Using Analogy to Learn About Phenomena at Scales Outside Human Perception Ilyse Resnick1*, Alexandra Davatzes2, Nora S

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Using Analogy to Learn About Phenomena at Scales Outside Human Perception Ilyse Resnick1*, Alexandra Davatzes2, Nora S View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Springer - Publisher Connector Resnick et al. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications (2017) 2:21 Cognitive Research: Principles DOI 10.1186/s41235-017-0054-7 and Implications ORIGINAL ARTICLE Open Access Using analogy to learn about phenomena at scales outside human perception Ilyse Resnick1*, Alexandra Davatzes2, Nora S. Newcombe3 and Thomas F. Shipley3 Abstract Understanding and reasoning about phenomena at scales outside human perception (for example, geologic time) is critical across science, technology, engineering, and mathematics. Thus, devising strong methods to support acquisition of reasoning at such scales is an important goal in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics education. In two experiments, we examine the use of analogical principles in learning about geologic time. Across both experiments we find that using a spatial analogy (for example, a time line) to make multiple alignments, and keeping all unrelated components of the analogy held constant (for example, keep the time line the same length), leads to better understanding of the magnitude of geologic time. Effective approaches also include hierarchically and progressively aligning scale information (Experiment 1) and active prediction in making alignments paired with immediate feedback (Experiments 1 and 2). Keywords: Analogy, Magnitude, Progressive alignment, Corrective feedback, STEM education Significance statement alignment and having multiple opportunities to make Many fundamental science, technology, engineering, and alignments are critical in supporting reasoning about mathematic (STEM) phenomena occur at extreme scales scale, and that the progressive and hierarchical that cannot be directly perceived. For example, the Geo- organization of scale information may provide salient logic Time Scale, discovery of the atom, size of the landmarks for estimation. Finally, we found that active universe, and rapidly developing field of nanotechnology prediction and corrective feedback are valuable in foster- are all based on phenomena occurring at scales that ing a linear representation of magnitude. These findings humans cannot directly experience. Unfortunately, nov- have practical implications, as they can guide development ices have trouble reasoning about phenomena outside hu- of supports for the acquisition of reasoning at extreme man perception, at least in part, because they do not scales, which is an important goal in STEM education. understand the relative magnitudes at these scales. Across two experiments we develop and test two successful in- structional analogies designed to teach geologic time. Background ’ These findings add to our theoretical understanding of How can we improve students reasoning about large how people reason about scale, and the role of analogical magnitudes? reasoning and active prediction in learning. We find that Skills in reasoning about size and scale are central to per- reasoning about different kinds of magnitude (that is, tem- formance across STEM disciplines (for example, Hawkins, poral and abstract) at different scales (that is, inside and 1978; Tretter, Jones, Andre, Negishi, & Minogue, 2006). outside human perception) have shared properties, and Many fundamental science, technology, engineering, and that people are able to use a spatial representation of tem- mathematic (STEM) phenomena occur at extreme scales poral magnitude to develop a more accurate understand- that cannot be directly perceived. For example, a core ing of geologic time. Our findings suggest that structural geologic concept is that geologic events can last billions of years (for example, the Earth formed approximately 4.6 billion years ago). Reasoning about geologic time allows * Correspondence: [email protected] 1 geologists to reconstruct the surface conditions of ancient Department of Psychology, Penn State University Lehigh Valley, 2809 ’ Saucon Valley Road, Center Valley, PA 18034, USA Earth, produce an accurate time line of Earthshistory, Full list of author information is available at the end of the article and understand the imperceptibly slow processes that © The Author(s). 2017 Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. Resnick et al. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications (2017) 2:21 Page 2 of 17 have led to the current environment. Practically, under- corresponding relations between the base concept and standing geologic time allows people to reason about the target concept (for example, Gentner, 1983; Kotovsky & sustainability of non-renewable resources and the conse- Gentner, 1996). It can also be difficult to identify the quences of anthropogenic climate change. Given the relevant relations to align if the base concept and target importance of reasoning about scale, it should be no sur- concept are different in many ways (Gentner, 1983; prise that the National Research Council in AFramework Gentner, 2001; Markman & Gentner, 1996, 1997; Kokinov for K-12 Science Education (National Research Council, & French, 2003). For example, Earth’s history is also 2011) and the American Association for the Advancement commonly mapped onto a 24-hour clock. However, this of Science in Benchmarks for Science Literacy (American analogy contains at least two differences in