where does human violence come from, and and Dale Peterson (9 YES why? Of course, there have been great advances in the way we think about these things. Most importantly, in the 1970s, the same decade as the Kahama killings, a new evolutionary theory emerged, the selfish-gene theory of natural selection, variously called inclusive fitness theory, sociobiology, or more Demonic Males: Apes and the broadly, behavioral ecology. Sweeping through the halls of academe, it revolu- fionized Darwinian thinking by its insistence that the ultimate explanation of Origins of Human Violence any individual's behavior considers only how the behavior tends to maximize genetic success: to pass that individual's genes into subsequent generations. The new theory, elegantly popularized in Richard Dawkins's The Selfish Gene, is Paradise Lost now the conventional wisdom in biological science because it explains animal behavior so well. It accounts easily for selfishness, even killing. And it has come The killer ape has long been part of our popular culture: Tarzan had to escape to be applied with increasing confidence to human behavior, though the from the bad apes, and King Kong was a murderous gorilla-like monster. But debate is still hot and unsettled. In any case, the general principle that behavior before the Kahama observations [in which males of one chimpanzee group evolves to serve selfish ends has been widely accepted, and the idea that killed the males of a neighboring group], few biologists took the idea seriously, humans might have been favored by natural selection to hate and to kill their The reason was simple. There was so little evidence of animals killing members enemies has become entirely, if tragically, reasonable. of their own species that biologists used to think animals killed each other only when something went wrong—an accident, perhaps, or unnatural crowding in Those are the general principles, and yet the specifics are lacking. Most zoos. The idea fit with the theories of animal behavior then preeminent, theo- animals are nowhere near as violent as humans, so why did such intensely ries that saw animal behavior as designed by evolution for mutual good. violent behavior evolve particularly in the human line? Why kill the enemy, Darwinian natural selection was a filter supposed to eliminate murderous rather than simply drive him away? Why rape? Why torture and mutilate? violence. Killer apes, like killers in any animal species, were merely a novelist's Why do we see these patterns both in ourselves and chimpanzees? Those sorts fantasy to most scientists before the 1970s. of questions have barely been asked, much less addressed. And so the behavior of people seemed very, very different from that of Because chimpanzees and humans are each other's closest relatives, such other animals. Killing, of course, is a typical result of human war, so one had to questions carry extraordinary implications, the more so because the study of presume that humans somehow broke the rules of nature. Still, war must have early human ancestry, unfolding in a fervor as we approach the century's end, come from somewhere. It could have come, for example, from the evolution of is bringing chimpanzees and humans even closer than we ever imagined. brains that happened to be smart enough to think of using tools as weapons, as Three dramatic recent discoveries speak to the relationship between chim- Konrad Lorenz argued in his famous book, On Aggression, published in 1963, panzees and humans, and all three point in the same direction: to a past, However it may have originated, more generally war was seen as one of around 5 million years ago, when chimpanzee ancestors and human ancestors the defining marks of humanity: To fight wars meant to be human and apart were indistinguishable. from nature. This larger presumption was true even of nonscientific theories, First, fossils recently dug up in Ethiopia indicate that over 4.5 million such as the biblical concept of an original sin taking humans out of Eden, or years ago there walked across African lands a bipedal ancestor of humans with the notion that warfare was an idea implanted by aliens, as Arthur C. Clarke a head strikingly like a chimpanzee's. imagined in 2001: A Space Odyssey. In science, in religion, in fiction, violence Second, laboratories around the world have over the last decade demon- and humanity were twinned. strated chimpanzees to be genetically closer to us than they are even to gorillas, The Kahama killings were therefore both a shock and a stimulus to despite the close physical resemblance between chimpanzees and gorillas.
Recommended publications
  • What, If Anything, Is a Darwinian Anthropology?
