Breaking the Cycle: Implicit Bias, Racial Anxiety, and Stereotype Threat Rachel D

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Breaking the Cycle: Implicit Bias, Racial Anxiety, and Stereotype Threat Rachel D January/February 2015 Volume 24: Number 1 Breaking the Cycle: Implicit Bias, Racial Anxiety, and Stereotype Threat Rachel D. Godsil Our country is in the midst of a obstacles rooted in racial or ethnic dif- scribe, “Breaking the Prejudice Habit” racial cataclysm. Deaths of black men ference with alarming frequency. And (Devine 2014) and research identify- and boys at the hands of police, com- yet most Americans espouse values of ing best practices to prevent implicit bined with grand juries’ failure to in- racial fairness. How can we make bias from affecting decision-making dict, have spurred grief, rage and pro- sense of these seeming contradictions? and behavior. test across the country. The reactions And how can we work to change the Understanding implicit bias can to the events are not uniform, how- conditions that set the stage for daily help explain why a black criminal de- ever. A deep polarization along racial challenges and tragic endings that are fendant charged with the same crime lines has emerged that contributes to linked to race? as a white defendant may receive a the feeling among many people of In November 2014, the Perception more draconian sentence, or why a color that black lives don’t matter. Institute, along with the Haas Institute resume from someone named Emily Neither these tragedies nor the ra- for a Fair and Inclusive Society, and will receive more callbacks than an cial disconnect that followed occur in the Center for Police Equity, issued otherwise identical resume from some- isolation. People of color experience the first in a series of reports entitled, one named Lakeisha. This work con- The Science of Equality: Addressing firms that people of color whose ex- Implicit Bias, Racial Anxiety, and Ste- periences of the world make abun- Rachel D. Godsil (rachel.godsil@ reotype Threat in Education and dantly clear that “race matters” are not gmail.com) is the Eleanor Bontecou Health Care, co-authored by Rachel simply oversensitive, while also ex- Professor of Law at Seton Hall Law Godsil, Linda Tropp, Phillip Atiba plaining how whites who consider School and Director of Research for Goff and john powell. The goal of this themselves non-racist may be sincere, the Perception Institute, a national series of reports is to synthesize and even if their behavior sometimes sug- consortium of social scientists, legal make accessible the advances in neu- gests otherwise. academics, lawyers and social justice roscience, social psychology and other This is not meant to suggest that advocates focusing on the implications “mind sciences” that have provided racialized outcomes are only a result of the mind sciences for policy, cul- insight into otherwise confounding of individual actions; cumulative ra- ture and institutions. She also chairs contradictions between our country’s cial advantages for whites as a group the New York City Rent Guidelines stated commitment to fairness and the have been embedded into society’s Board.This article is a précis of a behaviors that lead both to tragic out- structures and institutions. However, longer Perception Inst./Haas Inst. for comes and day-to-day indignities (Please turn to page 2) a Fair & Inclusive Society/Ctr. for linked to race. Police Equity report by Rachel Godsil, Our report includes a lengthy dis- Linda Tropp, Phillip Atiba Goff & cussion of social psychological re- CONTENTS: john powell, The Science of Equal- search focusing on “implicit bias”— ity: Addressing Implicit Bias, Racial the automatic association of stereotypes Implicit Bias ................ 1 Anxiety, and Stereotype Threat in Edu- or attitudes with particular social §8 Landlords ................ 3 cation and Health Care. The Percep- groups. We place particular emphasis Resources .................. 10 tion Inst., http://perception.org/our- on new research on reducing bias or, publications as Patricia Devine and colleagues de- Poverty & Race Research Action Council • 1200 18th Street NW • Suite 200 • Washington, DC 20036 202/906-8023 • FAX: 202/842-2885 • E-mail: [email protected] • www.prrac.org Recycled Paper (IMPLICIT BIAS: from page 1) how we navigate racial interactions. explored is the idea that whites can suf- These can translate into racial anxiety fer stereotype threat when concerned as john powell and I argued in these and stereotype threat which, indepen- that they may be perceived as racist. pages in 2011 ("Implicit Bias Insights dent of bias, can create obstacles for In the former context, the threat pre- as Preconditions to Structural institutions and individuals seeking to vents students from performing as well Change," P&R, Sept./Oct. 2011), adhere to antiracist practices. Indeed, as they ought, and so they themselves there are two key reasons why struc- research suggests that some forms of suffer the consequences of this phe- tural racism cannot be successfully anti-bias education may have detrimen- nomenon. Stereotype threat among challenged