Two Eyes See More Than One Human Beings Have Two Eyes Located About 6 Cm (About 2.4 In.) Apart

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Two Eyes See More Than One Human Beings Have Two Eyes Located About 6 Cm (About 2.4 In.) Apart ivi act ty 2 TTwowo EyesEyes SeeSee MoreMore ThanThan OneOne OBJECTIVES 1 straw 1 card, index (cut in half widthwise) Students discover how having two eyes helps us see in three dimensions and 3 pennies* increases our field of vision. 1 ruler, metric* The students For the class discover that each eye sees objects from a 1 roll string slightly different viewpoint to give us depth 1 roll tape, masking perception 1 pair scissors* observe that depth perception decreases *provided by the teacher with the use of just one eye measure their field of vision observe that their field of vision decreases PREPARATION with the use of just one eye Session I 1 Make a copy of Activity Sheet 2, Part A, for each student. SCHEDULE 2 Each team of two will need a metric ruler, Session I About 30 minutes three paper cups, and three pennies. Session II About 40 minutes Students will either close or cover their eyes, or you may use blindfolds. (Students should use their own blindfold—a bandanna or long strip VOCABULARY of cloth brought from home and stored in their science journals for use—in this depth perception and other activities.) field of vision peripheral vision Session II 1 Make a copy of Activity Sheet 2, Part B, for each student. MATERIALS 2 For each team, cut a length of string 50 cm (about 20 in.) long. Cut enough index For each student cards in half (widthwise) to give each team 1 Activity Sheet 2, Parts A and B half a card. Snip the corners of the cards to eliminate sharp edges. 1 blindfold (optional)* 3 Each team of two will need a field of vision For each team of two collar, a length of string, a straw, half an index card, and a 2-in. piece of masking 1 box crayons* tape. 3 cups, paper 1 field of vision collar delta science modules Using Your Senses 23 © Delta Education LLC. All rights reserved. BACKGROUND INFORMATION Activity Sheet 2, Part A Two Eyes See More Than One Human beings have two eyes located about 6 cm (about 2.4 in.) apart. Each eye sees 1. How far apart are your eyes? about 6 cm the world from a slightly different viewpoint, 2. Draw a straight line from each eye to the ✗. or angle, and sends its own set of signals to 3. What do the two lines the brain. The brain combines these signals show? into one composite image that has depth to how each eye sees the it. Two images combined into one gives us world from a different depth perception, the ability to judge how angle far away an object is. Depth perception enables us to see the world in three dimensions instead of two. Our field of vision includes everything we can see at any given moment without 4. Work with a partner to drop pennies into the cups. a. Using two eyes. How many pennies dropped into the moving our eyes. When we look straight cups when you used two eyes? ahead, our field of vision encompasses a Answers will vary. b. Predict. Predict how many pennies will drop into the cups roughly circular region from shoulder to when one eye is covered. shoulder, forehead to waist. Moving our Answers will vary. eyes or turning our head changes what we c. Using one eye. How many pennies dropped into the cups when you used one eye? see but does not enlarge our field of vision. Answers will vary. 5. Was your depth perception better when you used one eye or Peripheral vision is the outer edges of two eyes? better with two eyes our field of vision. It enables us to see something without looking directly at it. Peripheral vision is what we use when Activity Sheet 2, Part B we see something “out of the corner of Two Eyes See More Than One our eye.” 6. Measure your field of 7. Now color in the diagrams vision. Your partner will to show your field of vision Due to the arrangement of light-sensitive record your test results in each test, like this: 0 below. 0 4 1 9 3 cells in the retina, our sense of sight is less 2 8 3 3 7 3 4 6 3 5 5 3 6 4 3 7 3 3 8 2 3 9 1 1 3 0 30 acute along the periphery of our visual field. 