    JONATHAN MARKS What, if anything, is a Darwinian anthropology? Not too many years ago, I was scanning the job advertisements in anthropology and stumbled upon one for a faculty post in a fairly distinguished department in California. The ad specified that they were looking for someone who ‘studied culture from an evolutionary perspective’. I was struck by that, because it seemed to me that the alternative would be a creationist perspective, and I had never heard of anyone in this century who did that. Obviously my initial reading was incorrect. That department specifically wanted someone with a particular methodological and ideo- logical orientation; ‘evolutionary perspective’ was there as a code for something else. It has fascinated me for a number of years that Darwin stands as a very powerful symbol in biology. On the one hand, he represents the progressive aspect of science in its perpetual struggle against the perceived oppressive forces of Christianity (Larson 1997); and on the other, he represents as well the prevailing stodgy and stultified scientific orthodoxy against which any new bold and original theory must cast itself (Gould 1980). Proponents of the neutral theory (King and Jukes 1969) or of punctuated equilibria (Eldredge 1985) represented themselves as Darwinists to the outside worlds, and as anti-Darwinists to the inside world. Thus, Darwinism can be both the new and improved ideology you should bring home today, and is also the superseded Brand X ideology. That is indeed a powerful metaphor, to represent something as well as its opposite. Curiously, nobody ever told me in my scientific training that scientific progress was somehow predicated on the development of powerful metaphors.
    [Show full text]
  • Bringing in Darwin Bradley A. Thayer
    Bringing in Darwin Bradley A. Thayer Evolutionary Theory, Realism, and International Politics Efforts to develop a foundation for scientiªc knowledge that would unite the natural and social sci- ences date to the classical Greeks. Given recent advances in genetics and evolu- tionary theory, this goal may be closer than ever.1 The human genome project has generated much media attention as scientists reveal genetic causes of dis- eases and some aspects of human behavior. And although advances in evolu- tionary theory may have received less attention, they are no less signiªcant. Edward O. Wilson, Roger Masters, and Albert Somit, among others, have led the way in using evolutionary theory and social science to produce a synthesis for understanding human behavior and social phenomena.2 This synthesis posits that human behavior is simultaneously and inextricably a result of evo- lutionary and environmental causes. The social sciences, including the study of international politics, may build upon this scholarship.3 In this article I argue that evolutionary theory can improve the realist theory of international politics. Traditional realist arguments rest principally on one of two discrete ultimate causes, or intellectual foundations. The ªrst is Reinhold Niebuhr’s argument that humans are evil. The second is grounded in the work Bradley A. Thayer is an Assistant Professor of Political Science at the University of Minnesota—Duluth. I am grateful to Mlada Bukovansky, Stephen Chilton, Christopher Layne, Michael Mastanduno, Roger Masters, Paul Sharp, Alexander Wendt, Mike Winnerstig, and Howard Wriggins for their helpful comments. I thank Nathaniel Fick, David Hawkins, Jeremy Joseph, Christopher Kwak, Craig Nerenberg, and Jordana Phillips for their able research assistance.
    [Show full text]
  • The Descent of Edward Wilson
    prospectmagazine.co.uk http://www.prospectmagazine.co.uk/magazine/edward-wilson-social-conquest- earth-evolutionary-errors-origin-species/ The descent of Edward Wilson A new book on evolution by a great biologist makes a slew of mistakes The Social Conquest of Earth By Edward O Wilson (WW Norton, £18.99, May) When he received the manuscript of The Origin of Species, John Murray, the publisher, sent it to a referee who suggested that Darwin should jettison all that evolution stuff and concentrate on pigeons. It’s funny in the same way as the spoof review of Lady Chatterley’s Lover, which praised its interesting “passages on pheasant raising, the apprehending of poachers, ways of controlling vermin, and other chores and duties of the professional gamekeeper” but added: “Unfortunately one is obliged to wade through many pages of extraneous material in order to discover and savour these sidelights on the management of a Midland shooting estate, and in this reviewer’s opinion this book can not take the place of JR Miller’s Practical Gamekeeping.” I am not being funny when I say of Edward Wilson’s latest book that there are interesting and informative chapters on human evolution, and on the ways of social insects (which he knows better than any man alive), and it was a good idea to write a book comparing these two pinnacles of social evolution, but unfortunately one is obliged to wade through many pages of erroneous and downright perverse misunderstandings of evolutionary theory. In particular, Wilson now rejects “kin selection” (I shall explain this below) and replaces it with a revival of “group selection”—the poorly defined and incoherent view that evolution is driven by the differential survival of whole groups of organisms.
