The Influence of Phonology on Inflection. the Interplay Between

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Influence of Phonology on Inflection. the Interplay Between ADVERTIMENT. Lʼaccés als continguts dʼaquesta tesi queda condicionat a lʼacceptació de les condicions dʼús establertes per la següent llicència Creative Commons: http://cat.creativecommons.org/?page_id=184 ADVERTENCIA. El acceso a los contenidos de esta tesis queda condicionado a la aceptación de las condiciones de uso establecidas por la siguiente licencia Creative Commons: http://es.creativecommons.org/blog/licencias/ WARNING. The access to the contents of this doctoral thesis it is limited to the acceptance of the use conditions set by the following Creative Commons license: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/?lang=en The Influence of Phonology on Inflection The interplay between syllabification and lexical insertion in Pallarese Catalan Eduard Art´es Cuenca PhD Thesis Supervisor: Dr. Eul`alia Bonet Alsina Departament de Filologia Catalana Universitat Aut`onoma de Barcelona PhD Program in Cognitive Science and Language Barcelona, June 2016 2 A la meva mare Gender is a complexity whose totality is permanently deferred, never fully what it is at any given juncture in time. Gender Trouble Judith Butler 3 4 ABSTRACT This dissertation deals with the constraints that phonology imposes on the expo- nence of gender. In particular, it explores the interaction between epenthesis and nominal inflection in Pallarese Catalan clitics. Since inflection is located word-finally in Pallarese, I argue that right-edge epenthesis is avoided in order to maintain a strict correspondence between morphological and phonological structure. Inflectional vow- els may be used for syllabification purposes in word-final position instead. It is assumed in this thesis that every syntactic functional head projects a post- syntactic theme position (Oltra-Massuet, 1999) where gender is realized. A key aspect of the proposal is the idea that the exponents of gender are floating features, and thus inflection proceeds in two steps. First, the theme position is spelled out with an underspecified vowel (i.e., Th V) and, subsequently, the floating features ↔ associated with gender ([+fem] [+low], i.e., -[a]; [–fem] [+labial,–high], i.e., ↔ ↔ -[o]) are attached to the V-slot of the theme position. Couched within Optimality Theory, I argue that the constraint ranking determines whether the featural gender exponents surface or not. The feminine is always realized due to a constraint that favors parsing [+low] (which implies adding association lines that are absent in the input), whereas in the masculine the insertion of new association lines in the output is dispreferred and the features associated with [–fem] are not parsed, which accounts for default masculine -[Ø] exponence. If gender is part of the morphosyntatic com- position of a clitic, as in the 3rd person singular masculine accusative clitic, these floating features can nevertheless be attached to the V-slot of the theme position to improve syllabic structure under certain phonotactic conditions. This ‘morphologi- cal solution’ is less costly than (regular) word-initial epenthesis because it does not need to create a new skeletal position or insert new features. Impoverishment (Bonet, 1991) deletes gender features in the 3rd person plural accusative clitic, and thus the corresponding phonological features associated with gender cannot be used for syllabification purposes. The theme position with the 5 V-slot is maintained, though, and the default epenthetic features of Pallarese are inserted when required by phonotactics, which forces a thematic interpretation of this vowel. This solution is preferred over word-initial epenthesis because the theme position already provides a skeletal slot. The same procedure applies to other clitics that do not bear gender features either. Even though Pallarese shows a complex morphophonological intertwining regard- ing gender exponence, the OT analysis presented in this dissertation makes exclusive reference to phonological objects. The morphosyntactic structure of the nominal sys- tem constrains epenthesis, but strict modularity can be maintained. Furthermore, the use of floating features in the input can dispense with gender allomorphy (cf. Bonet et al. 2007). As for nouns and adjectives, the general process that spells out an underspecified vowel in the theme position, on the one hand, and floating place features for gender values, on the other, is only valid for default endings. That is, vowels other than -a (feminine) and -o (masculine) —when it surfaces— cannot be considered gender markers and need to be fully specified in the theme position of lexical entries instead. Therefore, nouns with non-regular endings are stored as complex representations and phonologically realized in one single step. This supports theories that assume that one exponent (or set of exponents) can spell out whole morphosyntactic structures (e.g., Siddiqi 2009, Caha 2009 or Berm´udez-Otero 2012). 