1 1 29 1 2 8 2 13 15 27 1 25 4 20 26 1 6 17 18 19 21 22 23 24 0 There we can detect motion, size, and With both eyes open I a. 0 a. 4 1 9 3 2 8 3 3 can see the index card 37 4 6 3 5 shape, but not color or fine detail. But 5 3 Varies 6 between 34 7 3 3 8 2 3 9 Varies 10 1 peripheral vision is important nonetheless. and . 3 30 11 29 1 2 8 1 2 3 1 7 1 5 5 2 4 20 2 26 It helps us cross the street safely! 16 17 18 19 21 22 23 24 0 0 4 1 b. With my left eye closed b. 9 3 2 8 3 3 37 4 I can see the index card 6 3 5 5 3 6 Depth perception is seriously compromised Varies 34 between 7 3 3 8 2 3 9 10 1 Varies 3 and . 30 11 29 with the use of just one eye. That is why 1 2 8 1 2 3 1 7 1 5 5 2 4 20 2 26 people who can see out of only one eye are 16 17 18 19 21 22 23 24 0 0 4 1 9 3 With my right eye closed 2 c. c. 8 3 3 often prohibited from driving or operating 37 4 6 3 5 I can see the index card 5 3 6 34 7 3 Varies 3 between 8 certain kinds of machinery. Likewise, our 2 3 9 10 1 3 30 Varies 11 and . 29 12 8 2 13 1 5 27 14 20 25 field of vision is much larger with two eyes 16 26 17 18 19 21 22 23 24 than it is with one. In short, one eye is not 8. Is your field of vision larger with one eye or with two eyes? simply a replacement, like a spare tire, for larger with two eyes the other. Our eyes work together to help us 9. How does your peripheral vision help you stay safe? negotiate our way in the world around us. Peripheral vision lets us see things, like a moving car, out of the corner of our eye. 24 activity 2 Two Eyes See More Than One © Delta Education LLC. All rights reserved. Guiding the Activity Additional Information Session I Safety Note: Encourage students to 1 Divide the class into teams of two. Distribute exercise caution when using the ruler a copy of Activity Sheet 2, Part A, to each around the eyes. student and a ruler to each team. Have students work together to measure the Tell students to hold the ruler flat against the distance between their eyes. tip of their nose (not in front of their eyes) while their partner looks at the ruler to see the distance from pupil to pupil. Ask, How far apart are your eyes? Students should find that their eyes are about 6 cm (2.4 in.) apart, or a little less. Tell students to record this measurement on question 1 of their activity sheet. 2 Draw an ✗ on the board. Tell students to hold out their thumb at arm’s length, close one eye, and line up their thumb with the ✗. Then tell students to switch eyes. Ask, What Students should notice that their thumb happens when you switch eyes? appears to “jump,” or change positions. It no longer aligns with the ✗. Ask, Has your thumb moved? no Explain that because our eyes are about 6 cm apart, each eye looks at the thumb from a slightly different viewpoint, or angle (see Figure 2-1). Switching eyes changes the angle from which the thumb is viewed, revealing the ✗ behind the thumb. Tell students to complete questions 2 and 3 on their activity sheet. Figure 2-1. At about 6 cm apart, each eye views the world from a slightly different angle. Ask, If each eye sees things from a different Students may suspect that the brain has angle, why don’t we see double? something to do with combining the images. Accept all reasonable answers. delta science modules Using Your Senses 25 © Delta Education LLC. All rights reserved. Guiding the Activity Additional Information Explain that when we look at an object with two eyes, each eye sends a set of signals to the brain. The brain combines the signals to form one image. Ask, Why do we need two eyes? Students may suggest that two are needed in case one gets injured. Tell students that one eye is not simply a replacement for the other. Our eyes work together to help us see better. They will learn how in the next exercise. 3 Distribute three paper cups and three pennies to each team. Ask students, Do you think you Students will probably say yes. could direct your partner’s hand to drop a penny into each cup with his or her eyes closed? With the help of a student volunteer, demonstrate how to do this activity, as follows: a) Place the cups on a desk or work table and sit about 4 feet away on one side of your student partner.