    [Show full text]
  • Human Adaptation to the Control of Fire
    Human Adaptation to the Control of Fire The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters Citation Wrangham, Richard W., and Rachel Naomi Carmody. 2010. Human adaptation to the control of fire. Evolutionary Anthropology 19(5): 187–199. Published Version doi:10.1002/evan.20275 Citable link http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:8944723 Terms of Use This article was downloaded from Harvard University’s DASH repository, and is made available under the terms and conditions applicable to Open Access Policy Articles, as set forth at http:// nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:dash.current.terms-of- use#OAP Human adaptation to the control of fire Richard Wrangham* and Rachel Carmody Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University 11 Divinity Avenue, Cambridge, MA 02138 USA For Evolutionary Anthropology * Corresponding Author Telephone: +1-617-495-5948 Fax: +1-617-496-8041 email: [email protected] Text Pages: 23 (pp. 3-25) References: 95 (pp. 26-35) Figures and Legends: 4 (pp. 36-41) Text boxes: 1 (pp. 42-45) Words: 10,479 Key words: cooking, life history, anatomy, behavior, cognition 1 About the authors: Richard Wrangham is a professor in the Department of Human Evolutionary Biology at Harvard University. Since 1987 he has directed a study of chimpanzee behavioral ecology in Kibale National Park, Uganda (currently co-director with Martin Muller). He is the author of Catching Fire: How Cooking Made Us Human (2009, Basic Books). E-mail: [email protected]. Rachel Carmody is a Ph.D.
    [Show full text]
  • Spring 2009 SPRING 2009
    Spring 2009 SPRING 2009 From the Director CONTENTS SINCE 1893, UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA PRESS has enriched General Interest 2 lives and contributed to the public good by fueling intellectual California 34 and creative endeavor. Poetry 38 This season follows tradition with a catalog full of books that build fields of knowledge, suggest solutions for challeng- Anthropology 40 ing environmental and social problems, and educate students, Sociology 45 policymakers, and curious readers alike. Our lead book, Elephant Reflections by Karl Ammann and History 50 Dale Peterson, illuminates the history and conservation of this Classics 57 singular creature. Other comprehensive reference volumes doc- Religion 60 ument the world’s wildlife, oceans, islands, and natural resources. A number of authors take on current issues such as organic Science 64 farming, human trafficking, the war on terror, and drug addic- GAIA/Series Monographs 69 tion, while historical studies reveal new information about topics Art 70 as diverse as ice cream, environmental change, the pineapple industry, Alcatraz, punk music, and Khubilai Khan’s fleet. Music 73 We also offer a cluster of biographies of iconic figures Walt Media 74 Whitman, Wallace Stegner, and Leonard Bernstein, as well as new works by returning authors Kevin Bales, Jann Pasler, Joan Film 75 Roughgarden, Neil Smelser, Robert Wuthnow, and many others Paperbacks 76 throughout the humanities, natural sciences, and social sciences. Huntington Library Press 103 I invite you to learn about these books and more in the pages that follow, and to visit www.ucpress.edu for our entire Ordering Information 106 selection of titles in print. Author Index 110 Title Index 111 Lynne Withey Director GENERAL INTEREST Photographs by Karl Ammann and Text by Dale Peterson Elephant Reflections Elephant Reflections brings award-winning wildlife photographer Karl Ammann’s gorgeous images together with a revelatory text by writer Dale Peterson to illuminate one of nature’s greatest and most original works of art: the elephant.
