SYNCHRONOUS HATCHING IN FRESHWATER : METABOLIC AND ENDOCRINE PHYSIOLOGICAL MECHANISMS

Jessica Kate McGlashan

A thesis submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the School of Science and Health, Western Sydney University. Submitted August 2015

Supervisory Panel: Ricky-John Spencer, Michael B. Thompson & Fredric J. Janzen

STATEMENT OF AUTHENTICATION

The work presented in this thesis is, to the best of my knowledge and belief, original except as acknowledged in the text. I hereby declare that I have not submitted this material, either in full or in part, for a degree at this or any other institution

Jessica Kate McGlashan August 2015

This thesis is dedicated to all the creatures that come and go in life, but forever leave a mark…..

All my love to Tia, Toby, Charlie and all the turtles

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Dr Ricky-John Spencer, thank you for the support and guidance you have provided me throughout my Honours and PhD years. Thank you for taking me on as an honours student and believing in me enough to support me during my PhD. I owe this PhD and all the experiences that have come with it to you. Thank you for all the encouragement, and allowing me to pursue opportunities internationally. Professor Michael Thompson (aka Magic Mike), no words can really express how thankful I am for your constant support and motivation over the last 4.5 years. You have been the best academic grandfather a girl could ask for. As much as I frustrated you at times, you have always been the encouraging and supportive voice in my ear that has helped me get back on track. Your belief in me has really helped me through this and I couldn’t have done it without you and the support from the Thompson lab and Sydney University. Professor Fred Janzen, you provided me with an amazing opportunity to work in your lab and see how science is done in the USA. On such short notice you took me in and allowed me the use of your amazing field site and lab to complete a part of this research. You were always available to chat and provided me with guidance when things got tough. Thank you for sending me to Camp, it will go down in my memories as one of the coolest experiences ever… despite the mozzies, dangerous plants and crazy storms.

To all the labs that have welcomed me and supported me in some way or another. Firstly to the Spencer Lab, Megan Callander, Jessica Dormer, Tim Finter, Katie Howard, Fiona Loudon, Kristen Petrov, Lisa Robertson, and James Van Dyke (Van). You guys are awesome and I wish you all the best. A special mention to Fiona Loudon and Megan Callander, you guys are amazing and I wish you all the luck in the future. To the members of the Thompson Lab at Sydney University, Matthew Brandley, Jessica Dudley, Jacqueline Herbert, Van, Kevin Hendrawan, Melanie Laird, Erin McDonald, Jessie McKenna, Francesca Van Den Berg and Camilla Whittington. In particular, thank you Jacquie for all your help, especially with checking my references, not only that but you are a great friend and an awesome netball buddy. A special mention must go to Van, thank you for everything (there’s too many things to list), you have been a fantastic addition to both my lab families. You have helped my scientific thinking and writing and you are someone that I can rely on for pretty much anything, except sharing your food. Thank you so much for your guidance and support and

for taking the time (and there was a lot of it) to help me become a better scientist. You are also an awesome friend and I will be more than happy to go dive with you and the Great Whites again. Finally, to my overseas family at Iowa State University and the Janzen Lab, especially Tim Mitchell, Daleen Badenhorst, Jess Maciel, Brooke Bodensteiner and Cecelia Hinsley. Thank you for your lab and field assistance during my time at ISU, and most importantly your friendship and hospitality. A special mention to Tim for all the support you gave me and continue to give. You are a great person, friend, scientist and fisherman and I can’t wait to out fish you again someday.

Thank you to the technical staff, in particular Mark Emanuel, for your time and technical assistance in the lab and all the other amazing people who have helped along the journey. I like to thank the Webb family for their time, help, and use of their property. I would not have been able to complete this research without the funding provided by a UWS Postgraduate Award, UWS funded top-up award and the UWS SSH Equipment Fund Grant.

To my great friends in life who have always been so supportive and caring, Kelly Hourigan, Zoe Isles and Aly Stringfellow, you guys were the perfect distractions when I needed a break. But a very special mention to my favourite couple, Leone (Leeroy) and Cameron, thank you for putting up with me for so long, and allowing me to live with you to make my life easier. It means so much to me that you could open your house to me and provide the family support I needed in Sydney. Thank you to Mischa, your company and support has been amazing, especially in the final days. I love you all.

Most importantly, I would like to thank my family Ruth, Darryl and Steve for your constant love and support. I love you guys. Mum, you have, and always will be my best friend. I’m sorry for being such a brat at times and I really want to thank you for everything you do for me. You’re the reason I have finished this, you’re the reason I continue to strive in life and will never give up (great now I’m crying). Dad, thanks for always for being there for me and believing in me, I know that you are proud of me and what I’ve achieved and I promise I will continue to make you proud. Steve, my big bro, the one person I wanted to be most like growing up. Your calm and loving nature makes you the amazing person you are, thank you for also putting up with my brattiness. Most importantly, thank you for being there when life gets tough. To all my family, thank you for caring.

TABLE OF CONTENTS

LIST OF FIGURES ...... iii LIST OF TABLES ...... v ABBREVIATIONS ...... vi ABSTRACT ...... vii CHAPTER 1 ...... 1 GENERAL INTRODUCTION ...... 1 Thesis structure ...... 19 References ...... 20 CHAPTER 2 ...... 32 Title: Hatching behaviour of eastern long-necked turtles ( longicollis): The influence of asynchronous environments on embryonic heart rate and phenotype ...... 33 Abstract ...... 34 Introduction ...... 35 Method ...... 38 Results ...... 43 Discussion ...... 49 Acknowledgements ...... 53 References ...... 54 CHAPTER 3 ...... 59 Title: Synchronous hatching without metabolic compensation in the painted turtle (Chrysemys picta) ...... 60 Abstract ...... 61 Introduction ...... 62 Method ...... 65 Results ...... 70 Discussion ...... 75 Acknowledgements ...... 80 References ...... 80 CHAPTER 4 ...... 85 Title: Thyroid hormones reduce incubation period without developmental and metabolic costs in Murray River short-necked turtles ( macquarii) ...... 86 Abstract ...... 87 Introduction ...... 88

i

Method ...... 91 Results ...... 99 Discussion ...... 105 Acknowledgements ...... 109 References ...... 109 CHAPTER 5 ...... 117 Title: Synchronous hatching: triiodothyronine and corticosterone concentrations in the yolk during metabolic adjustment in the Murray River turtle () ...... 118 Abstract ...... 119 Introduction ...... 120 Method ...... 123 Results ...... 129 Discussion ...... 134 Acknowledgements ...... 137 References ...... 137 CHAPTER 6 ...... 144 GENERAL DISCUSSION ...... 144 Concluding remarks ...... 150 References ...... 153 APPENDIX ...... 157

ii

LIST OF FIGURES

CHAPTER 1

Fig. 1: Environmentally cued hatching scenarios for incubation period for a clutch of Emydura macquarii embryos……………………………………………………..……...…....2 Fig. 2: The effect of incubation temperature on length of incubation in developing desert iguanas (Dipsosaurus dorsalis) (Muth, 1980)………...……………………………………….6

Fig. 3. Thermal gradient experienced by freshwater turtles, where eggs at the top experience warmer temperatures than eggs at the bottom of the nest (Photos: Dormer (2012))..…...……7

Fig. 4: Fig. 4. The difference in temperatures experienced by eggs at the top and bottom of a nest over a 24 h period (Thompson 1988)……..………………………………………………9

Fig. 5: Fig. 5. Hypothetical diagram of a thermal performance curve. The temperature where performance is optimal is termed Topt and the points where performance is zero are critical temperatures (CTmin and CTmax).…………………………...……………………………...11

Fig. 6: Stylised metabolic profile of turtle embryos (eggs) throughout incubation displaying early hatching and metabolic compensation…………………………………………………14

CHAPTER 2

Fig. 1. Incubation method for asynchronous clutch set-up…………………………………..40

Fig. 2. Mean (M) heart rates (bpm) of embryos in synchronous (dark grey) and asynchronous (light grey) groups……………..……………………………………………………………..44

Fig. 3. The difference from mean heart rate of synchronous (dark grey) and asynchronous (light grey) groups over a 24 h period. Data from weeks 4 to 9 before hatching are combined, x-axis represents duration of time (h) away from the peak heart rate and y-axis represents the difference from the mean heart rate (bpm)…………………………………………………...45

Fig. 4. Mean incubation period (days) of eggs in synchronous and asynchronous group…...46

Fig. 5. Mean incubation period of eggs in groups (dark grey) and individuals (light grey)....47

Fig. 6. Mean yolk sac size (mm3) of synchronous (dark grey) and asynchronous (light grey) hatchlings……………………………………………………………………………………..48

CHAPTER 3

Fig. 1. Incubation method for asynchronous clutch set-up…………………………………..66

Fig. 2. Mean incubation period of eggs in asynchronous and synchronous groups…………71

Fig. 3. LSMeans of oxygen consumption (mL/h) of embryos in asynchronous (dark grey) and synchronous (light grey) groups from week 4 to 8 of incubation…….………………….72

iii

Fig. 4. LSMeans of heart rate (bpm) of embryos in asynchronous (dark grey) and synchronous (light grey) groups from week 3 to 8 of incubation…………………………….73

Fig. 5. The difference from mean heart rate of asynchronous (dark grey) and synchronous (light grey) groups over a 24 h period across weeks 3 to 8 of incubation. The x-axis represents duration of time (h) away from the peak heart rate and the y-axis represents the difference from the mean heart rate (bpm)………………………………………………………………74

CHAPTER 4

Fig. 1. Mean weekly O2 of Emydura macquarii are plotted against the Julian day of each measurement to demonstrate how total O2 was calculated over incubation period and post hatching resting metabolic rate (RMR)………………………………………………………95

Fig. 2. LSMeans of incubation period of eggs in treatment and control groups…………….99

Fig. 3. LSMeans of O2 of embryos in control groups: DMSO (dark grey) and H2O (medium grey) and treatment group: T3 (light grey)…………………………………………………..100

Fig. 4. LSMeans of heart rates (bpm) of embryos in control groups: DMSO (dark grey) and H2O (medium grey) and treatment group: T3 (light grey)…………………………………..102

Fig. 5. LSMeans of righting time (s) in treatment and control groups……………………...103

Fig. 6. Neonatal thyroid gland from 2 month old Emydura macquarii hatchlings. a: Normal neonatal thyroid gland (x10), b: Thyroid gland (x20) from control (DMSO) hatchling, c: Thyroid gland (x20) from control (H2O) hatchling, d: Thyroid gland (x20) from treatment (T3) hatchling………………………………………………………………………………..105 CHAPTER 5

Fig. 1. Incubation method for asynchronous clutch set-up…………………………………125

Fig. 2: LSMeans of corticosterone concentrations (pg/g) of embryonic Emydura macquarii embryos throughout incubation in asynchronous (dark grey) and synchronous (light grey) clutches……………………………………………………………………………………..130

Fig. 3. LSMeans of triiodothyronine concentrations (pg/g) of embryonic Emydura macquarii embryos throughout incubation in asynchronous (dark grey) and synchronous (light grey) clutches……………………………………………………………………………………...131

Fig. 4. Mean incubation period of asynchronous- more advanced eggs and synchronous- not advanced eggs……………………………………………………………………………….132

Fig. 5. LSMeans of yolk mass (dark grey) and embryo mass (light grey) ratio throughout incubation a) asynchronous b) synchronous….…………………………………………..…133

iv

LIST OF TABLES

CHAPTER 4

Table 1: LSMeans of estimated total oxygen consumption throughout incubation (IncEE) N = 36, total oxygen consumption from hatching to RMR measurement (RMREE) N = 36, and total oxygen consumption (TotalEE) (IncEE + RMREE) N = 22, ± SE……………………101

Table 2: Mean measurements (± SE) of the thyroid gland (width) and epithelial follicular ratio of T3, DMSO and H2O treated embryos of Emydura macquarii……………………...105

v

ABBREVIATIONS

TSD: Temperature-dependent sex determination

ECH : Environmentally cued hatching

CTE: Constant temperature equivalents

CTMin: Minimum critical thermal limits

CTMax: Maximum critical thermal limits

T3: Triiodothyronine

T4: Thyroxine

HPT: hypothalamic-pituitary-thyroid axis

HPA: hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis bpm: Beats per minute

M: Mean

SE: Standard error

SD: Standard deviation d: day(s) h: hour(s)

vi

ABSTRACT

Synchronous hatching occurs in many aquatic turtles and appears to have evolved to maximise hatchling survival despite predation and stochastic environmental conditions. In most cases, synchronous hatching occurs when cooler, underdeveloped embryos in a nest either accelerate their development through metabolic compensation or hatch early.

Synchronous hatching is more broadly a form of environmentally cued hatching (ECH), which generally allows embryos to alter the time of hatching in relation to the environment through phenotypic plasticity and hatch early, delayed, and/or synchronously. The mechanisms of ECH and synchronous hatching in are still poorly understood but both cause variation in developmental stage at hatching and reduce variation in incubation time to increase an individual’s chance of survival through a tradeoff between the risks and benefits of hatching. Reptilian embryonic development and incubation period are sensitive to temperature changes. Thermal gradients in shallow nests influence rates of embryogenesis, but how these are influenced is diverse given daily temperature fluctuations and seasonal trends. Differences in incubation conditions cause eggs at the top of a nest to experience warmer temperatures than eggs at the bottom, which increases metabolic and developmental rates of embryos and causes asynchronous development within clutches. Despite asynchrony in development, most clutches still hatch synchronously, which is thought to be an important survival strategy for hatchlings. Cold-incubated embryos are triggered to either hatch early and at a less developed stage or metabolically compensate to accelerate developmental rate and hatch fully developed. Metabolic compensation and matching of circadian rhythms in heart rates of embryonic turtles indicate the potential for communication between embryos in a nest.

vii

I aimed to identify the mechanisms of hatching synchrony in the eastern long-necked turtle

(Chelodina longicollis) and the painted turtle (Chrysemys picta) to determine if they synchronously hatch via hatching early or metabolic compensation. The Murray River short- necked turtle (Emydura macquarii) is known to synchronously hatch by metabolically compensating during embryogenesis. In E. macquarii, metabolic compensation occurs because embryos initially incubated at different temperatures are still able to hatch at the same size and developmental stages after eggs are re-introduced to one another. Cold- incubated embryos appear to “catch up” by increasing their developmental rate, which is correlated with higher metabolic rates during late development and reduced residual yolk sacs at hatching. In this project, asynchronous conditions were induced in clutches of C. longicollis and C. picta, and incubation periods and hatchling morphology were compared to determine whether they still hatched synchronously and whether cold-incubated embryos

“caught up” developmentally with their warmer siblings. I also assessed metabolic rates via oxygen consumption and heart rates to determine if synchronous hatching occurred as a result of metabolic compensation. The effects of a group environment during incubation on egg development and incubation period were also investigated in C. longicollis during the final 3 weeks of development.

Both C. longicollis and C. picta hatched synchronously and showed circadian rhythms in heart rates, similar to E. macquarii, but only C. longicollis metabolically compensated.

Chelodina longicollis embryos adjusted their heart rates during development and hatched without any differences in hatchling size or performance. Heart rate profiles in both C. longicollis and C. picta had periods of peaked activity and reduced activity or resting, over a

24 h period, regardless of the initial incubation period. Chrysemys picta did not show evidence of metabolic compensation but instead hatched early. Chrysemys picta hatchlings

viii

overwinter in the nest, and require yolk reserves to sustain them until emergence in the following spring. Metabolic compensation requires embryos to catabolise more of their yolk reserves prior to hatching and thus could reduce the energy reserves available during winter.

The loss of energy could be detrimental to hatchling survival prior to emergence the following spring. Circadian rhythms in embryonic heart rate could be a potential mechanism for embryonic communication within the nest, thus making group environments necessary for synchronous hatching. Eggs that are incubated in groups had significantly reduced incubation periods compared to eggs incubated individually. Eggs incubate in close contact with each other and heart rates could be detected and used to communicate developmental stage among clutch-mates, thus mediating synchronous hatching.

The physiological mechanisms that enable metabolic compensation and early hatching are unknown, but hormones are likely to play a critical role. Thyroid hormones and glucocorticoids regulate embryogenesis and also play roles in birth/hatching events in many . I aimed to identify the effects of the thyroid hormone triiodothyronine (T3) on the developmental rate of E. macquarii and determine if T3 or corticosterone (the primary glucocorticoid in reptiles) increase in concentration during development and prior to hatching and therefore may be responsible for metabolic compensation.

Triiodothyronine was applied to E. macquarii eggs at the same stage of development, and heart rates, oxygen consumption rates, and incubation periods of all eggs were measured.

Triiodothyronine treated eggs hatched up to 3.5 days earlier than untreated eggs, indicating that exposure to increased T3 can reduce incubation period. Eggs treated with T3 did not exhibit elevated metabolic rates, which indicated that these changes in incubation period were not mediated via metabolic compensation. Morphology and performance of hatchlings did not

ix

differ between treated and untreated hatchlings. Histology of the thyroid gland showed no difference between hatchlings and T3 did not affect sex ratios. Together, these results suggest that T3 increases developmental rate, but does so without metabolic consequences to hatchlings.

I also carried out hormone analysis on the yolks of E. macquarii embryos developing in asynchronous clutches to determine if hormone concentrations change throughout incubation in embryos induced to metabolically compensate. Corticosterone and T3 assays were undertaken at 9 time points during incubation, but there were no significant differences in the concentrations of either hormone between asynchronous and synchronous clutches. There was an increase in T3 concentration in asynchronous embryos from the final two time points measured and an increase in corticosterone in both asynchronous and synchronous clutches in the last time point. These increases coincided with the stage of development when embryos are preparing for hatching, which may be stressful and energetically demanding.

Triiodothyronine and corticosterone regulate many physiological processes but do not appear to induce metabolic compensation in E. macquarii. Application of excess T3 reduces incubation period by accelerating development without affecting hatchling size and without incurring a metabolic cost. Likewise, T3 concentrations increased at the end of incubation just prior to hatching only in the asynchronous eggs, which suggests a potential role in accelerating development for hatching synchrony. Corticosterone concentrations also increased at the end of development but the increase was seen in both treatments, which suggests that it is necessary for hatching.

Comparing the mechanisms used to synchronously hatch, further improves the understanding of the different ways in which ECH has evolved in reptiles. Emydura macquarii and C.

x

longicollis synchronously hatched by metabolically compensating, which enabled embryos to adjust their developmental rates and catch-up to more advanced eggs in the nest with no developmental costs, but hatched with reduced residual yolk. Metabolic compensation might evolve in species that emerge from the nest shortly after hatching because it allows similarly- sized hatchlings to emerge en-masse and swamp potential predators. These species will also be able to forage as soon as they enter aquatic habitats, so the loss of yolk reserves is unlikely to affect their energetic requirements. In contrast, C. picta synchronously hatches via hatching early, which may result in reduced hatchling size and reduced locomotor performance in some cases, but benefits hatchlings that overwinter in the nest. Hatchlings of C. picta will not be disadvantaged by loss of energy reserves from reduced residual yolk sacs, which increases their ability to survive without feeding until the following spring. Together, my results show that synchronous hatching has evolved independently in different turtle lineages (pleurodiran vs cryptodiran) and that the specific mechanisms utilised to achieve synchronous hatching in each species are likely to maximise hatchling survival given the different hatching behaviours they exhibit.

xi

CHAPTER 1

GENERAL INTRODUCTION

Phenotypic plasticity can be described as the interaction between environmental factors and genotype resulting in the expression of an environment-specific phenotype, referred to as reaction norms, which can impact on fitness and survival (West-Eberhard, 1989; Scheiner,

1993). Changes to embryos’ environments during development can alter the behaviour, morphology, and physiology of the embryo (Doody, 2011). Plasticity in developmental response to changes in the environment can express traits that will reduce immediate threats to their survival, such as hatching behaviour and performance (Shine and Deeming, 2004 ;

Doody, 2011; Du et al., 2013). Reptilian embryos have the ability to alter their time of hatching in relation to environmental conditions using phenotypic plasticity within the boundaries set by incubation temperature (Spencer et al., 2001; Colbert et al., 2010; Doody,

2011; Spencer and Janzen, 2011; McGlashan et al., 2012). The timing of birth can impact on the survival of offspring both immediately and in the future (Brinkhof et al., 1993;

O'Donoghue and Boutin, 1995; Sheldon et al., 2003; Doody, 2011).

1

Environmentally cued hatching Environmentally cued hatching (ECH) is when environmental conditions facilitate hatching which can result in variation in developmental stage or incubation time, and increase an individual’s chance of survival through a tradeoff between the risks and benefits of hatching

(Gomez-Mestre et al., 2008; Warkentin and Calswell, 2009). Hatching behaviour is diverse, and embryos have the ability to hatch early, delayed or synchronously, and in some cases, multiple forms can be expressed in one species (Fig. 1) (Packard and Packard, 2000; Doody et al., 2001; Warkentin and Calswell, 2009; Doody, 2011; Spencer and Janzen, 2011). The period of hatching competence is the earliest possible point during embryogenesis that hatching can occur and the neonate will be viable (Doody, 2011). This period varies for individual species depending on the ECH behaviour that is displayed and the incubation conditions.

Early Synchronous Delayed

Hatching Competence

Fig. 1: Incubation period for a clutch of Emydura macquarii embryos incubated at 30 °C (45- 51 days), pipping begins at day 45 (based on the metabolic profile of E. macquarii (Thompson, 1989)). The light grey area indicates length of incubation; the dark grey area indicates a plastic period for hatching and the final stage of development. The blue marker shows the point where early hatching can commence, the red marker and arrow shows the point where synchronous hatching can occur and the green marker and arrow shows the point where delayed hatching can occur.

2

Early hatching

Early hatching occurs immediately at the onset of hatching competence (Gomez-Mestre et al.,

2008). It is triggered by abiotic or biotic cues in the environment, but most commonly through disturbances within the nest (Sih and Moore, 1993; Warkentin, 1995; Warkentin,

2000; Warkentin and Calswell, 2009). Early hatching is observed in the tuatara, crocodiles, many snakes, lizards, and a few turtles from a variety of triggers, but most commonly from vibrations and movement within the nest, as seen in the Australian pig-nosed turtle

(Carretochelys insulpta) (Webb et al., 1986; Vitt, 1991; Doody et al., 2001; Doody, 2011;

Doody et al., 2012). Hatching early can incur developmental costs to the individual, including hatching at an earlier developmental stage, reduced fitness and smaller body size (Warkentin,

1995; Doody, 2011).

Delayed hatching

Delayed hatching occurs when embryos reach hatching competence but remain inside the egg for some period after completing development. The period that hatching can occur spans from the onset of hatching competence up to weeks, or months later (Gomez-Mestre et al.,

2008). Delayed hatching is when an individual or a whole clutch delay hatching until triggered by certain conditions or events to ensure optimal foraging ability and greater protection from predators (Webb et al., 1986; Doody et al., 2001; Warkentin and Calswell,

2009; Rafferty and Reina, 2012). Other strategies that result in delayed hatching in reptiles, include embryonic diapause and cold torpor (Rafferty and Reina, 2012). The ability to delay time of hatching until specific conditions are experienced is evident in a number of species of turtles, lizards, and crocodiles (Thorbjarnarson and Hernández, 1993; Doody, 2011). Delayed hatching is observed in the Australian eastern-long-necked turtle (Chelodina longicollis), but the trigger for hatching in this species is not known (Spencer, 2012). In addition to hatching

3

early, C. insulpta can delay hatching and aestivate underground until the onset of the wet season when nests become inundated with water, causing hypoxic conditions and triggering synchronous hatching of all eggs in the nest (Webb et al., 1986; Doody et al., 2001).

Synchronous hatching

Synchronous hatching occurs when all the eggs in a nest hatch within a short timeframe after reaching hatching competence. It is commonly observed in a majority of freshwater and marine turtles, lizards and crocodiles (Ims, 1990; Thompson, 1993; Spencer et al., 2001;

Pike, 2009; Colbert et al., 2010; Doody, 2011). Hatching is usually triggered by movement, vibrations, and sound within the nest (Lang, 1987; Spencer et al., 2001; Colbert et al., 2010;

Doody, 2011), or other environmental cues such as flooding, waiting for optimal conditions and/or hypoxia as in C. insculpta (Webb et al., 1986). The painted turtle (Chrysemys picta) hatches synchronously which is an overwintering strategy to gain an optimal position in the nest until they emerge in the following spring (Colbert et al., 2010). The Murray short-necked turtle (Emydura macquarii) hatches synchronously, potentially as a predator avoidance strategy (Spencer et al., 2001; Tucker et al., 2008; McGlashan et al., 2012).

Synchronous hatching in turtles

Synchronised hatching reduces an individual’s exposure to prey-switching predators and improves survival rates of the new recruitments. Individuals that are first to perform

(hatching and emerging from the nest) have a better chance of survival due to a lag time from when they start to leave the nest to when predators become aware (Ims, 1990; Testa, 2002;

Tucker et al., 2008). Emydura macquarii emerge shortly after hatching, and the threat of predation favours synchronous hatching and optimal performance. Chelodina longicollis are thought to be solitary nesters, and hatching and emergence may only be a risk to

4

opportunistic or generalist predators so the evolutionary drive for synchronous hatching is reduced (Spencer, 2012). Little is known about the hatching behaviour in natural nests but there are a few reports that delayed emergence or overwintering of hatchlings might occur in natural nests (Ferronato, 2015). Overwintering is where hatchlings delay emergence and hibernate in the maternal nest until the following spring. Hatchlings will spend the cool autumn and winter months in a state of low metabolic activity and emerge several months later when environmental conditions are optimal (Costanzo et al., 1995; Costanzo et al.,

1999; Gibbons, 2013). Chrysemys picta frequently overwinter in the natal nest, but they exhibit synchronous hatching (Colbert et al. 2010).

In preparation for the nesting season, turtles simultaneously ovulate from each ovary. Eggs are retained in utero and embryogenesis is arrested at the gastrula stage until oviposition within the maternal nest (Ewert, 1985). Turtle nest environments vary considerably among species. Marine turtles construct nests as deep as 400 mm (Ackerman, 1977), a depth where temperatures can be relatively stable (Booth and Astill, 2001a), but generally diurnal temperatures will fluctuate within the nest. Smaller marine turtle species and majority of freshwater turtle embryos develop in shallow nests much closer to the surface (50-200mm).

Embryos in shallow nests can be affected by temperature variation within the nest caused by ambient temperatures (Carr and Hirth, 1961; Booth and Astill, 2001a; Maloney et al., 1990) and internal metabolic heating (Bustard, 1971; Wallace et al., 2004) which can have environmental impacts throughout embryogenesis (Wilson, 1998; Wallace et al., 2004).

Embryonic development and incubation period are sensitive to temperature changes, with warmer temperatures influencing growth and developmental rate and resulting in shorter incubation periods (Fig. 2) (Deeming and Ferguson, 1991; Booth, 1998; Monaghan, 2008). In

5

shallow nests there is a thermal gradient that occurs between the top and the bottom (Fig. 3)

(Thompson, 1988, 1989). Daily temperature fluctuations and seasonal changes in ambient temperature will affect nest temperatures and as a result, affect egg developmental outcomes and developmental time (Thompson, 1988, 1989; Booth and Thompson, 1991; Thompson et al., 1996; Thompson, 1997; Booth, 1998; Shine and Elphick, 2001). Eggs at the top of a nest generally experience warmer incubation temperatures than eggs at the bottom, and differences can be up to 6 °C (Fig. 3) (Thompson, 1988, 1997). Egg position and thermal gradients in a nest can alter developmental rates of embryos, and have the potential to cause asynchronous clutch conditions (Thompson, 1988; Deeming and Ferguson, 1991; Thompson,

1997).

Fig. 2: The effect of incubation temperature on length of incubation in developing desert iguanas (Dipsosaurus dorsalis) (Muth, 1980).

6

50-200 mm

Fig. 3: Thermal gradient experienced by freshwater turtles, where eggs at the top experience warmer temperatures than eggs at the bottom of the nest. Photos: Dormer (2012).

7

The average temperature difference between the top and the bottom eggs in the nest over a 24 h period is 3 °C (based on daily nest temperature variations in Thompson, 1988; Fig. 4).