6 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The time to thank all the people that have helped me in one way or another during this long journey has (finally!) come. Words cannot express how grateful I am to Eul`alia Bonet. She is the best supervisor one can ask for, and this thesis would not have been possible without her. I want to thank Eul`alia for her patience and availability in reading all the chapters of this thesis and answering all my questions (always with a smile), even the dumbest ones. Besides being an awesome linguist, Eul`alia is a humble person, something I truly appreciate. I am also very grateful to the members of my thesis committee. My heartfelt thanks to Joan Mascar´o(who has always been willing to help during my life as a PhD student at UAB) and Maria Rosa Lloret. I am particularly indebted to “Xico” Torres-Tamarit. Xico always encouraged me to submit abstracts to conferences to overcome my insecurities and, on many occasions, he had more faith in me than I did myself. In the end he was right and I was able to finish this thesis! I would like to thank all the people at the Centre de Ling¨u´ıstica Te`orica, where I felt at home during the four years of my grant, especially Mar Massanell (for her support with the fieldwork) and Teresa Cabr´eand Maria Ohannesian (both members of the Phonology Reading Group). I could not have survived the PhD without the countless coffee and lunch breaks at UAB with my colleagues: Teresa Blasco, Isabel Castro, Ekaterina Chernova, Adri- ana Fasanella, Javi Fern´andez, Jordi Fortuny, El´ıas Gallardo, Luc´ıaMedea Garc´ıa, Yurena Guti´errez, Maya Leela, Ares Llop (who also gave me the topic for this dissert- tion!), Cristina Real, Marina Roman, Carlos Rubio, Io Salmons and Qiuyue Zhong. I really miss my UAB life and the great moments we all spent together. I spent the Fall semester 2013 at the incredible MIT Department of Linguistics. I want to offer my thanks to Donca Steriade, who welcomed me into the department and transmitted her passion for phonology to me, and to Maria Luisa Freitas and Elena Nulvesu, for making a new place feel more familiar. 7 I also had a short stay at the University of Manchester in fall 2014. Yuni Kim always finds the time to help students —even when she does not have it— and I want to thank her for reading parts of this dissertation. I owe my deepest gratitude to Ricardo Berm´udez-Otero, as some of the theoretical assumptions of this thesis are based on our discussions. I really admire Ricardo’s capacity to find empirical evidence to support his theory in apparently unconnected phenomena. I have great memories of Manchester due to the people I shared my life with in those four months: Laura Arman, Fernanda Barrientos, James W. R. Brookes, Carlos Ivanhoe Gil Burgoin and Danielle Turton. Many thanks to Danielle Turton for last minute (almost literally) proofreading. Laura Arman has been my LaTeX Guardian Angel these last two years and also accepted proofreading the thesis at the very last moment. She is a great person and I cannot thank her enough for everything she has done for me. I am deeply thankful to the Consell Cultural de les Valls d’Aneu` (in particular, to Ferran Rella and Laura Grimau), the Ecomuseu de les Valls d’Aneu` , the town hall of Alins (Vall Ferrera), the Hotel Castellarnau (Escal´o), Marta Lluvich and N´uria Garcia Quera for helping me find participants for the study of Pallarese Catalan clitics. Special thanks should be given to the participants in the interviews held in May 2014 in Vall d’Aneu` and Vall Ferrera, from whom I learnt not only about language but also about rural anthropology. I would also like to thank Gus Mendia for the cover design. Finally, special thanks to Xavi, who has been very patient in this long process (especially these last two months), and my sister, because it is easier to feel lost in life by her side. Li dedico aquesta tesi a la meva mare, que sempre ens ha animat a estudiar. Arribar fins aqu´ıha valgut la pena per ella. 8 TABLE OF CONTENTS Abstract 5 Acknowledgments 7 1 Introduction 12 1.1 Thephonology-morphologyinterface . 12 1.1.1 Morphology and the lexicon . 12 1.1.2 TheoriesofInflection . .. .. 16 1.1.3 Morphology,phonologyorboth?. 22 1.2 Outlineofthethesis ........................... 28 2 The Pallarese Catalan clitic system 30 2.1 GeneralDescription............................ 30 2.1.1 Socio-GeographicInformation . 30 2.1.2 Methodology ........................... 31 2.1.3 Morphophonological description . 34 2.1.3.1 Combinations . 39 2.1.4 Syncretism in Pallarese Clitics . 42 2.2 Phonological Conditionings on Clitics’ Surface Forms . 44 2.2.1 Clitics in Isolation . 44 2.2.2 Clitic Clusters . 52 2.3 TheDefiniteArticle............................ 61 3 Nominal Inflection in Catalan and Spanish 64 3.1 Gender................................... 64 3.1.1 Catalan .............................. 65 3.1.1.1 Wheeler(1979) ..................... 67 3.1.1.2 Mascar´o(1985)and Mascar´o(1986) .