without an understanding of tal effects, if they increase bias aware- whites, by contrast, often causes be- how race operates psychologically. ness without also providing skills for havior that harms others—usually the First, public policy choices are often managing anxiety. very people they are worried about. affected by implicit bias or other Racial anxiety refers to discomfort Concern about being perceived as rac- racialized phenomena that operate im- about the experience and potential con- ist explains, for example, why some plicitly. As a result, the changes in sequences of inter-racial interactions. white teachers, professors and super- policy necessary to address institutional It is important to distinguish this defi- visors give less critical feedback to structures are dependent upon success- nition of racial anxiety from what so- black students and employees than to fully addressing implicit biases that can cial scientists refer to as “racial threat,” white ones (Harber et al., 2012) and affect political choices. Second, insti- which includes the anger, frustration, why white peer advisors may fail to tutional operations invariably involve uncertainty, feelings of deprivation and warn a black student but will warn a human behavior and interaction: Any white or Asian student that a certain policies to address racial inequities in Skills are needed for course load is unmanageable (Crosby schools, workplaces, police depart- managing racial anxiety. & Monin, 2007). ments, courthouses, government of- In other words, cognitive depletion fices and the like will only be success- or interference caused by stereotype ful if the people implementing the other emotions associated with concern threat can affect how one’s own ca- policy changes comply with them over loss of resources or dominance. pacity, such as the ability to achieve (Crosby & Monin, 2007). People of color may experience racial academically, will be judged; this Although implicit phenomena have anxiety that they will be the target of causes first-party harm to the indi- the potential to impede successful in- discrimination and hostile treatment. vidual whose performance suffers. stitutional change, implicit racial bias White people tend to experience anxi- However, as is explored in more de- is not the only psychological phenom- ety that they will be assumed to be racist tail below, stereotype threat about how enon that blocks society from achiev- and will be met with distrust or hostil- one’s character will be judged (i.e., ing racial equality. We risk being myo- ity. Whites experiencing racial anxi- being labeled a racist) can cause third- pic if we focus only on people’s cog- ety can seem awkward and maintain party harms when suffered by an indi- nitive processing, and we also risk un- less eye contact with people of color, vidual in a position of power. intended consequences if we focus our and ultimately these interactions tend Implicit bias, racial anxiety and ste- interventions only on addressing im- to be shorter than those without anxi- reotype threat have effects in virtually plicit bias. Our experiences, motiva- ety. If two people are both anxious that every important area of our lives. In tions and emotions are also integral to an interaction will be negative, it of- the first report, we illustrate the inter- ten is. So racial anxiety can result in a related implications of the three phe- Poverty & Race (ISSN 1075-3591) negative feedback loop in which both nomena in the domains of education is published six times a year by the Pov- parties’ fears seem to be confirmed by and healthcare. Education and health- erty and Race Research Action Council, the behavior of the other. care are of critical importance for ob- 1200 18th Street NW, Suite 200, Wash- Stereotype threat refers to the pres- vious reasons, and an abundance of ington, DC 20036, 202/906-8023, fax: 202/842-2885, E-mail: [email protected]. sure people feel when they fear that research has highlighted the role race Chester Hartman, Editor. Subscriptions their performance may confirm a nega- plays in unequal outcomes in both do- are $25/year, $45/two years. Foreign tive stereotype about their group mains. postage extra. Articles, article sugges- (Steele, 2010). This pressure is expe- The report also emphasizes the in- tions, letters and general comments are rienced as a distraction that interferes terventions that are emerging in the welcome, as are notices of publications, conferences, job openings, etc. for our with intellectual functioning. Although research that institutions can begin to Resources Section. Articles generally stereotype threat can affect anyone, it use to prevent continuing racialized may be reprinted, providing PRRAC has been most discussed in the context obstacles. Ideally, this work will hap- gives advance permission. of academic achievement among stu- pen at the structural and institutional © Copyright 2015 by the Poverty and dents of color, and among girls in sci- level—but many of us don’t want to Race Research Action Council. All rights reserved. ence, technology, engineering and wait, and the social science research math (STEM) fields.