addition to Association for the Advancement of Science, 1993) identi- differences in magnitude (24 hours versus billions of fied “size and scale” as fundamental scientific concepts, and years): clocks are cyclical whereas Earth’s history is linear, suggested them as a unifying theme in science education. and clocks are composed of equal temporal divisions Unfortunately, novices have trouble reasoning about whereas geologic time comprises unequal temporal phenomena outside human perception. Although nov- divisions based on major geologic events. Thus, students ices are sometimes reasonably accurate at ranking may not be able to identify the appropriate analogy to phenomena in a correct sequence, they have difficultly make and, subsequently, draw incorrect conclusions comparing the magnitude between phenomena at (Brown & Salter, 2010; Gentner, 1983). In this instance, extreme scales (for example, Delgado, Stevens, Shin, students may erroneously believe that, just like the Yunker, & Krajcik, 2007; Jones, Tretter, Taylor, & Oppewal, 24-hour clock, periods of Earth’s history are also evenly 2008; Libarkin, Anderson, Dahl, Beilfuss, & Boone, 2005). spaced, and fail to make the appropriate analogy between For example, although students are fairly accurate in iden- relative magnitudes of time between events. tifying a correct sequence of events in Earth’s geologic A review of analogical reasoning literature offers three history (Trend, 2001), they fail to understand the magni- techniques that may be useful in the development of tude of time between events (Tretter et al., 2006). effective analogies for phenomena at extreme scales. Analogy is potentially valuable for learning and First, the base concept and target concept should be reasoning about phenomena outside human perception structurally aligned; that is, as similar as possible with because such phenomena cannot be directly experienced just one “alignable difference” (Goldstone, 1994; Markman (Jones, Taylor, & Broadwell, 2009). Analogy refers to a & Gentner, 1993a, 1993b; Medin, Goldstone, & Gentner, process of aligning structural similarities between a base 1993). An alignable difference is a common relation concept and a target concept (Gentner, 1983). In fact, shared by the base concept and target concept which analogy is frequently used to teach phenomena at differs along one dimension. In the case of aligning an ex- extreme scales (for example, Clary & Wandersee, 2009; treme scale with a human scale, both scales should be Petcovic & Ruhf, 2008; Semken et al., 2009; Sibley, constructed in the same format (for example, a linear 2009). Unfortunately, even with a wide range of analo- number line), with the only difference being magnitude. gies, students struggle to comprehend phenomena However, because the difference in magnitude between outside human perception (for example, Delgado et al., extreme and human scales is so vast, it may not be 2007; Jones et al., 2008; Libarkin et al., 2005). Moreover, possible for the base concept and target concept to be analogies can mislead students (Brown & Salter, 2010; structurally aligned. Thus, a second technique to align Duit, 1991). For example, geologic time is often repre- very different concepts is called progressive alignment sented using a spatial analogy that compresses the time (Kotovsky & Gentner, 1996; Thompson & Opfer, 2010). before life on Earth becomes relatively more complex. Using progressive alignment, an analogy may consist of Although functional, learning from this nonlinear repre- more than one analogical step, beginning with a com- sentation can mislead students into thinking that parison of a base concept and a highly similar intermedi- biologic events occurred very early in the Earth’s history ate concept. Comparing two very similar concepts as an (Resnick, Davatzes, Newcombe, & Shipley, 2016;
Recommended publications
  • Analogical Processes and College Developmental Reading
    Analogical Processes and College Developmental Reading By Eric J. Paulson Abstract: Although a solid body of research is analogical. An analogical process involves concerning the role of analogies in reading processes the identification of partial similarities between has emerged at a variety of age groups and reading different objects or situations to support further proficiencies, few of those studies have focused on inferences and is used to explain new concepts, analogy use by readers enrolled in college develop- solve problems, and understand new areas and mental reading courses. The current study explores ideas (Gentner, & Colhoun, 2010; Gentner & Smith, whether 232 students enrolled in mandatory (by 2012). For example, when a biology teacher relates placement test) developmental reading courses the functions of a cell to the activities in a factory in in a postsecondary educational context utilize order to introduce and explain the cell to students, analogical processes while engaged in specific this is an analogical process designed to use what reading activities. This is explored through two is already familiar to illuminate and explain a separate investigations that focus on two different new concept. A process of mapping similarities ends of the reading spectrum: the word-decoding between a source (what is known) and a target Analogy appears to be a key level and the overall text-comprehension level. (what is needed to be known) in order to better element of human thinking. The two investigations reported here build on understand the target (Holyoak & Thagard, 1997), comparable studies of analogy use with proficient analogies are commonly used to make sense of readers.