Temperature and development in natural conditions are not linear and mean temperatures are not a good indication of the effects seen in natural conditions. Constant temperature equivalents (CTE) are designed to replicate fluctuating conditions and determine natural biological processes to compare to constant temperature regime experiments. The CTE of a fluctuating temperature regime should result in offspring with similar phenotypic expression and developmental time as the fluctuating temperature regime (Georges et al., 1994). Daily temperature fluctuations in the field affect development of the Australian scincid lizard

(Bassiana duperreyi) independent of the effects of mean temperature (Shine and Harlow,

1996) because of the variation within a nest (Fig. 4). As daily nest temperatures rise, developmental rate will increase and the effect of temperature on development will be greater, but variation into extreme highs or lows may slow development or result in developmental inhibition.

8

Fig. 4: The difference in temperatures experienced by eggs at the top and bottom of a nest over a 24 h period. Temperature was measured at the beginning and end of a season, M ± SD and sample size is in parentheses (Thompson 1988).

9

Temperature will affect eggs differently depending on their position in the nest (Georges et al., 1994). These thermal variances will strongly influence rates of embryogenesis and the hatchlings phenotype, including morphology, activity levels and performance such as running speed and thermoregulatory behaviour, independent of the effects of mean temperature

(Shine and Harlow, 1996). Despite the variation in temperature causing developmental differences among embryos, hatching synchrony still occurs in many freshwater turtle nests

(Thompson, 1989; Doody et al., 2001; Spencer et al., 2001; Colbert et al., 2010; McGlashan et al., 2012).

Embryonic development and the environment

Temperature significantly affects all biological processes in at an organisational level to whole-body processes, including metabolism and growth (Wilhoft, 1958; Packard et al.,

1988; Rome et al., 1992; Packard and Packard, 1994; Johnston et al., 1996). Typically, physiological rates increase with rising temperature until the optimal temperature for that process is reached, after which performance declines rapidly (Fig. 5) (Huey and Stevenson,

1979; Damme et al., 1990; Angilletta et al., 2002; Schulte et al., 2011). There are limits for optimal temperature and exceeding those limits into minimum critical thermal limits (CTMin) or maximum critical thermal limits (CTMax), may be detrimental to development and survival

(Angilletta et al., 2002; Zhu et al., 2006; Du et al., 2007).

10

Topt

Tmin Tmax

Fig. 5: Hypothetical diagram of a thermal performance curve. The temperature where performance is optimal is termed Topt and the points where performance is zero are critical temperatures (CTmin and CTmax).

Mechanisms of synchronous hatching

Hatching synchrony in reptiles can occur either by delaying hatching (eg. C. insculpta (Webb et al., 1986; Doody et al., 2001), hatching early (eg. C. picta (Colbert et al., 2010)) or through metabolic compensation (eg. Eastern three-lined skink (Bassiana duperreyi) (Du et al.,

2010a) and E. macquarii (McGlashan et al., 2012)). Cues from more advanced eggs stimulate less advanced eggs to either hatch early or accelerate developmental rate to hatch at the same developmental stage (Colbert et al., 2010; McGlashan et al., 2012). These cues could include

11

sibling heart rate, CO2 concentrations in the nest (Spencer et al., 2001; Spencer and Janzen,

2011) or acoustic communication through stridulation, vibration and audible clicks (Vergne et al., 2009; Ferrara et al., 2013; Ferrara et al., 2014).

Embryonic development and metabolic compensation

The incubation period of turtles can be divided in to two phases; primary development and secondary development. Primary development is associated with organ and tissue growth

(Birchard and Reiber, 1996; Booth, 2000; Wallace et al., 2004; Mortola, 2009; Colbert et al.,

2010) and secondary development is the last stage of incubation and is associated with maturation of the neuromuscular system (Wallace et al., 2004; Colbert et al., 2010).

Metabolic rate in many precocial animals increases in a sigmoidal pattern (having an “S” shape) throughout development until they peak several days before hatching, and then decrease steadily (Thompson, 1989; Booth, 2000; Booth and Astill, 2001; Du et al., 2010b;

McGlashan et al., 2012). This decline following the peak is the period of secondary development and is thought of as a ‘resting stage’, important for sensory, neuromuscular and thermoregulatory system growth (Wallace et al., 2004; Peterson and Kruegl, 2005). This marked fall in metabolic rate occurs in embryos of C. insculpta (Webb et al., 1986), E. macquarii and the American alligator (Alligator mississipiensis), 80-90% of the way through incubation (Thompson, 1989).

Hatching can occur at any time from the beginning of hatching competence, which is any time after peak embryonic metabolism. If turtles developing in cooler conditions in the nest do not adjust developmental rates, they can only achieve synchronous hatching by hatching early (Fig. 6; outcome 1), which requires shortening of the secondary developmental stage,

12

and could adversely affect their development, physiological performance, and ultimately survival (Colbert et al., 2010).

Emydura macquarii hatch synchronously via metabolic compensation when incubated next to more advanced eggs (Fig. 6; outcome 2). Metabolic compensation enables adjustments at many levels of biological organisation to complete development and hatch synchronously with no performance costs, but in doing so they utilise more of their yolk reserves to fuel greater metabolic demands (Hazel, 1989; Rome et al., 1992; McGlashan et al., 2012), It is not known whether the mechanism for synchronous hatching in C. picta incorporates metabolic compensation, but their reduced performance suggests that early hatching embryos potentially miss the resting period for secondary development (Vleck et al., 1979; Peterson and Kruegl, 2005; Colbert et al., 2010).

13

Early Hatching

Metabolic Compensation

Fig. 6: Stylised metabolic profile of turtle embryos (eggs) throughout incubation at 26 ºC and 30 °C (based on the metabolic profile of Emydura macquarii embryos incubated at 26 ºC (Thompson, 1989) and 30 °C (McGlashan et al., 2012). Eggs at the top of the nest are incubated in warmer conditions (red line) and have a shorter incubation period than eggs at the bottom of the nest in the cooler incubation conditions (blue line). Two possible outcomes can occur in a nest, (1) embryos will not be stimulated to increase their metabolic rates but instead hatch early and miss secondary development (blue arrow) or (2) embryos will adjust their metabolic rates during the final third of incubation (broken black line), thus completing secondary development. The light grey box represents secondary development for eggs 30 °C and the dark grey represents the secondary development for eggs from 26 °C.

.

14

To understand the mechanisms that have led to the evolution of synchronous hatching in a species, it is necessary to identify the of hatching behaviour. Whilst we are aware that E. macquarii metabolically compensates to catch-up to more advanced clutch-mates, there are still many gaps in the understanding of what other mechanisms drive synchronous hatching and why different hatching behaviours exist. Two freshwater turtles from geographically different locations and different turtle lineages (pleurodiran vs cryptodiran) were selected to further investigate the mechanism of ECH. I aimed to determine the different physiological mechanisms of hatching in C. picta and the C. longicollis, as both species overwinter in the nest, and to compare them against what is already known in E. macquarii to further develop the evolutionary perspective of synchronous hatching.

Endocrine system

Thyroid hormones and glucocorticoids are likely candidates for regulating metabolic compensation because they regulate embryonic development, growth, and metabolism in animals (Himms-Hagen, 1976; Dauncey, 1990; Silva, 2001; Hollenberg and Forrest, 2008).

Less advanced embryos that metabolically compensate to catch up with more advanced clutch-mates may do so by shifting hormone concentrations during development. Increased hormone concentrations may be responsible for accelerated development in freshwater turtles that synchronously hatch through metabolic compensation.

Thyroid hormones

Yolk is the primary supplier of thyroid hormones to the embryo for early tissue growth prior to thyroid gland differentiation and hormone synthesis (Escobar et al., 1985; Escobar et al.,

1988; Prati et al., 1992). Thyroid hormone concentrations in the yolk decrease in the early stages of development as they are taken up by tissues, particularly for neurodevelopment

15

(Prati et al., 1992; Bernal and Nunez, 1995; Escobar et al., 2004; Morreale de Escobar et al.,

2004), but concentrations begin to increase in the body following the onset of endogenous synthesis by the thyroid (Prati et al., 1992). After the thyroid gland develops and is functional, hormone secretion is regulated through the hypothalamic-pituitary-thyroid (HPT) axis, and the thyroid gland becomes the major thyroid hormone producer to the embryo

(Thommes et al., 1977; McNabb and Wilson, 1997). Thyroxine (T4) is the major secretory product of the thyroid gland and the primary circulating hormone in the blood and is converted into triiodothyronine (T3) in the liver, and its targets cells. Triiodothyronine is the active hormone that is primarily responsible for the physiological effects in tissues (Becker et al., 1997; O'Steen and Janzen, 1999; Rivera and Lock, 2008; Axelband et al., 2011).

The importance of thyroid hormones early in development is evidenced by the presence of thyroid hormone receptors in the brain, heart and yolk sac in early stages of embryonic development, before the thyroid gland is functional (Perez-Castillo et al., 1985; Bradley et al.,

1992; McNabb and King, 1993). Thyroid hormones are required for the growth and development of embryonic tissues throughout development, but increases in concentration are strongly correlated with the final stages of embryogenesis, prior to hatching and immediately post hatching in crocodiles and turtles (Dimond, 1954; Shepherdley et al., 2002).

Thyroid hormones can accelerate development in embryos and metabolic rates of hatchling turtles and adult lizards (Chandola-Saklani and Kar, 1990; O'Steen and Janzen, 1999).

Increased concentrations of thyroid hormones accelerated development in amphibians and fish by speeding up stages of metamorphosis, and a reduction of thyroid hormones caused deformities and arrested development of fins and limbs (Miwa and Inui, 1987; Brown, 1997;

Callery and Elinson, 2000; Escobar et al., 2004; Morreale de Escobar et al., 2004). I aimed to identify the effect thyroid hormone T3 has on development and incubation of embryonic E.

16

macquarii. Given the effect thyroid hormones have on development, it might be possible that increased concentrations during embryogenesis will accelerate development in embryonic turtles, and potentially stimulate metabolic compensation and synchronous hatching.

Glucocorticoids

Glucocorticoids are also of maternal origin prior to development of the adrenal glands, and arise via endogenous regulation through the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis later in embryonic development (Wise and Frye, 1973; Freeman, 1974; Tona et al., 2003).

Glucocorticoids control actions and physiological responses to immediate stressors and prepare and aid adaptation to chronic stressors (Wingfield and Ramenofsky, 1999; Sapolsky et al., 2000).

In reptiles, corticosterone is the primary hormone released to modulate actions and maintain homeostasis in response to a stressor (Hanke and Kloas, 1995). Corticosterone regulates many physiological and behavioural processes that help the body adjust to stress and restore homeostasis, including regulation of energy storage and utilization, such as maintaining normal concentrations of glucose in blood and supporting increased metabolic demands

(Wingfield and Ramenofsky, 1999; McEwen and Wingfield, 2003; Cote et al., 2006; Klose et al., 2006; Landys et al., 2006; Dallman and Bhatnagar, 2010). Increased corticosteroid concentrations in blood plasma influences survival, size and body condition, stamina and metabolism i.e. oxygen consumption in birds and reptiles (Wikelski et al., 1999; Miles et al.,

2007; DuRant et al., 2008; Meylan et al., 2010). Corticosterone is synthesised by the adrenal glands from mid-development and increases in concentration in the embryo late in development, until hatching and is thought to be important for the change of respiration mode

17

after hatching (Pedernera, 1972; Wise and Frye, 1973; Kalliecharan and Hall, 1974; Medler and Lance, 1998; Thompson, 2007; Mortola, 2009).

Corticosterone and T3 have very similar biological effects on metabolism and neuromuscular development. Both hormones increase in the plasma of embryos around the time of hatching and are required for yolk absorption, egg emergence and post hatching performance and survival (Dimond, 1954; Wikelski et al., 1999; Miles et al., 2007). Embryos of the snapping turtle (Chelydra serpentina) have reduced yolk sac absorption and a greater rate of failure to complete hatching when treated with a thyroid inhibitor during embryogenesis (Dimond,

1954). In the side-blotched lizards (Uta stansburiana), increased corticosterone concentrations results in improved stamina by enhancing locomotor abilities and reducing recovery times and resting metabolic rates (Miles et al., 2007).

The physiological mechanisms that underlie metabolic compensation are unknown, and corticosterone and thyroid hormones could be responsible for increasing metabolic rates.

Embryonic stress responses trigger changes in corticosterone concentrations, and can increase circulating thyroid hormone T3 because it is linked to the conversion of T4 to T3 in avian embryos (Decuypere et al., 1983; Meeuwis et al., 1989). Similar patterns of increased corticosterone and T3 concentrations occur during development and metamorphosis in anurans (Kikuyama et al., 1993; Becker et al., 1997; Brown, 1997; Hayes, 1997; Fort et al.,

2007). Nest environments that result in asynchronous development of eggs may impact on the long-term stress of the embryo (length of stress period relative to incubation length). Stress may stimulate an increase in corticosterone secretion, causing an increase in T3 conversion that ultimately stimulates an increase in metabolic rates in the stressed embryo.

18

Production of T3 and corticosterone has been correlates and both hormones have similar physiological functions. I aimed to analyse both corticosterone and T3 hormone concentrations throughout the incubation period in E. macquarii embryos developing in synchronous and asynchronous clutch environments. Emydura macquarii are known to metabolically compensate, and this method is intended to identify if thyroid hormones and corticosterone are involved in regulating metabolic compensation and synchronous hatching.

I aimed to analyse both corticosterone and T3 hormone concentrations throughout the incubation period in E. macquarii embryos developing in synchronous and asynchronous clutch environments. Emydura macquarii are known to metabolically compensate, and this method is intended to identify if thyroid hormones and corticosterone are involved in regulating metabolic compensation and synchronous hatching

Thesis structure In this thesis, I analysed synchronous hatching in three species of freshwater turtles, C. longicollis, C.picta and E. macquarii. Specifically, I aimed to determine the hatching behaviour of C. longicollis embryos during asynchronous clutch incubation environments. I also assessed the heart rate of embryos and egg position in the nest to determine if either factor may influence hatching behaviour (Chapter 2). Chapter 2 is a published manuscript that identifies the hatching behaviour and the mechanism of synchronous hatching in C. longicollis. In Chapter 3 I aimed to determine the mechanism of synchronous hatching in C. picta and whether neonates are hatching early or metabolically compensating throughout incubation. Metabolic data, including heart rate and rate of oxygen consumption were used to assess metabolic rates in asynchronous clutch environments, and morphometric measurements are used to determine phenotypic consequences in both Chapters 2 and 3.

19

Chapters 4 and 5 address the function of the endocrine system during embryonic development and hatching in freshwater turtles. The hormones T3 and corticosterone have important functions in embryonic development and may trigger synchronous hatching through metabolic compensation and or early hatching. I aimed to test the effects of the exogenous application of T3 hormone on the developmental rate, hatching time and phenotypic traits of the freshwater turtle E. macquarii (Chapter 4). Hormone concentrations during asynchronous development will indicate the role of the both T3 and corticosterone hormones during metabolic compensation. I aimed to further investigate the role of T3, as well as corticosterone concentration throughout development in E. macquarii embryos in asynchronous clutch environments, through yolk hormone assays taken throughout incubation.

Finally, in Chapter 6 I present concluding thoughts on the mechanisms of synchronous hatching and the biological and ecological significance as well as the future directions in the field of ECH. The thesis is set out as a series of four manuscripts and may contain repetition of content between chapters due to the manuscript formatting.

References Ackerman, R. A. 1977. The respiratory gas exchange of sea turtle nests (Chelonia, Caretta). Respiration Physiology, 31, 19-38.

Angilletta, M. J., Niewiarowski, P. H. & Navas, C. A. 2002. The evolution of thermal physiology in ectotherms. Journal of Thermal Biology, 27, 249-268.

Axelband, F., Dias, J., Ferrão, F. M. & Einicker-Lamas, M. 2011. Nongenomic signaling pathways triggered by thyroid hormones and their metabolite 3-iodothyronamine on the cardiovascular system. Journal of Cellular Physiology, 226, 21-28.

Becker, K. B., Stephens, K. C., Davey, J. C., Schneider, M. J. & Galton, V. A. 1997. the type 2 and type 3 iodothyronine deiodinases play important roles in coordinating development in Rana catesbeiana tadpoles. Endocrinology, 138, 2989-2997.

20

Bernal, J. & Nunez, J. 1995. Thyroid hormones and brain development. European Journal of Endocrinology, 133, 390-398.

Birchard, G. & Reiber, C. 1996. Heart rate during development in the turtle embryo: effect of temperature. Journal of Comparative Physiology, B, 166, 461-466.

Booth, D. T. 1998. Incubation of turtle eggs at different temperatures: do embryos compensate for temperature during development? Physiological Zoology, 71, 23-26.

Booth, D. T. 2000. Incubation of eggs of the Australian broad-shelled turtle, (Testudinata : ), at different temperatures: effects on pattern of oxygen consumption and hatchling morphology. Australian Journal of Zoology, 48, 369-378.

Booth D.T. & Astill, K. 2001a. Temperature variation within and between nests of the green sea turtle, Chelonia mydas (Chelonia: Cheloniidae) on Heron Island, Great Barrier Reef. Australian Journal of Zoology, 49, 71–84.

Booth, D. T. & Astill, K. 2001b. Incubation temperature, energy expenditure and hatchling size in the green turtle (Chelonia mydas), a species with temperature-sensitive sex determination. Australian Journal of Zoology, 49, 389-396.

Booth, D. T. & Thompson, M. B. 1991. A comparison of reptilian eggs with those of megapode birds. In: Deeming, D. C. & Ferguson, M. W. J. (eds.) Egg Incubation: Its Effects on Embryonic Development in Birds and Reptiles. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Bradley, D., Towle, H. & Young, W. 1992. Spatial and temporal expression of alpha- and beta-thyroid hormone receptor mRNAs, including the beta 2-subtype, in the developing mammalian nervous system. Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 2288-2302.

Brinkhof, M. W. G., Anton, J. C., Hage, F. J. & Simon, V. 1993. Timing of reproduction and fledging success in the coot Fulica atra: evidence for a causal relationship. Journal of Ecology, 62, 577-587.

Brown, D. D. 1997. The role of thyroid hormone in zebrafish and axolotl development. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 94, 13011-13016.

Bull, J. & Vogt, R. 1979. Temperature-dependent sex determination in turtles. Science, 206, 1186-1188.

Bull, J. J. 1980. Sex determination in reptiles. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 55, 3-21.

Bustard H. R. 1971. Temperature and water tolerances of incubating sea turtle eggs, British Journal of Herpetology, 4, 196-198.

21

Callery, E. M. & Elinson, R. P. 2000. Thyroid hormone-dependent metamorphosis in a direct developing frog. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 2615-2620.

Carr, A., & Hirth, H. 1961. Social facilitation in green turtle siblings. Animal Behaviour, 9, 68-70.

Chandola-Saklani, A. & Kar, A. 1990. Evidence for the role of thyroxine as a hormone in the physiology of a lizard. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 78, 173-179.

Colbert, P. L., Spencer, R.-J. & Janzen, F. J. 2010. Mechanism and cost of synchronous hatching. Functional Ecology, 24, 112-121.

Costanzo, J., P. , Iverson, J. B., Wright, M. F. & Lee, R. E., Jr. 1995. Cold hardiness and overwintering strategies of hatchlings in an assemblage of northern turtles. Ecology, 76, 1772-1785.

Costanzo, J. P., Litzgus, J. D. & Lee Jr, R. E. 1999. Behavioral responses of hatchling painted turtles (Chrysemys picta) and snapping turtles (Chelydra serpentina) at subzero temperatures. Journal of Thermal Biology, 24, 161-166.

Cote, J., Clobert, J., Meylan, S. & Fitze, P. S. 2006. Experimental enhancement of corticosterone levels positively affects subsequent male survival. Hormones and Behavior, 49, 320-327.

Dallman, M. F. & Bhatnagar, S. 2010. Chronic stress and energy balance: role of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis. Comprehensive Physiology. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Damme, R. V., Bauwens, D. & Verheyen, R. F. 1990. Evolutionary Rigidity of Thermal Physiology: The Case of the Cool Temperate Lizard Lacerta vivipara. Oikos, 57, 61- 67.

Dauncey, M. J. 1990. Thyroid hormones and thermogenesis. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society, 49, 203-215.

Decuypere, E., Scanes, C. G. & Kühn, E. R. 1983. Effects of glucocorticoids on circulating concentrations of thyroxine (T4) and triiodothyronine (T3) and on peripheral monodeiodination in pre- and post-hatching chickens. Hormone and Metabolic Research, 15, 233-236.

Deeming, D. C. & Ferguson, M. W. J. 1991. Egg Incubation: Its Effects on Embryonic Development in Birds and Reptiles, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.

Dimond, S. M. T. 1954. The reactions of developing snapping turtles, Chelydra serpentina serpentina, to thiourea. Journal of Experimental Zoology, 127, 93-115.

22

Doody, J. S. 2011. Environmentally cued hatching in reptiles. Integrative and Comparative Biology, 51, 49-61.

Doody, J. S., Georges, A., Young, J. E., Pauza, M. D., Pepper, A. L., Alderman, R. L. & Welsh, M. A. 2001. Embryonic aestivation and emergence behaviour in the pig-nosed turtle, Carettochelys insculpta. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 79, 1062-1072.

Doody, J. S., Stewart, B., Camacho, C. & Christian, K. 2012. Good vibrations? Sibling embryos expedite hatching in a turtle. Animal Behaviour, 83, 645-651.

Dormer, J. 2012. Significance of incubation temperature on the life history of an Australian freshwater turtle. Honours, University Of Western Sydney.

Du, W.-G., Hu, L.-J., Lu, J.-L. & Zhu, L.-J. 2007. Effects of incubation temperature on embryonic development rate, sex ratio and post-hatching growth in the Chinese three- keeled pond turtle, Chinemys reevesii. Aquaculture, 272, 747-753.

Du, W.-G., Radder, R. S., Sun, B. & Shine, R. 2009. Determinants of incubation period: do reptilian embryos hatch after a fixed total number of heart beats? Journal of Experimental Biology, 212, 1302-1306.

Du, W.-G., Thompson, M. B. & Shine, R. 2010a. Facultative cardiac responses to regional hypoxia in lizard embryos. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology, Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology, 156, 491-494.

Du, W.-G., Tu, M.-C., Radder, R. S. & Shine, R. 2013. Can embryos influence their own rates of heating and cooling? PLoS ONE, 8, e67095.

Du, W.-G., Zhao, B. & Shine, R. 2010b. Embryos in the fast lane: high-temperature heart rates of turtles decline after hatching. PLoS ONE, 5, e9557.

DuRant, S. E., Romero, L. M., Talent, L. G. & Hopkins, W. A. 2008. Effect of exogenous corticosterone on respiration in a reptile. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 156, 126-133.

Escobar, G. M. d., Obregón, M. a. J. & Rey, F. E. d. 2004. Maternal thyroid hormones early in pregnancy and fetal brain development. Best Practice & Research Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 18, 225-248.

Escobar, G. M. D., Obregon, M. J., Oña, C. R. D. & Rey, F. E. D. 1988. Transfer of thyroxine from the mother to the rat fetus near term: effects on brain 3,5,3'- triiodothyronine deficiency. Endocrinology, 122, 1521-1531.

Escobar, G. M. D., Pastor, R., Obregon, M. J. & Rey, F. E. D. 1985. Effects of maternal hypothyroidism on the weight and thyroid hormone content of rat embryonic tissues, before and after onset of fetal thyroid function. Endocrinology, 117, 1890-1900.

23

Ewert, M. A. 1985. Embryology of turtles. Biology of the Reptilia. C. Gans, F. Billett, P.F.A. Maderson ed. New York: Development A. Wiley.

Ferrara, C. R., Vogt, R. C., Harfush, M. R., Sousa-Lima, R. S., Albavera, E. & Tavera, A. 2014. First evidence of leatherback turtle (Dermochelys coriacea) embryos and hatchlings emitting sounds. Chelonian Conservation and Biology, 13, 110-114.

Ferrara, C. R., Vogt, R. C. & Sousa-Lima, R. S. 2013. Turtle vocalizations as the first evidence of posthatching parental care in chelonians. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 127, 24-32.

Ferronato, B. d. O. 2015. Ecology of the eastern long-necked turtle (chelodina longicollis) along a natural-urban gradient, ACT, Australia. Doctor of Philosophy, University of Canberra.

Fort, D. J., Degitz, S., Tietge, J. & Touart, L. W. 2007. The hypothalamic-pituitary-thyroid (hpt) axis in frogs and its role in frog development and reproduction. Critical Reviews in Toxicology, 37, 117-161.

Freeman, B. M. 1974. Hormones in development In: Freeman, B. M. & Vince, M. V. (eds.) Development of the Avian Embryo. London: Chapman and Hall.

Georges, A., Limpus, C. & Stoutjesdijk, R. 1994. Hatchling sex in the marine turtle Caretta caretta is determined by proportion of development at a temperature, not daily duration of exposure. Journal of Experimental Zoology, 270, 432-444.

Gibbons, J. W. 2013. A long-term perspective of delayed emergence (aka overwintering) in hatchling turtles: some they do and some they don't, and some you just can't tell. Journal of Herpetology, 47, 203-214.

Gomez-Mestre, I., Wiens, J. J. & Warkentin, K. M. 2008. Evolution of Adaptive Plasticity: Risk-Sensitive Hatching in Neotropical Leaf-Breeding Treefrogs. Ecological Monographs, 78, 205-224.

Hanke, W. & Kloas, W. 1995. Comparative aspects of regulation and function of the adrenal complex in different groups of vertebrates. Hormone and Metabolic Research, 27, 389-397.

Hayes, T. B. 1997. Steroids as potential modulators of thyroid hormone activity in anuran metamorphosis. American Zoologist, 37, 185-194.

Hazel, J. R. 1989. Cold adaptation in ectotherms: regulation of membrane function and cellular metabolism. In: Wang, L. H. (ed.) Animal Adaptation to Cold. Springer Berlin Heidelberg.

Himms-Hagen, J. 1976. Cellular thermogenesis. Annual Review of Physiology, 38, 315-351.

24

Hollenberg, A. N. & Forrest, D. 2008. The thyroid and metabolism: the action continues. Cell Metabolism, 8, 10-12.

Huey, R. B. & Stevenson, R. D. 1979. Integrating Thermal Physiology and Ecology of Ectotherms: A Discussion of Approaches. American Zoologist, 19, 357-366.

Ims, R. A. 1990. The ecology and evolution of reproductive synchrony. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 5, 135-140.

Johnston, I. A. & Temple, G. K. 2002. Thermal plasticity of skeletal muscle phenotype in ectothermic vertebrates and its significance for locomotory behaviour. Journal of Experimental Biology, 205, 2305-2322.

Johnston, I. A., Vieira, V. L. A. & Hill, J. 1996. Temperature and ontogeny in ectotherms: muscle phenotype in fish. In: Press, C. U. (ed.) Animals and Temperature: Phenotypic and Evolutionary Adaptation. I.A. Johnston, A.F. Bennett ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Kalliecharan, R. & Hall, B. K. 1974. A developmental study of the levels of progesterone, corticosterone, cortisol, and cortisone circulating in plasma of chick embryos. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 24, 364-372.

Kikuyama, S., Kawamura, K., Tanaka, S. & Yamamoto, K. 1993. Aspects of amphibian metamorphosis: Hormonal control. International Review of Cytol.

Klose, S., Smith, C., Denzel, A. & Kalko, E. 2006. Reproduction elevates the corticosterone stress response in common fruit bats. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology, 192, 341-350.

Landys, M. M., Ramenofsky, M. & Wingfield, J. C. 2006. Actions of glucocorticoids at a seasonal baseline as compared to stress-related levels in the regulation of periodic life processes. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 148, 132-149.

Lang, J. W. 1987. Crocodilian behaviour: implications for management. In: Webb, C. J. W., Manolis, S. C. & Whitehead, P. J. (eds.) Wildlife Management: Crocodiles and Alligators. Sydney: Surrey Beatty and Sons.

McEwen, B. S. & Wingfield, J. C. 2003. The concept of allostasis in biology and biomedicine. Hormones and Behavior, 43, 2-15.

McGlashan, J. K., Spencer, R.-J. & Old, J. M. 2012. Embryonic communication in the nest: metabolic responses of reptilian embryos to developmental rates of siblings. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279, 1709-1715.

McNabb, A. F. M. & Wilson, C. M. 1997. Thyroid hormone deposition in avian eggs and effects on embryonic development. American Zoologist, 37, 553-560.

25

McNabb, F. M. A. & King, D. B. 1993. Thyroid hormone effects on growth, development, and metabolism. The Endocrinology of Growth, Development, and Metabolism in Vertebrates. New York: Academic Press.