Recommended publications
  • The Force of Fear: Police Stereotype Threat, Self-Legitimacy, and Support for the Use Force
    The force of fear: Police stereotype threat, self-legitimacy, and support for the use force Rick Trinknera,1, Phillip Atiba Goffb a,School of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Arizona State University, 411 N. Central Ave, Suite 600, Phoenix, AZ 85004; and b,Center for Policing Equity, John Jay College of Criminal Justice, 524 West 59th Street, Room 6.63.14, New York, NY 10019 1To whom correspondences should be addressed. Email: [email protected]. Abstract increased endorsement of unreasonable use of force—in turn possibly further eroding Researchers have linked concern about public trust. These findings serve as the first appearing racist among police officers—a integration of two psychological literatures: kind of stereotype threat—to racial stereotype threat and procedural justice. disparities in the use of force. This study They also highlight an under-explored presents the first empirical test of the mechanism of urgent scientific and policy psychological mechanism linking stereotype concern. threat to support for police violence among officers. Drawing from psychology and Introduction criminology, we argue that threat undermines officers’ self-legitimacy, which Popular explanations of excessive police use encourages overreliance on force and of force—especially racial disparities in that coercion to maintain situational control. To force—often reduce to issues of officer test this argument, surveys were distributed characteristics (e.g., general aggression, to officers from the patrol division of a large racial bias). Despite the widespread urban police force. Respondents completed popularity of this lay theory, it is at odds measures assessing their experience of with the scientific consensus that attitudes stereotype threat, self-legitimacy, resistance towards the department’s use of force are relatively weak predictors of behavior, policy, approval of using excessive force, only explaining about 10% across contexts and endorsement of procedurally fair (Dovidio, 2001; LaPiere, 1934; Wicker, policing.
    [Show full text]
  • Avoiding the Conjunction Fallacy: Who Can Take a Hint?
    Avoiding the conjunction fallacy: Who can take a hint? Simon Klein Spring 2017 Master’s thesis, 30 ECTS Master’s programme in Cognitive Science, 120 ECTS Supervisor: Linnea Karlsson Wirebring Acknowledgments: The author would like to thank the participants for enduring the test session with challenging questions and thereby making the study possible, his supervisor Linnea Karlsson Wirebring for invaluable guidance and good questions during the thesis work, and his fiancée Amanda Arnö for much needed mental support during the entire process. 2 AVOIDING THE CONJUNCTION FALLACY: WHO CAN TAKE A HINT? Simon Klein Humans repeatedly commit the so called “conjunction fallacy”, erroneously judging the probability of two events occurring together as higher than the probability of one of the events. Certain hints have been shown to mitigate this tendency. The present thesis investigated the relations between three psychological factors and performance on conjunction tasks after reading such a hint. The factors represent the understanding of probability and statistics (statistical numeracy), the ability to resist intuitive but incorrect conclusions (cognitive reflection), and the willingness to engage in, and enjoyment of, analytical thinking (need-for-cognition). Participants (n = 50) answered 30 short conjunction tasks and three psychological scales. A bimodal response distribution motivated dichotomization of performance scores. Need-for-cognition was significantly, positively correlated with performance, while numeracy and cognitive reflection were not. The results suggest that the willingness to engage in, and enjoyment of, analytical thinking plays an important role for the capacity to avoid the conjunction fallacy after taking a hint. The hint further seems to neutralize differences in performance otherwise predicted by statistical numeracy and cognitive reflection.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Bias Mitigation: How to Make Decision-Making More Rational?