Recommended publications
  • Specific Eye Conditions with Corresponding Adaptations/Considerations
    Specific Eye Conditions with Corresponding Adaptations/Considerations # Eye Condition Effect on Vision Adaptations/Considerations 1 Achromotopsia colors are seen as shades of grey, tinted lenses, reduced lighting, alternative nystagmus and photophobia improve techniques for teaching colors will be with age required 2 Albinism decreased visual acuity, photophobia, sunglasses, visor or cap with a brim, nystagmus, central scotomas, strabismus reduced depth perception, moving close to objects 3 Aniridia photophobia, field loss, vision may tinted lenses, sunglasses, visor or cap with fluctuate depending on lighting brim, dim lighting, extra time required to conditions and glare adapt to lighting changes 4 Aphakia reduced depth perception, inability to sunglasses, visor or cap with a brim may accommodate to lighting changes be worn indoors, extra time required to adapt to lighting changes 5 Cataracts poor color vision, photophobia, visual bright lighting may be a problem, low acuity fluctuates according to light lighting may be preferred, extra time required to adapt to lighting changes 6 Colobomas photophobia, nystagmus, field loss, sunglasses, visor or cap with a brim, reduced depth perception reduced depth perception, good contrast required 7 Color Blindness difficulty or inability to see colors and sunglasses, visor or cap with a brim, detail, photophobia, central field reduced depth perception, good contrast scotomas (spotty vision), normal required, low lighting may be preferred, peripheral fields alternative techniques for teaching colors
    [Show full text]
  • 3D Computational Modeling and Perceptual Analysis of Kinetic Depth Effects
    Computational Visual Media DOI 10.1007/s41095-xxx-xxxx-x Vol. x, No. x, month year, xx–xx Research Article 3D Computational Modeling and Perceptual Analysis of Kinetic Depth Effects Meng-Yao Cui1, Shao-Ping Lu1( ), Miao Wang2, Yong-Liang Yang3, Yu-Kun Lai4, and Paul L. Rosin4 c The Author(s) 2020. This article is published with open access at Springerlink.com Abstract Humans have the ability to perceive 3D shapes from 2D projections of rotating 3D objects, which is called Kinetic Depth Effects. This process is based on a variety of visual cues such as lighting and shading effects. However, when such cues are weakened or missing, perception can become faulty, as demonstrated by the famous silhouette illusion example – the Spinning Dancer. Inspired by this, we establish objective and subjective evaluation models of rotated 3D objects by taking their projected 2D images as input. We investigate five different cues: ambient Fig. 1 The Spinning Dancer. Due to the lack of visual cues, it confuses humans luminance, shading, rotation speed, perspective, and color as to whether rotation is clockwise or counterclockwise. Here we show 3 of 34 difference between the objects and background. In the frames from the original animation [21]. Image courtesy of Nobuyuki Kayahara. objective evaluation model, we first apply 3D reconstruction algorithms to obtain an objective reconstruction quality 1 Introduction metric, and then use a quadratic stepwise regression analysis method to determine the weights among depth cues to The human perception mechanism of the 3D world has represent the reconstruction quality. In the subjective long been studied.
    [Show full text]
  • Interacting with Autostereograms
    See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/336204498 Interacting with Autostereograms Conference Paper · October 2019 DOI: 10.1145/3338286.3340141 CITATIONS READS 0 39 5 authors, including: William Delamare Pourang Irani Kochi University of Technology University of Manitoba 14 PUBLICATIONS 55 CITATIONS 184 PUBLICATIONS 2,641 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE Xiangshi Ren Kochi University of Technology 182 PUBLICATIONS 1,280 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects: Color Perception in Augmented Reality HMDs View project Collaboration Meets Interactive Spaces: A Springer Book View project All content following this page was uploaded by William Delamare on 21 October 2019. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. Interacting with Autostereograms William Delamare∗ Junhyeok Kim Kochi University of Technology University of Waterloo Kochi, Japan Ontario, Canada University of Manitoba University of Manitoba Winnipeg, Canada Winnipeg, Canada [email protected] [email protected] Daichi Harada Pourang Irani Xiangshi Ren Kochi University of Technology University of Manitoba Kochi University of Technology Kochi, Japan Winnipeg, Canada Kochi, Japan [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] Figure 1: Illustrative examples using autostereograms. a) Password input. b) Wearable e-mail notification. c) Private space in collaborative conditions. d) 3D video game. e) Bar gamified special menu. Black elements represent the hidden 3D scene content. ABSTRACT practice. This learning effect transfers across display devices Autostereograms are 2D images that can reveal 3D content (smartphone to desktop screen). when viewed with a specific eye convergence, without using CCS CONCEPTS extra-apparatus.