Maloney J. E., Darian-Smith, C., Takahashi, Y., & Limpus, C. J. 1990. The environment of the embryonic loggerhead turtle (Caretta caretta) in Queensland. Copeia, 1990, 378– 387.

Medler, K. F. & Lance, V. A. 1998. Sex differences in plasma corticosterone levels in alligator (Alligator mississippiensis) embryos. Journal of Experimental Zoology, 280, 238-244.

Meeuwis, R., Michielsen, R., Decuypere, E. & Kühn, E. R. 1989. Thyrotropic activity of the ovine corticotropin-releasing factor in the chick embryo. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 76, 357-363.

Meylan, S., Haussy, C. & Voituron, Y. 2010. Physiological actions of corticosterone and its modulation by an immune challenge in reptiles. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 169, 158-166.

Miles, D. B., Calsbeek, R. & Sinervo, B. 2007. Corticosterone, locomotor performance, and metabolism in side-blotched lizards (Uta stansburiana). Hormones and Behavior, 51, 548-554.

Miwa, S. & Inui, Y. 1987. Effects of various doses of thyroxine and triiodothyronine on the metamorphosis of flounder (Paralichthys olivaceus). General and Comparative Endocrinology, 67, 356-363.

Monaghan, P. 2008. Early growth conditions, phenotypic development and environmental change. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 363, 1635-1645.

Morreale de Escobar, G., Obregon, M. & Escobar del Rey, F. 2004. Role of thyroid hormone during early brain development. European Journal of Endocrinology, 151, U25-U37.

Mortola, J. P. 2009. Gas exchange in avian embryos and hatchlings. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology, Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology, 153, 359- 377.

Muth, A. 1980. Physiological Ecology of Desert Iguana (Dipsosaurus Dorsalis) Eggs: Temperature and Water Relations. Ecology, 61, 1335-1343.

O'Donoghue, M. & Boutin, S. 1995. Does reproductive synchrony affect juvenile survival rates of northern mammals? Oikos, 74, 115-121.

26

O'Steen, S. & Janzen, Fredric J. 1999. Embryonic temperature affects metabolic compensation and thyroid hormones in hatchling snapping turtles. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology, 72, 520-533.

Packard, G., Packard, M., Miller, K. & Boardman, T. 1988. Effects of temperature and moisture during incubation on carcass composition of hatchling snapping turtles (Chelydra serpentina). Journal of Comparative Physiology, B, 158, 117-125.

Packard, G. C. & Packard, M. J. 1988. The physiological ecology of reptilian eggs and embryos. Gans, C., Huey, R.B. (eds.), Biology of the Reptilia. New York: Wiley.

Packard, G. C. & Packard, M. J. 1994. The physiological ecology of reptilian eggs and embryos. Biology of the Reptilia. C. Gans and R.B. Huey ed. Ann Arbor, Mich: Branta.

Packard, G. C. & Packard, M. J. 2000. Developmental plasticity in painted turtles, Chrysemys picta. Functional Ecology, 14, 474-483.

Pedernera, E. A. 1972. Adrenocorticotropic activity in vitro of the chick embryo pituitary gland. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 19, 589-591.

Perez-Castillo, A., Bernal, J., Ferreiro, B. & Pans, T. 1985. The early ontogenesis of thyroid hormone receptor in the rat fetus. Endocrinology, 117, 2457-2461.

Peterson, C. C. & Kruegl, A. 2005. Peaked temporal pattern of embryonic metabolism in an emydid turtle (Chrysemys picta picta). Journal of Herpetology, 39, 678-681.

Pike, D. A. 2009. Do green turtles modify their nesting seasons in response to environmental temperatures? Chelonian Conservation and Biology, 8, 43-47.

Prati, M., Calvo, R., Morreale, G. & Escobar, G. M. d. 1992. L-thyroxine and 3,5,3'- triiodothyronine concentrations in the chicken egg and in the embryo before and after the onset of thyroid function. Endocrinology, 130, 2651-2659.

Price, T. D., Qvarnström, A. & Irwin, D. E. 2003. The role of phenotypic plasticity in driving genetic evolution. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 270, 1433-1440.

Rafferty, A. R. & Reina, R. D. 2012. Arrested embryonic development: a review of strategies to delay hatching in egg-laying reptiles. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279, 2299-2308.

Rivera, S. & Lock, B. 2008. The reptilian thyroid and parathyroid glands. Veterinary Clinics of North America: Exotic Animal Practice, 11, 163-175.

27

Rome, L. C., Stevens, E. D. & John-Alder, H. B. 1992. The influence of temperature and thermal acclimation on physiological function. Environmental Physiology of the Amphibians. M.E. Feder and W.W. Burggren ed. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Sapolsky, R. M., Romero, L. M. & Munck, A. U. 2000. How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocrine Reviews, 21, 55-89.

Scheiner, S. M. 1993. Genetics and Evolution of Phenotypic Plasticity. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 24, 35-68.

Schulte, P. M., Healy, T. M. & Fangue, N. A. 2011. Thermal performance curves, phenotypic plasticity, and the time scales of temperature exposure. Integrative and Comparative Biology.

Sheldon, B. C., Kruuk, L. E. B. & Merila, J. 2003. Natural selection and inheritance of breeding time and clutch size in the collared flycatcher. Evolution, 57, 406-420.

Shepherdley, C. A., Daniels, C. B., Orgeig, S., Richardson, S. J., Evans, B. K. & Darras, V. M. 2002. Glucocorticoids, thyroid hormones, and iodothyronine deiodinases in embryonic saltwater crocodiles. American Journal of Physiology: Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 283, 1155-1163.

Shine, R. & Deeming, D. C. 2004 Adaptive consequences of developmental plasticity. Reptilian incubation: environment, evolution and behaviour.

Shine, R. & Elphick, M. J. 2001. The effect of short-term weather fluctuations on temperatures inside lizard nests, and on the phenotypic traits of hatchling lizards. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 72, 555-565.

Shine, R. & Harlow, P. S. 1996. Maternal manipulation of offspring phenotypes via nest-site selection in an oviparous lizard. Ecology, 77, 1808-1817.

Sih, A. & Moore, R. D. 1993. Delayed hatching of salamander eggs in response to enhanced larval predation risk. American Naturalist, 142, 947-960.

Silva, J. E. 2001. The multiple contributions of thyroid hormone to heat production. Journal of Clinical Investigation, 108, 35-37.

Spencer, R.-J. & Janzen, F. J. 2011. Hatching behavior in turtles. Integrative and Comparative Biology, 51, 100-110.

Spencer, R.-J., Thompson, M. B. & Banks, P. B. 2001. Hatch or wait? A dilemma in reptilian incubation. Oikos, 93, 401-406.

28

Spencer, R. J. 2012. Embryonic heart rate and hatching behavior of a solitary nesting turtle. Journal of Zoology, 287, 169-174.

Testa, J. W. 2002. Does predation on neonates inherently select for earlier births? Journal of Mammalogy, 83, 699-706.

Thommes, R. C., Vieth, R. L. & Levasseur, S. 1977. The effects of hypophysectomy by means of surgical decapitation on thyroid function in the developing chick embryo. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 31, 29-36.

Thompson, M. B. 1988. Nest temperatures in the pleurodiran turtle, Emydura macquarii. Copeia, 1988, 996-1000.

Thompson, M. B. 1989. Patterns of metabolism in embryonic reptiles. Respiration Physiology, 76, 243-255.

Thompson, M. B. 1993. Oxygen consumption and energetics of development in eggs of the leatherback turtle, Dermochelys coriacea. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Physiology, 104, 449-453.

Thompson, M. B. 1997. Egg Physiology and Biology. The Biology, Husbandry And Healthcare of Reptile. New Jersey: TFH Publications Inc.

Thompson, M. B. 2007. Comparison of the respiratory transition at birth or hatching in viviparous and oviparous amniote vertebrates. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology, 148, 755-760.

Thompson, M. B., Packard, G. C., Packard, M. J. & Rose, B. 1996. Analysis of the nest environment of tuatara Sphenodon punctatus. Journal of Zoology, 238, 239-251.

Thorbjarnarson, J. B. & Hernández, G. 1993. Reproductive Ecology of the Orinoco Crocodile (Crocodylus intermedius) in Venezuela. i. Nesting Ecology and Egg and Clutch Relationships. Journal of Herpetology, 27, 363-370.

Tona, K., Malheiros, R., Bamelis, F., Careghi, C., Moraes, V., Onagbesan, O., Decuypere, E. & Bruggeman, V. 2003. Effects of storage time on incubating egg gas pressure, thyroid hormones, and corticosterone levels in embryos and on their hatching parameters. Poultry Science, 82, 840-845.

Tucker, J. K., Paukstis, G. L. & Janzen, F. J. 2008. Does predator swamping promote synchronous emergence of turtle hatchlings among nests? Behavioral Ecology, 19, 35-40.

Vergne, A. L., Pritz, M. B. & Mathevon, N. 2009. Acoustic communication in crocodilians: from behaviour to brain. Biological Reviews, 84, 391-411.

29

Vitt, L. J. 1991. Ecology and life history of the scansorial arboreal lizard Plica plica (Iguanidae) in Amazonian Brazil. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 69, 504-511.

Vleck, C. M., Hoyt, D. F. & Vleck, D. 1979. Metabolism of avian embryos: patterns in altricial and precocial birds. Physiological Zoology, 363-377.

Wallace, B.P., Sotherland, P.R., Spotila, J.R., Reina, R.D., Franks, B.F. & Paladino, F.V. 2004. Biotic and Abiotic Factors Affect the Nest Environment of Embryonic Leatherback Turtles, Dermochelys coriacea. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology, 77, 423-432.

Wallace, B.P., Sotherland, P.R., Spotila, J.R., Reina, R.D., Franks, B.R. & Paladino, F.V. 2004. Biotic and abiotic factors affect the nest environment of embryonic leatherback turtles, Dermochelys coriacea. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology. 77, 423-432.

Warkentin, K. M. 1995. Adaptive plasticity in hatching age: a response to predation risk trade-offs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 92, 3507-3510.

Warkentin, K. M. 2000. Wasp predation and wasp-induced hatching of red-eyed treefrog eggs. Animal Behaviour, 60, 503-510.

Warkentin, K. M. & Calswell, M. S. 2009. Assessing risk: embryos, information, and escape hatching. In: Dukas, R. & Ratcliffe, J. (eds.) Cognitive Ecology II. The Evolutionary Ecology of Learning, Memory, and Information Use. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Warner, D. A. & Shine, R. 2011. Interactions among thermal parameters determine offspring sex under temperature-dependent sex determination. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278, 256-265.

Webb, G. J. W., Choquenot, D. & Whitehead, P. J. 1986. Nests, eggs, and embryonic development of Carettochelys insculpta (Chelonia: Carettochelidae) from Northern Australia. Journal of Zoology, 1, 521-550.

West-Eberhard, M. J. 1989. Phenotypic plasticity and the origins of diversity. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 20, 249-278.

West-Eberhard, M. J. 2003. Developmental Plasticity and Evolution, Oxford, Oxford University Press.

Wikelski, M., Lynn, S., Breuner, J. C., Wingfield, J. C. & Kenagy, G. J. 1999. Energy metabolism, testosterone and corticosterone in white-crowned sparrows. Journal of Comparative Physiology, A, 185, 463-470.

Wilhoft, D. C. 1958. The effect of temperature on thyroid histology and survival in the lizard, Sceloporus occidentalis. Copeia, 1958, 265-276.

30

Wilson, D. S. 1998. Nest-site selection: Microhabitat variation and its effects on the survival of turtle embryos. Ecology, 79, 1884-1892.

Wingfield, J. C. & Ramenofsky, M. 1999. Hormones and the behavioral ecology of stress. Stress Physiology in Animals. P. H. M. Balm ed. Sheffield: Sheffield Academic Press.

Wise, P. M. & Frye, B. E. 1973. Functional development of the hypothalamo-hypophyseal- adrenal cortex axis in the chick embryo, Gallus domesticus. Journal of Experimental Zoology, 185, 277-291.

Yntema, C. L. 1968. A Series of stages in the embryonic development of Chelydra serpentina Journal of Morphology, 125, 219-252.

Yntema, C. L. 1978. Incubation times for eggs of the turtle Chelydra serpentina (Testudines: Chelydridae) at various temperatures. Herpetologica, 34, 274-277.

Zhu, X.-P., Wei, C.-Q., Zhao, W.-H., Du, H.-J., Chen, Y.-L. & Gui, J.-F. 2006. Effects of incubation temperatures on embryonic development in the Asian yellow pond turtle. Aquaculture, 259, 243-248.

31

CHAPTER 2

HATCHING BEHAVIOUR OF EASTERN LONG-NECKED TURTLES

(CHELODINA LONGICOLLIS): THE INFLUENCE OF

ASYNCHRONOUS ENVIRONMENTS ON EMBRYONIC HEART RATE

AND PHENOTYPE

A paper published in Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular &

Integrative Physiology

McGlashan, J. K., Loudon, F. K., Thompson, M. B. & Spencer, R.-J. 2015. Hatching behavior of eastern long-necked turtles (Chelodina longicollis): The influence of asynchronous environments on embryonic heart rate and phenotype. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology, 188, 58-64.

32

Title: Hatching behaviour of eastern long-necked turtles (Chelodina longicollis): The influence of asynchronous environments on embryonic heart rate and phenotype

Jessica K. McGlashan*a, Fiona K. Loudona, Michael B. Thompsonb and Ricky-John

Spencera

aSchool of Science and Health, University of Western Sydney, Locked Bag 1797, Penrith

South DC, NSW 2751, Australia. bSchool of Biological Sciences, The University of Sydney NSW 2006, Australia

Running title: Heart rates of Chelodina longicollis during embryogenesis

*Author for correspondence: Jessica McGlashan

Email: [email protected]

Phone: T +61 2 4570 1195

Author Contributions

This manuscript is jointly authored, I am the primary author and I designed the experiment. I took all the heart rate measurements, maintained the eggs throughout incubation, recorded the pipping and hatching times, took hatchling measurements and performed the post-hatching righting tests and carried out data analysis. Dr Ricky-John Spencer supervised the work and provided feedback on earlier versions of the manuscript and Fiona Loudon and Mike Thompson critiqued the manuscript. I collected the eggs with the help of Fiona Loudon, Jessica Dormer and Heidi Stricker.

33

Abstract Variable temperatures within a nest cause asynchronous development within clutches of freshwater turtle embryos, yet synchronous hatching occurs and is thought to be an important survival strategy for hatchlings. Metabolic compensation and circadian rhythms in heart rates of embryonic turtles indicate the potential of communication between embryos in a nest.

Heart rates were used to identify metabolic circadian rhythms in clutches of an Australian freshwater turtle (Chelodina longicollis) and determine whether embryos metabolically compensate and hatch synchronously when incubated in asynchronous environments. The effects of a group environment during incubation on egg development and incubation period were also investigated during the final 3 weeks of development. Chelodina longicollis hatch synchronously and metabolically compensate so that less advanced embryos catch-up to more advanced clutch-mates. Heart rates of embryos remained stable from week 4-7 in asynchronous (M = 89 bpm) and synchronous (M = 92 bpm) groups and declined in the final two weeks of incubation (M = 72 and 77 bpm). Circadian rhythms were present throughout development and diel heart rates of embryos in asynchronous groups showed less deviation from the mean (M = - 0.5 bpm) than synchronous groups (M = - 4 bpm). Eggs incubated in groups had a significantly shorter incubation period than eggs incubated individually.

Phenotypic traits including size, performance and growth of all hatchlings were not affected.

Egg position within a turtle nest is important for coordinating development throughout incubation and facilitating synchronous hatching.

Key words: Egg, embryo, embryonic communication, embryonic development, environmentally cued hatching, turtle, incubation, metabolic compensation, nest environment, synchronous hatching

34

Introduction Environment affects embryonic development of an organism and alters offspring fitness and survival, including variations in an individual's behaviour, physiology, morphology, performance, and phenotypic characteristics (Deeming and Ferguson, 1991; West-Eberhard,

2003; Monaghan, 2008). Collectively, these variations are referred to as phenotypic plasticity

(Price et al., 2003; Doody, 2011). Intrinsic and extrinsic factors, such as timing of ovulation, nutrient acquisition, and dissimilar thermal environments in the nest, cause variation in development and incubation period of the embryo (Deeming and Ferguson, 1991).

Environmental conditions, such as temperature, influence embryonic development, including incubation length (Deeming and Ferguson, 1991; Booth, 1998; Monaghan, 2008), sex determination (TSD) (Yntema, 1968, 1978; Bull and Vogt, 1979; Bull, 1985), and physiological changes i.e. metabolic rate (developmental rate and cardiovascular function) of embryos (Packard and Packard, 1988; Du et al., 2009, 2010b). In natural nests of pleurodiran

(side-necked) turtles, temperature fluctuates daily and results in a temperature gradient that varies up to 6 °C between the top and the bottom of the nest. Eggs at the top experience greater shifts in temperature and are warmer for more than 75% of the day (Thompson, 1988,

1997). Temperature determines the length of incubation, with warmer incubation temperatures resulting in higher embryonic metabolic rate, including cardiac function, the number of heart beats, and faster development (Birchard and Reiber, 1996; Du et al., 2009) than do cooler temperatures (Packard and Packard, 1988; Deeming and Ferguson, 1991;

Monaghan, 2008).

Despite the environmental influence of temperature on incubation length, many avian and reptilian embryos are capable of altering their development and hatching time (Hoyt, 1979;

Vleck et al., 1980; Thompson, 1989; Du et al., 2011; McGlashan et al., 2012). Embryos have the ability to alter time of hatching in relation to environment through phenotypic plasticity 35

(Packard and Packard, 2000; Doody, 2011; Spencer and Janzen, 2011). A recent focal point for embryonic studies, and in particular reptilian embryos, is environmentally cued hatching

(ECH) (Warkentin and Calswell, 2009). ECH can increase an individual's chance of survival through a tradeoff between risks (Gomez-Mestre et al., 2008; Warkentin and Calswell, 2009).

Phenotypic plasticity facilitates ECH where embryos can hatch early, delay hatching, or hatch synchronously in response to environmental conditions. These behaviours are not mutually exclusive and some species show multiple forms of ECH (Warkentin and Calswell, 2009;

Doody, 2011).

The event and timing of hatching can affect survival of offspring both immediately (risk of predation) and in the future (resource availability, size, and performance of individual)

(Spencer and Janzen, 2011). Fully developed pig-nosed turtle (Carretochelys insculpta) embryos delay hatching until the onset of the wet season when nests become inundated with water, causing spontaneous hatching of all eggs in the nest (Webb et al., 1986; Doody et al.,

2001). Embryos hatch synchronously in response to hypoxia caused by flooding or vibrations from movement of hatchlings (Doody et al., 2012). Conversely, the Murray short-necked turtle (Emydura macquarii), and the North American painted turtle (Chrysemys picta) hatch synchronously by either catching up or hatching prematurely. E.macquarii embryos increase metabolic rates throughout the last third of development to catch up and hatch at a similar developmental stage to more advanced clutch-mates. Metabolic compensation does not affect hatchling size and performance immediately and up to 5 months post hatching (Spencer et al.,

2001; McGlashan et al., 2012). Less advanced embryos of C. picta also hatch prematurely in response to more advanced siblings, but metabolic compensation is not apparent, in terms of neonatal performance post hatching (Colbert et al., 2010).

36

The cardiovascular system is vital for oxygen and nutrient delivery during development

(Birchard and Reiber, 1996; Tazawa, 2005; Monaghan, 2008). Heart rate of precocial embryos is dependent on temperature and stage of development (Cain et al., 1967; Tazawa,

2005) and is used as an indicator for metabolic rate in avian and reptilian embryos (Ar and

Tazawa, 1999; Pearson and Tazawa, 1999; Tazawa, 2005; Du et al., 2009, 2010b, 2011).

Metabolic rate, or heart rate, can show circadian rhythms which can be endogenously driven over a 24 h period. Within a nest of buried eggs, where there are no external signals such as daylight, rhythms can be variable and lacking any temporal organization (Laposky et al.,

2008). Embryonic reptiles show circadian rhythms in metabolic rates during incubation in the absence of environmental cues. Circadian rhythms occur in rates of oxygen consumption in groups of embryonic snakes (Dmi'el, 1969) and in heart rates of turtle embryos (Loudon et al., 2013). Metabolic compensation and synchronized circadian rhythms could suggest that there is communication between embryos within a clutch during development, but how individuals communicate developmental stage is unknown. For eggs that are incubating in close contact, there is potential for individuals to detect heart beats of siblings and adjust developmental rates accordingly. Preliminary data suggest that eggs of E. macquarii incubated in groups have less variation in heart rates and incubation periods are significantly shorter compared to eggs incubated in isolation (Loudon et al., unpublished data). The aim of this study was to further explore the phenomenon of ECH that's evidently increasing in reported occurrences. We aim to identify, if hatching synchrony occurs in the Australian chelid, Chelodina longicollis, and the mechanisms utilized (metabolic compensation and circadian rhythms). If C. longicollis hatches synchronously through metabolic compensation, it should be evident in the diel patterns of embryonic heart rates and incubation periods of embryos incubated next to more advanced eggs. We also aim to test the effect of egg positioning within a nest during incubation. Eggs that are incubated in groups will have a

37

shorter incubation period and hatch synchronously compared to eggs incubated individually, due to the potential for embryo–embryo communication.

Method Study species

Chelodina longicollis has a wide distribution in south-eastern Australia. It occupies shallow, transient waterholes, remote water bodies, and permanent rivers (Chessman, 1988). Females may travel 500 m or more to nest (Cann, 1998; Kennett, 1999; Kennett et al., 2009) and predation on nests is high (Thompson, 1983; Beck, 1991). Clutch sizes vary between 6 and

23 eggs, each egg weighing between 4 and 8 g. Natural incubation and emergence takes from

110 to 150 d, with overwintering of hatchlings in the nest in populations in southern Australia

(Parmenter, 1985).

Female C. longicollis were captured from the Hawkesbury region, New South Wales

(−33.61°S, 150.75°E) from 1 to 14 November 2011 using lobster and cathedral traps.

Females were palpated and all gravid females were given a subcutaneous injection of oxytocin (1 mL/kg) (Spencer et al., 2001) to stimulate oviposition. Females were then placed in water in individual containers in a quiet dark room. Eggs were collected from containers over a period of 5 to 24 h, weighed and individually marked in order of oviposition using a

HB pencil. Eggs were then placed in containers with moist vermiculite and kept cool until all eggs from the clutch were collected (Spencer et al., 2001; Colbert et al., 2010). A total of 20 clutches of 6–12 eggs were collected for the asynchrony experiment and 5 clutches of 7–10 eggs for the egg positioning experiment. Prior to release, all females were palpated to ensure all eggs were laid; three of the 20 females were injected twice to complete oviposition.

38

Incubation methods

Eggs were half buried in a mixture of vermiculite and deionized water (1:1) and incubated in clear plastic containers (200 × 100 × 50 mm). Eggs were removed from containers weekly and vermiculite and water mixture was weighed and rehydrated to maintain water potential of vermiculite. The initial weight of the container and vermiculite were used to account for evaporation and net water gain from the eggs. Containers were also rotated weekly within environmental chambers to counteract potential thermal gradients (Janzen, 1993; Colbert et al., 2010).

Asynchrony experiment

Six eggs from each of the 20 clutches were used for this experiment. Asynchrony within clutches of C. longicollis was established in 10 of the clutches at the beginning of the experiment using protocols similar to Spencer et al. (2001) and Colbert et al. (2010). To establish developmental asynchrony, two of the six eggs from each clutch were incubated at

30 °C and the remaining four eggs were incubated at 26 °C for 8 d. After 8 d, the four eggs incubated at 26 °C were reunited with the two more advanced eggs at 30 °C and positioned so that they were not in contact but were within 10 mm of each other. The two more advanced eggs were placed between the four eggs incubated at 26 °C. We refer to this group of eggs as the ‘asynchronous’ group as eggs will be at different developmental stages (Fig. 1). The remaining 10 clutches were used as the ‘synchronous’ groups and were set up using the same procedure, with the two not advanced eggs being incubated at 26 °C for the 8 d period. The remaining four eggs were also incubated at 26 °C so that all six eggs are still at the same developmental stage and therefore synchronized when reunited (Spencer et al., 2001). After eggs were reunited into containers, containers were placed in a 30 °C incubator until development was completed. Each clutch was incubated in separate containers throughout the

39

experiment. Heart rates of embryos from asynchronous and synchronous groups were monitored weekly and containers were opened every 2–3 d to allow fresh air exchange throughout the incubation period until the commencement of pipping/hatching.

Fig. 1: Incubation method- Eggs from each clutch were divided and incubated separately for 8 d, then returned to the same containers to continue incubation at 30 °C. Asynchronous group- two more advanced eggs (black) were incubated at 30 °C and four eggs (white) were incubated at 26 °C for 8 d. Synchronous group- two not advanced eggs (black) were incubated at 26 °C and four eggs (white) were incubated at 26 °C for 8 d. All eggs were then placed in 30 °C incubators until the completion of incubation.

Egg position: Group vs individual incubation experiment

Clutches consisting of 7–10 eggs were used in an egg positioning experiment. Each clutch was incubated in a single container at 26 °C for the first 9 weeks. After 9 weeks, each clutch was divided into a group of three or four eggs and all remaining eggs were separated into individual containers. Grouped eggs were moved into a new container of the same size and individuals were each moved into new, smaller containers (100 × 100 × 50 mm) to complete

40

the final two to 3 weeks of incubation. Containers of grouped and individual eggs were put back into 26 °C incubators to complete incubation.

Heart rates: Asynchronous experiment

Heart rates were taken on the four eggs that were incubated next to the more advanced eggs in the asynchronous groups and the not advanced eggs in the synchronous groups. All results relating to heart rates will be referring to those four eggs in both asynchronous and synchronous groups. Heart rates, recorded as beats per minute (bpm), of embryos from both asynchronous and synchronous groups were measured to determine 1) heart rate profile, 2) circadian rhythm, and 3) metabolic compensation. Heart rates were used to infer metabolic rate (Wallace and Jones, 2008; Du et al., 2009) of embryos and identify any difference between asynchronous and synchronous groups. One of the four eggs from each clutch was selected to measure heart rate over 24 h at 4 time intervals (5:00, 11:00, 17:00, and 23:00 h), once a week. Eggs were removed from the incubation container and immediately placed in a

Buddy digital egg monitor system (Avian Biotech, England) in complete darkness. Buddy monitors provide a non-invasive method to sense and amplify cardiovascular signals of an embryo using variations of infrared radiation intensity that is absorbed and reflected by arterial vasculature (Lierz et al., 2006). Eggs were placed in monitors and three readings were taken when heart rates stabilized (within 2 min). Care was taken not to exceed this time due to the potential heating effect from the Buddy system (Sartori et al., 2015). The mean of three readings, taken at 30 s intervals, was calculated. Eggs were not disturbed during the recording period and any embryonic movement could be detected and distinguished from heart rate readings. Heart rates were measured weekly from week 4 to 9 (earliest and latest time heart rates could be detected by the monitors).

41

Pipping and post hatching measurements

To determine the incubation period, all eggs in both experiments were inspected for signs of pipping (initial break of egg with caruncle) twice daily (Gutzke et al., 1984). The incubation period was measured as time in days from oviposition until pipping.

Within 12 h of hatching, neonates were weighed, measured (carapace and plastron straight length and width), and performance tests were undertaken. The residual yolk sac that had not yet been drawn into the body through the umbilical fontanel at the time of hatching was measured and an estimate of the size was calculated using length, width, and height measurements (volume of an ellipsoid). Performance and coordination of hatchlings were assessed through righting times. All hatchlings were placed on their back (carapace) and the time taken to right themselves was measured and used as an index of neuromuscular development (Vleck et al., 1979, 1980; Peterson and Kruegl, 2005; Colbert et al., 2010).

Righting times were recorded to the nearest second using a digital stopwatch and all tests were stopped at 300 s. Hatchling growth in the asynchronous experiment was measured every month for 4 months to assess short-term costs to individuals.