    Cognitive Bias Mitigation: How to make decision-making more rational? Abstract Cognitive biases distort judgement and adversely impact decision-making, which results in economic inefficiencies. Initial attempts to mitigate these biases met with little success. However, recent studies which used computer games and educational videos to train people to avoid biases (Clegg et al., 2014; Morewedge et al., 2015) showed that this form of training reduced selected cognitive biases by 30 %. In this work I report results of an experiment which investigated the debiasing effects of training on confirmation bias. The debiasing training took the form of a short video which contained information about confirmation bias, its impact on judgement, and mitigation strategies. The results show that participants exhibited confirmation bias both in the selection and processing of information, and that debiasing training effectively decreased the level of confirmation bias by 33 % at the 5% significance level. Key words: Behavioural economics, cognitive bias, confirmation bias, cognitive bias mitigation, confirmation bias mitigation, debiasing JEL classification: D03, D81, Y80 1 Introduction Empirical research has documented a panoply of cognitive biases which impair human judgement and make people depart systematically from models of rational behaviour (Gilovich et al., 2002; Kahneman, 2011; Kahneman & Tversky, 1979; Pohl, 2004). Besides distorted decision-making and judgement in the areas of medicine, law, and military (Nickerson, 1998), cognitive biases can also lead to economic inefficiencies. Slovic et al. (1977) point out how they distort insurance purchases, Hyman Minsky (1982) partly blames psychological factors for economic cycles. Shefrin (2010) argues that confirmation bias and some other cognitive biases were among the significant factors leading to the global financial crisis which broke out in 2008.
    [Show full text]
  • Stereotypes and Prejudice
    Stereotypes and Prejudice Their Overt and Subtle Influence in the Classroom CONNIE T. WOLFE University of Michigan STEVEN J. SPENCER Hope College The authors examine overt and subtle forms of stereotyping and prejudice. Two theories that explain overt prejudice are reviewed: realistic conflict theory and social identity theory. Although overt prejudice seems to have declined, subtle stereotyping is still pervasive. The authors review one theory, aversive racism theory, that explains this phenomenon. They also discuss two perspectives, attributional ambiguity and stereotype threat, which provide accounts of the impact of subtle racism. Both overt and subtle prejudice present challenges for the classroom. The authors describe one intervention called the jigsaw classroom that encourages work toward common goals and helps reduce the expression and impact of overt discrimination. A second intervention program, wise schooling, is presented, which aims to reduce the impact of subtle stereotypes by reducing stereotype threat. Why do prejudice and discrimination exist? Has overt racism been replaced by more subtle forms of prejudice? How does stereotyping affect its targets? In this article we describe two theories, realistic conflict theory and social identity theory, which provide an answer to the first question. We address the second question by noting that although overt discrimination has decreased, subtle forms of prejudice are still quite common and we describe one theory, aversive racism, that provides a compelling account of this change in the expression of prejudice. Finally, we answer the third question by describing two phenomena, attributional ambiguity and stereotype threat, that result from the pervasive nature of subtle stereotyping. This article is a selective overview of what social psychology has to say about these crucial issues.
    [Show full text]
  • Graphical Techniques in Debiasing: an Exploratory Study
    GRAPHICAL TECHNIQUES IN DEBIASING: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY by S. Bhasker Information Systems Department Leonard N. Stern School of Business New York University New York, New York 10006 and A. Kumaraswamy Management Department Leonard N. Stern School of Business New York University New York, NY 10006 October, 1990 Center for Research on Information Systems Information Systems Department Leonard N. Stern School of Business New York University Working Paper Series STERN IS-90-19 Forthcoming in the Proceedings of the 1991 Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences Center for Digital Economy Research Stem School of Business IVorking Paper IS-90-19 Center for Digital Economy Research Stem School of Business IVorking Paper IS-90-19 2 Abstract Base rate and conjunction fallacies are consistent biases that influence decision making involving probability judgments. We develop simple graphical techniques and test their eflcacy in correcting for these biases. Preliminary results suggest that graphical techniques help to overcome these biases and improve decision making. We examine the implications of incorporating these simple techniques in Executive Information Systems. Introduction Today, senior executives operate in highly uncertain environments. They have to collect, process and analyze a deluge of information - most of it ambiguous. But, their limited information acquiring and processing capabilities constrain them in this task [25]. Increasingly, executives rely on executive information/support systems for various purposes like strategic scanning of their business environments, internal monitoring of their businesses, analysis of data available from various internal and external sources, and communications [5,19,32]. However, executive information systems are, at best, support tools. Executives still rely on their mental or cognitive models of their businesses and environments and develop heuristics to simplify decision problems [10,16,25].