    [Show full text]
  • Stereopsis and Stereoblindness
    Exp. Brain Res. 10, 380-388 (1970) Stereopsis and Stereoblindness WHITMAN RICHARDS Department of Psychology, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge (USA) Received December 20, 1969 Summary. Psychophysical tests reveal three classes of wide-field disparity detectors in man, responding respectively to crossed (near), uncrossed (far), and zero disparities. The probability of lacking one of these classes of detectors is about 30% which means that 2.7% of the population possess no wide-field stereopsis in one hemisphere. This small percentage corresponds to the probability of squint among adults, suggesting that fusional mechanisms might be disrupted when stereopsis is absent in one hemisphere. Key Words: Stereopsis -- Squint -- Depth perception -- Visual perception Stereopsis was discovered in 1838, when Wheatstone invented the stereoscope. By presenting separate images to each eye, a three dimensional impression could be obtained from two pictures that differed only in the relative horizontal disparities of the components of the picture. Because the impression of depth from the two combined disparate images is unique and clearly not present when viewing each picture alone, the disparity cue to distance is presumably processed and interpreted by the visual system (Ogle, 1962). This conjecture by the psychophysicists has received recent support from microelectrode recordings, which show that a sizeable portion of the binocular units in the visual cortex of the cat are sensitive to horizontal disparities (Barlow et al., 1967; Pettigrew et al., 1968a). Thus, as expected, there in fact appears to be a physiological basis for stereopsis that involves the analysis of spatially disparate retinal signals from each eye. Without such an analysing mechanism, man would be unable to detect horizontal binocular disparities and would be "stereoblind".
    [Show full text]
  • Relative Importance of Binocular Disparity and Motion Parallax for Depth Estimation: a Computer Vision Approach
    remote sensing Article Relative Importance of Binocular Disparity and Motion Parallax for Depth Estimation: A Computer Vision Approach Mostafa Mansour 1,2 , Pavel Davidson 1,* , Oleg Stepanov 2 and Robert Piché 1 1 Faculty of Information Technology and Communication Sciences, Tampere University, 33720 Tampere, Finland 2 Department of Information and Navigation Systems, ITMO University, 197101 St. Petersburg, Russia * Correspondence: pavel.davidson@tuni.fi Received: 4 July 2019; Accepted: 20 August 2019; Published: 23 August 2019 Abstract: Binocular disparity and motion parallax are the most important cues for depth estimation in human and computer vision. Here, we present an experimental study to evaluate the accuracy of these two cues in depth estimation to stationary objects in a static environment. Depth estimation via binocular disparity is most commonly implemented using stereo vision, which uses images from two or more cameras to triangulate and estimate distances. We use a commercial stereo camera mounted on a wheeled robot to create a depth map of the environment. The sequence of images obtained by one of these two cameras as well as the camera motion parameters serve as the input to our motion parallax-based depth estimation algorithm. The measured camera motion parameters include translational and angular velocities. Reference distance to the tracked features is provided by a LiDAR. Overall, our results show that at short distances stereo vision is more accurate, but at large distances the combination of parallax and camera motion provide better depth estimation. Therefore, by combining the two cues, one obtains depth estimation with greater range than is possible using either cue individually.
    [Show full text]
  • Chromostereo.Pdf
    ChromoStereoscopic Rendering for Trichromatic Displays Le¨ıla Schemali1;2 Elmar Eisemann3 1Telecom ParisTech CNRS LTCI 2XtremViz 3Delft University of Technology Figure 1: ChromaDepth R glasses act like a prism that disperses incoming light and induces a differing depth perception for different light wavelengths. As most displays are limited to mixing three primaries (RGB), the depth effect can be significantly reduced, when using the usual mapping of depth to hue. Our red to white to blue mapping and shading cues achieve a significant improvement. Abstract The chromostereopsis phenomenom leads to a differing depth per- ception of different color hues, e.g., red is perceived slightly in front of blue. In chromostereoscopic rendering 2D images are produced that encode depth in color. While the natural chromostereopsis of our human visual system is rather low, it can be enhanced via ChromaDepth R glasses, which induce chromatic aberrations in one Figure 2: Chromostereopsis can be due to: (a) longitunal chro- eye by refracting light of different wavelengths differently, hereby matic aberration, focus of blue shifts forward with respect to red, offsetting the projected position slightly in one eye. Although, it or (b) transverse chromatic aberration, blue shifts further toward might seem natural to map depth linearly to hue, which was also the the nasal part of the retina than red. (c) Shift in position leads to a basis of previous solutions, we demonstrate that such a mapping re- depth impression. duces the stereoscopic effect when using standard trichromatic dis- plays or printing systems. We propose an algorithm, which enables an improved stereoscopic experience with reduced artifacts.