Data analysis

A general linear model (GLM) was used to analyze group vs. individual incubation periods in the egg positioning experiment. A GLM was also used to assess incubation periods in the asynchronous experiment to test whether asynchronous or synchronous group incubation regime affected incubation period and if the asynchronous group hatched synchronously, using egg mass as a covariate. We compared the incubation periods of eggs within each clutch in the asynchronous group and synchronous group to assess if there were differences in incubation periods within clutches and therefore if hatching synchrony occurred. Incubation

42

period of eggs in asynchronous groups were compared to the synchronous groups, to determine if asynchronous groups hatched earlier than expected. To confirm a difference in incubation period that would be expected from the initial asynchrony set-up, the incubation period of the two more advanced eggs in the asynchronous group were compared to the two not advanced eggs in the synchronous group. Heart rates of embryos were analyzed using

GLM for every week to test whether the asynchronous treatment affected metabolic rate, with egg mass as a covariate. Circadian rhythms were determined through daily heart rate fluctuations, which were calculated as a difference from the mean diel rate (bpm) at each time point. Peaks were used to align circadian rhythms with time of day in either asynchronous or synchronous groups and analyzed using an ANOVA. Hatchling mass, morphology, and righting ability were individually analyzed using a GLM with egg mass as a covariate. A binomial test was used to assess propensity to right. All analyses were performed using SAS

9.2 (SAS Institute Inc.).

Results Heart rate

Heart rate (bpm) of embryos from asynchronous and synchronous groups decreased from week 4 to 9 of incubation from 93 to 73 bpm (21.5 %; t4 = 6.42, p = 0.002) and 95 to 66 bpm

(30.5 %; t4 = 6.8, p = 0.001), respectively (Fig. 2). Heart rates of asynchronous and synchronous embryos did not differ significantly throughout incubation with the exception of week 8 when synchronous group heart rates were 17.6 % higher than asynchronous embryos

(F1,13 = 36.49, p = 0.0001; Fig. 2). From weeks 4 to 6, heart rate of all embryos remained relatively constant 89.5–92.1 bpm, after which a significant decline occurred in both asynchronous and synchronous groups. From week 7 to week 8, asynchronous embryos heart rates dropped by 13.2 bpm (15.6 %; F1,14 = 4.6, p = 0.0001) and synchronous embryos

43

dropped by 3.6 bpm (4 %) and continued to decline significantly by 19.8 bpm from weeks 8 to 9 (22.9 %; F1,9 = 61.28, p = 0.00003) (Fig. 2) however, asynchronous embryos increased heart rate by 2.5 bpm in weeks 8–9 (3.4 %).

120

100 a

80 a

60

Heart Rate (bpm) Rate Heart 40

20

0 Week 4 Week 5 Week 6 Week 7 Week 8 Week 9

Fig. 2: Mean (M) heart rates (bpm) of embryos in synchronous (dark grey) and asynchronous

(light grey) groups: synchronous, M = 90 bpm; asynchronous, M = 84 bpm (week 8: F1,13 = 36.49, p = 0.0001 a) ± SE, N = 70. a) significant difference in heart rates between asynchronous and synchronous groups, N is the total number of eggs used.

Circadian rhythms in heart rates were present with a clear peak in heart rates throughout the day (Fig. 3). Individuals showed minimum and maximum heart rates over a 24 h period, but they were not correlated with the time of day in both asynchronous and synchronous groups.

In general, heart rate of asynchronous groups did not vary greatly from the mean heart rate as did synchronous groups. There was no significant difference in the peak heart rates between

44

groups, suggesting that asynchronous groups did not exceed the potential for periods of higher peak daily rate, relative to synchronous groups. In the 6 h leading up to the maximum rate, there was a greater negative deviation, or period of daily ‘resting’, in the synchronous group (M = -4 bpm) than the asynchronous group (M = -0.5 bpm) (F1,10 = 4.96, p = 0.04)

(Fig. 3). Heart rate deviation from the mean was also significantly different 12 h before the peak between synchronous groups (M = 0.3 bpm) and asynchronous groups (M = -1.6 bpm)

(F1,10 = 4.96, p = 0.03), most evident in the diel heart rate profiles in week 5, 7, and 8.

6

4

2 a b 0

-2 a

-4

Difference from mean ratefrom heartmean Difference (bpm) b -6 -12 h -6 h Peak 6 h

Fig. 3: The difference from mean heart rate of synchronous (dark grey) and asynchronous (light grey) groups over a 24 h period. Data from weeks 4 to 9 before hatching are combined, x-axis represents duration of time (h) away from the peak heart rate and y-axis represents the difference from the mean heart rate (bpm), ±SE, N = 70. a) significant difference in heart rate deviation 12 h prior to the peak heart rate between synchronous and asynchronous groups

(F1,10 = 4.96, p = 0.03), b) significant difference in heart rate deviation 6 h prior to the peak heart rate between synchronous and asynchronous groups (F1,10 = 4.96, p = 0.04).

45

Incubation period

Incubation period did not vary between clutch-mates in both asynchronous and synchronous groups. All eggs within a clutch in the asynchronous groups hatched within 1 d of each other

(F1,43 = 0.11, p = 0.74) and all eggs within a clutch in the synchronous group hatched within 3 d of each other (F1,43 = 0.80, p = 0.38). There was a significant difference in incubation periods between eggs in asynchronous and synchronous groups (4–7 d) (F1,43 = 4.00, p =

0.05) and an 8 d difference in the incubation period of the two more advanced eggs from the asynchronous groups compared to the two not advanced eggs in the synchronous group (F1,43

= 9.62, p = 0.003) (Fig. 4). In the egg position experiment, grouped embryos hatched up to 3 d earlier than embryos that were incubated individually (F1,33 = 4.45, p = 0.04) (Fig. 5).

b a 80

78

76

74 b 72 a

70

68

66 Incubation Period (days) Period Incubation

64

62

60 Synchronous Not advanced eggs Asynchronous More advanced eggs

Fig. 4: Mean incubation period (days) of eggs in synchronous and asynchronous groups, ± SE, N = 48. a) significant difference between more advanced eggs and not advanced eggs

(F1,43 = 9.62, p = 0.003), b) significant difference of both synchronous and not advanced eggs compared with asynchronous eggs (F1,43 = 4.00, p = 0.05). 46

90 b

89

88

87 a

86

85 IncubationPeriod (days) 84

83

82 Group Individual

Fig. 5: Mean incubation period of eggs in groups (dark grey) and individuals (light grey) ±

SE, N = 35, (F1,33 = 4.45, p = 0.04).

Hatching and post hatching development

A total of 27 asynchronous and 21 synchronous hatchlings representing all clutches were used to assess hatching and post hatching development. Yolk sac size differed significantly between neonates from asynchronous and synchronous groups at the time of hatching.

Hatchlings from the asynchronous groups hatched with significantly smaller residual yolk

3 3 sacs (30 ± 19 mm ) than those from synchronous groups (92 ± 21 mm ) (F1,43 = 4.45, p =

0.04), but there was no difference in yolk sacs between neonates within clutches in the asynchronous group (F1,26 = 0.01, p = 0.92) and synchronous group (F1,20 = 0.81, p = 0.38)

(Fig. 6). There was no difference in hatchling mass (F1,43 = 0.47, p = 0.5) and carapace length

47

(F1,43= 0.83, p = 0.36) between neonates from asynchronous and synchronous groups at the time of hatching and at 4 months; mass (F1,15 = 0.87, p = 0.37) and carapace length (F1,15 =

0.10, p = 0.76).

120 a

100

) 3 80 a

60

Yolk Sac Volume Sac Volume Yolk (mm 40

20

0 Synchronous Not advanced eggs Asynchronous More advanced eggs

Fig. 6: Mean yolk sac size (mm3) of synchronous (dark grey) and asynchronous (light grey) hatchlings, ± SE, N = 48. a) significant difference between asynchronous and synchronous groups (F1,43 = 4.45, p = 0.04).

Performance (as assessed by righting times) was not affected by incubation treatment. There was no significant difference in performance (total time to right) of neonates from asynchronous and synchronous groups at the time of hatching (F1,27 = 0.97, p = 0.33) and 48

after 4 months of growth (F1,41= 0.00, p = 0.96). There was also no significant difference in righting times between hatchlings from asynchronous groups and all other hatchlings from the experiment. Propensity to right was assessed and there was no significant difference between all groups at the time of hatching (F3,44 = 1.51, p = 0.23) and at 4 months of age

(F3,44 = 0.24, p = 0.87). No phenotypic differences occurred between grouped embryos and individual embryos in the egg positioning experiment.

Discussion In a natural nest, eggs of C. longicollis take 110–150 d to hatch and emerge (Parmenter,

1985), but they incubate for 62–79 d at a constant temperature of 30 °C and 75–94 d at a constant temperature of 26 °C in the laboratory (J. K. McGlashan, unpublished data). Much of this variation in incubation periods may relate primarily to complex interactions between siblings within individual nests. Here we show that eggs of C. longicollis hatch synchronously and embryos are able to metabolically compensate and catch up to more advanced clutch-mates by remarkably reducing incubation periods by up to 7 d (Fig. 4).

Group environment also influences hatching times, with eggs incubated in groups hatching up to 3 d earlier than eggs incubated in isolation (Fig. 5).

Hatching synchrony in reptiles can occur either by delaying hatching (eg. C. insculpta (Webb et al., 1986; Doody et al., 2001)), hatching prematurely (eg. C. picta (Colbert et al., 2010)) or through metabolic compensation (eg. Eastern three-lined skink (Bassiana duperreyi) (Du et al., 2010a) and embryonic E. macquarii (McGlashan et al., 2012)). Neonatal C. longicollis show a level of developmental plasticity that allows them to hatch synchronously, but we found no significant evidence of metabolic compensation in mean daily heart rate throughout the 9 weeks of incubation, similar to Spencer (2012). Metabolic compensation in C.

49

longicollis may be stimulated by communication through diel adjustments of heart rate.

Circadian rhythms are present in developing C. longicollis embryos, as they are in embryonic

E. macquarii (Loudon et al., 2013), despite no environmental influences such as daylight.

Heart rates are elevated or ‘peak’ during the day, indicating periods of high developmental activity and drop, indicating periods of low developmental activity or ‘resting’. These rhythms are not synchronized to the time of day among eggs of C. longicollis, and there is no indication of synchrony between individuals when compared from week to week. The mean weekly heart rate can be used as an indicator for metabolic rate of embryos, and deviations from the mean rate throughout the day may indicate positive or negative compensatory changes in embryo metabolism. Asynchronous groups showed less overall deviation from the mean heart rate in relation to synchronous groups and instead remained relatively constant throughout incubation (Fig. 3). In daily periods of ‘rest’ where heart rate drops, asynchronous embryos showed less negative deviation, compared to synchronous groups where the drop in heart rate was up to 5 bpm (t10 = 2.99, p = 0.006). Maintaining a constant heart rate in these periods of resting throughout incubation is indicative of metabolic compensation. Changes in relative heart rate are more subtle in asynchronous embryos, as they remain at a constant higher rate compared to synchronous groups, this is necessary to metabolically compensate, hence ‘catch up’ to more advanced embryos and facilitate synchronous hatching.

Embryonic oxygen consumption and heart rate increase in a sigmoidal pattern (having an ‘S’ shape) throughout development in many turtles, until they reach a peak several days before hatching and then decrease steadily (Thompson, 1989; Booth, 2000; Booth and Astill, 2001;

Du et al., 2010b; McGlashan et al., 2012). In many precocial animals, this decline following the peak is thought of as a ‘resting stage’. The resting stage is one where tissue growth is essentially complete and the sensory, neuromuscular, and thermoregulatory systems mature.

50

Referred to as secondary development, it is an important stage for species with little to no parental care post hatching (Vleck et al., 1979; Peterson and Kruegl, 2005). The heart rate of embryos of C. longicollis remains stable from weeks 4 to 7, a pattern that is seen in semi- precocial birds (Cain et al., 1967; Tazawa et al., 1991a,b; Tazawa, 2005). In week 8

(asynchronous) and week 9 (synchronous), heart rate declines significantly (Fig. 2), indicating a potential resting stage. This marked fall in metabolic rate has also been shown in embryos of C. insculpta (Webb et al., 1986), E. macquarii, and the American alligator

(Alligator mississipiensis), 80–90% of the way through incubation (Thompson, 1989). The embryos of the more advanced eggs from the asynchronous group were 8 d more advanced than the embryos of the not advanced eggs of the synchronous groups. The significant decline in heart rate by the asynchronous embryos is potentially a result of embryos responding to the developmental rates of more advanced eggs in the clutch and therefore beginning their resting stage, in this case, a week earlier than expected, relative to synchronous groups. Both asynchronous and synchronous groups began their resting stage 80–85% of the way through incubation. The asynchronous group, as a result of being incubated with more advanced eggs, shifted their resting stage to coincide with the more advanced eggs, ensuring enough time to complete secondary development with no developmental consequences. Embryos of the asynchronous group hatched up to 7 d earlier than the synchronous group, assumed to be at the same stage, thus hatching synchronously with clutch-mates.

The cost of metabolic compensation during development may relate to increased yolk reserve utilization. Certain incubation conditions that demand increased developmental rates or metabolic compensation during development often results in greater utilization of yolk reserves (Bhan et al., 2002; Radder et al., 2002, 2004; Van Dyke et al., 2011). Residual yolk at hatching is a result of left-over yolk from incubation but is used for post hatching energy

51

supply and growth (Tucker et al., 1998; Radder et al., 2004; Van Dyke et al., 2011). Evidence of metabolic compensation is apparent in diel heart rates of embryos (Fig. 3) and is also supported by asynchronous group hatchlings having significantly smaller residual yolk volumes than synchronous groups (Fig. 6). Yolk sac sizes within the asynchronous group and the synchronous group are not significantly different between hatchlings so asynchronous embryos utilized the same amount of yolk during the period of increased demand as the more advanced eggs did at the beginning of incubation when asynchrony was established (Fig. 1).

E. macquarii embryos catabolize yolk reserves to increase metabolism during incubation, resulting in smaller residual yolk sacs at hatching and no difference in hatchling size

(McGlashan et al., 2012). Chelodina longicollis hatchlings that hatch with smaller yolk sacs are not phenotypically disadvantaged in regards to size and performance at the time of hatching and up to 4 months post hatching, indicating that development is completed.

Nests are versatile environments for developing embryos. Until recently, it was believed that the microenvironment of a nest, such as temperature and humidity, was the primary factor that determined how reptiles and embryos grow and develop, but it is apparent that reptilian embryogenesis is far more complex than previously assumed. The mechanisms of ECH in turtles are still poorly understood, and although temperature and nest ecology can impact on incubation length and development of ectothermic embryos, there is evidence of plasticity in development (West-Eberhard, 1989, 2003; Deeming and Ferguson, 1991; Packard and

Packard, 2000; Johnston and Temple, 2002; Price et al., 2003; Doody, 2011). It is not known how C. longicollis and E. macquarii accelerate development, but heart rates are not fixed and environmental conditions merely determine the limits of heart rate and thus developmental rate (McGlashan et al., 2012; Loudon et al., 2013). Synchronous hatching has evolved in many aquatic turtles with many abiotic and biotic factors serving as the cues (Webb et al.,

52

1986; Thompson, 1989; Lindeman, 1991; Packard et al., 1997; Doody et al., 2001; Spencer et al., 2001; Packard and Packard, 2004; Tucker et al., 2008; Colbert et al., 2010; Spencer and

Janzen, 2011). Synchronous hatching is not a passive action, it is driven by embryo–embryo communication in the nest. It is not clear how embryos are communicating, but it is apparent that the final 2 weeks of incubation are important for signaling and responding to cues in the nest as evident in both the asynchronous and egg position experiment. Egg positioning in the nest potentially facilitates communication between embryos, and this may be due to auditory cues from embryos or vibrations from heart beats during development.

Thermal and phenotypic plasticity of embryos, communication between embryos within the nest, and most recently, embryonic circadian rhythms have all been discovered in recent years (McGlashan et al., 2012; Loudon et al., 2013). The mechanism and evolutionary purpose that stimulates and enables early/premature synchronous hatching could vary among species of turtles, but it is evident that cues that occur within a nest are capable of altering a very important stage in the life history of these animals to allow synchronous hatching, thereby maximizing the chance of survival for the individual.

Acknowledgements Research was supported by a UWS SSH Equipment Fund Grant and was conducted under UWS

ACEC no. A9300, NSW NPWS Permit SL101065. Karen Harland helped capture turtles and collect eggs. Research was conducted in the Evolution and Developmental Laboratory and K1 Aquatic

Facility at the University of Western Sydney.

53

References Ar, A. & Tazawa, H. 1999. Analysis of heart rate in developing bird embryos: effects of developmental mode and mass. Comp. Biochem. Physiol., A: Mol. Integr. Physiol., 124, 491-500.

Beck, R. 1991. The common long-necked tortoise Chelodina longicollis (Shaw 1802)(Testudines: Chelidae): a comparative study of the morphology and behaviour. Vic. Nat., 66, 4-21.

Birchard, G. & Reiber, C. 1996. Heart rate during development in the turtle embryo: effect of temperature. J. Comp. Physiol., B, 166, 461-466.

Booth, D. T. 1998. Incubation of turtle eggs at different temperatures: do embryos compensate for temperature during development? Physiol. Zool., 71, 23-26.

Booth, D. T. 2000. Incubation of eggs of the Australian broad-shelled turtle, Chelodina expansa (Testudinata : Chelidae), at different temperatures: effects on pattern of oxygen consumption and hatchling morphology. Aust. J. Zool., 48, 369-378.

Booth, D. T. & Astill, K. 2001. Incubation temperature, energy expenditure and hatchling size in the green turtle (Chelonia mydas), a species with temperature-sensitive sex determination. Aust. J. Zool., 49, 389-396.

Bull, J. J. 1985. Sex determining mechanisms: an evolutionary perspective. Experientia, 41, 1285-1296.

Bull, J. J. & Vogt, R. C. 1979. Temperature-dependent sex determination in turtles. Science, 206, 1186-1188.

Cain, J. R., Abbott, U. K. & Rogallo, V. L. 1967. Heart rate of the developing chick embryo. Exp. Biol. Med., 126, 507-510.

Cann, J. 1998. Australian Freshwater Turtles, Singapore, Beaumont publishing.

Chessman, B. C. 1988. Habitat preferences of freshwater turtles in the Murray Valley, Victoria and New South Wales Aust. Wildl. Res., 15 485-491.

Colbert, P. L., Spencer, R.-J. & Janzen, F. J. 2010. Mechanism and cost of synchronous hatching. Funct. Ecol., 24, 112-121.

Deeming, D. C. & Ferguson, M. W. J. 1991. Egg Incubation: Its Effects on Embryonic Development in Birds and Reptiles, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.

Dmi'el, R. 1969. Circadian rhythm of oxygen consumption in snake embryos. Life Sci., 8, 1333-1341.

54

Doody, J. S. 2011. Environmentally cued hatching in reptiles. Integr. Comp. Biol., 51, 49-61.

Doody, J. S., Georges, A., Young, J. E., Pauza, M. D., Pepper, A. L., Alderman, R. L. & Welsh, M. A. 2001. Embryonic aestivation and emergence behaviour in the pig-nosed turtle, Carettochelys insculpta. Can. J. Zool., 79, 1062-1072.

Doody, J. S., Stewart, B., Camacho, C. & Christian, K. 2012. Good vibrations? Sibling embryos expedite hatching in a turtle. Anim. Behav., 83, 645-651.

Du, W.-G., Radder, R. S., Sun, B. & Shine, R. 2009. Determinants of incubation period: do reptilian embryos hatch after a fixed total number of heart beats? J. Exp. Biol., 212, 1302-1306.

Du, W.-G., Thompson, M. B. & Shine, R. 2010a. Facultative cardiac responses to regional hypoxia in lizard embryos. Comp. Biochem. Physiol., A: Mol. Integr. Physiol., 156, 491-494.

Du, W.-G., Ye, H., Zhao, B., Pizzatto, L., Ji, X. & Shine, R. 2011. Patterns of interspecific variation in the heart rates of embryonic reptiles. PloS one, 6, e29027.

Du, W.-G., Zhao, B. & Shine, R. 2010b. Embryos in the fast lane: high-temperature heart rates of turtles decline after hatching. PLoS ONE, 5, e9557.

Gomez-Mestre, I., Wiens, J. J. & Warkentin, K. M. 2008. Evolution of adaptive plasticity: risk-sensitive hatching in neotropical leaf-breeding treefrogs. Ecol. Monogr., 78, 205- 224.

Gutzke, W. H., Paukstis, G. L. & Packard, G. C. 1984. Pipping versus hatching as indices of time of incubation in reptiles. J. Herpetol., 494-496.

Hoyt, D. F. 1979. Osmoregulation by avian embryos: the allantois functions like a toad's bladder. Physiol. Zool., 52, 354-362.

Janzen, F. J. 1993. An experimental analysis of natural selection on body size of hatchling turtles. Ecology, 74, 332-341.

Johnston, I. A. & Temple, G. K. 2002. Thermal plasticity of skeletal muscle phenotype in ectothermic vertebrates and its significance for locomotory behaviour. J. Exp. Biol., 205, 2305-2322.

Kennett, R. 1999. Reproduction of two species of freshwater turtle, Chelodina rugosa and dentata, from the wet–dry tropics of northern Australia. J. Zool., 247, 457-473.

Kennett, R., Roe, J., Hodges, K. & Georges, A. 2009. Chelodina longicollis (Shaw 1794): Eastern long-necked turtle, Common long-necked turtle, Common snake-necked turtle. Chelonian Res Monogr, 5, 31.1-31.8.

55

Laposky, A. D., Bass, J., Kohsaka, A. & Turek, F. W. 2008. Sleep and circadian rhythms: Key components in the regulation of energy metabolism. FEBS Lett., 582, 142-151.

Lierz, M., Gooss, O. & Hafez, H. M. 2006. Noninvasive Heart Rate Measurement Using a Digital Egg Monitor in Chicken and Turkey Embryos. J. Avian Med. Surg., 20, 141- 146.

Loudon, F. K., Spencer, R.-J., Strassmeyer, A. & Harland, K. 2013. Metabolic circadian rhythms in embryonic turtles. Integr. Comp. Biol., 53, 175-182.

McGlashan, J. K., Spencer, R.-J. & Old, J. M. 2012. Embryonic communication in the nest: metabolic responses of reptilian embryos to developmental rates of siblings. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci., 279, 1709-1715.

Monaghan, P. 2008. Early growth conditions, phenotypic development and environmental change. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., Ser. B: Biol. Sci., 363, 1635-1645.

Packard, G. C. & Packard, M. J. 1988. The physiological ecology of reptilian eggs and embryos. Gans, C., Huey, R.B. (eds.), Biology of the Reptilia. New York: Wiley.

Packard, G. C. & Packard, M. J. 2000. Developmental plasticity in painted turtles, Chrysemys picta. Funct. Ecol., 14, 474-483.

Parmenter, C. J. 1985. Reproduction and survivorship of Chelodina longicollis ( Testudinata: Chelidae). The biology of Australasian frogs and reptiles, 53-61.

Pearson, J. T. & Tazawa, H. 1999. Development of cardiac rhythms in altricial avian embryos. Comp. Biochem. Physiol., A: Mol. Integr. Physiol., 124, 475-482.

Peterson, C. C. & Kruegl, A. 2005. Peaked temporal pattern of embryonic metabolism in an emydid turtle (Chrysemys picta picta). J. herpetol., 39, 678-681.

Price, T. D., Qvarnström, A. & Irwin, D. E. 2003. The role of phenotypic plasticity in driving genetic evolution. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci., 270, 1433-1440.

Radder, R. S., Shanbhag, B. A. & Saidapur, S. K. 2002. Pattern of yolk internalization by hatchlings is related to breeding timing in the garden lizard, Calotes versicolor. Curr. Sci., 82, 1484-1486.

Radder, R. S., Shanbhag, B. A. & Saidapur, S. K. 2004. Yolk partitioning in embryos of the lizard, Calotes versicolor: maximize body size or save energy for later use? J. Exp. Zool. A Comp. Exp. Biol., 301, 783-785.

Sartori, M. R., Leite, C. A. C., Abe, A. S., Crossley Ii, D. A. & Taylor, E. W. 2015. The progressive onset of cholinergic and adrenergic control of heart rate during

56

development in the green iguana, Iguana iguana. Comp. Biochem. Physiol., A: Mol. Integr. Physiol., 188, 1-8.

Spencer, R.-J. & Janzen, F. J. 2011. Hatching behavior in turtles. Integr. Comp. Biol., 51, 100-110.

Spencer, R.-J., Thompson, M. B. & Banks, P. B. 2001. Hatch or wait? A dilemma in reptilian incubation. Oikos, 93, 401-406.

Spencer, R. J. 2012. Embryonic heart rate and hatching behavior of a solitary nesting turtle. J. Zool., 287, 169-174.

Tazawa, H. 2005. Cardiac rhythms in avian embryos and hatchlings. Avian Poult. Biol. Rev., 16, 123-150.

Tazawa, H., Hiraguchi, T., Kuroda, O., Tullett, S. & Deeming, D. 1991a. Embryonic heart rate during development of domesticated birds. Physiol. Zool., 1002-1022.

Tazawa, H., Kuroda, O. & Whittow, G. C. 1991b. Noninvasive determination of embryonic heart rate during hatching in the brown noddy (Anous stolidus). The Auk, 594-601.

Thompson, M. 1983. Populations of the Murray River tortoise, Emydura (Chelodina): the effect of egg predation by the red fox, Vulpes vulpes. Wildl. Res., 10, 363-371.

Thompson, M. B. 1988. Nest temperatures in the pleurodiran turtle, Emydura macquarii. Copeia, 1988, 996-1000.

Thompson, M. B. 1989. Patterns of metabolism in embryonic reptiles. Respir. Physiol., 76, 243-255.

Thompson, M. B. 1997. Egg Physiology and Biology. The Biology, Husbandry And Healthcare of Reptile. New Jersey: TFH Publications Inc.

Tucker, J. K., Filoramo, N. I., Paukstis, G. L. & Janzen, F. J. 1998. Residual yolk in captive and wild-caught hatchlings of the red-eared slider turtle (Trachemys scripta elegans). Copeia, 1998, 488-492.

Van Dyke, J. U., Plummer, M. V. & Beaupre, S. J. 2011. Residual yolk energetics and postnatal shell growth in Smooth Softshell Turtles, Apalone mutica. Comp. Biochem. Physiol., A: Mol. Integr. Physiol., 158, 37-46.

Vleck, C. M., Hoyt, D. F. & Vleck, D. 1979. Metabolism of avian embryos: patterns in altricial and precocial birds. Physiol. Zool., 363-377.

Vleck, D., Vleck, C. M. & Hoyt, D. F. 1980. Metabolism of avian embryos: ontogeny of oxygen consumption in the rhea and emu. Physiol. Zool., 53, 125-135.

57

Wallace, B. P. & Jones, T. T. 2008. What makes marine turtles go: a review of metabolic rates and their consequences. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol., 356, 8-24.

Warkentin, K. M. & Calswell, M. S. 2009. Assessing risk: embryos, information, and escape hatching. In: Dukas, R. & Ratcliffe, J. (eds.) Cognitive Ecology II. The Evolutionary Ecology of Learning, Memory, and Information Use. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Webb, G. J. W., Choquenot, D. & Whitehead, P. J. 1986. Nests, eggs, and embryonic development of Carettochelys insculpta (Chelonia: Carettochelidae) from Northern Australia. J. Zool., 1, 521-550.

West-Eberhard, M. J. 1989. Phenotypic plasticity and the origins of diversity. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 20, 249-278.

West-Eberhard, M. J. 2003. Developmental Plasticity and Evolution, Oxford, Oxford University Press.

Yntema, C. L. 1968. A series of stages in the embryonic development of Chelydra serpentina. J. Morphol., 125, 219-251.

Yntema, C. L. 1978. Incubation times for eggs of the turtle Chelydra serpentina (Testudines: Chelydridae) at various temperatures. Herpetologica, 34, 274-277.

58

CHAPTER 3

SYNCHRONOUS HATCHING WITHOUT METABOLIC

COMPENSATION IN THE PAINTED TURTLE (CHRYSEMYS PICTA)

Chapter formatted for submission to Journal of Experimental Biology

59

Title: Synchronous hatching without metabolic compensation in the painted turtle (Chrysemys picta)

Jessica K. McGlashan1*, Michael B. Thompson2, Fredric J. Janzen3 and Ricky-John Spencer1

1School of Science and Health, University of Western Sydney, Locked Bag 1797, Penrith

South DC, NSW 2751, Australia.