Recommended publications
  • Systemic Contrast and Catalan Rhotics*
    Systemic Contrast and Catalan Rhotics* Jaye Padgett - University of California, Santa Cruz (January 2003) 1. Introduction Catalan, a Romance language spoken primarily in the northeast of Spain, has two contrasting rhotics: a tap [r] and a trill [r]. The two sounds contrast only between vowels with the distribution otherwise being predictable. The facts closely parallel the better known facts of Spanish, and the problems they raise are similar. In this paper I take a new look at Catalan rhotics, arguing that we can achieve a better account of them if we explicitly acknowledge the role of contrast in their behavior. Specifically, the explanation calls on constraints requiring that contrast be maintained, on the one hand, and on constraints requiring that contrasts be perceptually distinct, on the other. The account is cast within Dispersion Theory (Flemming 1995, to appear), which is designed to express such notions. In approaching the Catalan facts in this way I follow Bradley (2001), who applies Dispersion Theory to Spanish rhotics. Some of the main conclusions we reach differ, all the same. A good deal of recent work argues that both neutralization avoidance and perceptual distinctiveness play important roles in shaping phonologies. (Besides the above, see Ní Chiosáin and Padgett 2001, Sanders 2002, Padgett to appear-a,b, and Sanders in progress.) The theoretical framework of these works, Dispersion Theory, remains rather new, and this paper offers another case study. The appeal to contrast leads to a simpler and more explanatory account of Catalan (and Spanish) rhotics compared to previous ones. The analysis also illustrates how it is possible to incorporate more systematic phonetic detail into phonology, increasing our descriptive coverage, without predicting the overgeneration of contrast.
    [Show full text]
  • Analyzing the Status of Catalan Schwa in Barcelona
    ICPhS XVII Regular Session Hong Kong, 17-21 August 2011 ANALYZING THE STATUS OF CATALAN SCHWA IN BARCELONA Susana Cortés , Conxita Lleó & Ariadna Benet Research Centre on Multilingualism, University of Hamburg, Germany [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected] ABSTRACT underlying schwa phoneme (the latter author points out that this schwa has the following This paper analyzes the production of Catalan distinctive features: [medial central-labial]). schwa by children and adults in two areas of Another approach is that of van Oostendorp's Barcelona differing in degree of presence of Projection Theory [9], based on metrical Spanish (which does not have schwa in its sound phonology. According to him, schwa is a defective inventory). The schwa production in different vowel, with unspecified distinctive features, kinds of words or in different positions in the word suitably fitting in weak and in extrametrical provides evidence for the status of this vowel. positions. Keywords: schwa, production, bilingualism, These theories, together with certain Catalan, phonological status assumptions about sound change, make different predictions about the substitution or maintenance 1. INTRODUCTION of schwa: For the last forty years, it has been observed that a) If non-alternating schwas are maintained the usage of schwa in Catalan is decreasing (Lóp ez more often than alternating schwas, this would del Castillo [7], Veny [13], Payrató [10], Pla argue for the phonological character of non- Fulquet [11], Herrick [4]). This reduction of the alternating schwa. phonological system seems to be due to external b) Position in the word (Catalan stress system (e.g., Spanish influence) or internal factors (e.g., being basically trochaic): If schwa is kept in markedness and complexity).