    [Show full text]
  • Demonstrative and Non-Demonstrative Reasoning by Analogy
    Demonstrative and non-demonstrative reasoning by analogy Emiliano Ippoliti Analogy and analogical reasoning have more and more become an important subject of inquiry in logic and philosophy of science, especially in virtue of its fruitfulness and variety: in fact analogy «may occur in many contexts, serve many purposes, and take on many forms»1. Moreover, analogy compels us to consider material aspects and dependent-on-domain kinds of reasoning that are at the basis of the lack of well- established and accepted theory of analogy: a standard theory of analogy, in the sense of a theory as classical logic, is therefore an impossible target. However, a detailed discussion of these aspects is not the aim of this paper and I will focus only on a small set of questions related to analogy and analogical reasoning, namely: 1) The problem of analogy and its duplicity; 2) The role of analogy in demonstrative reasoning; 3) The role of analogy in non-demonstrative reasoning; 4) The limits of analogy; 5) The convergence, particularly in multiple analogical reasoning, of these two apparently distinct aspects and its philosophical and methodological consequences; § 1 The problem of analogy and its duplicity: the controversial nature of analogical reasoning One of the most interesting aspects of analogical reasoning is its intrinsic duplicity: in fact analogy belongs to, and takes part in, both demonstrative and non-demonstrative processes. That is, it can be used respectively as a means to prove and justify knowledge (e.g. in automated theorem proving or in confirmation patterns of plausible inference), and as a means to obtain new knowledge, (i.e.
    [Show full text]
  • SKETCHING, ANALOGIES, and CREATIVITY on the Shared
    J. S. Gero, B. Tversky and T. Purcell (eds), 2001, Visual and Spatial Reasoning in Design, II Key Centre of Design Computing and Cognition, University of Sydney, Australia, pp. SKETCHING, ANALOGIES, AND CREATIVITY On the shared research interests of psychologists and designers ILSE M. VERSTIJNEN, ANN HEYLIGHEN, JOHAN WAGEMANS AND HERMAN NEUCKERMANS K.U.Leuven Belgium Abstract. Paper and pencil sketching, visual analogies, and creativity are intuitively interconnected in design. This paper reports on previous and current research activities of a psychologist (the 1st author) and an architect/designer (the 2nd author) on issues concerning sketching and analogies, and analogies and creativity respectively. In this paper we tried to unite these findings into a combined theory on how sketching, analogies, and creativity interrelate. An appealing theory emerges. It is hypothesized that with no paper available or no expertise to use it, analogies can be used to support the creative process instead of sketches. This theory, however, is a tentative one that needs more research to be confirmed. 1. Sketching Within creative professions, mental imagery often interacts with paper and pencil sketching. Artists and designers (among others) frequently engage into sketching and, without any proper paper available, will often report frustration and resort to newspaper margins, back of envelops, beer mats or the like. Although anecdotally, this frustration seems to be quite common, and demonstrates the importance of paper and pencil sketching as a necessary aid to mental imagery. Paper and pencil sketches are one form of externalization of mental images, and therefore can inform us about characteristics of these images.