2School of Biological Sciences, The University of Sydney NSW 2006, Australia

3Department of Ecology, Evolution and Organismal Biology, Iowa State University, 251

Bessey Hall, Ames, Iowa 50011 USA

Short title: Synchronous hatching in Chrysemys picta

*Author for correspondence: Jessica McGlashan

Email: [email protected]

Phone: T +61 2 4570 1195

Author Contributions

This manuscript is jointly authored, I am the primary author and I designed the experiment. I took all the heart rate and respiration measurements, maintained the eggs throughout incubation, recorded the pipping and hatching times, took hatchling measurements and performed the post- hatching righting tests and carried out data analysis. Dr Ricky-John Spencer and Dr Fredric Janzen supervised the work and provided feedback on earlier versions of the manuscript and Mike Thompson critiqued the manuscript.

60

Abstract Environmentally cued hatching allows embryos to alter time of hatching in relation to environment through phenotypic plasticity. Variable temperatures within shallow nests of many freshwater turtles cause asynchronous development of embryos within clutches, yet neonates still synchronously hatch. Metabolic compensation and changes in circadian rhythms enable embryos to adjust their developmental rates and catch-up to more-advanced eggs in the nest. Chrysemys picta is a North American freshwater turtle and many populations overwinter within the natal nest, to emerge the following spring. Synchronous hatching is thought to occur when cooler, underdeveloped embryos accelerate their development to hatch early which may ensure an optimal overwintering position in the nest.

We investigated metabolic rates, through oxygen consumption and heart rate profiles, of clutches of C. picta developing in asynchronous conditions to determine if any compensatory changes during incubation occur. Embryos hatched synchronously and displayed circadian rhythms throughout incubation, but exhibited no evidence of metabolic compensation.

Phenotypic traits, including size and righting performance, of hatchlings were not affected.

Thus C. picta appear to hatch synchronously regardless of differences in developmental rate that accompany it. The proximate and ultimate mechanisms that drive synchronous hatching in freshwater turtles vary, probably as a result of differing selective pressures on different species.

Key words: Egg, embryo, embryonic development, circadian rhythms, environmentally cued hatching, turtle, incubation, metabolic compensation, nest environment, synchronous hatching, overwintering, evolution

61

Introduction Timing of hatching can affect survival of offspring both immediately (risk of predation) and in the future (resource availability, size and performance of individual) (Tucker et al., 2008;

Colbert et al., 2010; Spencer and Janzen, 2011). Embryos have the ability to alter time of hatching in relation to environmental cues via environmentally cued hatching (ECH)

(Packard and Packard, 2000; Doody, 2011; Spencer and Janzen, 2011). There are three forms of ECH; early, synchronous, and delayed hatching (Doody, 2011; Spencer and Janzen, 2011).

Early hatching can be triggered through disturbances within the nest, usually from predatory attacks (Sih and Moore, 1993; Warkentin, 1995; Warkentin, 2000). Synchronous hatching occurs despite environmentally induced developmental differences of embryos (Thompson,

1989; Spencer et al., 2001; Colbert et al., 2010), and delayed hatching is when embryos wait until more favourable conditions are available (Doody, 2011; Spencer and Janzen, 2011).

These behaviours are not mutually exclusive and some species show multiple forms of ECH

(Warkentin and Calswell, 2009; Doody, 2011).

In shallow nests of many freshwater turtles, a natural thermal gradient exists due to diel and seasonal changes in ambient temperatures (Thompson, 1988; Thompson et al., 1996;

Thompson, 1997; Shine and Elphick, 2001). Eggs at the top of a nest generally experience warmer incubation temperatures than eggs at the bottom, with differences up to 6 °C

(Thompson, 1988, 1997), and have the potential to cause embryos to develop asynchronously within a nest. Despite the variation in developmental rate among embryos, many species nonetheless hatch synchronously (Thompson, 1989; Spencer et al., 2001; Colbert et al., 2010;

McGlashan et al., 2012).

Predator avoidance likely drives the evolution of synchronous hatching in turtles.

Synchronous hatching allows all hatchlings to dig out of the nest together, thus increasing 62

their chance of survival by swamping predators or reducing their exposure (Sih and Moore,

1993; Warkentin, 1995; Warkentin, 2000; Bradbury et al., 2004; Tucker et al., 2008).

Embryos can hatch early and synchronously in response to predatory attacks on nests

(Warkentin, 1995; Warkentin, 2000) or when risk of predation is reduced, due to the time of day (Bradbury et al., 2004). Environmental pressures can also stimulate synchronous hatching, as in the pig-nosed turtle (Carettochelys insculpta), which delays hatching until an environmental trigger (hypoxia) stimulates synchronised hatching (Webb et al., 1986; Doody et al., 2001; Doody et al., 2012). The painted turtle (Chrysemys picta) hatches synchronously through early hatching, which is thought to be an overwintering strategy to gain an optimal position in the nest to increase their chance of survival until they emerge in the following spring (Colbert et al., 2010). In contrast, the Australian Murray River short-neck turtle

(Emydura macquarii) and the eastern long-necked turtle (Chelodina longicollis) both hatch synchronously as a predator avoidance strategy (Tucker et al., 2008), through metabolic compensation, which is evident in increasing oxygen consumption rates ( O2) and heart rate patterns during development (McGlashan et al., 2012; McGlashan et al., 2015). Circadian rhythms in heart rate are evident in both species, and embryos might cue into the heart rate rhythm of their clutch-mates to communicate developmental stage, and thus respond to differences by metabolically compensating (Loudon et al., 2013; McGlashan et al., 2015). In

E. macquarii and C. longicollis metabolic compensation ensures completion of development in embryos developing in the cooler parts of the nest, which is essential to survival, as any reduction in developmental time can have adverse effects on the growth and performance of the hatchling (Vince and Chinn, 1971; Janzen, 1993; Warkentin, 1995). It is unknown whether C. picta also utilises metabolic compensation in order to hatch early.

For C. picta, winter is a critical time for hatchling survival, as nest temperatures can drop below -10 °C (Packard and Packard, 2004). Ensuring an optimal overwintering position in the

63

nest, where conditions are ideal for reducing the amount of energy expended or the chance of mortality from freezing, would be a significant benefit of hatching synchrony (Colbert et al.,

2010; Spencer and Janzen, 2014). Early hatching C. picta exhibit reduced performance, which continues for up to 9 months post hatching (Colbert et al., 2010). Reduced hatchling performance suggests that embryos are missing an important stage of development, which may be vital for precocial species (Vleck et al., 1979; Peterson and Kruegl, 2005). Precocial species have a metabolic profile where there is an evident decline in metabolic rate several days before hatching (Thompson, 1989; Booth, 2000; Booth and Astill, 2001; Du et al.,

2010b; McGlashan et al., 2012). This period is considered a ‘resting stage’ where tissue growth is essentially complete, sensory, neuromuscular, and thermoregulatory systems mature, and any necessary acclimation can occur with minimal metabolic cost.

Metabolic compensation enables embryos to complete development which prevents them from being disadvantaged developmentally (McGlashan et al., 2012). Metabolic compensation can be costly in terms of yolk reserves that are necessary for post hatching growth and development (Radder et al., 2002; Radder et al., 2004; McGlashan et al., 2012).

Because neonates of C. picta typically remain in the nest over winter, we would not expect metabolic compensation, as utilising yolk reserves prior to hatching could be detrimental to their survival in the nest (Willette et al., 2005). The aim of this study was to test the hypothesis that synchronous hatching occurs in C. picta by hatching early and not through metabolic compensation. We investigated metabolic rate by measuring ( O2) and heart rate profiles to detect variation of embryos incubated in controlled conditions of synchronous and asynchronous incubation.

64

Method Egg collection and incubation

Eggs of C. picta were collected from freshly built nests at the Thomson Causeway Recreation

Area, Thomson, IL, USA from the 4th-7th June 2012. Eggs were excavated from nest within 5 h of oviposition and were placed in plastic containers with moist soil. A total of 10 clutches of nine eggs each were collected within two days, and were returned to the laboratory at Iowa

State University. All eggs were uniquely marked using a HB pencil, weighed and were placed in clear plastic incubator containers (200 x 100 x 50 mm), half buried in a 1:1 mixture of vermiculite and deionised water (Spencer et al., 2001; Colbert et al., 2010). To account for evaporation and maintain water potential of vermiculite, containers were weighed and rehydrated weekly. To counteract potential thermal gradients, containers were also rotated weekly within incubators (Janzen, 1993).

Incubation method

Treatment and control groups containing five individual clutches in each were established.

The treatment (asynchronous) group and the control (synchronous) group were set up using protocols similar to Spencer et al. (2001) and Colbert et al. (2010). To establish asynchrony within the five asynchronous clutches, three of the nine eggs from each clutch were randomly selected to be incubated at 30 °C (asynchronous-more advanced eggs) and the remaining six eggs (asynchronous-target eggs) were incubated at 26 °C for the first 7 d of incubation. After

7 d, the asynchronous-target eggs were reunited with the three asynchronous-more advanced eggs at 30 °C. The asynchronous-more advanced eggs were positioned between the asynchronous-target eggs incubated at 26 °C, within 10 mm of each other (Fig. 1). Containers were then incubated at 28 °C until they hatched. The remaining five clutches in the synchronous group were set up using the same procedure, with the three synchronous-not advanced eggs being incubated at 26 °C for the 7 d period. The remaining six eggs 65

(synchronous- target eggs) were also incubated at 26 °C so that all nine eggs were still at the same developmental stage and were therefore already synchronised by incubation temperature when reunited (Spencer et al., 2001). After eggs were reunited into containers, containers were incubated at 28 °C until they hatched. Each clutch was incubated in a separate container throughout the experiment. Heart rates and O2 of target embryos from asynchronous and synchronous groups were monitored weekly and containers were opened every 2-3 days to allow fresh air exchange throughout the incubation period until the commencement of pipping/hatching.

Fig. 1: Eggs from each clutch were divided and incubated separately for 7 d, then returned to the same containers to continue incubation at 28 °C. Asynchronous group: three more- advanced eggs (black) were incubated at 30°C and six eggs (white) were incubated at 26 °C for 7 d. Synchronous group- three not advanced eggs (black) were incubated at 26 °C and six eggs (white) were incubated at 26 °C for 7 d. All eggs were then placed in 28 °C incubators until the completion of incubation.

66

Oxygen consumption

Oxygen consumption was measured from week 4 to 8 of an eight week incubation period.

One target egg from each clutch (5 asynchronous and 5 synchronous), incubating next to either an asynchronous-more advanced egg or a synchronous-not advanced egg, was selected for O2 analysis each week. O2 of each egg was measured twice within a 24 h period and the average of both analyses was used. Oxygen (%) was measured in a closed system using an S-

3A oxygen analyser (AEI Technologies, USA). The analyser was first calibrated (20.94 %

O2) using 50 cc of atmospheric air injected using a syringe and syringe pump. Unknown samples were also collected and analysed through the same method (Vleck, 1987). Airtight chambers were constructed from 500 mL metal paint cans and sealed with lids that were fitted with a 2-way stopcock. Eggs were placed on moist cotton wool, to keep them from desiccating, in the opened chamber and allowed to come to thermal equilibrium in the 28 °C incubator approximately 5 min prior to sealing. The lid was sealed and an initial (FI) air sample was taken from the outflow port with a syringe fitted with a stopcock. The chamber pressure was allowed to equilibrate then the outflow port was sealed and containers were carefully placed in the incubator at 28 °C. A final (FE) sample was taken at the conclusion before the egg was then placed back into its original incubator container with its clutch- mates. To measure O2, eggs remained in the air-tight chambers for a period between 1 and 5 h, depending on the stage of incubation. A negative control was in place to account for any leakage or spontaneous change in O2 not caused by the egg. Temperature and air pressure were recorded at the beginning of the experiment to determine standard temperature pressure volume of each can. FI and FE samples were then put through the O2 analyser, via two 3 mL syringe barrels, one with silica gel and the other with a combination of ascarite and soda lime to remove CO2 and moisture. Fractional O2 concentrations of FI and FE samples were

67

– analysed and O2 was calculated from the equation (Vleck et al., 1979; –

Vleck, 1987).

Heart rate measurements

Heart rates (bpm) were measured from weeks 3 to 8 of incubation to determine 1) heart rate profile, 2) circadian rhythm, and 3) metabolic compensation. Heart rates were used to infer metabolic rate (Wallace and Jones, 2008; Du et al., 2009) of embryos and identify any difference between asynchronous and synchronous groups. One target egg from each clutch was removed from its incubator container and immediately placed in a Buddy digital egg monitor system (Avian Biotech, England) in complete darkness. Three readings were taken once heart rates stabilised (within 2 min) (McGlashan et al., 2015; Sartori et al., 2015). The mean of three readings, taken at 30 s intervals, was calculated. Eggs were not disturbed during the recording period and any embryonic movement could be detected and distinguished from heart rate readings, in which case we would wait for movement to cease and heart rate to stabilise before recording again (McGlashan et al., 2015). The heart rate of each egg was taken at four time intervals over a 24 h period (0500 h, 1100 h, 1700 h and

2300 h) once a week and an average daily heart rate was calculated for weekly profile. Each week a different egg was used to record heart rate.

Pipping and post hatching measurements

To determine incubation period, eggs were inspected for signs of pipping (initial break of egg with caruncle) twice daily (Gutzke et al., 1984). The incubation period was measured as time in days from oviposition until pipping, and was used to demonstrate that the “target” eggs hatched at the same time as the asynchronous-more advanced eggs. Neonates were allowed to hatch unassisted, after which they were weighed (g), straight carapace length and width and

68

straight plastron length and width were measured and performance tests (righting ability) were undertaken within 12 h of hatching. Performance and coordination of hatchlings was assessed through righting times. All hatchlings were placed on their back (carapace) and the time taken to right themselves was measured as well as latency to begin moving and total active righting time, with all tests terminated after 180s. Measurements were used as an index of neuromuscular development (Vleck et al., 1979; Vleck et al., 1980; Peterson and Kruegl,

2005; Colbert et al., 2010).

Data analysis

Incubation period was analysed using a mixed linear model analysis in SAS (PROC MIXED;

SAS, version 9.3, SAS Institute, Cary, NC). Embryonic O2 and heart rate were compared among weeks and among asynchronous and synchronous groups using a mixed linear model analysis (PROC MIXED). Egg mass (g) and week of incubation were included in the model as covariates. Circadian rhythms were determined through daily heart rate fluctuations, which were calculated as a difference from the mean diel rate (bpm) at each time point. Peaks were aligned with time of day in both asynchronous and synchronous groups to determine circadian rhythms. Differences from the mean diel rate were analysed between asynchronous and synchronous groups each week and at each time point using a mixed linear model analysis (PROC MIXED) with egg mass as a covariate. The difference between each time point (i.e. -6 h to peak), within treatment groups were analysed using an ANOVA; Single factor.

Phenotypic differences in carapace length and width, plastron length and width and hatchling mass were compared among asynchronous and synchronous groups using a multivariate analysis of variance (MANCOVA) with egg mass as a covariate in SAS (PROC GLM).

69

Hatchling righting ability was individually analysed using a mixed linear model analysis

(PROC MIXED) with hatchling mass as a covariate. A binomial test was used to assess propensity of hatchlings to right themselvesin SAS (PROC GLIMMIX). If treatment effects were found to be significant, then post-hoc Tukey tests were used to determine where pairwise differences occurred. All O2, heart rate, and growth data analysed with PROC

MIXED were log-transformed before analysis to ensure linearity in the models. Examination of residuals ensured that all assumptions of univariate parametric statistics were met. In the multivariate analysis, the assumption of multivariate normality could not be directly tested due to sample size limitations. Thus, Pillai’s Trace was used as a test statistic because it is robust to violations of multivariate normality (Scheiner, 2001). Statistical significance was determined at the 0.05 Type I error level, and data are presented as the mean ± SE.

Results Incubation period

Of the 89 eggs, 11 failed to hatch (2 asynchronous-target eggs-, 4 synchronous-focus eggs, 3 asynchronous-more advanced eggs and 2 synchronous-not advanced eggs). Hatching synchrony occurred within clutches in both asynchronous (treatment) (t73 = 1.84, p = 0.2641) and synchronous groups (control) (t73 = 0.04, p = 1), with incubation period averaging 53 ±

0.29 and 54 ± 0.3 days respectively (Fig. 2). The incubation period of eggs in the asynchronous or synchronous clutches was significantly affected by treatment group (F3,73 =

4.3, p = 0.0076). Further pairwise comparisons showed that asynchrony was established, as the incubation period of asynchronous-more advanced and synchronous-not advanced eggs differed by 2 d (t73 =2.84, p = 0.0293). Asynchronous-more advanced eggs also hatched significantly earlier than the synchronous-target eggs and the synchronous-not advanced eggs

70

within clutches (t73 = 3.25, p = 0.0091), but there was no significant difference in incubation period of asynchronous-target eggs and synchronous-target eggs (t73 = 1.9, p = 0.2361).

55.5 b b 55

54.5 a b

54

a 53.5

53

Incubation period (days) period Incubation 52.5

52

51.5 Focus eggs- More-advanced eggs Focus eggs- Not-advanced eggs Asynchronous Synchronous

Fig. 2: Mean incubation period of eggs in asynchronous and synchronous groups, ± SE, N = 78. Letters indicate significant pairwise differences among weeks in both asynchronous and synchronous groups, identical letters indicate no difference.

Oxygen consumption

Treatment and control groups did not differ in O2 at any time point over the course of incubation (F1,30 = 0.01, p = 0.9149; Fig. 3), but there was a time effect (F4,30 = 3.33, p =

0.0226). O2 increased with egg mass (F1,30 = 15.78, p = 0.0004), and there was an interaction effect between egg mass and time (F4,30 = 0.27, p = 0.432) but no interaction of treatment and egg mass (F1,30 = 0.12. p = 0.7289), treatment and time (F4,30 = 0.27 p =

71

0.8969), or egg mass, treatment and time (F4,30 = 0.28, p = 0.886). O2 of asynchronous- target eggs peaked at 0.48 mL/h in week 6 of incubation and declined to 0.16 mL/h prior to hatching in week 8. Synchronous-target eggs peaked at 0.45 mL/h in week 5 then gradually declined to 0.17 mL/h in week 8. O2 increased from week 4-5 within each group (t30 = -5.15, p < 0.0001) and declined from week 7 to 8 of incubation (t30 = 6.77, p < 0.0001).

0.6 b b 0.5 b

0.4 a

(mL/h) 0.3

c

2 VO 0.2

0.1

0 4 5 6 7 8 Week

Fig. 3: LSMeans of oxygen consumption (mL/h) of embryos in asynchronous (dark grey) and synchronous (light grey) groups from week 4 to 8 of incubation, ± SE, n = 10, N = 50. Letters indicate significant pairwise differences among weeks in both asynchronous and synchronous groups.

Heart rate

Heart rates also did not differ significantly between asynchronous-target eggs and synchronous-target eggs at any time point during the incubation period (F1,36 = 1.19, p =

72

0.2819; Fig. 4) and were further unaffected by egg mass and by interactions of treatment and egg mass. Heart rates did vary among weeks of incubation (F5,36 = 5.55, p = 0.0007), gradually decreasing from 96 to 60 in asynchronous groups, and 100 to 67 bpm in synchronous groups from week 3 to 8 of incubation. There was no interaction effect of egg mass and time, treatment and time, nor egg mass, treatment and time. Heart rate in both treatment groups declined significantly from week 6 to 7 (t47 = 3.71, p = 0.0069) and week 7 to 8 (t47 = 3.60, p = 0.0094) of incubation.

120 a a b 100 b c

d 80

60

Heart Rate(bpm) Heart 40

20

0 3 4 5 6 7 8 Week

Fig. 4: LSMeans of heart rate (bpm) of embryos in asynchronous (dark grey) and synchronous (light grey) groups from week 3 to 8 of incubation, ± SE, n = 10, N = 60. Letters indicate significant pairwise differences among weeks in both asynchronous and synchronous groups.

Circadian rhythms in heart rates were present with a clear peak in heart rates throughout the day in both asynchronous-target eggs (-6 h to peak- F1,14 = 18.92, p = 0.0007; peak to 6 h-

73

F1,14 = 20.41, p = 0.0005; Fig. 5) and synchronous-target eggs (-6 h to peak- F1,14 = 5.59, p =

0.03; peak to 6 h- F1,14 = 24.69, p = 0.0002). Individuals showed minimum and maximum heart rates over a 24 h period that varied up to 6 bpm, but they were not correlated with the time of day in both asynchronous and synchronous groups. There was no significant difference between asynchronous and synchronous groups at each time point; -6 h (F1,30 =

0.22, p = 0.64), peak (F1,30 = 0.00, p = 0.99), 6 h (F1,30 = 1.39, p = 0.25) nor 12 h (F1,30 = 0.01, p = 0.94). There was a modest week effect (F4,30 = 2.8, p = 0.044) and egg mass by week interaction effect (F4,30 = 3.38, p = 0.021) during the peak time point but further pairwise tests revealed no significant differences between weeks. No other main effects or interaction effects were significant at any time point.

12 b

10

8

6

4

2 a 0 a a -2

Difference from Differencefrom mean heartrate (bpm) -4 -6 h Peak 6 h 12 h -6

Fig. 5: The difference from mean heart rate of asynchronous (dark grey) and synchronous (light grey) groups over a 24 h period across weeks 3 to 8 of incubation. The x-axis represents duration of time (h) away from the peak heart rate and the y-axis represents the difference from the mean heart rate (bpm), ± SE, N = 62. Letters indicate significant pairwise differences among 6 h time periods in both asynchronous and synchronous groups. 74

Post hatching measurements

Multivariate analysis of all measureable phenotypes (carapace length and width, plastron length and width and mass) post hatching revealed that hatching size was not affected by the interaction between treatment and egg mass (Pillai = 0.2142, F15,174 = 0.89 p = 0.5742).

Treatment alone also did not have an effect on size of hatchlings (Pillai = 0.165, F10,114 = 1.03 p = 0.4265), with egg mass alone explaining the majority of variation in body size (Pillai =

0.647, F5,56 = 20.54 p < 0.0001). The average measurements for carapace length in asynchronous groups was 25.6 ± 0.37 mm and synchronous group was 25.57 ± 0.39 mm.

Plastron length in asynchronous groups was 24.69 ± 0.42 mm and synchronous groups was

24.76 and hatchling mass was 4.96 ± 0.31 g and 4.99 ± 0.2 g respectively.

Performance and neuromuscular development, as assessed by righting ability, was not affected by treatment group. There was no significant difference between any of the treatment groups in ability to right successfully (F2,65 = 1.3, p = 0.2783). Latency to right, total time to right and active righting time were also not affected by treatment group.

Discussion Chrysemys picta neonates hatched synchronously within both asynchronous and synchronous clutches, with no indication of metabolic compensation. Asynchronous-target eggs hatched within 1 day of the asynchronous-more advanced eggs with which they were incubated, similar to the synchronous group, where synchrony was expected. The incubation period of asynchronous-more advanced eggs was significantly different from the synchronous-not advanced eggs, indicating that the asynchronous setup (Fig. 1) was enough to result in a substantial difference in incubation period, similar to that detected in prior work (Colbert et al., 2010).

75

There was no evidence of metabolic compensation in heart rate and O2 of developing target eggs in both asynchronous and synchronous clutches as they were similar to each other at each time point of incubation. O2 peaked in weeks 5 and 6 and declined in the final 2 weeks, as expected (Thompson, 1989; Booth et al., 2000; Booth and Astill, 2001; Du et al., 2010a;

McGlashan et al., 2012). Heart rates were also similar between target eggs in both asynchronous and synchronous clutches, gradually decreasing from week 3 to 8 of incubation, a pattern seen in semi-precocial birds (Cain et al., 1967; Tazawa et al., 1991a;

Tazawa et al., 1991b; Tazawa, 2005) and the snapping turtle (Chelydra serpentina) (Birchard and Reiber, 1996). The observed fall in metabolic rate prior to hatching, known as the resting period, in both heart rate and O2 has also been detected in embryos of C. longicollis

(McGlashan et al., 2015), C. insculpta (Webb et al., 1986), E. macquarii (Thompson, 1989;

McGlashan et al., 2012), and the American alligator (Alligator mississipiensis) (Thompson,

1989), 80-90% of the way through incubation. The resting period is when acclimation occurs and metabolic costs for growth are not usually as high and the effect of temperature is not as great as it is during early development (Birchard and Reiber, 1995; Birchard and Reiber,

1996). Hence, if metabolic compensation was to occur, it would be expected during this fall in metabolic rate, as seen in E. macquarii (McGlashan et al., 2012). Synchronous hatching with no evidence of metabolic compensation suggests embryos are hatching early with the more-advanced eggs in the clutch and possibly missing out on an important stage of development.

Circadian rhythms were present in developing C. picta embryos despite an absence of environmental cues, such as daylight. Heart rates were elevated and reached a ‘peak’ during the day, indicating high developmental activity, and then dropped, suggesting low developmental activity or daily ‘resting’. These rhythms are not synchronised to the time of

76

day among eggs and there is no indication of synchrony within clutches when compared from week to week, similar to C. longicollis and E. macquarii (McGlashan et al., 2012; Loudon et al., 2013). The mean weekly heart rate can reflect metabolic rate of embryos, and deviations from the mean rate throughout the day may suggest positive or negative compensatory changes in embryonic metabolism. Asynchronous groups did not exceed the potential for periods of higher peak daily rate or reduced resting periods relative to synchronous groups.

There was no difference in any deviation from the mean between asynchronous and synchronous groups at each time point indicating that there was no metabolic compensation within diel heart rate fluctuations.

Hatchlings were not obviously disadvantaged in terms of growth and development. The lack of a difference in size or mass of hatchlings, and their ability to right themselves, indicated that hatching early does not substantially affect development. Although previous studies found that neonates are affected by synchronous hatching, it is likely that a difference in the length of time used to establish asynchrony might be the cause. Colbert et al. (2010) kept more advanced embryos at a warmer temperature for a period of 11 d. Neonates were most likely too under-developed and, when stimulated to hatch synchronously by more advanced clutch-mates, were ultimately disadvantaged in performance.

In terms of biological significance, eggs at the top of the nest experience greater temperature fluctuations throughout the day and are warmer for more than 75% of the day compared to eggs at the bottom (Thompson, 1988, Thompson et al., 1996, Thompson, 1997, Shine and

Elphick, 2001). Nest temperatures are also influenced by air temperature fluctuations, substrate thermal conductance, and maternal effects, such as nest-site selection (Thompson,

1988). Each of these factors will determine the extent to which temperature differences

77

within a nest might cause differences in embryonic metabolisms and growth rates. It is possible that natural conditions in the nest could stimulate a greater difference between clutch-mates than elicited experimentally, and thus result in neonates with poor performance.

Further studies are needed to determine the upper limit of synchronous hatching in natural conditions. The final 10 % of incubation is when hatching synchrony can take place once hatching competence is achieved. The asynchronous set up in this study was 12 % of the total incubation period, but in Colbert et al. (2010) the incubation set up was proportionally 20 % of the total incubation period. Nevertheless, both this study and Colbert et al. (2010) demonstrated that synchronous hatching occurs in C. picta regardless of developmental differences at the beginning of incubation and the potential costs that may come as a result of hatching early. If the difference in development is small, then synchrony will not incur performance costs.

Although temperature is the major determinant for embryonic development in ectotherms

(Deeming and Ferguson, 1991; Booth, 1998; Monaghan, 2008), hatching time is nonetheless plastic (Packard and Packard, 2000; Doody, 2011; Spencer and Janzen, 2011). Environmental cues within the nest may trigger hatching (Doody, 2011). Whilst synchronous hatching is driven by a particular abiotic or biotic force in many species, it does not appear to be the case in C. picta. Indeed, offspring in many populations of freshwater turtles in the northern hemisphere overwinter in the natal nest (Gibbons, 2013).Winter nest temperatures are variable (Weisrock and Janzen, 1999) and could influence the overall energy expenditures of neonates in the nest. During winter, metabolic demands are generally low but maternally sourced yolk provisions are utilised during this time, and the warmer the winter, the more energy is used (Willette et al., 2005; Muir et al., 2013). Yolk stores are extremely important in C. picta, a species that has temperature-dependent sex determination, as males that have

78

developed in cooler nest conditions will utilise less yolk than females in cooler over wintering nests, and females who develop in warmer incubation conditions will utilise less yolk than males in warmer overwintering nests (Spencer and Janzen, 2014). Thus, it would be important for their survival to hold onto these energy stores in preparation for atypical overwintering conditions, as this can result in up to 50% loss of energy (Spencer and Janzen,

2014). By not metabolically compensating during development, embryos are able to reserve more energy stores to support them through the autumn and winter months and the critical time of emergence and migration.