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Biases in Economic Decisions – Three Essays on the Impact of Debiasing
    TECHNISCHE UNIVERSITÄT MÜNCHEN Lehrstuhl für Betriebswirtschaftslehre – Strategie und Organisation Univ.-Prof. Dr. Isabell M. Welpe Cognitive biases in economic decisions – three essays on the impact of debiasing Christoph Martin Gerald Döbrich Abdruck der von der Fakultät für Wirtschaftswissenschaften der Technischen Universität München zur Erlangung des akademischen Grades eines Doktors der Wirtschaftswissenschaften (Dr. rer. pol.) genehmigten Dissertation. Vorsitzender: Univ.-Prof. Dr. Gunther Friedl Prüfer der Dissertation: 1. Univ.-Prof. Dr. Isabell M. Welpe 2. Univ.-Prof. Dr. Dr. Holger Patzelt Die Dissertation wurde am 28.11.2012 bei der Technischen Universität München eingereicht und durch die Fakultät für Wirtschaftswissenschaften am 15.12.2012 angenommen. Acknowledgments II Acknowledgments Numerous people have contributed to the development and successful completion of this dissertation. First of all, I would like to thank my supervisor Prof. Dr. Isabell M. Welpe for her continuous support, all the constructive discussions, and her enthusiasm concerning my dissertation project. Her challenging questions and new ideas always helped me to improve my work. My sincere thanks also go to Prof. Dr. Matthias Spörrle for his continuous support of my work and his valuable feedback for the articles building this dissertation. Moreover, I am grateful to Prof. Dr. Dr. Holger Patzelt for acting as the second advisor for this thesis and Professor Dr. Gunther Friedl for leading the examination board. This dissertation would not have been possible without the financial support of the Elite Network of Bavaria. I am very thankful for the financial support over two years which allowed me to pursue my studies in a focused and efficient manner. Many colleagues at the Chair for Strategy and Organization of Technische Universität München have supported me during the completion of this thesis.
    [Show full text]
  • Sticks and Stones May Break My Bones, but Words May Also Hurt Me: a Comparison of United States and German Hate Speech Laws
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Fordham University School of Law Fordham International Law Journal Volume 41, Issue 5 2018 Article 9 Sticks and Stones May Break my Bones, But Words May Also Hurt Me: A Comparison of United States and German Hate Speech Laws Deborah Levine∗ ∗ Copyright c 2018 by the authors. Fordham International Law Journal is produced by The Berke- ley Electronic Press (bepress). https://ir.lawnet.fordham.edu/ilj NOTE STICKS AND STONES MAY BREAK MY BONES, BUT WORDS MAY ALSO HURT ME: A COMPARISON OF UNITED STATES AND GERMAN HATE SPEECH LAWS Deborah Levine* I. INTRODUCTION ........................................................1294 II. THE INTERPLAY OF HATE SPEECH, DIGNITY, AND PSYCHOLOGY ...........................................................1295 A. Defining Hate Speech, Dignity, and Liberty ..........1296 B. The Pros and Cons of Hate Speech Regulation ......1298 1. Arguments in Favor of Regulation of Hate Speech ..............................................................1298 2. Arguments in Favor of Free Speech ...................1303 III. HISTORY AND LAW OF THE UNITED STATES AND GERMANY REGARDING HATE SPEECH ..............1305 A. History and Law Pertaining to Hate Speech in the United States ..........................................................1305 1. History and its Effect on Freedom of Speech ....1306 2. The Constitution, the First Amendment, and Statutes and Their Effect on Free Speech ........1308 3. Cases and Their Effect, or Lack Thereof, on Hate Speech Regulation ...........................................1311 a. Supreme Court Cases in Line with Chaplinksy and Brandenburg .......................................1313 * J.D. Candidate, 2019, Fordham University School of Law; B.A., 2016 Barnard College of Columbia University. I would like to thank Professor Abner Greene for his guidance and input in the earlier drafts, the editors of the Fordham International Law Journal, and the staff members of the Fordham International Law Journal who edited this Note.