    [Show full text]
  • A Reduction and Spreading Theory of Voicing and Other Sound Effects""
    CatWPL 412, 1995, 267-328 A Reduction and Spreading Theory of Voicing and Other Sound Effects"" Joan Mascaró Universitat Autbnoma de Barcelona. Departament de Filologia Catalana 08193 Bellaterra (Barcelona). Catalonia (Spain) [email protected] Abstract A set of different surface effects commonly found in the phonology of natural lan- guages, voicing assimilation and final devoicing, are derived from two parameters, Reduction, the loss of phonological structure, and Spreading, the association of two unas- sociated nodes. Reduction is an operation on a node (e.g. voice), and it can take three values: no operation, Deassociation of the node, or Deletion of the node. Spreading has just the two minimal values, + and -. Spreading can combine with Reduction, or with Deassociation, resulting in two different assimilatory effects. When Spreading takes the negative value, or when contextually no spreading is possible, the surface effect that results is devoicing. The different parametric settings of Recluction and Spreading coupled with syllable control, and stages of application, predict the appropriate surfa- ce clusters of voicing effects. These results are extended to other effects like place centralization, unmarked assimilation, and to a set of residual or sporadic effects like progressive assimilation in regressive systems. Key words: phonological theory, voicing, assimilation, devoicing. Resum. Una teoria de reducció i d'escampament de la sonoritat i d'altres efectes sonors Tota una sbrie d'efectes superficials que es troben sovint en la fonologia de les llengües naturals, l'assimilació i l'ensordiment final, es deriven de dos parimetres: la Reducció, O NOTE OF THE EDITORS. Although CatWPL normally publishes the results of ongoing research, the editors have considered that this 1987 manuscript was worth publishing.
    [Show full text]
  • Vocale Incerta, Vocale Aperta*
    Vocale Incerta, Vocale Aperta* Michael Kenstowicz Massachusetts Institute of Technology Omaggio a P-M. Bertinetto Ogni toscano si comporta di fronte a una parola a lui nuova, come si nota p. es. nella lettura del latino, scegliendo costantamente, e inconsciamente, il timbro aperto, secondo il principio che il Migliorini ha condensato nella formula «vocale incerta, vocale aperta»…è il processo a cui vien sottoposto ogni vocabolo importato o adattato da altri linguaggi. (Franceschi 1965:1-3) 1. Introduction Standard Italian distinguishes seven vowels in stressed nonfinal syllables. The open ɛ,ɔ vs. closed e,o mid-vowel contrast (transcribed here as open è,ò vs. closed é,ó) is neutralized in unstressed position (1). (1) 3 sg. infinitive tócca toccàre ‘touch’ blòcca bloccàre ‘block’ péla pelàre ‘pluck’ * A preliminary version of this paper was presented at the MIT Phonology Circle and the 40th Linguistic Symposium on Romance Languages, University of Washington (March 2010). Thanks to two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments as well as to Maria Giavazzi, Giovanna Marotta, Joan Mascaró, Andrea Moro, and Mario Saltarelli. 1 gèla gelàre ‘freeze’ The literature uniformly identifies the unstressed vowels as closed. Consequently, the open è and ò have more restricted distribution and hence by traditional criteria would be identified as "marked" (Krämer 2009). In this paper we examine various lines of evidence indicating that the open vowels are optimal in stressed (open) syllables (the rafforzamento of Nespor 1993) and thus that the closed é and ó are "marked" in this position: {è,ò} > {é,ó} (where > means “better than” in the Optimality Theoretic sense).
    [Show full text]
  • The Syllabification of Catalan Clitics
    1 MORE ON ALIGNMENT AS AN ALTERNATIVE TO DOMAINS: THE SYLLABIFICATION OF CATALAN CLITICS Eulàlia Bonet Maria-Rosa Lloret (Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona) (Universitat de Barcelona) [email protected] [email protected] Abstract Catalan has fourteen pronominal clitic forms, each with different realizations depending on the context. Vowel epenthesis and consonant deletion, which are common in Catalan, seem to have a different conditioning in simple words, in Verb-clitic or clitic-Verb sequences, and in clitic-clitic sequences (where an emergence of the unmarked effect with respect to syllable structure is found). In this paper, devoted mainly to the phonological behavior of pronominal clitics in Catalan (specifically, the variety spoken in the Barcelona area) and framed within Optimality Theory, it is argued that, in spite of the apparent need for an analysis that resorts to levels, all the facts concerning epenthesis and deletion can be accounted for without them and with a single constraint hierarchy. The differences in behavior are the consequence of the different ranking of morphological Alignment constraints and an Alignment constraint that makes reference to subsyllabic constituents. 1. Introduction Catalan has fourteen pronominal clitic forms, and each of these forms can surface with several different realizations depending mostly on the phonological context. In addition, most clitics can combine with each other, and sequences can have more than four clitics at the same time. In (1) the variation in clitic shape is illustrated for the variety of Catalan spoken in the Barcelona area (Barceloní, from now on), which is the dialect that is examined in this paper.