Environmentally cued hatching is evident in many oviparous species, including birds (Vince and Chinn, 1971), fishes (Bradbury et al., 2004), amphibians (Sih and Moore, 1993;

Warkentin, 1995; Warkentin, 2000) and reptiles (Ferguson, 1985; Vitt, 1991; Doody et al.,

2001; Spencer et al., 2001; Colbert et al., 2010). Importantly, the mechanisms driving synchronous hatching in C. picta differ from the mechanisms present in other turtles (E. macquarii, C. longicollis or C. insculpta) and could have evolved independently. By overwintering in the nest, C. picta hatchlings experienced a more favourable environment and reduced risk of predation when emerging in the following spring (Wilbur, 1975; Gibbons,

2013). Carettochelys insulpta also wait until more favourable conditions and lower predation risk, but do so by initially delaying hatching until triggered to synchronously hatch by seasonal flooding (Webb et al., 1986; Doody et al., 2001). For E. macquarii and C. longicollis, synchronous hatching is important for predator avoidance through predator swamping or reducing exposure to prey switching predators (Ims, 1990; Tucker et al., 2008).

Whether emergence is triggered immediately after hatching or several months later could depend on environmental factors unique to a particular region, or by genetic factors unique to some populations (Breitenbach et al., 1984). Synchronous hatching through the mechanism of

79

early hatching does not have any obvious benefit or disadvantage to C. picta because hatchlings usually remain in the nest for several months after hatching. Thus, synchronous hatching may be an ancestral trait in C. picta which has been retained through descent and natural selection because its effect is not deleterious to hatchling survival.

Acknowledgements Research was supported by a UWS SSH Equipment Fund Grant and was conducted under permits from the Illinois Department of Natural Resources and Iowa State Institutional

Animal Care and Use Committee. We thank the US Army Corps of Engineers for access to the Thomson Causeway, T. S. Mitchell, J. Maciel, B. Bodensteiner and C. Hinsley for lab and field assistance. We also thank J. U. Van Dyke for statistical advice and helpful comments and D. Vleck for use of his laboratory and respirometry equipment.

References Birchard, G. & Reiber, C. 1996. Heart rate during development in the turtle embryo: effect of temperature. J. Comp. Physiol., B, 166, 461-466.

Birchard, G. F. & Reiber, C. L. 1995. Growth, metabolism, and chorioallantoic vascular density of developing snapping turtles (Chelydra serpentina): influence of temperature. Physiol. Zool., 68, 799-811.

Booth, D. T. 1998. Incubation of turtle eggs at different temperatures: do embryos compensate for temperature during development? Physiol. Zool., 71, 23-26.

Booth, D. T. 2000. Incubation of eggs of the Australian broad-shelled turtle, Chelodina expansa (Testudinata : Chelidae), at different temperatures: effects on pattern of oxygen consumption and hatchling morphology. Aust. J. Zool., 48, 369-378.

Booth, D. T. & Astill, K. 2001. Incubation temperature, energy expenditure and hatchling size in the green turtle (Chelonia mydas), a species with temperature-sensitive sex determination. Australian Journal of Zoology, 49, 389-396.

Booth, D. T., Thompson, M. B. & Herring, S. 2000. How incubation temperature influences the physiology and growth of embryonic lizards. J. Comp. Physiol., B, 170, 269-276.

80

Bradbury, I. R., Campana, S. E., Bentzen, P. & Snelgrove, P. V. R. 2004. Synchronized hatch and its ecological significance in rainbow smelt Osmerus mordax in St. Mary's Bay, Newfoundland. Limnol. Oceanogr., 49, 2310-2315.

Breitenbach, G. L., Congdon, J. D. & Sels, R. C. v. L. 1984. Winter temperatures of Chrysemys picta nests in Michigan: effects on hatchling survival. Herpetologica, 40, 76-81.

Cain, J. R., Abbott, U. K. & Rogallo, V. L. 1967. Heart rate of the developing chick embryo. Exp. Biol. Med., 126, 507-510.

Colbert, P. L., Spencer, R.-J. & Janzen, F. J. 2010. Mechanism and cost of synchronous hatching. Funct. Ecol., 24, 112-121.

Deeming, D. C. & Ferguson, M. W. J. 1991. Egg Incubation: Its Effects on Embryonic Development in Birds and Reptiles, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.

Doody, J. S. 2011. Environmentally cued hatching in reptiles. Integr. Comp. Biol., 51, 49-61.

Doody, J. S., Georges, A., Young, J. E., Pauza, M. D., Pepper, A. L., Alderman, R. L. & Welsh, M. A. 2001. Embryonic aestivation and emergence behaviour in the pig-nosed turtle, Carettochelys insculpta. Can. J. Zool., 79, 1062-1072.

Doody, J. S., Stewart, B., Camacho, C. & Christian, K. 2012. Good vibrations? Sibling embryos expedite hatching in a turtle. Anim. Behav., 83, 645-651.

Du, W.-G., Radder, R. S., Sun, B. & Shine, R. 2009. Determinants of incubation period: do reptilian embryos hatch after a fixed total number of heart beats? J. Exp. Biol., 212, 1302-1306.

Du, W.-G., Thompson, M. B. & Shine, R. 2010a. Facultative cardiac responses to regional hypoxia in lizard embryos. Comp. Biochem. Physiol., A: Mol. Integr. Physiol., 156, 491-494.

Du, W.-G., Zhao, B. & Shine, R. 2010b. Embryos in the fast lane: high-temperature heart rates of turtles decline after hatching. PLoS ONE, 5, e9557.

Ferguson, M. W. J. 1985. Reproductive biology and embryology of the crocodilians. Biology of the Reptilia. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Gibbons, J. W. 2013. A long-term perspective of delayed emergence (aka overwintering) in hatchling turtles: some they do and some they don't, and some you just can't tell. J. Herpetol., 47, 203-214.

Gutzke, W. H. N., Paukstis, G. L. & Packard, G. C. 1984. Pipping versus hatching as indices of time of incubation in reptiles. J. Herpetol., 18, 494-496.

81

Ims, R. A. 1990. The ecology and evolution of reproductive synchrony. Trends Ecol. Evol., 5, 135-140.

Janzen, F. J. 1993. An experimental analysis of natural selection on body size of hatchling turtles. Ecology, 74, 332-341.

Loudon, F. K., Spencer, R.-J., Strassmeyer, A. & Harland, K. 2013. Metabolic circadian rhythms in embryonic turtles. Integr. Comp. Biol., 53, 175-182.

McGlashan, J. K., Loudon, F. K., Thompson, M. B. & Spencer, R.-J. 2015. Hatching behavior of eastern long-necked turtles (Chelodina longicollis): the influence of asynchronous environments on embryonic heart rate and phenotype. Comp. Biochem. Physiol., A: Mol. Integr. Physiol., 188, 58-64.

McGlashan, J. K., Spencer, R.-J. & Old, J. M. 2012. Embryonic communication in the nest: metabolic responses of reptilian embryos to developmental rates of siblings. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci., 279, 1709-1715.

Monaghan, P. 2008. Early growth conditions, phenotypic development and environmental change. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., Ser. B: Biol. Sci., 363, 1635-1645.

Muir, T. J., Dishong, B. D., Lee, R. E. & Costanzo, J. P. 2013. Energy use and management of energy reserves in hatchling turtles (Chrysemys picta) exposed to variable winter conditions. J. Therm. Biol., 38, 324-330.

Packard, G. C. & Packard, M. J. 2000. Developmental plasticity in painted turtles, Chrysemys picta. Funct. Ecol., 14, 474-483.

Packard, G. C. & Packard, M. J. 2004. To freeze or not to freeze: adaptations for overwintering by hatchlings of the North American painted turtle. J. Exp. Biol., 207, 2897-2906.

Peterson, C. C. & Kruegl, A. 2005. Peaked temporal pattern of embryonic metabolism in an emydid turtle (Chrysemys picta picta). J. Herpetol., 39, 678-681.

Radder, R. S., Shanbhag, B. A. & Saidapur, S. K. 2002. Pattern of yolk internalization by hatchlings is related to breeding timing in the garden lizard, Calotes versicolor. Curr. Sci., 82 1484-1485.

Radder, R. S., Shanbhag, B. A. & Saidapur, S. K. 2004. Yolk partitioning in embryos of the lizard, Calotes versicolor: maximize body size or save energy for later use? J. Exp. Zool. A Comp. Exp. Biol., 301, 783-785.

Sartori, M. R., Leite, C. A. C., Abe, A. S., Crossley Ii, D. A. & Taylor, E. W. 2015. The progressive onset of cholinergic and adrenergic control of heart rate during

82

development in the green iguana, Iguana iguana. Comp. Biochem. Physiol., A: Mol. Integr. Physiol., 188, 1-8.

Scheiner, S. M. 2001. MANOVA: multiple response variables and multispecies interactions. Design and Analysis of Ecological Experiments. Scheiner SM, Gurevitch J ed. New York, NY: Oxford University.

Shine, R. & Elphick, M. J. 2001. The effect of short-term weather fluctuations on temperatures inside lizard nests, and on the phenotypic traits of hatchling lizards. Biol. J. Linn. Soc., 72, 555-565.

Sih, A. & Moore, R. D. 1993. Delayed hatching of salamander eggs in response to enhanced larval predation risk. Am. Nat., 142, 947-960.

Spencer, R.-J. & Janzen, F. J. 2011. Hatching behavior in turtles. Integr. Comp. Biol., 51, 100-110.

Spencer, R.-J., Thompson, M. B. & Banks, P. B. 2001. Hatch or wait? A dilemma in reptilian incubation. Oikos, 93, 401-406.

Spencer, R. J. & Janzen, F. J. 2014. A novel hypothesis for the adaptive maintenance of environmental sex determination in a turtle. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci., 281, 20140831.

Tazawa, H. 2005. Cardiac rhythms in avian embryos and hatchlings. Avian Poult. Biol. Rev., 16, 123-150.

Tazawa, H., Hiraguchi, T., Kuroda, O., Tullett, S. G. & Deeming, D. C. 1991a. Embryonic heart rate during development of domesticated birds. Physiol. Zool., 64, 1002-1022.

Tazawa, H., Kuroda, O. & Whittow, G. C. 1991b. Noninvasive determination of embryonic heart rate during hatching in the brown noddy (Anous stolidus). The Auk, 108, 594- 601.

Thompson, M. B. 1988. Nest temperatures in the pleurodiran turtle, Emydura macquarii. Copeia, 1988, 996-1000.

Thompson, M. B. 1989. Patterns of metabolism in embryonic reptiles. Respir. Physiol., 76, 243-255.

Thompson, M. B. 1997. Egg Physiology and Biology. The Biology, Husbandry And Healthcare of Reptile. New Jersey: TFH Publications Inc.

Thompson, M. B., Packard, G. C., Packard, M. J. & Rose, B. 1996. Analysis of the nest environment of tuatara Sphenodon punctatus. J. Zool., 238, 239-251.

83

Tucker, J. K., Paukstis, G. L. & Janzen, F. J. 2008. Does predator swamping promote synchronous emergence of turtle hatchlings among nests? Behav. Ecol., 19, 35-40.

Vince, M. A. & Chinn, S. 1971. Effect of accelerated hatching on the initiation of standing and walking in the Japanese quail. Anim. Behav., 19, 62-66.

Vitt, L. J. 1991. Ecology and life history of the scansorial arboreal lizard Plica plica (Iguanidae) in Amazonian Brazil. Can. J. Zool., 69, 504-511.

Vleck, C. M., Hoyt, D. F. & Vleck, D. 1979. Metabolism of avian embryos: patterns in altricial and precocial birds. Physiol. Zool., 52, 363-377.

Vleck, D. 1987. Measurement of O2 consumption, CO2 production, and water vapor production in a closed system. J. Appl. Physiol., 62, 2103-2106.

Vleck, D., Vleck, C. M. & Hoyt, D. F. 1980. Metabolism of avian embryos: ontogeny of oxygen consumption in the rhea and emu. Physiol. Zool., 53, 125-135.

Wallace, B. P. & Jones, T. T. 2008. What makes marine turtles go: a review of metabolic rates and their consequences. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol., 356, 8-24.

Warkentin, K. M. 1995. Adaptive plasticity in hatching age: a response to predation risk trade-offs. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci., 92, 3507-3510.

Warkentin, K. M. 2000. Wasp predation and wasp-induced hatching of red-eyed treefrog eggs. Anim. Behav., 60, 503-510.

Warkentin, K. M. & Calswell, M. S. 2009. Assessing risk: embryos, information, and escape hatching. In: Dukas, R. & Ratcliffe, J. (eds.) Cognitive Ecology II. The Evolutionary Ecology of Learning, Memory, and Information Use. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Webb, G. J. W., Choquenot, D. & Whitehead, P. J. 1986. Nests, eggs, and embryonic development of Carettochelys insculpta (Chelonia: Carettochelidae) from Northern Australia. J. Zool., 1, 521-550.

Weisrock, D. W. & Janzen, F. J. 1999. Thermal and fitness-related consequences of nest location in painted turtles (Chrysemys picta). Funct. Ecol., 13, 94-101.

Wilbur, H. M. 1975. The evolutionary and mathematical demography of the turtle Chrysemys picta. Ecology, 56, 64-77.

Willette, D. A. S., Tucker, J. K. & Janzen, F. J. 2005. Linking climate and physiology at the population level for a key life-history stage of turtles. Can. J. Zool., 83, 845-850.

84

CHAPTER 4

THYROID HORMONES REDUCE INCUBATION PERIOD WITHOUT

DEVELOPMENTAL AND METABOLIC COSTS IN MURRAY RIVER

SHORT-NECKED TURTLES (EMYDURA MACQUARII)

Chapter in review in Physiological and Biochemical Zoology

85

Title: Thyroid hormones reduce incubation period without developmental and metabolic costs in Murray River short-necked turtles (Emydura macquarii)

Jessica K. McGlashan1, Michael B. Thompson 2, James Van Dyke 1 2and Ricky-John

Spencer1

1School of Science and Health, University of Western Sydney, Locked Bag 1797, Penrith

South DC, NSW 2751, Australia.

2School of Biological Sciences, The University of Sydney NSW 2006, Australia

*Author for correspondence: Jessica McGlashan

Email: [email protected]

Phone: T +61 2 4570 1195

Author Contributions

This manuscript is jointly authored, I am the primary author and I designed the experiment. I applied the hormones, took all the heart rate and respiration measurements, maintained the eggs throughout incubation, recorded the pipping and hatching times, took hatchling measurements and performed the post-hatching righting tests. I undertook all histological methods and carried out data analysis. Dr Ricky-John Spencer and Professor Michael Thompson supervised the work and provided feedback on earlier versions of the manuscript and James Van Dyke provided feedback, critiqued and edited the manuscript.

86

Abstract Metabolic processes are affected by both temperature and thyroid hormones in vertebrates.

Temperature is the major determinant of incubation length in oviparous vertebrates, but turtles can also alter developmental rate independent of temperature. Temperature gradients within natural nests cause different developmental rates of turtle embryos within nests.

Despite temperature-induced reductions in developmental rate, cooler-incubated neonates often hatch synchronously with warmer siblings, via metabolic compensation. The physiological mechanisms underlying metabolic compensation are unknown, but thyroid hormones may play a critical role. We applied excess triiodothyronine (T3) to developing eggs of Murray River short-necked turtle (Emydura macquarii), a species that exhibits metabolic compensation and synchronous hatching, to determine whether T3 influences developmental rate, and whether any changes incur metabolic costs. We measured heart rate, oxygen consumption and incubation period of eggs, and morphology and performance of hatchlings. Embryos that were exposed to T3 pipped up to 3.5 days earlier than untreated controls, despite no change in total metabolic expenditure, and there were no treatment differences in hatchling morphology. Hatchlings treated with T3 demonstrated similar righting ability to hatchlings from the control groups. Exposure to T3 shortens incubation length by accelerating embryonic development, but without noticeably increasing embryonic metabolism. Thus, T3 is a mechanism that cooler-incubated reptiles could use to accelerate their development to allow synchronous hatching with their warmer clutch mates, but at little or no metabolic cost. Thus, “metabolic compensation” for synchronous hatching may not be metabolically expensive, if T3 is the underlying mechanism.

Key words: Triiodothyronine, T3, synchronous hatching, environmentally cued hatching, phenotypic plasticity, embryogenesis, egg

87

Introduction All physiological processes in animals are temperature-dependent (Johnston et al. 1996;

Packard et al. 1988; Packard and Packard 1988; Rome et al. 1992; Wilhoft 1958). In developing oviparous vertebrates, warmer incubation temperatures accelerate embryogenesis and reduce incubation periods (Birchard and Reiber 1996; Colbert et al. 2010; Lin et al. 2008;

Spencer et al. 2001). Oxygen consumption ( O2) and heart rates of embryos are also temperature-dependent, partially as a result of accelerated embryogenesis (Birchard and

Reiber 1996; Booth et al. 2000; Du et al. 2011; Du et al. 2010; Lillywhite et al. 1999;

Vladimirova et al. 2005). Ultimately, incubation temperature also influences aspects of offspring phenotype, including morphology (Du et al. 2009; Du et al. 2010; Packard and

Packard 1988), performance (Booth et al. 2004; Colbert et al. 2010; Dial 1987; Janzen 1993;

Nelson et al. 2004; Weisrock and Janzen 1999) and sex differentiation in species that have temperature-dependent sex determination (Bull 1985; Bull and Vogt 1979; Yntema 1968,

1978).

In reptile nests, mean temperature differences occur throughout development as a result of diel and seasonal changes in ambient temperature (Shine and Elphick 2001; Thompson 1988,

1997; Thompson et al. 1996). Within shallow nests, a temperature gradient occurs during the day, where eggs at the bottom of the nest experience cooler temperatures and eggs at the top of the nest experience warmer temperatures, by up to 6 °C (Thompson 1988; Thompson et al.

1996). Due to being closer to the ground surface, eggs from the top of the nest experience higher temperatures during the day than do eggs at the bottom, while at night all eggs within the next experience similar temperatures (Thompson 1988; Thompson et al. 1996). These variable temperatures cause asynchronous development between the top and bottom eggs within a clutch (Thompson 1988, 1997). For example, in captivity, the Murray River short-

88

neck turtle (Emydura macquarii) eggs incubated at 30 °C can hatch up to 3 weeks earlier than do sibling eggs incubated at 26 °C (Dormer et al. In Press). Despite considerable variation in incubation temperatures, many reptilian embryos still hatch synchronously (Cannon et al.

1986; Colbert et al. 2010; Du et al. 2011; McGlashan et al. 2012; Spencer et al. 2001;

Thompson 1989; Vleck et al. 1980).

The majority of evidence for synchronous hatching in the literature is anecdotal, with only a few species studied in depth. The pig-nosed turtle (Carettochelys insculpta) delays hatching until an environmental trigger (hypoxia) stimulates a synchronised hatching event (Doody et al. 2001; Doody et al. 2012; Webb et al. 1986). Synchronous hatching occurs in E. macquarii and the North American painted turtle (Chrysemys picta) when less developed embryos, incubating at cooler temperatures, either adjust their developmental rate through metabolic compensation (McGlashan et al. 2012) or hatch early (Colbert et al. 2010; Spencer and

Janzen 2011). Metabolic compensation in E. macquarii has been defined as an increase in developmental rate in cool-incubated embryos, independent of incubation temperature, that allows them to developmentally “catch up” with warm-incubated embryos, likely with a metabolic cost (McGlashan et al. 2012). Synchronous hatching through metabolic compensation is also evident through circadian rhythms in the eastern long-necked turtle

(Chelodina longicollis), this may be stimulated by communication through diel adjustments of heart rate (McGlashan et al. 2015), as they are in embryonic E. macquarii (Loudon et al.,

2013). Remarkably, these changes in metabolic and developmental rates do not incur costs to hatchling morphology or locomotor performance (McGlashan et al. 2015; McGlashan et al.

2012).

89

The physiological mechanisms that underlie metabolic compensation are not known. Thyroid hormones are likely candidates for regulating metabolic compensation because they regulate embryonic development, growth, and metabolism in animals (Dauncey 1990; Himms-Hagen

1976; Hollenberg and Forrest 2008; Silva 2001). Triiodothyronine (T3) and thyroxine (T4) are secreted from the thyroid gland into circulation for distribution to target tissues (Ford et al.

1957; Norris 1997; Rivera and Lock 2008). Responsiveness to thyroid hormones occurs early in embryonic development and maternal thyroid hormones deposited into the yolk or transferred via a placenta are the primary supply for the early embryo (Forrest et al. 1990;

McComb et al. 2005; McNabb and Wilson 1997). Thyroid hormone concentrations decrease throughout the early stages of development when there is a significant biological requirement for the hormone, particularly for neurodevelopment (Bernal and Nunez 1995; Escobar et al.

2004; Morreale de Escobar et al. 2004), but concentrations increase following development of the thyroid gland and the onset of organ function (Prati et al. 1992). Exposure to more T3 and T4 can accelerate development in the embryos of vertebrates and conversely, lack of thyroid hormones can significantly arrest development and result in deformity (Brown 1997;

Callery and Elinson 2000; Escobar et al. 2004; Miwa and Inui 1987; Morreale de Escobar et al. 2004). Thus, differences in the production of thyroid hormone within individual embryos may explain why some individuals develop faster than others. In reptiles, metabolic compensation may occur because cooler-incubated embryos are stimulated to produce greater amounts of thyroid hormone in order to catch up with warmer-incubated siblings.

In reptile nests, eggs incubate in close contact with each other. Cues from more advanced warmer eggs could stimulate cooler, less advanced eggs to secrete more thyroid hormone to accelerate development to catch-up and hatch synchronously. These cues could include sibling heart rate, CO2 concentrations in the nest (Spencer and Janzen 2011; Spencer et al.

90

2001) and acoustic communication through stridulation, vibration and audible clicks (Ferrara et al. 2014; Ferrara et al. 2013; Vergne et al. 2009). Natural increases of thyroid hormone concentrations may be responsible for accelerated development in freshwater turtles that result in synchronous hatching through metabolic compensation.

The aim of this study was to determine whether thyroid hormones facilitate metabolic compensation and thus, synchronous hatching. We focused on T3 because it is the active form of thyroid hormone in mammals (Braverman et al. 1970; DiStefano et al. 1982a; DiStefano et al. 1982b; Hollenberg and Forrest 2008; Schwartz et al. 1971) and would be most likely to influence embryonic metabolic rate in turtles (O'Steen and Janzen 1999). T3 is also the primary hormone that increases O2 consumption in thyroidectomised lizards (Chandola-

Saklani and Kar 1990). In addition, we have preliminary evidence that metabolically- compensating E. macquarii exhibit slightly elevated T3 concentrations during development

(McGlashan 2016). We specifically tested the hypothesis that increased T3 would accelerate developmental rate in embryos of the turtle Emydura macquarii by inducing positive metabolic compensation (eg. increased heart rate and O2).

Method Study species

Emydura macquarii is the focal species for this study because embryos can be stimulated to accelerate development through metabolic compensation when incubated with more advanced embryos (McGlashan et al. 2012). Emydura macquarii is an Australian pleurodiran

(side-necked) turtle that lays clutches of 10-30 eggs in relatively shallow terrestrial nests from October-December. Females usually emerge to nest in the riparian zones close to aquatic habitat during or after rain (Chessman 1986).

91

Egg collection and incubation

Female E. macquarii were captured from the Murray River, Albury, New South Wales from

30th October to 1st November 2012, using lobster and cathedral traps. Gravidity was determined by palpation. Gravid females were injected subcutaneously with oxytocin (Ilium

Syntocin 10 IU/ml, Troy Laboratories PTY LTD) at a dose of 1mL/kg, to stimulate oviposition, and placed in 150 mm deep water in tubs 600 x 400 mm in a dark quiet room for up to 24 h (Spencer et al. 2001). Eggs were collected from tubs, weighed, individually marked in laying order using a HB pencil, and placed in moist vermiculite. Prior to release, all females were palpated to ensure all eggs were oviposited; if not, then a second oxytocin injection was administered to complete oviposition. Clutches were transported to the

University of Western Sydney for incubation.

Three eggs from each of the twelve clutches were used for this experiment. One of the three eggs from each clutch was assigned to one of three containers, one for thyroid hormone treatment and two for controls (vehicle and H2O). Each container held a total of 12 eggs.

Eggs were half buried in a 1:1 mixture of vermiculite and deionised water and incubated in clear plastic incubator containers (200 x 100 x 50 mm). To account for evaporation and maintain water potential of vermiculite, containers were weighed and rehydrated weekly.

Containers were incubated in environmental chambers at 30 °C and were rotated weekly within chambers to counteract potential thermal gradients in the incubator (Janzen 1993).

Hormone application

Eggs in the treatment group were treated with the thyroid hormone 3,3’5-triiodo-L-thyronine

(T3; Sigma-Aldrich, NSW, Australia) dissolved in the vehicle dimethyl sulfoxide (DMSO,

Sigma-Aldrich Co. Australia). Eggs in the vehicle control group were treated with DMSO

92

only, while eggs in the H2O control group were treated with deionised water (H2O). In the treatment group, 2.0 μg T3 was dissolved in 5.0 μL of DMSO- 0.4 μg/μL (O'Steen and Janzen

1999). DMSO was used due to its ability to transport and penetrate through membranes (Kolb et al. 1967).

The dosage determined was based on the dose of the same hormone applied to Chelydra serpentina embryos, observed to have increased resting metabolic rates (RMR) of hatchlings and reduced incubation periods (O'Steen and Janzen 1999). The dose given to C. serpentina at embryonic stages 18-19 was calculated for E. macquarii embryo size (crown to rump length) at approximately the same stage of development (Dormer 2012; Greenbaum and Carr

2001; Yntema 1968). Emydura macquarii reached stages 18-19 at approximately day 20 of development at an incubation temperature of 30 °C (Dormer 2012). At this stage of embryonic development the thyroid gland is differentiated and functional (Dimond 1954;

Ewert 1985; O'Steen and Janzen 1999), so a single dose of 5 μL of T3/DMSO and controls

(DMSO and H2O) was applied to the top surface of each egg in the corresponding container using a pipette.

Metabolic measurements

Oxygen consumption ( O2) was measured in weeks 4, 5 and 6, on all 36 eggs to determine whether added T3 increased metabolic rate in embryonic turtles. O2 was measured using

Oxy-4 mini: 4-Channel Fiber optic oxygen transmitter (PreSens, Precision Sensing,

Regensburg, Germany). Eggs were placed in a glass container (50 x 50 x 50 mm) in 100 g of moist vermiculite (1:1), sealed and placed in an incubator for a period between 4 and 24 h, depending on the stage of development based on incubation week. An initial reading was taken immediately after the container was sealed and a final reading was taken at the end of

93

the time period. The differences in individuals’ initial and final readings (mL/h) were compared between treatment and control groups to see if T3 exposure affected O2. One empty container containing only moist vermiculite was used as a control. Hourly O2 of hatchlings were also measured after hatching to determine if hormone application had any lasting effects on resting metabolic rates (RMR) of neonates. Hatchlings between 35 and 40 d old were placed in the same glass containers, previously used for measuring embryonic O2, on a moist paper towel for a period up to 3 h. Initial and final readings were taken and the difference was used to determine total O2 for each individual. O2 rates were also used to calculate individual total O2 during incubation.

Heart rate measurements

We measured heart rates (bpm) of all 36 eggs weekly to infer metabolic rate of embryos (Du et al. 2009; Wallace and Jones 2008). Eggs were individually removed from the incubation container and immediately placed in a Buddy digital egg monitor system (Avian Biotech,

England) in complete darkness. Three readings were taken once heart rates stabilised (within

2 min; McGlashan et al. 2015; Sartori et al. 2015). The mean of three readings, taken at 30 s intervals, was calculated. Eggs were not disturbed during the recording period and any embryonic movement could be detected and distinguished from heart rate readings, in which case we waited for movement to cease and heart rate to stabilise before recording again

(McGlashan et al. 2015).

Calculation of total oxygen consumption

Total daily O2 was calculated for each embryo during incubation using each individuals weekly O2 per hour and multiplying it by 24. O2 increases in a sigmoid pattern throughout development in many turtles, (Booth 2000; Booth and Astill 2001; Du et al. 2010;

94

McGlashan et al. 2012; Thompson 1989), but embryonic O2 was only collected in the final weeks of incubation (weeks 4 to 6). We plotted O2 against Julian day for each measurement

(Fig. 1), and the linear equation for each week to week line was calculated. Each linear equation was then integrated to estimate the total O2 over each weekly interval. The values for each weekly interval were summed to estimate total oxygen consumed during incubation

(Van Dyke and Beaupre 2011). We used the same procedure to estimate O2 of hatchlings from the last measurement of egg metabolism prior to hatching to the RMR measurement 35-

40 days after hatching.