    [Show full text]
  • Exploring Gender Identification Effects on Stereotype Threat Lauren
    Exploring Gender Identification Effects on Stereotype Threat Lauren Westendorf Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Prerequisite for Honors in Psychology April 2015 © 2015 Lauren Westendorf EXPLORING GENDER IDENTIFICATION EFFECTS ON STEREOTYPE THREAT 2 Acknowledgements Special thanks to the Dean’s Office of Wellesley College and the Wellesley College Psychology Department for offering the funding needed to execute this project, to Professor Margaret Keane for all of her moral support and logistical help, to Phil Peake of Smith College and Amber Douglas of Mount Holyoke College for graciously assisting in my ambitious intercollegiate data collection, and to Professors Stephen Chen, Linda Carli, and Irene Mata for serving on my thesis defense committee. Endless gratitude, of course, goes to my devoted thesis advisor, Professor Julie Norem, for pushing me in the most wonderful ways, and to Professor Angela Bahns, for inspiring my interest in these topics with her incredible courses. Lastly, a very special thanks goes to my parents, the Wellesley Widows, and Lallie Lukens for their unwavering moral support. EXPLORING GENDER IDENTIFICATION EFFECTS ON STEREOTYPE THREAT 3 Table of Contents Abstract ........................................................................................................................................... 5 Introduction ..................................................................................................................................... 6 Techniques for Reducing Stereotype Threat ......................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Working Memory, Cognitive Miserliness and Logic As Predictors of Performance on the Cognitive Reflection Test
    Working Memory, Cognitive Miserliness and Logic as Predictors of Performance on the Cognitive Reflection Test Edward J. N. Stupple ([email protected]) Centre for Psychological Research, University of Derby Kedleston Road, Derby. DE22 1GB Maggie Gale ([email protected]) Centre for Psychological Research, University of Derby Kedleston Road, Derby. DE22 1GB Christopher R. Richmond ([email protected]) Centre for Psychological Research, University of Derby Kedleston Road, Derby. DE22 1GB Abstract Most participants respond that the answer is 10 cents; however, a slower and more analytic approach to the The Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT) was devised to measure problem reveals the correct answer to be 5 cents. the inhibition of heuristic responses to favour analytic ones. The CRT has been a spectacular success, attracting more Toplak, West and Stanovich (2011) demonstrated that the than 100 citations in 2012 alone (Scopus). This may be in CRT was a powerful predictor of heuristics and biases task part due to the ease of administration; with only three items performance - proposing it as a metric of the cognitive miserliness central to dual process theories of thinking. This and no requirement for expensive equipment, the practical thesis was examined using reasoning response-times, advantages are considerable. There have, moreover, been normative responses from two reasoning tasks and working numerous correlates of the CRT demonstrated, from a wide memory capacity (WMC) to predict individual differences in range of tasks in the heuristics and biases literature (Toplak performance on the CRT. These data offered limited support et al., 2011) to risk aversion and SAT scores (Frederick, for the view of miserliness as the primary factor in the CRT.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effect of Overweight Stereotype Threat on Children's Motor Learning*
    PSIHOLOGIJA, 2020, Online First, 1–12 UDC: © 2020 by authors DOI: https://doi.org/10.2298/PSI200413036R The effect of overweight stereotype threat on children’s motor learning* Maryam Rabeinia, Esmaeel Saemi, and Rasool Abedanzadeh Faculty of Sport Sciences, Shahid Chamran University of Ahvaz, Ahvaz, Iran The purpose of the present study was to investigate the effect of overweight stereotype threat on children’s motor learning. Twenty-four children (Mage = 9.25 ± 0.53 years) were conveniently selected and randomly assigned into two groups of stereotype threat (with emphasis on individual characteristics, namely, overweight) and control group. The task consisted of maintaining the balance on the stabilometer during 30-second trials. One trial in the pre-test, 10 trials in the acquisition phase and 5 trials in the retention test were run. The findings revealed lower motor learning among the overweight stereotype group compared to the control group. Therefore, it is suggested that in similar situations, coaches can reduce stereotype threat emphasizing on the ineffectiveness of individual characteristics, i.e., overweight