    [Show full text]
  • Primates, Plants, and Parasites: the Evolution of Animal Self-Medication and Ethnomedicine - Michael A
    ETHNOPHARMACOLOGY - Vol. II - Primates, Plants, and Parasites: The Evolution of Animal Self-Medication and Ethnomedicine - Michael A. Huffman, Sylvia K. Vitazkova PRIMATES, PLANTS, AND PARASITES: THE EVOLUTION OF ANIMAL SELF-MEDICATION AND ETHNOMEDICINE Michael A. Huffman, Section of Ecology, Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, JAPAN Sylvia K. Vitazkova, EPSCoR, University of Virgin Islands, 2 Brewer’s Bay, St. Thomas, USVI, USA. Keywords: African great apes, zoopharmacognosy, self-medication, diet, ethnomedicine, parasite control, evolution, phytochemistry. Contents 1. Introduction 2. Animal self-medication and ethnomedicine 3. The impact of parasites on the evolution of self-medicative behavior 4. Food as medicine in animals and humans 5. Use of plants as medicine by chimpanzees in the wild 5.1. Whole leaf swallowing and the physical expulsion of parasite 5.2. Vernonia amygdalina and bitter pith chewing behavior 5.3. The ethnomedicine and phytochemistry of bitter pith chewing 6. A link between animal self-medication and ethnomedicine 7. Tongwe ethnozoology and health care 8. Future studies and directions of research Acknowledgments Glossary Bibliography Biographical Sketches Summary Early in the co-evolution of plant–animal relationships, some arthropod species began to utilize the chemical defenses of plants to protect themselves from their own predators and parasites. It is likely thus that the origins of herbal medicine have their roots deep within the animalUNESCO kingdom. Humans have looked – to wildEOLSS and domestic animals for sources of herbal remedies since prehistoric times. Both folklore and living examples provide accounts of howSAMPLE medicinal plants were obtained CHAPTERS by observing the behavior of animals. Animals too learn about the details of self-medication by watching each other.
    [Show full text]
  • The Human Family—Its Evolutionary Context and Diversity
    social sciences $€ £ ¥ Review The Human Family—Its Evolutionary Context and Diversity Karen L. Kramer Department of Anthropology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT 84112, USA; [email protected] Abstract: The family defines many aspects of our daily lives, and expresses a wide array of forms across individuals, cultures, ecologies and time. While the nuclear family is the norm today in developed economies, it is the exception in most other historic and cultural contexts. Yet, many aspects of how humans form the economic and reproductive groups that we recognize as families are distinct to our species. This review pursues three goals: to overview the evolutionary context in which the human family developed, to expand the conventional view of the nuclear family as the ‘traditional family’, and to provide an alternative to patrifocal explanations for family formation. To do so, first those traits that distinguish the human family are reviewed with an emphasis on the key contributions that behavioral ecology has made toward understanding dynamics within and between families, including life history, kin selection, reciprocity and conflict theoretical frameworks. An overview is then given of several seminal debates about how the family took shape, with an eye toward a more nuanced view of male parental care as the basis for family formation, and what cooperative breeding has to offer as an alternative perspective. Keywords: behavioral ecology; family studies; cooperative breeding; patrilineal Citation: Kramer, Karen L. 2021. The 1. Introduction Human Family—Its Evolutionary Family formation shapes many aspects of our daily lives—who we work, eat and sleep Context and Diversity.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of Homoerotic Behavior in Humans
    The Evolution of Homoerotic Behavior in Humans Frank Muscarella, PhD Barry University ABSTRACT. This paper presents a theoretical model for the evolution of same-sex sexual behavior, or homoerotic behavior, in humans. Con- trary to the traditional study of the topic, the emphasis is on homoerotic behavior itself, irrespective of sexual orientation. It is an inferential mod- el drawn from cross-species evidence, cross-cultural evidence, and cur- rent evolutionary theory. It is posited that humans evolved a disposition for homoerotic behavior because it increased same-sex affiliation among peripheralized hominids and indirectly influenced rates of survival and reproductive success. The implications for the