Week 7- Week 8 (47) Week 7- 40 d Post –Hatching (30) Week 6- Week 7 DMSO (48) H2O Oviposition - Week 6 T3 (24)

Pipping (d: 349-351)

Fig. 1: Mean weekly O2 of Emydura macquarii are plotted against the Julian day of each measurement to demonstrate how total O2 was calculated over incubation period and post hatching resting metabolic rate (RMR). Solid lines represent changes in measured oxygen consumption from measurement to measurement. The numbers within parentheses are sample sizes for each time point. Black line represents control DMSO, dark grey line represents control H2O and light grey line represents treatment T3 ± SE. The broken black line represents the day pipping commenced for treatment (T3) neonates and the thick black unbroken line represents the day pipping commenced for control group neonates. No significant difference in embryo total O2 (F2,17 = 2.21 p = 0.1406), no significant difference in total RMR O2 (F2,6 = 2.19 p = 0.1310).

95

Pipping, post hatching measurements and hatchling care

Eggs were inspected twice daily for signs of pipping (initial break of egg with caruncle)

(Gutzke et al. 1984). The incubation period was measured as time in days from oviposition until pipping. Neonates were allowed to hatch unassisted (within 1-4 d), after which they were weighed (g), and straight carapace length (CL) and straight plastron length (PL) were measured. Performance tests (righting ability) were undertaken within 12 h of hatching.

Residual yolk sac volumes (Packard and Packard 1988) were estimated as volumes of ellipsoids using length, width and height measurements. Performance and coordination of hatchlings were assessed using a righting ability test to determine if the T3 treatment affected neuromuscular development which could affect post hatching survival. Essentially, this is to test whether experimental manipulation of T3 had negative consequences for hatchlings that could not be observed by studying incubation period, metabolism, or hatchling morphology alone. Hatchlings were placed on their carapace and the times taken to right themselves were measured as an index of neuromuscular development (Colbert et al. 2010; Peterson and

Kruegl 2005; Vleck et al. 1979; Vleck et al. 1980). Righting times were recorded once, to the nearest second using a digital stopwatch and all tests were stopped at 180 s if a turtle did not right itself.

Hatchlings were initially marked with an identifiable number using enamel paint and within 2 weeks their marginal scutes were individually notched using dissecting scissors (Spencer et al. 2001). Hatchlings were housed together in a 70 L aquarium that was filtered using an external filter (Eheim 440 power filters, Germany). Turtles were fed 3-4 times a week on a diet of white bait (Richmond Seafoods, Richmond Market place), commercial frozen turtle foods (Fish Fuel Co.) and bloodworms (Orca). UVB lighting (Exo Terra: Repti Glow 2.0

40W, Rolf C. Hagen Inc.) was supplied on a 7 am to 7 pm timer and a foam floating dock for

96

basking was available at all times. All hatchlings were treated the same for 2 months before being dissected to determine if there were any effects of excess T3 on internal morphology.

Histology

Out of the 36 eggs, 34 hatched successfully. Neonates were euthanased via an overdose of inhaled isoflurane followed by pithing for measurements of the thyroid gland, including thyroid gland width (TW) and the ratio of epithelial height and follicular diameter (EFR;

Wilhoft 1958). We also identified the sexes of hatchlings and examined gross morphology to determine whether T3 exposure affected development of internal organs. Thyroid gland and gonads were removed and fixed in 10% neutral buffered formalin for 1-2 weeks in 4 °C.

Tissues were processed through graded alcohol steps and xylene, prior to being embedded in paraffin wax (Wilhoft 1958). Samples were cut into 10 μm sections, mounted on silane- coated slides (ProSciTech, Queensland) and stained with haematoxylin and eosin (Bancroft

1975) and cover slipped with DPX. The slides were viewed and photographed using an

Olympus BX60 compound microscope, Jenoptik ProgRes C14 digital camera and Image

Pro+ 6.2 software. The slides were examined to determine the sex of each hatchling and the thyroid gland and follicular development (De Solla et al. 2006; Ross et al. 2009; Wilhoft

1958; Yntema 1981).

Data analysis

Incubation period was analysed using a mixed linear model analysis (PROC MIXED; SAS, version 9.3, SAS Institute, Cary, NC) with egg mass included as the covariate. Embryonic

and heart rate were compared among weeks and among treatment and control groups using a repeated measures analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) in SAS (PROC MIXED). Egg mass (g) was included in the model as a covariate. A non-repeated ANCOVA was used to

97

compare hatchling RMR 35-40 days after hatching, using hatchling mass as a covariate.

Estimated total O2 of embryos and hatchlings were also compared using ANCOVA models in PROC MIXED. Egg mass was included as a covariate for the comparison among embryos, while hatchling mass was used as a covariate for the comparison among hatchlings.

Phenotypic differences in carapace length, plastron length, mass and yolk sac volume were compared among treatment and controls using a multivariate analysis of variance

(MANCOVA) with egg mass as a covariate in SAS (PROC GLM). Hatchling righting ability was individually analysed using a mixed linear model analysis (PROC MIXED) with egg mass as a covariate. A binomial test was used to assess propensity to right in SAS (PROC

GLIMMIX). Differences in thyroid width and epithelial follicular ration were analysed using

ANCOVA with hatchling mass (g) as covariate.

Clutch was included as a random effect in all models, and if treatment effects were found to be significant, then post-hoc Tukey Tests were used to determine where pairwise differences occurred. All data analysed with ANCOVA were log-transformed before analysis to ensure linearity in the models. Examination of residuals ensured that all assumptions of univariate parametric statistics were met. In the multivariate analysis, the assumption of multivariate normality could not be directly tested due to sample size limitations. Thus, Pillai’s Trace was used as a test statistic because it is robust to violations of multivariate normality (Scheiner

2001). Statistical significance was determined at the 0.05 Type I error level, and data are presented as means ± SE.

98

Results Incubation period

Only two of the 36 eggs failed to hatch. One egg pipped but failed to hatch and the other failed to pip, both were from the T3 treatment group. Triiodothyronine reduced the incubation period of embryos by 2-3 days (F2,17 = 4.25 p = 0.033; Fig. 2). Eggs that were treated with T3 hatched on average 2.5 d earlier than DMSO treated eggs (t17 = 3.18 p = 0.014) and 3 d earlier than eggs treated with H2O alone (t17 = 3.10 p = 0.014).

49 a a

48

47

b 46

45

44 Incubation Period (days) Period Incubation

43

42 DMSO H2OH O T3T 2 3

Fig. 2: LSMeans of incubation period of eggs in treatment and control groups (F2,17 = 4.27 p = 0.031). Letters indicate significant pairwise differences among treatments within each measurement (T3-DMSO, t17 = 3.15 p = 0.015, T3-H2O, t17 = 3.07 p = 0.018) ± SE. T3; n =

10, DMSO and H2O; n = 12.

Oxygen consumption

There was a time effect on O2 over the course of incubation (F3,92 = 36.07 p < 0.0001; Fig.

3). Within the T3 treatment, significant pairwise differences occurred between week 6 of 99

incubation and hatchling RMR (T3; t92 = 4.2, p < 0.0001). Significant pairwise differences also occurred at the same time points within both control groups (DMSO; t92 = 5.93, p <

0.0001; H2O; t92 = 5.27, p < 0.0001, p = <0.0001). There was no significant difference in O2 between treatment and control groups at any time point (F2,92 = 0.66, p = 0.5183), or the interaction of treatment and time throughout the incubation period (F6,92 = 0.42, p = 0.8630), but O2 increased with increasing egg mass (F1,92 = 9.27 p = 0.003). Analysis of post hatching RMR also revealed no significant differences among treatment and controls (F2,5 =

0.45 p = 0.6636; Fig. 3).

3.5 a

3

a 2.5

2 b

b 1.5

1 Oxygen Consumption Consumption Oxygen (mL/h)

0.5

0 Week 4 Week 5 Week 6 Hatchling RMR

Fig. 3: LSMeans of O2 of embryos in control groups: DMSO (dark grey) and H2O (medium grey) and treatment group: T3 (light grey), ± SE, N = 36 across 117 technical replicates. No significant difference among treatment groups (F2,92 = 0.66 p = 0.5183), O2 did change with time (F3,92 = 36.07 p < 0.0001), letters indicate significant pairwise differences between O2 of embryos in week 6 of incubation and hatchling RMR within treatment groups (DMSO; t92

= 5.93, p < 0.0001; H2O; t92 = 5.27, p < 0.0001; T3; t92 = 4.2, p < 0.0001).

100

Estimated total oxygen consumption

Embryo total VO2 (IncEE) did not differ among treatment and control groups (F2,17 = 2.21 p =

0.1406; Fig. 1; Table 1) and there was no interaction of egg mass and treatment (F2,17 = 2.40 p = 0.1207). Egg mass did not significantly affect total VO2 of embryos (F1,17 = 0.39 p =

0.5412). Estimated total RMR VO2 from hatchlings did not differ significantly between treatment and control groups (F2,6 = 2.91 p = 0.1310) and there was no interaction effect of egg mass and treatment (F2,6 = 3.12 p = 0.1176). Egg mass, treatment or the interaction of both did not significantly affect RMREE (Table 1).

Table 1: LSMeans of estimated total oxygen consumption (mL/h) throughout incubation (IncEE) N = 33, total oxygen consumption from hatching to RMR measurement (RMREE) N = 33, and total oxygen consumption (TotalEE) (IncEE + RMREE) N = 22, ± SE.

Treatment IncEE RMREE TotalEE 1068.2 ± 103.35 1674.84 ± 166.55 2333.07 ± 278.98 DMSO (n = 11) 860.20 ± 103.33 1427.53 ± 208.64 1541.99 ± 283.65 T3 (n = 10) 1034.61 ± 113.49 1659.47 ± 193.98 2222.23 ± 310.45 H2O (n = 12)

Heart rate

Heart rates did not differ among treatment and control groups at any point during incubation

(F2,155 = 0.16 p = 0.8512; Fig. 4), and were not affected by the interactions of treatment and egg mass or of treatment, egg mass and time. Heart rate was significantly affected by egg mass (F1,155 = 6.58, p = 0.0113) and time (F5,155 = 3.10, p = 0.0107) individually and as an interaction effect (F5,155 = 2.52, p = 0.0317).

101

110

100

90

80

Heart Rate(bpm) Heart 70

60

50 2 3 4 5 6 7 Incubation Week

Fig. 4: LSMeans of heart rates (bpm) of embryos in control groups: DMSO (dark grey) and

H2O (medium grey) and treatment group: T3 (light grey), ± SE, N = 36 across 202 technical replicates.

Heart rates were affected by week of incubation (F5,155 = 3.1 p = 0.0107; Fig. 4). Heart rate gradually increased in both control groups from week 2 to week 4 of incubation and weeks 2-

5 for the treatment group but there was no interaction effect of treatment and time (F10,155 =

0.78 p = 0.6434). Heart rate began to decline in week 5 in control groups and week 6 in the treatment group, prior to hatching.

Post hatching measurements

A multivariate analysis of all measurable hatchling phenotypes (carapace length, plastron length, mass and yolk sac volume) revealed that size was not affected by the interaction of

102

treatment and egg mass (Pillai = 0.1819, F8,28 = 0.35 p = 0.9377), treatment alone (Pillai =

0.1604, F8,28 = 0.31 p = 0.9577), or egg mass alone (Pillai = 0.2876, F4,13 = 1.31 p = 0.3166).

Righting ability was not affected by treatment with T3. There was no significant difference in ability to successfully right (F2,22 = 0.01 p = 0.999) and total time for neonates to right themselves at the time of hatching (F2,17 = 0.12 p = 0.888; Fig. 5). Twenty-four out of the 34 hatchings righted themselves within the time frame.

180

160

140

120

100

80

Righting time (s) time Righting 60

40

20

0 DMSO H2O T3

Fig. 5: LSMeans of righting time (s) in treatment and control groups ± SE. T3; n = 8, DMSO; n = 6 and H2O; n = 10.

Histological measurements

The thyroid width and the epithelial follicular ratio were only measured in 32 hatchlings as two samples were destroyed during the embedding process. There was no significant

103

difference in either measurement (F2,15 = 1.81 p = 0.197 and F2,15 = 0.60 p = 0.56; Table 2).

Gonads of hatchings were also assessed to determine sex ratio. There was no significant difference in the ratio of males to females in all groups (F2,19 = 0.53 p = 0.6).

Table 2: Mean measurements (± SE) of the thyroid gland (width) and epithelial follicular ratio of T3, DMSO and H2O treated embryos of Emydura macquarii.

Treatment Thyroid Width (µ) Epithelial Follicular Ratio (%) DMSO (n = 11) 983 ± 48 10.6 ± 1.1

T3 (n = 10) 877 ± 66 10 ± 0.8

H2O (n = 12) 963 ± 31 10 ± 1.1

The histological appearance of thyroid glands from hatchlings exposed to T3 did not differ from control groups (Fig. 6). The thyroid gland is made up of tightly packed follicles, enclosed with simple cuboidal epithelium. Follicles typically contain colloid that is homogenous in appearance (Fig. 6a). Epithelial cells of follicles are sometimes squamous indicating little absorption and continued colloid production; this usually results in larger follicles (Fig. 6b). C-cells were present in the thyroid glands of hatchling E. macquarii, lying between follicles (Fig. 6b and d). Vacuolation in the peripheral colloid was generally minimal, but evident in few samples (Fig. 6b) and generally indicative of increased rate of colloid production. A single layer of cuboidal follicular epithelial cells was present in glands of treatment and control hatchlings (Fig. 6c and d) and capillary networks were abundant throughout glands (Fig. 6b, c and d).

104

Fig. 6: Neonatal thyroid gland from 2 month old Emydura macquarii hatchlings. a: Normal neonatal thyroid gland (x10) from control (DMSO) hatchling, 1: follicles, 2: simple cuboidal epithelium, 3: colloid. b: Thyroid gland (x20) from control (DMSO) 2 hatchling, 1: squamous epithelial cells, 2: C-cell, 3: vacuoles, 4: capillary network. c: Thyroid gland (x20) from control (H2O) hatchling, 1: simple cuboidal epithelial cells, 2: capillary network. d:

Thyroid gland (x20) from treatment (T3) hatchling, 1: simple cuboidal epithelial cells, 2: C- cell, 3: capillary network.

Discussion Metabolic compensation in E. macquarii has been defined as an increase in developmental rate, with a metabolic cost, to allow cool-incubated embryos to developmentally “catch up” with warm-incubated embryos (McGlashan et al. 2012). Embryos may be able to accelerate

105

development late in incubation by producing excess T3, however, our results suggest that excess T3 accelerates development without incurring developmental or metabolic compensatory costs. Indeed, the addition of T3 reduces incubation period in embryonic E. macquarii by up to 3 days, but does not compromise embryonic development in any way revealed by our methods. Throughout incubation, there were no significant differences in heart rate or O2 between treatment and control embryos at any time point. Furthermore, total oxygen consumption by eggs over the course of development was not affected by the application of exogenous T3. Thus, T3 causes a small reduction in incubation period, but that this reduction does not incur a metabolic cost over the entirety of incubation. In addition, total and daily oxygen consumption are unchanged but incubation period is slightly reduced, indicating that embryos exposed to exogenous T3 might experience slightly higher per-day metabolic rates at any given point during development. Otherwise, we would expect a reduction in total oxygen consumption concomitant with a reduction in incubation period.

However, the potential differences in per-day oxygen expenditure were not sufficiently large to be detectable in our study, possibly due to either the relatively small sample size of our study or to random variation in the relative timing of metabolic measurements at each time point. Ultimately, embryos that hatched earlier used similar amounts of energy during development as did those that hatched later, such that all hatchlings should have similar post- hatch energy reserves.

Exposure to excess T3 did not affect hatchling size, residual yolk internalisation, or post- hatching righting ability. Thyroid growth and development were also not affected by the application of T3, suggesting that an increased production or synthesis of thyroid hormones does not affect thyroidal development, nor lead to long term effects on the growth and development of the turtle. Taken together, our results show that elevated concentrations of T3

106

can reduce incubation period in E. macquarii, but do not compromise embryonic development in any obvious way. Remarkably, these results suggest that cooler-incubated embryos might be able to use increased T3 to accelerate development and catch up with warmer-incubated siblings without significant metabolic or developmental costs. There could still be a metabolic cost in producing extra T3 during embryonic development; as any compensatory change during development could potentially come at a cost (Metcalfe and

Monaghan 2001), but it is likely to be small and it is not addressed in this study.

At developmental stages 18-19, when we applied T3, thyroid follicles have begun to appear and colloid is present within, thus the thyroid should be well-enough developed to synthesise hormones (Dimond 1954; Lynn 1970). Increased production of T3 in mid-late development might allow cooler incubated embryos to reduce incubation period to that of the warmer incubated embryos in the nest to ensure synchronous hatching of neonates. Preliminary evidence indicates that T3 concentrations in yolk increase in metabolically compensating

E.macquarii embryos during the final stages of incubation just prior to hatching (McGlashan

2016). Increases in plasma thyroid hormone concentrations have also been correlated with hatching and post hatching in crocodiles and turtles (Dimond 1954; Shepherdley et al. 2002).

Thus, it is possible that elevated production of T3 in late development could be the mechanism underlying synchronised hatching in turtles, but our results suggest that excess T3 might also accelerate embryogenesis much earlier in development, if it were produced at that time. Thus, increased T3 concentrations at any stage of development appear to increase developmental rates.

In addition to potentially regulating metabolic compensation, thyroid hormones are important regulators of embryogenesis more generally (Morvan-Dubois et al. 2013). Triiodothyronine,

107

T4, and their receptors are present in the eggs and embryos of vertebrates (Power et al. 2001;

Prati et al. 1992; Shepherdley et al. 2002). Evidence for thyroid hormone function in embryogenesis comes from endocrine disruption studies. Thyroid hormones regulate growth and pigmentation in embryonic zebrafish, Danio rerio (Walpita et al. 2009) and brain development in the African clawed frog, Xenopos laevis (Fini et al. 2012). Embryonic chickens exposed to PCBs exhibited reduced circulating thyroid hormone concentrations, which was associated with delayed hatching (Roelens et al. 2005).

Synchronous hatching has evolved in species where group emergence is important (Doody et al. 2001; Gyuris 1993; Lang 1987; Spencer et al. 2001; Thompson 1988). If excess T3 does cause embryos to accelerate development, cues within the nest such are heart rate, carbon dioxide concentrations, or auditory cues from the embryos could stimulate its production

(Ferrara et al. 2014; Ferrara et al. 2013; Spencer and Janzen 2011; Vergne et al. 2009). Our observation that development is accelerated without metabolic or morphological consequences is important. Completion of development is essential as any reduction in developmental time can have adverse effects on the growth, performance and survival of the hatchling (Janzen 1993; Vince and Chinn 1971; Warkentin 1995). Predation risk may be higher in individuals that have reduced performance and agility, particularly in turtles during terrestrial movement. Hatchlings that are slow or unable to right themselves may have an increased exposure risk to predators (Burger 1976) so selection favours hatching without any cost to performance. Hatchling size can also influence survival cost (Doody 2011; Janzen

1993; Warkentin 1995), so may be favoured by natural selection. The apparent lack of energetic or fitness cost to T3-stimulated accelerated development, combined with the potential benefits of synchronous hatching, suggests that if T3 is the mechanism underlying synchronous hatching, then it should be strongly favoured by natural selection.

108

Acknowledgements Research was supported by a UWS SSH Equipment Fund Grant and was conducted under

UWS ACEC no. A9908, NSW NPWS Permit SL101065. Fiona Loudon, Jessica Dormer,

Heidi Stricker helped capture turtles and collect eggs. We thank the Webb family for their time, help, and use of their property. Mark Emanuel, Megan Callander, Gabriele Bobek and

Liz Kabanoff for their knowledge and guidance. Research was conducted in the Evolution and Developmental Laboratory and K1 Aquatic Facility at the University of Western Sydney.

References Bancroft, J.D. 1975. Histochemical Techniques. Butterworths, London.

Bernal, J., and J. Nunez. 1995. Thyroid hormones and brain development. European Journal of Endocrinology 133: 390-398.

Birchard, G., and C. Reiber. 1996. Heart rate during development in the turtle embryo: effect of temperature. Journal of Comparative Physiology B: Biochemical, Systemic, and Environmental Physiology 166: 461-466.

Booth, D.T. 2000. Incubation of eggs of the Australian broad-shelled turtle, Chelodina expansa (Testudinata : Chelidae), at different temperatures: effects on pattern of oxygen consumption and hatchling morphology. Australian Journal of Zoology 48: 369-378.

Booth, D.T., and K. Astill. 2001. Incubation temperature, energy expenditure and hatchling size in the green turtle (Chelonia mydas), a species with temperature-sensitive sex determination. Australian Journal of Zoology 49: 389-396.

Booth, D.T., E. Burgess, J. McCosker and J.M. Lanyon. 2004. The influence of incubation temperature on post-hatching fitness characteristics of turtles. International Congress Series 1275: 226-233.

Booth, D.T., M.B. Thompson and S. Herring. 2000. How incubation temperature influences the physiology and growth of embryonic lizards. Journal of Comparative Physiology B 170: 269-276.

Braverman, L.E., S.H. Ingbar and K. Sterling. 1970. Conversion of Thyroxine (T4) to triiodothyronine (T3) in athyreotic human subjects. Journal of Clinical Investigation 49: 855-864.

109

Brown, D.D. 1997. The role of thyroid hormone in zebrafish and axolotl development. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 94: 13011-13016.

Bull, J.J. 1985. Sex determining mechanisms: an evolutionary perspective. Experientia 41: 1285-1296.

Bull, J.J., and R.C. Vogt. 1979. Temperature-dependent sex determination in turtles. Science 206: 1186-1188.

Burger, J. 1976. Behavior of Hatchling Diamondback Terrapins (Malaclemys terrapin) in the Field. Copeia 1976: 742-748.

Callery, E.M., and R.P. Elinson. 2000. Thyroid hormone-dependent metamorphosis in a direct developing frog. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 97: 2615- 2620.

Cannon, M.E., R.E. Carpenter and R.A. Ackerman. 1986. Synchronous Hatching and Oxygen Consumption of Darwin's Rhea Eggs (Pterocnemia pennata). Physiological Zoology 59: 95-108.

Chandola-Saklani, A., and A. Kar. 1990. Evidence for the role of thyroxine as a hormone in the physiology of a lizard. General and Comparative Endocrinology 78: 173-179.

Chessman, B. 1986. Diet of the Murray Turtle, Emydura-Macquarii (Gray) (Testudines, Chelidae). Wildlife Research 13: 65-69.

Colbert, P.L., R.-J. Spencer and F.J. Janzen. 2010. Mechanism and cost of synchronous hatching. Functional Ecology 24: 112-121.

Dauncey, M.J. 1990. Thyroid hormones and thermogenesis. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 49: 203-215.

De Solla, S.R., P.A. Martin, K.J. Fernie, B.J. Park and G. Mayne. 2006. Effects of environmentally relevant concentrations of atrazine on gonadal development of snapping turtles (Chelydra serpentina). Environmental Toxicology and Chemistry 25: 520-526.

Dial, B.E. 1987. Energetics and Performance during Nest Emergence and the Hatchling Frenzy in Loggerhead Sea Turtles (Caretta caretta). Herpetologica 43: 307-315.

Dimond, S.M.T. 1954. The reactions of developing Snapping Turtles, Chelydra serpentina serpentina (Linné), to thiourea. Journal of Experimental Zoology 127: 93-115.

DiStefano, J.J., M.T. K. and J. M. 1982a. Comprehensive Kinetics of Thyroxine Distribution and Metabolism in Blood and Tissue Pools of the Rat from only Six Blood Samples: Dominance of Large, Slowly Exchanging Tissue Pools. Endocrinology 111: 108-117.

110

DiStefano, J.J., J. M., M.T. K. and B. M. 1982b. Comprehensive Kinetics of Triiodothyronine Production, Distribution, and Metabolism in Blood and Tissue Pools of the Rat Using Optimized Blood-Sampling Protocols. Endocrinology 110: 198-213.

Doody, J.S. 2011. Environmentally Cued Hatching in Reptiles. Integrative and Comparative Biology 51: 49-61.

Doody, J.S., A. Georges, J.E. Young, M.D. Pauza, A.L. Pepper, R.L. Alderman and M.A. Welsh. 2001. Embryonic aestivation and emergence behaviour in the pig-nosed turtle, Carettochelys insculpta. Canadian Journal of Zoology 79: 1062-1072.

Doody, J.S., B. Stewart, C. Camacho and K. Christian. 2012. Good vibrations? Sibling embryos expedite hatching in a turtle. Animal Behaviour 83: 645-651.

Dormer, J. 2012. Significance of incubation temperature on the life history of an Australian freshwater turtle. p. 132. School of Science and Health.

Dormer, J., J.M. Old, J.U. Van Dyke and R.-J. Spencer. In Press. Incubation temperature affects development order of morphological features and staging criteria in turtle embryos. Journal of Zoology DOI: 10.1111/jzo.12346.

Du, W.-G., R.S. Radder, B. Sun and R. Shine. 2009. Determinants of incubation period: do reptilian embryos hatch after a fixed total number of heart beats? Journal of Experimental Biology 212: 1302-1306.

Du, W.-G., B. Zhao, Y. Chen and R. Shine. 2011. Behavioral thermoregulation by turtle embryos. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108: 9513-9515.

Du, W.-G., B. Zhao and R. Shine. 2010. Embryos in the fast lane: high-temperature heart rates of turtles decline after hatching. PLoS ONE 5: e9557.

Escobar, G.M.d., M.a.J. Obregón and F.E.d. Rey. 2004. Maternal thyroid hormones early in pregnancy and fetal brain development. Best Practice & Research Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism 18: 225-248.

Ewert, M.A. 1985. Embryology of turtles. pp. 75–267. Biology of the Reptilia. Development A. Wiley, New York.

Ferrara, C.R., R.C. Vogt, M.R. Harfush, R.S. Sousa-Lima, E. Albavera and A. Tavera. 2014. First evidence of leatherback turtle (Dermochelys coriacea) embryos and hatchlings emitting sounds. Chelonian Conservation and Biology 13: 110-114.

Ferrara, C.R., R.C. Vogt and R.S. Sousa-Lima. 2013. Turtle vocalizations as the first evidence of posthatching parental care in chelonians. Journal of Comparative Psychology 127: 24-32.

111

Fini, J.B., S.L. Mevel, L. Palmier, V.M. Darras, I. Punzon, S.J. Richardson, M.S. Clerget- Froidevaux and B.A. Demeneix. 2012. Thyroid hormone signaling in the Xenopus laevis embryo is functional and susceptible to endocrine disruption. Endocrinology 153: 5068-5081.

Ford, D.H., K.R. Corey and J. Gross. 1957. The localization of thyroid hormones in the organs and tissues of the guinea pig: An autoradiographic and chromatographic study. Endocrinology 61: 426-447.

Forrest, D., M. Sjöberg and B. Vennström. 1990. Contrasting developmental and tissue- specific expression of alpha and beta thyroid hormone receptor genes. The EMBO Journal 9: 1519-1528.

Greenbaum, E., and J.L. Carr. 2001. Sexual differentiation in the spiny softshell turtle (Apalone spinifera), a species with genetic sex determination. Journal of Experimental Zoology 290: 190-200.

Gutzke, W.H., G.L. Paukstis and G.C. Packard. 1984. Pipping versus hatching as indices of time of incubation in reptiles. Journal of herpetology: 494-496.

Gyuris, E. 1993. Factors that control the emergence of green turtle hatchlings from the nest. Wildlife Research 20: 345-353.

Himms-Hagen, J. 1976. Cellular Thermogenesis. Annual Review of Physiology 38: 315-351.

Hollenberg, A.N., and D. Forrest. 2008. The Thyroid and Metabolism: The Action Continues. Cell Metabolism 8: 10-12.

Janzen, F.J. 1993. An experimental analysis of natural selection on body size of hatchling turtles. Ecology 74: 332-341.