    [Show full text]
  • Voicing and Continuancy in Catalan a Nonvacuous Duke-Of-York Gambit and a Richness-Of-The-Base Paradox
    Voicing and continuancy in Catalan A nonvacuous Duke-of-York gambit and a Richness-of-the-Base paradox Ricardo Bermúdez-Otero University of Manchester 23 November 2001 Please cite as: Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo (2001). “Voicing and continuancy in Catalan: a nonvacuous Duke- of-York gambit and a Richness-of-the-Base paradox.” Ms., University of Manchester. Available online at www.bermudez-otero.com/Catalan.pdf Linguistics and English Language University of Manchester Manchester M13 9PL United Kingdom [email protected] Contents 1. Optimality-theoretic phonology in crisis: the problem of opacity xx 2. The solution: interleaving xx 3. Voicing and continuancy in Catalan obstruents: an interleaved analysis xx 3.1. Postlexical spirantization xx 3.2. A puzzle: the failure of spirantization in the coda xx 3.2.1. Mascaró's solution xx 3.2.2. Harris's solution xx 3.3. Word-level devoicing and syllabification xx 3.3.1. Prefixes xx 3.3.2. Enclitics xx 3.4. The segment-rich lexicon, the stem level, and devoicing xx 3.5. Voice neutralization xx 3.5.1. The constraints xx 3.5.2. The rankings xx 3.5.3. Prevocalic prefix-final and word-final fricatives xx 3.6. Voice neutralization and spirantization: ordering relations in interleaved OT xx 4. A nonvacuous Duke-of-York gambit xx Ricardo Bermúdez-Otero Voicing and continuancy in Catalan — 2 3 Voicing and continuancy in Catalan obstruents: an interleaved analysis 3.1. Postlexical spirantization Standard Catalan possesses the following set of distinctive consonantal segments; see Wheeler (1979: xxii, 222, 234, chs V and VI), Recasens (1991: 173), and Hualde (1992: §3.1.1).
    [Show full text]
  • Stressed Postverbal Pronominals in Catalan
    Stressed postverbal pronominals in Catalan Item Type Article Authors Nadeu, Marianna; Simonet, Miquel; Llompart, Miquel Citation Stressed postverbal pronominals in Catalan 2017, 29 (1) Probus DOI 10.1515/probus-2016-0016 Publisher DE GRUYTER MOUTON Journal Probus Rights Copyright © 2016, Walter de Gruyter GmbH. Download date 05/10/2021 15:20:34 Item License http://rightsstatements.org/vocab/InC/1.0/ Version Final published version Link to Item http://hdl.handle.net/10150/623956 Probus 2017; 29(1): 119–162 Marianna Nadeu*, Miquel Simonet and Miquel Llompart Stressed postverbal pronominals in Catalan DOI 10.1515/probus-2016-0016 Abstract: Majorcan Catalan postverbal pronominal elements are typically described as being prominent due to stress shift from their host. This study sheds light on the prosodic phonology of these pronouns through the analysis of duration, vowel quality, and f0 in verb + pronominal sequences, which are compared to a baseline condition without pronominals and to the same sequences in a Catalan variety without stress shift. Our results show acoustic differences in the realization of pronominals in these varieties. The duration and vowel quality patterns are consistent with the stress shift account of postverbal pronominals in Majorcan Catalan. Analysis of f0 contours also reveals phono- logical differences across varieties. Whereas stressed postverbal pronominals are not rare in Romance, Majorcan Catalan is one of a much reduced number of varieties within the Romance domain, where the attachment of a pronominal element to a host triggers “true” stress shift rather than an additional promi- nence on the pronominal element, like Sardinian or Neapolitan. Keywords: stress shift, Catalan, postverbal pronominals, clitics, prosody 1 Introduction The current study is concerned with the phonetics and phonology of prosodic prominence in pronominal groups – i.