    [Show full text]
  • Binocular Vision
    BINOCULAR VISION Rahul Bhola, MD Pediatric Ophthalmology Fellow The University of Iowa Department of Ophthalmology & Visual Sciences posted Jan. 18, 2006, updated Jan. 23, 2006 Binocular vision is one of the hallmarks of the human race that has bestowed on it the supremacy in the hierarchy of the animal kingdom. It is an asset with normal alignment of the two eyes, but becomes a liability when the alignment is lost. Binocular Single Vision may be defined as the state of simultaneous vision, which is achieved by the coordinated use of both eyes, so that separate and slightly dissimilar images arising in each eye are appreciated as a single image by the process of fusion. Thus binocular vision implies fusion, the blending of sight from the two eyes to form a single percept. Binocular Single Vision can be: 1. Normal – Binocular Single vision can be classified as normal when it is bifoveal and there is no manifest deviation. 2. Anomalous - Binocular Single vision is anomalous when the images of the fixated object are projected from the fovea of one eye and an extrafoveal area of the other eye i.e. when the visual direction of the retinal elements has changed. A small manifest strabismus is therefore always present in anomalous Binocular Single vision. Normal Binocular Single vision requires: 1. Clear Visual Axis leading to a reasonably clear vision in both eyes 2. The ability of the retino-cortical elements to function in association with each other to promote the fusion of two slightly dissimilar images i.e. Sensory fusion. 3. The precise co-ordination of the two eyes for all direction of gazes, so that corresponding retino-cortical element are placed in a position to deal with two images i.e.
    [Show full text]
  • Collaborative Treatment for Vision After TBI
    Collaborative Treatment for Vision After TBI Tom Wilson, OD, COVD Terri Cassidy, OTR, CDRS Vision is Primary • Human vision is a complex process that involves the coordinated efforts of both the eyes and the brain. • We use central vision (focal) to see detail. • We use peripheral vision (ambient) to inform us of movement or a direction we should point our eyes. • Even if we feel like our eyes (and brain) are not giving us accurate information – we believe our eyes. 2 Case Study 1 56 y/o woman involved in a motor vehicle crash. She was able to walk and talk. MRI was negative. Not long after she notices symptoms of not being able to read correctly, headaches, motion sickness is much worse than normal, she can’t go back to work on the computer, she is very tentative about driving. Her eye doctor tells her that her eyes are fine, it must be her medications. Finally she is referred to Dr. Wilson who does an evaluation and refers her to vision rehab with OT. Presentation Title – Date (month #, ####) 3 Evaluation Neuro-optometric Assessment • Eye health • Binocular Vision • Prism evaluation • Function Occupational Therapy Vision Assessment • Goal of OT is to improve the client’s level of performance in a needed ADL. • Deficiencies in visual processing are significant to the OT Process if they interfere with occupational performance – doing the tasks that she wants and needs to do. Presentation Title – Date (month #, ####) 4 Evaluation Results OD Finds: OT Finds: Characteristics of Post Trauma Pt is not able to work more than 2 Vision Syndrome
    [Show full text]
  • Motion-In-Depth Perception and Prey Capture in the Praying Mantis Sphodromantis Lineola
    © 2019. Published by The Company of Biologists Ltd | Journal of Experimental Biology (2019) 222, jeb198614. doi:10.1242/jeb.198614 RESEARCH ARTICLE Motion-in-depth perception and prey capture in the praying mantis Sphodromantis lineola Vivek Nityananda1,*, Coline Joubier1,2, Jerry Tan1, Ghaith Tarawneh1 and Jenny C. A. Read1 ABSTRACT prey as they come near. Just as with depth perception, several cues Perceiving motion-in-depth is essential to detecting approaching could contribute to the perception of motion-in-depth. or receding objects, predators and prey. This can be achieved Two of the motion-in-depth cues that have received the most using several cues, including binocular stereoscopic cues such attention in humans are binocular: changing disparity and as changing disparity and interocular velocity differences, and interocular velocity differences (IOVDs) (Cormack et al., 2017). monocular cues such as looming. Although these have been Stereoscopic disparity refers to the difference in the position of an studied in detail in humans, only looming responses have been well object as seen by the two eyes. This disparity reflects the distance to characterized in insects and we know nothing about the role of an object. Thus as an object approaches, the disparity between the stereoscopic cues and how they might interact with looming cues. We two views changes. This changing disparity cue suffices to create a used our 3D insect cinema in a series of experiments to investigate perception of motion-in-depth for human observers, even in the the role of the stereoscopic cues mentioned above, as well as absence of other cues (Cumming and Parker, 1994).