on motor performance of children thereby prevent performance and learning disruptions. Keywords: balance, overweight, stereotype threat, children Highlights: • Balance performance in children was lower in overweight stereotype threat. • Overweight stereotype threat conditions had a negative effect on motor learning as well as performance among children. • It is suggested that educators and practitioners decrease the stereotype threat by emphasizing the ineffectiveness of individual characteristics such as being overweight on performance and thereby prevent the destruction of children’s performance and learning. Corresponding author: [email protected] Note. The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or finantial relations that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
    [Show full text]
  • The Challenge of Debiasing Personnel Decisions: Avoiding Both Under- and Overcorrection
    University of Pennsylvania ScholarlyCommons Management Papers Wharton Faculty Research 12-2008 The Challenge of Debiasing Personnel Decisions: Avoiding Both Under- and Overcorrection Philip E. Tetlock University of Pennsylvania Gregory Mitchell Terry L. Murray Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.upenn.edu/mgmt_papers Part of the Business Administration, Management, and Operations Commons Recommended Citation Tetlock, P. E., Mitchell, G., & Murray, T. L. (2008). The Challenge of Debiasing Personnel Decisions: Avoiding Both Under- and Overcorrection. Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 1 (4), 439-443. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1754-9434.2008.00084.x This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. https://repository.upenn.edu/mgmt_papers/32 For more information, please contact [email protected]. The Challenge of Debiasing Personnel Decisions: Avoiding Both Under- and Overcorrection Disciplines Business Administration, Management, and Operations This journal article is available at ScholarlyCommons: https://repository.upenn.edu/mgmt_papers/32 1 The Challenge of Debiasing Personnel Decisions: Avoiding Both Under- and Over-Correction Philip E. Tetlock, * Gregory Mitchell, ** and Terry L. Murray *** Introduction This commentary advances two interrelated scientific arguments. First, we endorse Landy's (2008) concerns about the insufficient emphasis placed on individuating information by scholars eager to import social-cognition work on stereotyping into employment law. Building on Landy’s analysis, we emphasize that greater attention needs to be given to the power of accountability and teamwork incentives to motivate personnel decision-makers to seek and utilize individuating information that is predictive of job-relevant behavior. Second, we note how easy it is for exchanges between proponents and skeptics of unconscious stereotyping to lead to ideological stalemates.
    [Show full text]
  • Stereotype Threat Effects in Afro-Caribbean Immigrant Groups
    #3247—Social Psychology Quarterly—VOL. 70 NO. 4—70408-Deaux Social Psychology Quarterly 2007, Vol. 70, No. 4, 384–404 Becoming American: Stereotype Threat Effects in Afro-Caribbean Immigrant Groups* KAY DEAUX Graduate Center, City University of New York NIDA BIKMEN Denison University ALWYN GILKES Medgar Evers College, City University of New York ANA VENTUNEAC YVANNE JOSEPH Graduate Center, City University of New York YASSER PAYNE University of Delaware CLAUDE STEELE Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences Educational and occupational data suggest that second-generation West Indian immigrants have less favorable outcomes than their first-generation counterparts, who are typically shown to outperform comparison groups of African Americans. In two studies, we explore the social psychological process of stereotype threat as it differentially affects the performance of first- and second-generation West Indian students. An initial questionnaire study of 270 West Indian students provided data on perceived favorability of African American and West Indian stereotypes, ethnic identification, and perceptions of discrimination. An experimental study of stereotype threat showed a significant interaction between generation and stereotype threat condition: first- and second- generation West Indian students performed equally in neutral conditions, but differed significantly when stereotype threat was present. While first- generation students increased their performance in the threat condition, second-generation students showed the performance decrements characteristic of African American students. Effects due to the race of the experimenter were also found, suggesting the importance of context in testing situations. Overall, the findings argue for the relevance of psychological processes in understanding broader demographic patterns of immigration and change.
    [Show full text]