    [Show full text]
  • Systemic Contrast and Catalan Rhotics1
    Systemic contrast and Catalan rhotics1 JAYE PADGETT Abstract It is argued here that an appealing approach to the distribution and behavior of rhotics in Catalan can be had by appealing to the maintenance, and the perceptual goodness, of contrast. Since the Catalan facts are very similar to those of Spanish, the relevance of these ideas to Spanish is also discussed. The analysis is cast within Dispersion Theory (Flemming 1995, 2004), in a version following Padgett (2003a,b). The analysis is interesting because it (i) provides novel insight into the rhotic facts, (ii) extends dispersion-theoretic thinking into the area of consonants, and (iii) contributes to a program of research showing how allophonic generalizations can be explained by appeal to contrast. 1. Introduction Catalan, a Romance language spoken primarily in the northeast of Spain, has two contrasting rhotics: a tap [R]andatrill[r].Thetwosoundscontrastonly between vowels, with the distribution otherwise being predictable. The facts parallel the better known facts of Spanish in many respects. This article takes anewlookatCatalanrhotics,arguingthatwecanbestexplaintheirbehavior by appealing to the role that contrast plays. Specifically, the explanation calls 1. I am very grateful to Blanca Palmada, Jordi Cicres, Junko Itô, Ellen Kaisse, Shigeto Kawa- hara, Robert Kirchner, Silvia Llach, Vera Lee-Schoenfeld, Anya Lunden, Armin Mester, Máire Ní Chiosáin, and especially Travis Bradley, James Harris, and an anonymous reviewer for this journal, for reading versions of this article and providing feedback leading to its improvement. Thanks are also due to the audience at a presentation at the Zentrum für All- gemeine Sprachwissenschaft in Berlin, especially Laura Downing, Caroline Féry, Tracy Hall, Marzena Zygis,˙ and Hubert Truckenbrodt.
    [Show full text]
  • The Morphophonology of Russian Adjectival Inflection Morris Halle Ora Matushansky
    The Morphophonology of Russian Adjectival Inflection Morris Halle Ora Matushansky In this article, we present the morphosyntactic structure underlying the Russian adjectival declension and the phonological rules that apply to it to derive the surface representations. We describe the two declen- sion classes of Russian adjectives and argue that adjectives and nouns employ the same theme suffixes (-oj- and -o-) and, importantly, that choice of theme suffix also determines choice of Case exponents. On this view, there is no special adjectival declension class; instead, Case exponents are shared between adjectives and nouns, and the choice of a ‘‘paradigm’’ is determined by the choice of the theme suffix. The article covers all adjectival inflections, including those of the possess- ives, demonstratives, interrogatives, and paucal numerals. Keywords: Russian, adjectives, declension, theme suffixes, morphol- ogy, phonology 1 Introduction The goal of this article is to determine the underlying syntactic structure of Russian adjectives in all of their inflected forms. We aim to show that all inflected forms of an adjective have a simple, transparent morphological composition that is obscured by the operation of various phonological rules, independently motivated in Russian. Our more specific objectives are (a) to describe the two declension paradigms of Russian adjectives and (b) to determine the function of the theme suffix -oj- and of its absence from the surface representation. We are also concerned in this article with elucidating the nature of theme suffixes (so called by analogy with theme vowels of Latin and Catalan, on which see Oltra-Massuet 2000) and with comparing theme suffixes characteristic of adjectival declension classes with those of nominal declension classes and with those of verbal conjugation classes (Halle and Matushansky, in preparation).