    [Show full text]
  • Aristotle's Concept of Analogy and Its Function in the Metaphysics By
    Aristotle’s Concept of Analogy and its Function in the Metaphysics by Alexander G. Edwards Submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts at Dalhousie University Halifax, Nova Scotia August 2016 © Copyright by Alexander G. Edwards, 2016 for my parents, Jeff and Allegra τὸ δυνατὸν γὰρ ἡ φιλία ἐπιζητεῖ, οὐ τὸ κατ’ ἀξίαν· οὐδὲ γὰρ ἔστιν ἐν πᾶσι, καθάπερ ἐν ταῖς πρὸς τοὺς θεοὺς τιμαῖς καὶ τοὺς γονεῖς· οὐδεὶς γὰρ τὴν ἀξίαν ποτ’ ἂν ἀποδοίη, εἰς δύναμιν δὲ ὁ θεραπεύων ἐπιεικὴς εἶναι δοκεῖ. – EN VIII.14 ii Table of Contents Abstract iv List of Abbreviations Used v Acknowledgements vi Chapter 1: Introduction 1 Chapter 2: Analogy, Focality, and Aristotle’s concept of the analogia entis 4 Chapter 3: The Function of Analogy within the Metaphysics 45 Chapter 4: Conclusion 71 Bibliography 74 iii Abstract This thesis aims to settle an old dispute concerning Aristotle’s concept of analogy and its function in the Metaphysics. The question is whether Aristotle’s theory of pros hen legomena, things predicated in reference to a single term, is implicitly a theory of analogy. In the Middle Ages, such unity of things said in reference to a single source, as the healthy is said in reference to health, was termed the analogy of attribution. Yet Aristotle never explicitly refers to pros hen unity as analogical unity. To arrive at an answer to this question, this thesis explores Aristotle’s concept of analogy with an eye to its actual function in the argument of the Metaphysics. As such, it offers an account of the place and role of analogy in Aristotelian first philosophy.
    [Show full text]
  • Analogical Thinking for Idea Generation to Some Extent, Everyone Uses Analogies As a Thinking Mechanism in Daily Life (Holyoak & Thagard, 1996)
    Interdisciplinary Journal of Information, Knowledge, and Management Volume 11, 2016 Cite as: Kim, E., & Horii, H. (2016). Analogical thinking for generation of innovative ideas: An exploratory study of influential factors. Interdisciplinary Journal of Information, Knowledge, and Management, 11, 201-214. Retrieved from http://www.informingscience.org/Publications/3539 Analogical Thinking for Generation of Innovative Ideas: An Exploratory Study of Influential Factors Eunyoung Kim and Hideyuki Horii Center for Knowledge Structuring, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan [email protected]; [email protected] Abstract Analogical thinking is one of the most effective tools to generate innovative ideas. It enables us to develop new ideas by transferring information from well-known domains and utilizing them in a novel domain. However, using analogical thinking does not always yield appropriate ideas, and there is a lack of consensus among researchers regarding the evaluation methods for assessing new ideas. Here, we define the appropriateness of generated ideas as having high structural and low superficial similarities with their source ideas. This study investigates the relationship be- tween thinking process and the appropriateness of ideas generated through analogical thinking. We conducted four workshops with 22 students in order to collect the data. All generated ideas were assessed based on the definition of appropriateness in this study. The results show that par- ticipants who deliberate more before reaching the creative leap stage and those who are engaged in more trial and error for deciding the final domain of a new idea have a greater possibility of generating appropriate ideas. The findings suggest new strategies of designing workshops to en- hance the appropriateness of new ideas.