conceptualization of same-sex sexual behavior and future research are indicated. [Article copies available for a fee from The Haworth Document Delivery Service: 1-800-342-9678. E-mail address: <[email protected]> Website: <http://www.HaworthPress.com>] KEYWORDS. Homoerotic behavior, homosexual behavior, evolution- ary psychology, evolution and homosexuality Evolutionary psychology is fascinating new field. The evolutionary model has proven to be rich in heuristic value and has generated a wealth of academic dialogue (Buss, Haselton, Shackelford, Bleske, & Frank Muscarella is Associate Professor of Psychology at Barry University. The author is grateful to Michael R. Cunningham, Douglas J. Garber, James Gregg, Linda M. Peterson, Christopher Starratt, and Lenore T. Szuchman for valu- able comments on the manuscript. Correspondence may be addressed to: Frank Muscarella, Department of Psychology, Barry University, 11300 Northeast Second Avenue, Miami Shores, FL 33161-6695. Journal of Homosexuality, Vol. 40(1) 2000 E 2000 by The Haworth Press, Inc. All rights reserved. 51 52 JOURNAL OF HOMOSEXUALITY Wakefield, 1998).
    [Show full text]
  • Celebrating the Life, Works, and Legacy of Henry David Thoreau
    THOREAU SOCIETY BICENTENNIAL ANNUAL GATHERING 2017 Celebrating the Life, Works, and Legacy of Henry David Thoreau Be it life or death, we crave only reality. Henry D. Thoreau JULY 11TH-16TH 2017 CONCORD, MA THOREAU SOCIETY STAFF ANNUAL GATHERING STAFF Michael J. Frederick, Executive Director Clarissa Eaton, Registration Coordinator & Program Editor Chynna Lemire, Business Manager Rachel Gulick, Registration Coordinator Magdalena Bermudez, Annual Gathering Coordinator Finley Janes, Director of Hospitality Jon Fadiman, Shop Supervisor William Bermudez, Audio-Visual Technician Richard Smith, Shop Manager & Thoreau Living History Conrad Demasi, Audio-Visual Technician HONORARY ADVISOR Edward O. Wilson BOARD OF DIRECTORS Michael Schleifer, CPA Brooklyn, New York Tom Potter President Martinsville, IN Robert Clarke Rochelle Johnson, PhD Woodbury, CT Caldwell, ID Treasurer Paul Schacht, PhD Gayle Moore Rochester, NY Martinsville, IN Clerk Michael Stoneham, PhD Washingtonville, NY Barry Andrews, D.Min. Bainbridge Island, WA Andrew Celentano Stoneham, MA Harrison A. Glasgow Manassas, VA Allen Harding Matamoras, PA Ronald Hoag, PhD Grimesland, NC Michael Lorence Williamsburg, VA The Thoreau Society Annual Gathering & Bicentennial EVENT MAP Old Manse Thoreau Farm & Thoreau Birthhouse 341 Virginia Road Monument Street Bedford Street Virginia Road Lowell Street Colonial Inn Masonic Temple Old Bedford Road Keyes Road Alcott House Keyes Road Parking Lot First Parish Church Lexington Road Concord Museum Main Street Emerson House Concord Free Public Library
    [Show full text]
  • Harvard Museums of Science & Culture
    FALL 2013 PROGRAMS HARVARD MUSEUMS OF SCIENCE & CULTURE • One world…four museums The Harvard Museums of Science and Culture (HMSC) offer outstanding permanent galleries, dynamic special exhibits, and captivating public programs for all ages and interests. From a 42-foot long Kronosaurus to exquisite glass flowers; from massive Maya and Egyptian monuments to finely-calibrated scientific instruments used by Benjamin Franklin; and from exceptional faculty lectures to gala celebrations, and children’s programs, the HMSC museum partnership is a portal to Harvard University’s world-renowned collections and vital research on human civilizations, biodiversity, Earth’s origins, and the history of science. Come see the world, in four museums. • Collection of Scientific Historical Instruments • Harvard Museum of Natural History • Harvard Semitic Museum • Peabody Museum of Archaeology & Ethnology • 1 •SEPTEMBER• Bee Day at Harvard Special Event. HMSC All activities are free and open to the public. For event details, visit the HMSC website: www.hmsc.harvard.edu Tuesday, September 10, 12:00–9:00 pm. The Plaza and the Science Center, 1 Oxford St. Jointly sponsored by the Harvard Museum of Natural History, Harvard Undergraduate Beekeepers, the Harvard Farmers’ Market, the Food Literacy Project, and the Cambridge Entomological Club Participate in a day of events and activities highlighting the importance of honey bees and beekeeping to biodiversity, the food supply, and human health. Learn more about the alarming decline of beehives (colony collapse disorder) widely documented in the U.S. and Europe. Event Schedule 12:00–6:00 pm: The Plaza (in front of the Science Center) Enjoy organic honey tasting at the Harvard Farmers’ Market.