    [Show full text]
  • From Stereogram to Surface: How the Brain Sees the World in Depth
    Spatial Vision, Vol. 22, No. 1, pp. 45–82 (2009) © Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 2009. Also available online - www.brill.nl/sv From stereogram to surface: how the brain sees the world in depth LIANG FANG and STEPHEN GROSSBERG ∗ Department of Cognitive and Neural Systems, Center for Adaptive Systems and Center of Excellence for Learning in Education, Science, and Technology, Boston University, 677 Beacon St., Boston, MA 02215, USA Received 19 April 2007; accepted 31 August 2007 Abstract—When we look at a scene, how do we consciously see surfaces infused with lightness and color at the correct depths? Random-Dot Stereograms (RDS) probe how binocular disparity between the two eyes can generate such conscious surface percepts. Dense RDS do so despite the fact that they include multiple false binocular matches. Sparse stereograms do so even across large contrast-free regions with no binocular matches. Stereograms that define occluding and occluded surfaces lead to surface percepts wherein partially occluded textured surfaces are completed behind occluding textured surfaces at a spatial scale much larger than that of the texture elements themselves. Earlier models suggest how the brain detects binocular disparity, but not how RDS generate conscious percepts of 3D surfaces. This article proposes a neural network model that predicts how the layered circuits of visual cortex generate these 3D surface percepts using interactions between visual boundary and surface representations that obey complementary computational rules. The model clarifies how interactions between layers 4, 3B and 2/3A in V1 and V2 contribute to stereopsis, and proposes how 3D perceptual grouping laws in V2 interact with 3D surface filling-in operations in V1, V2 and V4 to generate 3D surface percepts in which figures are separated from their backgrounds.
    [Show full text]
  • 20/20 VISION Part I: Eyesight, Perception and Vision
    20/20 VISION Part I: Eyesight, perception and vision In this first part of our three-part exploration of 20/20 vision, we are going to look at our physical eyes and how they work. Then we will explore perception and how the brain actually converts all that sensory information into images. Finally, we will look at some of the techniques for improving our vision naturally and ask if these really work. 20/20 vision is a term used to express normal visual acuity (the clarity or sharpness of vision) measured at a distance of 20 feet. ... If you have 20/100 vision, it means that you must be as close as 20 feet to see what a person with normal vision can see at 100 feet. Having 20/20 vision does not necessarily mean you have perfect vision. 20/20 vision only indicates the sharpness or clarity of vision at a distance. Other important vision skills, including peripheral awareness or side vision, eye coordination, depth perception, focusing ability and colour vision, contribute to your overall visual ability. Some people can see well at a distance but are unable to bring nearer objects into focus. This condition can be caused by hyperopia (far-sightedness) or presbyopia (loss of focusing ability). Others can see items that are close but cannot see those far away. This condition may be caused by myopia (near-sightedness). Is it possible to see better than 20/20? Yes, it's indeed possible to have sharper than 20/20 vision. In fact, most people with young, healthy eyes are capable of identifying at least some of the letters on the 20/15 line or even smaller letters on the Snellen chart.
    [Show full text]
  • Stereo Depth Perception
    Stereo Optical Company Vision Tester Slide Package: Driver Rehabilitation Slide Package For complete vision screening needs. Ideal for School, Athletic Physicals, Employment Physicals, Driver Licensing Exams, D.O.T., and General Check-Ups. Slide # 1: 2000-003 Slide # 2: 2000-010 Slide # 3: 2000-007 Slide # 4: 2000-012 Slide # 5: 2000-025 Slide # 6: 2000-013 Slide # 7: 2000-024 Slide # 8: 2000-087 Slide # 9: 2000-004 Slide #10: 2000-065 Slide #11: 2000-148 Slide #12: 2000-149 8600 W. Catalpa Ave., Suite 703 I Chicago, IL 60656 USA I 1.773.867.0380 I Toll-free 1.800.344.9500 Fax: 1.773.867.0388 I Email: [email protected] I www.StereoOptical.com P/N 56219 Slide 2000-003 “FAR” VISUAL ACUITY LETTERS 1. Dial at 1 (Yellow) indicator. 2. Far Point switch illuminated 3. Right and Left eye switches illuminated L R QUESTION: How many columns of letters do you see? SCORING: The answer is 3. Ask the subject to read line 5 completely. If this is correct, proceed to line 6, if correct proceed to line 7, if correct, the subject has 20/20 vision or better, at FAR point for the right eye, left eye and both eyes together. The correct scoring key is printed on the record form. Two or more letters incorrectly identified on any line (3 to 7) is considered a FAIL for that acuity level IN THAT COLUMN. A different acuity reading for each column is possible. The center column is critical because this is the binocular acuity test, while the right and left columns are monocular acuity tests.
    [Show full text]