    [Show full text]
  • Resources for Research on Analogy: a Multi- Disciplinary Guide Marcello Guarini University of Windsor
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Scholarship at UWindsor University of Windsor Scholarship at UWindsor Philosophy Publications Department of Philosophy 2009 Resources for Research on Analogy: A Multi- disciplinary Guide Marcello Guarini University of Windsor Amy Butchart Paul Smith Andrei Moldovan Follow this and additional works at: http://scholar.uwindsor.ca/philosophypub Part of the Philosophy Commons Recommended Citation Guarini, Marcello; Butchart, Amy; Smith, Paul; and Moldovan, Andrei. (2009). Resources for Research on Analogy: A Multi- disciplinary Guide. Informal Logic, 29 (2), 84-197. http://scholar.uwindsor.ca/philosophypub/18 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Department of Philosophy at Scholarship at UWindsor. It has been accepted for inclusion in Philosophy Publications by an authorized administrator of Scholarship at UWindsor. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Resources for Research on Analogy: A Multi-disciplinary Guide MARCELLO GUARINI* Department of Philosophy University of Windsor Windsor, ON Canada [email protected] AMY BUTCHART Department of Philosophy Guelph University Gulph, ON Canada [email protected] PAUL SIMARD SMITH Department of Philosophy University of Waterloo Waterloo, ON Canada [email protected] ANDREI MOLDOVAN Faculty of Philosophy Department of Logic, History and Philosophy of Science University of Barcelona Barcelona, Spain [email protected] * The first author wishes to thank the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada for financial support over the four years during which this project was completed. ©Marcello Guarini, Amy Butchart, Paul Simard Smith, Andrei Moldovan. Informal Logic, Vol. 29, No.2, pp.
    [Show full text]
  • Textual Analogy Parsing: What’S Shared and What’S Compared Among Analogous Facts
    Textual Analogy Parsing: What’s Shared and What’s Compared among Analogous Facts Matthew Lamm1;3 Arun Tejasvi Chaganty2;3∗ Christopher D. Manning1;2;3 Dan Jurafsky1;2;3 Percy Liang2;3 1Stanford Linguistics 2Stanford Computer Science 3Stanford NLP Group mlamm,jurafsky @stanford.edu chaganty,manning,pliangf g @cs.stanford.edu f g Abstract mention According to the U.S. Census , almost 10.9 million African parsing Americans , or 28% , live To understand a sentence like “whereas at or below the poverty line , analogy frame only 10% of White Americans live at or compared with 15% of Lati- source U.S. Census nos and approximately 10% below the poverty line, 28% of African quant live at or below the povery line of White Americans . Americans do” it is important not only to whole African Americans value 28% identify individual facts, e.g., poverty rates visualization whole Latinos of distinct demographic groups, but also the value 15% higher-order relations between them, e.g., the 25 whole White Americans value 10% disparity between them. In this paper, we 20 15 propose the task of Textual Analogy Parsing plotting (TAP) to model this higher-order meaning. 10 The output of TAP is a frame-style meaning % at/below poverty line Wh. Lat. AA representation which explicitly specifies what Figure 1: In textual analogy parsing (TAP), one is shared (e.g., poverty rates) and what is maps analogous facts to semantic role represen- compared (e.g., White Americans vs. African tations and identifies analogical relations between Americans, 10% vs. 28%) between its com- them.