    [Show full text]
  • Complete Paper
    Shattering Paradigms: An Attempt to Formalize Pressures within Subparadigms Maria Ohannesian and Clàudia Pons Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona 1. Introduction Paradigmatic pressures do not work in a homogeneous or symmetric way. As already noted by many scholars, factors such as the degree of phonological similarity, the degree of semantic closeness, the degree of productivity between the members of a paradigm, or the number of grammatical properties which these members share are directly correlated with the degree of phonological pressure exerted between them. Optimality Theory has built up many proposals to account for surface similarities between the members of a paradigm but none explicitly deals with the problem of inclusion, that is, with those cases in which given the paradigm set <A, B, C, D>, only a partial subset of the paradigm, say <A, B> or <A, B, C>, is under paradigmatic pressure. This paper is an attempt to deal with the notion of inclusion within paradigms. We show how the Optimal Paradigms model (McCarthy [2001] 2005) and the Transderivational Correspondence Theory (Benua [1997] 2000) can be extended to apply not only within flat paradigms but also within paradigms with an internal uneven structure, in such a way that nominal morphological categories like Gender and Number, verbal morphological categories like Tense, Number, Person, etc., or the productivity of a given derivative process are explicit in the formal machinery of the theory. Due to space reasons, we illustrate our proposal by focusing on two Romance phenomena – overapplication of cluster reduction in Catalan and overapplication of diphthongization in Spanish – but it can be extended to many other phenomena which will be referred to briefly when necessary.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 2 Conflicts
    Chapter 2 Conflicts Summary of the Chapter The issue of the kind of conflicts that are pervasive in natural language grammars is a good starting point for a detailed consideration of Optimality Theory. There are various driving forces in the constitution of grammars: lexical contrasts must be maintained in the interest of expressivity and contrast (this yields a set of faithfulness constraints), linguistic structures differ in terms of simplicity, possibly with respect to a number of dimensions (the realm of markedness constraints), and finally, the individual elements in a structure must be ordered relative to each other, and different levels of representations must be tied to each other (the domain of alignment). Conflicts between faithfulness and markedness, and among markedness and alignment principles, simply cannot be circumvented. Because it focuses on the resolution of these conflicts, Optimality Theory is the proper architecture for a theory of natural language. 2.1 Faithful or simple? A first source for conflicts The exploitation of the descriptive potential of conflicts among linguistic principles and their resolution is the key feature of Optimality Theory. Although conflicts have always figured in linguistic analyses (see the preceding chapter and chapter 5), one may wonder why a theory of language should focus on them so heavily. In a certain sense, conflicts always reside in the eye of the beholder only: the preceding chapter has revealed that conflict-free formulations of grammatical principles are certainly possible, to the extent that one is willing to complicate each linguistic rule by a list of exceptions. Admitting conflicts and optimality in the theory of grammar may make its overall architecture more complex, but the spirit of Optimality Theory is that this is a price one should be willing to pay: it allows a considerable simplification of the formulation of the individual principles of grammar.
    [Show full text]
  • Prosodic and Analogical Effects in Lexical Glide Formation in Catalan1
    Prosodic and analogical effects in lexical glide formation in Catalan1 TERESA CABRÉ AND PILAR PRIETO Abstract This paper provides a broad empirical description and a close examination of how Catalan speakers sillabify sequences of vocoids of rising sonority within the lexicon (e.g., piano ‘piano’, clariana ‘clearing’ or àvia ‘grandmother’). A survey with 381 words administered to 60 speakers has enabled us to identify two distinct varieties of Central Catalan: a more innovative variety (which dis- plays a stronger tendency to glide formation) and a more conservative variety. This situation, together with a certain degree of inter-speaker variation found in the data, reveals the existence of language change in progress. Both vari- eties display clear prosodic regularities: word-initial positional effects (that is, gliding tends to be blocked in word-initial position; cf. m[i"O]l ‘mewl’, p[i"a]no ‘piano’, d[i@]dema ‘diadem’) and distance-to-stress effects (that is, gliding in- creases when the distance to the tonic syllable is greater; cf. d[i@]lecte ‘dialect’ 1. We are grateful to E. Bonet, I. Chitoran, S. Frota, M.R. Lloret, M. Vigário and M. Wheeler for helpful comments and suggestions and, especially, to J.I. Hualde, M. Kenstowicz and J. Mascaró for lengthy discussions of some parts of the manuscript. Thanks are due to Francesc Rosés, who helped us in retrieving relevant data for preparing the questionnaire from the MOTZ database, and to all our informants and colleagues who have patiently answered our poll. Earlier versions of this work were presented in the following forums: XXIII In- contro di Grammatica Generativa (Trieste, March 2001), XXI Colloquium on Generative Grammar (Zaragoza, April 2001), and GLOW Colloquium 25 (Amsterdam, April 2002).
    [Show full text]