    [Show full text]
  • Equal, Similar, but Different: Convergent Bonobos and Conserved Chimpanzees
    Equal, Similar, but Different: Convergent Bonobos and Conserved Chimpanzees The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters Citation Hare, B., and R. W. Wrangham. 2017. Equal, Similar, but Different: Convergent Bonobos and Conserved Chimpanzees. In Chimpanzees and human evolution, ed. M. N. Muller, R. W. Wrangham, & D. R. Pilbeam, 142-173. Cambridge, MA, US: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. Citable link http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:42656533 Terms of Use This article was downloaded from Harvard University’s DASH repository, and is made available under the terms and conditions applicable to Open Access Policy Articles, as set forth at http:// nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:dash.current.terms-of- use#OAP 1 2 3 4 5 6 Equal, similar but different: convergent bonobos and conserved chimpanzees 7 8 9 10 11 12 Brian Hare1,2 and Richard Wrangham3 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 1Department of Evolutionary Anthropology, Duke University 20 21 2Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University 22 23 3Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University 24 25 26 27 28 Abstract 29 Humans are lucky to have two closest relatives and not just one. While chimpanzees 30 have been the focus of far more research and are thought to more closely represent 31 our last common ape ancestor, the bonobo will be as important as the chimpanzee 32 in reconstructing our past. Understanding human evolution requires not only 33 knowing what traits are derived but also identifying what process may have shaped 34 these traits as they evolved.
    [Show full text]
  • Men and Force: Saying Yes to Aggression and No to Violence
    Thomas Owen-Towle Minns lecture V MEN AND FORCE SAYING YES TO AGGRESSION AND NO TO VIOLENCE Men are not flawed by nature. We become destructive when our masculinity is damaged. Violence springs from desperation and fear rather than authentic manhood. Aaron Kipnis Introduction If you haven’t discerned by now, I’m neither a cynic nor an optimist. Instead I’m a pragmatic hoper. In the final analysis, I agree with Stephen Boyd who says that “men are not inherently or irreversibly violent, relationally incompetent, emotionally constipated, and sexually compulsive. To the extent that we manifest these characteristics, we do so not because we are male, but because we have experienced violent socialization and conditioning processes that have required or produced this kind of behavior and we have chosen to accept, or adopt, these ways of being, thinking, and acting.” I contend that men can transform our histories of damaged and damaging masculinity. We can, indeed must, become more mature liberal religious brothers. Changing men will change our world. I behave my life on that premise and promise. One of the thorny yet useful distinctions for contemporary men to negotiate lies at the heart of lecture V: namely, saying yes to aggression or soul-force and saying no to violence, or unwarranted destruction. Our Demonic Male Legacy 1 There is no such thing as paradise, not in the South Seas, not in southern Greece, not anywhere. There never has been. To find a better world we must look not to a romanticized and dishonest dream forever receding into the primitive past, but to a future that rests on a proper understanding of ourselves.
    [Show full text]