    [Show full text]
  • How to Use Analogies for Breakthrough Innovations
    A Service of Leibniz-Informationszentrum econstor Wirtschaft Leibniz Information Centre Make Your Publications Visible. zbw for Economics Schild, Katharina; Herstatt, Cornelius; Lüthje, Christian Working Paper How to use analogies for breakthrough innovations Working Paper, No. 24 Provided in Cooperation with: Hamburg University of Technology (TUHH), Institute for Technology and Innovation Management Suggested Citation: Schild, Katharina; Herstatt, Cornelius; Lüthje, Christian (2004) : How to use analogies for breakthrough innovations, Working Paper, No. 24, Hamburg University of Technology (TUHH), Institute for Technology and Innovation Management (TIM), Hamburg, http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:gbv:830-opus-1332 This Version is available at: http://hdl.handle.net/10419/55474 Standard-Nutzungsbedingungen: Terms of use: Die Dokumente auf EconStor dürfen zu eigenen wissenschaftlichen Documents in EconStor may be saved and copied for your Zwecken und zum Privatgebrauch gespeichert und kopiert werden. personal and scholarly purposes. Sie dürfen die Dokumente nicht für öffentliche oder kommerzielle You are not to copy documents for public or commercial Zwecke vervielfältigen, öffentlich ausstellen, öffentlich zugänglich purposes, to exhibit the documents publicly, to make them machen, vertreiben oder anderweitig nutzen. publicly available on the internet, or to distribute or otherwise use the documents in public. Sofern die Verfasser die Dokumente unter Open-Content-Lizenzen (insbesondere CC-Lizenzen) zur Verfügung gestellt haben sollten, If the documents have been made available under an Open gelten abweichend von diesen Nutzungsbedingungen die in der dort Content Licence (especially Creative Commons Licences), you genannten Lizenz gewährten Nutzungsrechte. may exercise further usage rights as specified in the indicated licence. www.econstor.eu How to use analogies for breakthrough innovations Dipl.
    [Show full text]
  • High-Level Perception, Representation, and Analogy: a Critique of Artificial Intelligence Methodology
    High-Level Perception, Representation, and Analogy: A Critique of Artificial Intelligence Methodology David J. Chalmers, Robert M. French, Douglas R. Hofstadter Center for Research on Concepts and Cognition Indiana University Bloomington, Indiana 47408 CRCC Technical Report 49 — March 1991 E-mail addresses: [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] To appear in Journal of Experimental and Theoretical Artificial Intelligence. High-Level Perception, Representation, and Analogy: A Critique of Artificial Intelligence Methodology Abstract High-level perception—the process of making sense of complex data at an abstract, conceptual level—is fundamental to human cognition. Through high-level perception, chaotic environmen- tal stimuli are organized into the mental representations that are used throughout cognitive pro- cessing. Much work in traditional artificial intelligence has ignored the process of high-level perception, by starting with hand-coded representations. In this paper, we argue that this dis- missal of perceptual processes leads to distorted models of human cognition. We examine some existing artificial-intelligence models—notably BACON, a model of scientific discovery, and the Structure-Mapping Engine, a model of analogical thought—and argue that these are flawed pre- cisely because they downplay the role of high-level perception. Further, we argue that perceptu- al processes cannot be separated from other cognitive processes even in principle, and therefore that traditional artificial-intelligence models cannot be defended by supposing the existence of a “representation module” that supplies representations ready-made. Finally, we describe a model of high-level perception and analogical thought in which perceptual processing is integrated with analogical mapping, leading to the flexible build-up of representations appropriate to a given context.
    [Show full text]
  • Reflections on the Growth and Development of Islamic Philosophy
    Ilorin Journal of Religious Studies, (IJOURELS) Vol.3 No.2, 2013, Pp.169-190 REFLECTIONS ON THE GROWTH AND DEVELOPMENT OF ISLAMIC PHILOSOPHY Abdulazeez Balogun SHITTU Department of Philosophy & Religions, Faculty of Arts, University of Abuja, P. M. B. 117, FCT, Abuja, Nigeria. [email protected], [email protected] Phone Nos.: +2348072291640, +2347037774311 Abstract As a result of secular dimension that the Western philosophy inclines to, many see philosophy as a phenomenon that cannot be attributed to religion, which led to hasty conclusion in some quarters that philosophy is against religion and must be seen and treated as such. This paper looks at the concept of philosophy in general and Islamic philosophy in particular. It starts by examining Muslim philosophers’ understanding of philosophy, and the wider meanings it attained in their philosophical thought, which do not only reflect in their works but also manifest in their deeds and lifestyles. The paper also tackles the stereotypes about the so-called “replication of Greek philosophy in Islamic philosophy.” It further unveils a total transformation and a more befitting outlook of Islamic philosophy accorded the whole enterprise of philosophy. It also exposes the distinctions between the Islamic philosophy and Western philosophy. Keywords: Islam, Philosophy, Reason, Revelation Introduction From the onset of the growth of Islam as a religious and political movement, Muslim thinkers have sought to understand the theoretical aspects of their faith by using philosophical concepts. The discussion of the notion of meaning in Islamic philosophy is heavily influenced by theological and legal debates about the interpretation of Islam, and about who has the right to pronounce on interpretation.
    [Show full text]
  • Analogy As the Core of Cognition
    Analogy as the Core of Cognition by Douglas R. Hofstadter Once upon a time, I was invited to speak at an analogy workshop in the legendary city of Sofia in the far-off land of Bulgaria. Having accepted but wavering as to what to say, I finally chose to eschew technicalities and instead to convey a personal perspective on the importance and centrality of analogy-making in cognition. One way I could suggest this perspective is to rechant a refrain that I’ve chanted quite oft in the past, to wit: One should not think of analogy-making as a special variety of reasoning (as in the dull and uninspiring phrase “analogical reasoning and problem-solving,” a long-standing cliché in the cognitive-science world), for that is to do analogy a terrible disservice. After all, reasoning and problem-solving have (at least I dearly hope!) been at long last recognized as lying far indeed from the core of human thought. If analogy were merely a special variety of something that in itself lies way out on the peripheries, then it would be but an itty- bitty blip in the broad blue sky of cognition. To me, however, analogy is anything but a bitty blip — rather, it’s the very blue that fills the whole sky of cognition — analogy is everything, or very nearly so, in my view. End of oft-chanted refrain. If you don’t like it, you won’t like what follows. The thrust of my chapter is to persuade readers of this unorthodox viewpoint, or failing that, at least to give them a strong whiff of it.
    [Show full text]
  • Greco-Islamic Philosophy and Islamic Jurisprudence in the Ottoman Empire (-): Aristotle’S Theory of Sciences in Works on Uŝūl Al-Fiqh Abdurrahman Atçıl*
    Greco-Islamic Philosophy and Islamic Jurisprudence in the Ottoman Empire (-): Aristotle’s Theory of Sciences in Works on Uŝūl al-Fiqh Abdurrahman Atçıl* Osmanlı İmparatorluğu’nda Yunan-İslâm Felsefesi ve Fıkıh İlmi (1300-1600): Usûl-i Fıkıh Çalışmalarında Aristo’nun Bilimler Teorisi Öz Bu makale Osmanlı İmparatorluğu’nun merkezi topraklarında 1300-1600 yıl- ları arasında dini ilimlerdeki gelişmelerin özelliklerinin tespit edilmesi yolunda küçük bir katkı sunmayı hedeflemektedir. Bu makalede Osmanlı usûl-i fıkh yazarlarının bu ilmin tabiatı ve içeriğine dair düşünceleri incelenmektedir. Bu yazarlar Aristo mantı- ğını benimseyen ve bunu kitaplarında kullanan seleflerinin görüşlerini benimsediler ve geliştirdiler. Onlara göre usûl-i fıkh ve füru-ı fıkhı kapsayan fıkıh ilmi Aristo’nun bir araştırma dalının bilim olarak adlandırılabilmesi için gerekli gördüğü tüm şartları taşıyordu ve bu ispatlanabilirdi. Anahtar kelimeler: Fıkıh; usûl-i fıkh; felsefe; Aristo mantığı; bilimler teorisi; Osmanlı İmparatorluğu’nda hukuk çalışmaları. A. Introduction Until the eleventh century, Islamic religious scholars had a cool attitude to- ward philosophy and Aristotelian logic. They considered the first as the reflec- tion on issues related to religion on the basis of pure reason, and the second as its method.1 The point of departure for scholars was the scriptural texts, the Qur’an and ģadith (reports of the prophetic tradition). Thus, uŝūl al-fiqh (theo- * Queens College, City University of New York. I am grateful to Cornell H. Fleis- cher, Baber Johansen and Muzaffar Alam for reading and commenting on an earlier version of this study. Two anonymous readers offered helpful criticism and saved me from many errors. All remaining ones are mine.
    [Show full text]