Johnston, I.A., V.L.A. Vieira and J. Hill. 1996. Temperature and ontogeny in ectotherms: muscle phenotype in fish. in C.U. Press, ed. Animals and Temperature: Phenotypic and Evolutionary Adaptation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

Kolb, K.H., G. Jaenicke, M. Kramer and P.E. Schulze. 1967. Absorption, distribution and elimination of labeled dimethyl sulfoxide in man and animals. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 141: 85-95.

Lang, J.W. 1987. Crocodilian behaviour: implications for management. pp. 274-294 in C.J.W. Webb, S.C. Manolis and P.J. Whitehead, eds. Wildlife Management: Crocodiles and Alligators. Surrey Beatty and Sons, Sydney.

Lillywhite, H.B., K.C. Zippel and A.P. Farrell. 1999. Resting and maximal heart rates in ectothermic vertebrates. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology - Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology 124: 369-382.

112

Lin, L.-H., H. Li, H. An and X. Ji. 2008. Do temperature fluctuations during incubation always play an important role in shaping the phenotype of hatchling reptiles? Journal of Thermal Biology 33: 193-199.

Lynn, W.G. 1970. The thyroid. pp. 201-234. Biology of reptilia. Academic Press, New York.

McComb, D.M., J. Gelsleichter, C.A. Manire, R. Brinn and C.L. Brown. 2005. Comparative thyroid hormone concentration in maternal serum and yolk of the bonnethead shark (Sphyrna tiburo) from two sites along the coast of Florida. General and Comparative Endocrinology 144: 167-173.

McGlashan, J.K. 2016. Synchronous hatching in freshwater turtles: Metabolic and endocrine physiological mechanisms. School of Science and Health. Western Sydney University.

McGlashan, J.K., F.K. Loudon, M.B. Thompson and R.-J. Spencer. 2015. Hatching behaviour of eastern long-necked turtles (Chelodina longicollis): The influence of asynchronous environments on embryonic heart rate and phenotype. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Physiology 188: 58-64.

McGlashan, J.K., R.-J. Spencer and J.M. Old. 2012. Embryonic communication in the nest: metabolic responses of reptilian embryos to developmental rates of siblings. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 279: 1709-1715.

McNabb, A.F.M., and C.M. Wilson. 1997. Thyroid hormone deposition in avian eggs and effects on embryonic development. American Zoologist 37: 553-560.

Metcalfe, N.B., and P. Monaghan. 2001. Compensation for a bad start: grow now, pay later? Trends in Ecology & Evolution 16: 254-260.

Miwa, S., and Y. Inui. 1987. Effects of various doses of thyroxine and triiodothyronine on the metamorphosis of flounder (Paralichthys olivaceus). General and Comparative Endocrinology 67: 356-363.

Morreale de Escobar, G., M. Obregon and F. Escobar del Rey. 2004. Role of thyroid hormone during early brain development. European Journal of Endocrinology 151: U25-U37.

Morvan-Dubois, G., J.B. Fini and B.A. Demeneix. 2013. Is thyroid hormone signaling relevant for vertebrate embryogenesis? pp. 365-396 in Y.-B. Shi, ed. Current Topics in Developmental Biology.

Nelson, N.J., M.B. Thompson, S. Pledger, S.N. Keall and C.H. Daugherty. 2004. Egg mass determines hatchling size, and incubation temperature influences post-hatching growth, of tuatara Sphenodon punctatus. Journal of Zoology 263: 77-87.

113

Norris, D.O. 1997. Vertebrate endocrinology. Academic Press, San Diego.

O'Steen, S., and Fredric J. Janzen. 1999. Embryonic temperature affects metabolic compensation and thyroid hormones in hatchling snapping turtles. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology 72: 520-533.

Packard, G., M. Packard, K. Miller and T. Boardman. 1988. Effects of temperature and moisture during incubation on carcass composition of hatchling snapping turtles (Chelydra serpentina). Journal of Comparative Physiology B 158: 117-125.

Packard, G.C., and M.J. Packard. 1988. The physiological ecology of reptilian eggs and embryos. pp. 523–605. Gans, C., Huey, R.B. (Eds.), Biology of the Reptilia. Wiley, New York.

Peterson, C.C., and A. Kruegl. 2005. Peaked temporal pattern of embryonic metabolism in an emydid turtle (Chrysemys picta picta). Journal of Herpetology 39: 678-681.

Power, D.M., L. Llewellyn, M. Faustino, M.A. Nowell, B.T. Björnsson, I.E. Einarsdottir, A.V.M. Canario and G.E. Sweeney. 2001. Thyroid hormones in growth and development of fish. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part C: Toxicology & Pharmacology 130: 447-459.

Prati, M., R. Calvo, G. Morreale and G.M.d. Escobar. 1992. L-thyroxine and 3,5,3'- triiodothyronine concentrations in the chicken egg and in the embryo before and after the onset of thyroid function. Endocrinology 130: 2651-2659.

Rivera, S., and B. Lock. 2008. The reptilian thyroid and parathyroid glands. Veterinary Clinics of North America: Exotic Animal Practice 11: 163-175.

Roelens, S.A., V. Beck, J. Maervoet, G. Aerts, G.E. Reyns, P. Schepens and V.M. Darras. 2005. The dioxin-like PCB 77 but not the ortho-substituted PCB 153 interferes with chicken embryo thyroid hormone homeostasis and delays hatching. General and Comparative Endocrinology 143: 1-9.

Rome, L.C., E.D. Stevens and H.B. John-Alder. 1992. The influence of temperature and thermal acclimation on physiological function. pp. 183–205 Environmental Physiology of the Amphibians. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

Ross, M.H., W. Pawlina and T.A. Barnash. 2009. Atlas of Descriptive Histology. Sinauer Associates, Massachusetts, USA.

Sartori, M.R., C.A.C. Leite, A.S. Abe, D.A. Crossley Ii and E.W. Taylor. 2015. The progressive onset of cholinergic and adrenergic control of heart rate during development in the green iguana, Iguana iguana. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology.

114

Scheiner, S.M. 2001. MANOVA: multiple response variables and multispecies interactions. pp. 99-115. Design and analysis of ecological experiments. Oxford University, New York, NY.

Schwartz, H.L., M.I. Surks and J.H. Oppenheimer. 1971. Quantitation of extrathyroidal conversion of l-thyroxine to 3,5,3′-triiodo-l-thyronine in the rat. Journal of Clinical Investigation 50: 1124-1130.

Shepherdley, C.A., C.B. Daniels, S. Orgeig, S.J. Richardson, B.K. Evans and V.M. Darras. 2002. Glucocorticoids, thyroid hormones, and iodothyronine deiodinases in embryonic saltwater crocodiles. American Journal of Physiology- Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology 283: 1155-1163.

Shine, R., and M.J. Elphick. 2001. The effect of short-term weather fluctuations on temperatures inside lizard nests, and on the phenotypic traits of hatchling lizards. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 72: 555-565.

Silva, J.E. 2001. The multiple contributions of thyroid hormone to heat production. Journal of Clinical Investigation 108: 35-37.

Spencer, R.-J., and F.J. Janzen. 2011. Hatching behavior in turtles. Integrative and Comparative Biology 51: 100-110.

Spencer, R.-J., M.B. Thompson and P.B. Banks. 2001. Hatch or wait? A dilemma in reptilian incubation. Oikos 93: 401-406.

Thompson, M.B. 1988. Nest temperatures in the Pleurodiran turtle, Emydura macquarii. Copeia 1988: 996-1000.

Thompson, M.B. 1989. Patterns of metabolism in embryonic reptiles. Respiration Physiology 76: 243-255.

Thompson, M.B. 1997. Egg physiology and biology. pp. 88-121. The Biology, Husbandry And Healthcare of Reptile. TFH Publications Inc., New Jersey.

Thompson, M.B., G.C. Packard, M.J. Packard and B. Rose. 1996. Analysis of the nest environment of tuatara Sphenodon punctatus. Journal of Zoology 238: 239-251.

Van Dyke, J.U., and S.J. Beaupre. 2011. Bioenergetic components of reproductive effort in viviparous snakes: Costs of vitellogenesis exceed costs of pregnancy. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology 160: 504- 515.

Vergne, A.L., M.B. Pritz and N. Mathevon. 2009. Acoustic communication in crocodilians: from behaviour to brain. Biological Reviews 84: 391-411.

115

Vince, M.A., and S. Chinn. 1971. Effect of accelerated hatching on the initiation of standing and walking in the Japanese quail. Animal Behaviour 19: 62-66.

Vladimirova, I., T. Alekseeva and M. Nechaeva. 2005. Growth and oxygen consumption in embryonic and early postembryonic development of European pond turtle Emys orbicularis (Reptilia: Emydidae). Biology Bulletin 32: 172-178.

Vleck, C.M., D.F. Hoyt and D. Vleck. 1979. Metabolism of avian embryos: patterns in altricial and precocial birds. Physiological Zoology 52: 363-377.

Vleck, D., C.M. Vleck and D.F. Hoyt. 1980. Metabolism of avian embryos: ontogeny of oxygen consumption in the rhea and emu. Physiological Zoology 53: 125-135.

Wallace, B.P., and T.T. Jones. 2008. What makes marine turtles go: A review of metabolic rates and their consequences. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 356: 8-24.

Walpita, C.N., A.D. Crawford, E.D. Janssens, S. Van der Geyten and V.M. Darras. 2009. Type 2 iodothyronine deiodinase is essential for thyroid hormone-dependent embryonic development and pigmentation in zebrafish. Endocrinology 150: 530-539.

Warkentin, K.M. 1995. Adaptive plasticity in hatching age: a response to predation risk trade-offs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 92: 3507-3510.

Webb, G.J.W., D. Choquenot and P.J. Whitehead. 1986. Nests, eggs, and embryonic development of Carettochelys insculpta (Chelonia: Carettochelidae) from Northern Australia. Journal of Zoology 1: 521-550.

Weisrock, D.W., and F.J. Janzen. 1999. Thermal and fitness-related consequences of nest location in Painted Turtles (Chrysemys picta). Functional Ecology 13: 94-101.

Wilhoft, D.C. 1958. The effect of temperature on thyroid histology and survival in the lizard, Sceloporus occidentalis. Copeia 1958: 265-276.

Yntema, C.L. 1968. A Series of stages in the embryonic development of Chelydra serpentina Journal of Morphology 125: 219-252.

Yntema, C.L. 1978. Incubation times for eggs of the turtle Chelydra serpentina (Testudines: Chelydridae) at various temperatures. Herpetologica 34: 274-277.

Yntema, C.L. 1981. Characteristics of gonads and oviducts in hatchlings and young of Chelydra serpentina resulting from three incubation temperatures. Journal of Morphology 167: 297-304.

116

CHAPTER 5

SYNCHRONOUS HATCHING: TRIIODOTHYRONINE AND

CORTICOSTERONE CONCENTRATIONS IN THE YOLK DURING

METABOLIC ADJUSTMENT IN THE MURRAY RIVER TURTLE

(EMYDURA MACQUARII)

Chapter formatted for submission to the Journal of Experimental Zoology

117

Title: Synchronous hatching: triiodothyronine and corticosterone concentrations in the yolk during metabolic adjustment in the Murray River turtle (Emydura macquarii)

Jessica K. McGlashan1*, Michael B. Thompson2 and Ricky-John Spencer1

1School of Science and Health, University of Western Sydney, Locked Bag 1797, Penrith South DC, NSW 2751, Australia. 2School of Biological Sciences (A08), University of Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia

*Author for correspondence: Jessica McGlashan

Email: [email protected]

Phone: T +61 2 4570 1195

Author Contributions

This manuscript is jointly authored, I am the primary author and I designed the experiment. I took all the heart rate and respiration measurements, maintained the eggs throughout incubation, recorded the pipping and hatching times, took hatchling measurements and performed the post- hatching righting tests and carried out data analysis. Dr Ricky-John Spencer and Professor Michael Thompson supervised the work and provided feedback on earlier versions of the manuscript.

118

Abstract Embryonic development is sensitive to external factors such as temperature and moisture.

Within a natural nest, variable temperatures can alter metabolic rate of embryos and cause asynchronous development of clutch-mates. Synchronous hatching is a phenomenon that occurs in several species of turtles despite differences in incubation conditions. Emydura macquarii hatch synchronously by metabolically compensating during the final third of incubation, so less advanced eggs can catch up to more advanced eggs. This study investigates the concentration of endogenous thyroid hormone (triiodothyronine- T3) and gluccocorticoid (corticosterone) throughout the incubation period of developing E. macquarii eggs to determine if their influence on metabolism and growth may stimulate metabolic compensation. Asynchrony was established in clutches of developing E. macquarii eggs and concentrations of T3 and corticosterone were measured in the yolk at nine time points. There were no significant differences between asynchronous and synchronous clutches in concentrations of either hormone. There was an increase in T3 concentration from 6-11 pg/g in the yolk of eggs at day 52 and 57 of incubation measured and an increase in corticosterone at day 57. The increases coincide with the stage of development when embryos are preparing for hatching, which may be stressful and energetically expensive. Corticosterone and T3 do not appear to be the hormones responsible for regulating metabolic compensation and synchronous hatching. Both corticosterone and T3 increased at the end of incubation which suggests they are necessary for hatching and preparing the embryo for life outside of the egg, due to their strong physiological effects during stressful environmental conditions.

Key Words: Triiodothyronine, T3, glucocorticoid, corticosterone, synchronous hatching, environmentally cued hatching, phenotypic plasticity, embryonic development, egg

119

Introduction Embryonic development is sensitive to external factors such as temperature and moisture

(Packard et al., 1982; Packard et al., 1983; Packard et al., 1988; Deeming and Ferguson,

1991; Booth, 1998; Monaghan, 2008). These factors alter metabolic rate of an embryo throughout incubation and can affect the rate of oxygen consumption, heart rate and hormonal balance, specifically, thyroid hormones and corticosterone (Decuypere et al., 1979;

Tullett, 1990; Tona et al., 2003). Conversely, hormonal disruptions can alter metabolism

(Wikelski et al., 1999; Miles et al., 2007; DuRant et al., 2008; Meylan et al., 2010). Thyroid hormones and glucocorticoids (corticosterone) are necessary for embryonic development and maintaining homeostasis when under stress (Karnofsky et al., 1951; Scott et al., 1981;

Gorbman et al., 1983; McNabb, 1992 ; McNabb and King, 1993b). Hormones can cross the placenta to reach the embryo in mammals (Wourms, 1981; Vulsma et al., 1989; McNabb and

Wilson, 1997; Escobar et al., 2004; Morreale de Escobar et al., 2004), or are deposited into the oocytes during vitellogenesis via lipids and binding proteins for embryonic development in oviparous animals (lecithotrophy) (review, McNabb & Wilson, 1997; McNabb & King,

1993). The amount of hormone that is deposited into the oocyte depends on the maternal hormone status, but it is still not known what proportion of hormones used to regulate embryonic development comes from the mother (yolk), or from the embryos own production

(Wilson and McNabb, 1997).

Thyroid hormones are required for the growth and development of embryonic tissues, by triggering and accelerating cartilage and bone differentiation and maturation (Burch and

Lebovitz, 1982; Wilson and McNabb, 1997), development of the brain and central nervous system (Bernal and Nunez, 1995; Morreale de Escobar et al., 2004) and cardiac function, including contraction and relaxation rates, cardiac output and heart rate (Morkin et al., 1983;

Klein, 1990; Little and Seebacher, 2014). Thyroid hormones are also strongly linked to 120

reduced incubation periods (Chapter 4), hatching success and yolk retraction in turtles

(Dimond, 1954) and increase in the plasma of embryonic crocodiles late in development, prior to hatching and immediately post hatching (Shepherdley et al., 2002). Thyroid function of the embryo is limited in ability during the first half of incubation in birds (Thommes,

1987; McNichols and McNabb, 1988), so maternal hormones in the yolk are taken up by the embryo during development for early tissue growth prior to thyroid gland differentiation and hormone production and synthesis (Escobar et al., 1985; Escobar et al., 1988; Prati et al.,

1992). Thyroid hormone receptors are present in the brain and heart early in embryonic development before the thyroid gland is functional (Perez-Castillo et al., 1985; Bradley et al.,

1992; McNabb and King, 1993b). Receptors are also present in the brain, blood and yolk sac in early stages of embryonic development in birds (Haidar et al., 1983; Forrest et al., 1990).

After the thyroid gland is developed and functional through the hypothalamic-pituitary- thyroid (HPT) axis, the thyroid gland become the major supplier of hormones to the embryo

(Thommes et al., 1977; McNabb and Wilson, 1997).

Glucocorticoids are also of maternal origin prior to development of the adrenal glands, and arise via endogenous regulation through the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis later in embryonic development (Wise and Frye, 1973; Freeman, 1974; Tona et al., 2003).

Glucocorticoids control actions and physiological responses to immediate stressors and prepare and aid adaptation to chronic stressors by stimulating a rise in hormone secretion

(Wingfield and Ramenofsky, 1999; Sapolsky et al., 2000). In reptiles, corticosterone is the primary hormone released to modulate actions in response to a stressor (Hanke and Kloas,

1995). Corticosterone is secreted and synthesised from mid-development and increases in concentration late in development, right through to hatching in the chicken and alligator

(Pedernera, 1972; Wise and Frye, 1973; Kalliecharan and Hall, 1974; Medler and Lance,

121

1998), but decreases soon after hatching. The decrease of plasma corticosterone concentrations after hatching also occurs in crocodiles (Shepherdley et al., 2002) and chickens (Thommes and Hylka, 1977). Corticosteroid is involved in many physiological and behavioural processes that help the body to adjust to stress and restore homeostasis, including regulation of energy storage and utilization (Dallman and Bhatnagar, 2010), stabilisation of oxidative stress and regulation of immune functions (McEwen et al., 1997; Lin et al., 2004;

Spencer et al., 2005). Increased corticosteroid concentrations in blood plasma improves stamina and metabolism in reptiles (Miles et al., 2007; DuRant et al., 2008; Meylan et al.,

2010) and increases oxygen consumption in birds (Wikelski et al., 1999).

Synchronous hatching is a phenomenon that occurs in several species of turtles (Webb et al.,

1986; Spencer et al., 2001; Colbert et al., 2010; McGlashan et al., 2015). Nest temperature gradients cause asynchronous development among embryos in a nest (Thompson, 1988,

1997), but they still hatch synchronously. Hatching synchrony happens through metabolic compensation, early hatching or by environmental triggers that stimulate a spontaneous hatching event (Doody et al., 2001; Colbert et al., 2010; Doody, 2011; McGlashan et al.,

2012; McGlashan et al., 2015;Chapter 2; 3). Asynchronous environments in a nest may cause a stress response from embryos that are less developed than their clutch-mates, and stimulate them to metabolically compensate and catch-up. Both thyroid and corticosterone hormones could be necessary to stimulate metabolic compensation and synchronous hatching in freshwater turtles due to their strong physiological effects and their influence on metabolism and hatching time (Chapter 4). We tested the hypothesis that thyroid hormones and corticosterone are involved in regulating metabolic compensation and synchronous hatching.

122

Method Egg collection and incubation

Emydura macquarii was chosen for this study as they can be stimulated to accelerate development through metabolic compensation by incubating eggs with more advanced eggs

(McGlashan et al., 2012). Twenty female E. macquarii were captured from the Murray River,

Albury, New South Wales from 30th October to 1st November 2012, using lobster and cathedral traps. Females were palpated to determine gravidity. They were injected subcutaneously with oxytocin (Ilium Syntocin 10 IU/ml, Troy Laboratories PTY LTD) at a dose of 1mL/kg to stimulate oviposition and placed in tubs 600 x 400 mm with 150 mm deep water in a dark quiet room for up to 24 h (Spencer et al., 2001, McGlashan et al., 2015). Eggs were collected from the tubs, weighed, individually marked in laying order using a HB pencil, and placed in moist vermiculite. Prior to release, females were palpated to ensure all eggs were oviposited; if not, then a second oxytocin injection was administered to complete oviposition. The 20 clutches were transported to the University of Western Sydney,

Hawkesbury, for incubation.

Eggs were half buried in a 1:1 mixture of vermiculite and deionised water and incubated in clear plastic containers with lids (200 x 100 x 50 mm). The initial weight of the container and vermiculite were used to account for evaporation and net water gain by the eggs. Containers were also rotated weekly within environmental chambers to counter the effects of potential thermal gradients (Janzen, 1993, Colbert et al., 2010).

Developmental asynchrony in clutches of eggs was established using protocols similar to

McGlashan et al. (2012). Twelve eggs from each clutch were used in either the treatment

(asynchronous) or control (synchronous) groups. Another egg from each clutch was frozen and stored in -20 °C, directly following oviposition, for the initial hormone analysis. Both 123

asynchronous and synchronous groups consisted of 10 clutches each. To establish asynchrony within the asynchronous clutches, four of the eggs were randomly selected from each clutch, moved to a separate container (200 x 100 x 50 mm) and placed in an incubator at 30 °C

(asynchronous- more advanced eggs) for 7 d. The remaining eight eggs (asynchronous- target eggs) from each clutch were kept in their original containers in an incubator at 26 °C for 7 d.

After 7 d the eggs at 30 °C, which were now more advanced that the asynchronous- target eggs, were reunited with the target eggs incubated at 26 °C. One asynchronous- more advanced egg was positioned between two target eggs, within 10 mm of each other, i.e. not in direct contact (Fig. 1). Containers were incubated at 26 °C for the rest of incubation. The synchronous groups were set up using the same procedure, but the four randomly selected eggs (synchronous- not advanced eggs) were moved into a separate container and placed in an incubator at 26 °C for the 7 d period. The remaining eight eggs (synchronous- target eggs) were also incubated at 26 °C so that all eggs within the clutch were at the same developmental stage and therefore still synchronised when reunited. After eggs were reunited into original containers, they continued to be incubated at 26 °C. Each clutch was incubated in separate containers throughout the experiment. Containers were opened every 2-3 days to allow fresh air exchange throughout the incubation period until the commencement of pipping/hatching. Egg handling was minimal and all eggs were treated the same throughout incubation until euthanasia, therefore any stress induced by handling would be redundant. All target eggs were sacrificed for yolk analysis during the experiment, so we were not able to determine incubation period and whether synchronous hatching would result from the asynchrony set-up. To ensure asynchrony within clutches we followed the asynchrony set-up method used by Spencer et al. (2001), Colbert et al. (2010), McGlashan et al. (2012) and

McGlashan et al. (2015). This method has previously resulted in synchronous hatching within

124

clutches whilst still causing a difference in incubation periods between asynchronous and synchronous clutches.

Fig. 1: Incubation method- Eggs from each clutch were divided and incubated separately for 7 d, then returned to the same containers to continue incubation at 26 °C. Asynchronous group- four more advanced eggs (black) were incubated at 30°C and eight target eggs (white) were incubated at 26 °C for 7 d. Synchronous group- four not advanced eggs (black) were incubated at 26 °C and eight target eggs (white) were incubated at 26 °C for 7 d. All eggs were then placed in 26 °C incubators until the completion of incubation.

Assay procedure

Yolks from 110 target eggs were used to measure concentrations of T3 and corticosterone at different stages of development throughout the incubation period. Each week following the incubation set up, one egg from each clutch was selected to monitor daily heart rate. Each egg was then snap frozen in liquid nitrogen, and stored at -20 °C until hormone analysis (Bowden

125

et al., 2001). Whole yolks were separated from the shell, albumin and embryo while frozen, and all yolks and embryos were weighed.

Yolk preparation and extraction

Yolks from each egg were thawed and homogenised. Hormone concentrations in yolk were determined as picogram per millilitre of yolk using hormone-specific assays. For T3 analysis, a 0.5 g sample of yolk was transferred to a 10 mL glass tube and combined with 0.2 mL of

PBS (phosphate buffered solution) and vortexed for 2 min. A 0.2 g sample of yolk was transferred into a 5 mL glass tube and combined with 0.3 mL of PBS for corticosterone analysis.

Triiodothyronine (T3)

The methods of Tagawa and Hirano (1987) and Ho et al. (2011) were used to measure T3.

Each yolk sample had 2 mL of methanol 99.93% (Sigma-Aldrich, MO, USA) added and the sample was vortexed for 2 min, shaken for 10 min (150 oscillations/min) and centrifuged

(5000 rpm/4 min). The supernatant was decanted into a new 10 mL glass tube. The remaining precipitate in the original tube was then resuspended in 1 mL of methanol, vortexed for 2 min, shaken for 10 min and centrifuged and the supernatant decanted into a second 10 mL glass tube. The two separate supernatants were given 5 mL of chloroform (CHCI3)

–1 (UNIVAR, APS-NSW, Australia) and 0.5 mL ammonium hydroxide (2mol l NH4OH) each and were vortexed, shaken and centrifuged as above. The upper phase, consisting of the methanol/aqueous phase in both tubes was removed and added to a new 10 mL glass tube.

The remaining yolk/CHCI3 in each tube was extracted again with 0.5 mL NH4OH as above and the upper phase from each tube was added to the methanol/aqueous pool. Each sample was then dried under an air stream of filtered nitrogen (N2) gas on a hot plate between 24-28

126

°C (for 4-5 h). Once dried, the sample was resuspended in 1 mL NH4OH, vortexed and shaken, then 1 mL of CHCI3 was added to tube, vortexed, shaken and centrifuged and the upper phase was decanted into a new 10 mL tube. The supernatant was then dried under an air stream of filtered N2 gas on a hot plate between 24-28 °C (for 2 h). To determine extraction efficiency a known amount of the hormone was added to an extracted yolk sample, vortexed and allowed to equilibrate throughout the yolk. The sample was then extracted following the same procedure. Dried samples were stored at -20 °C until analysis. Samples were resuspended in 200 μL of PBS immediately prior to analysis.

Corticosterone

Following procedures similar to Schwabl (1993) and Addison et al. (2008), corticosteroide was extracted with 2 mL of diethyl ether (AnalaR, Merck PTY. LTD., VIC, Australia), vortexed for 2 min, shaken (150 oscillations/min) for 10 min and centrifuged at 3000 rpm for

4 min. The supernatant was decanted into a new 5 mL glass tube. The precipitate was then extracted two more times, again with 2 mL of diethyl ether and the last with 1 mL of diethyl ether, each time the sample was vortexed, shaken and centrifuged and decanted into the same glass tube with the pooled ether and extract. The sample was dried under a continuous air stream of filtered N2 gas on a hot plate at 24-28 °C. The dried extract was redissolved in 1 mL of 90 % ethanol and stored overnight at -20 °C to allow for sedimentation of neutral lipids. The samples were then centrifuged and the ethanol phase was decanted from the lipid protein precipitate into a new glass tube. Samples were dried under filtered N2 (~ 1 h). To determine extraction efficiency, a known amount of the hormone was added to an extracted yolk sample, vortexed and allowed to equilibrate throughout the yolk. The sample was then extracted following the same procedure. Dried samples were stored at -20 °C until analysis.

Prior to analysis, samples were reconstituted in ~300 µL of assay buffer provided in kits.

127

Hormone concentrations were measured by T3 in-vitro competitive enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA) kits (Sapphire Bioscience, Australia- Abcam, USA) and corticosterone enzyme immunoassay (EIA) kits (Sapphire Bioscience, Australia- Enzo Life

Sciences, USA) according to the instruction manuals supplied with each kit. Yolk samples were run in duplicate and compared to a standard curve that ranged from 0-19 pg/ml for T3 and 32-20000 pg/ml for corticosterone. Replicate yolks were run between each assay kit to control for inter-assay variation. Intra-assay variation, the variation of results within the data set, calculated as the coefficient of variation of the standards, for T3 was 4.79 %, 4.41 %, 7.76

%, 5.46 % and 4.17 % with an inter-assay variation, the variation of results obtained from repeated experiments, of 15.44 %. The intra-assay variation for corticosterone was 3.65 %,

3.5 %, 2.71 %, 2.15 %, 5.06 % and 3.28 % with an inter-assay variation of 3.43 %.

Statistical analysis

We performed analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) in SAS (PROC MIXED; SAS, version 9.3,

SAS Institute, Cary, NC) on differences among asynchronous and synchronous groups in both T3 and corticosterone hormone concentrations in yolk, using egg mass and clutch as the covariate (Schwabl, 1993, Bowden et al., 2001). Embryo and yolk size were expressed as a percentage of egg mass and were normally distributed (Shapiro-Wilks test for normality).

The effects of treatment on embryonic growth and yolk utilisation were compared in SAS

(PROC GLM) (unbalanced). If treatment effects were significant, then post-hoc Tukey Tests were used to determine where pairwise differences occurred. Linear regression analysis was used to examine associations between corticosterone and T3 hormone concentrations. All data were log-transformed before analysis to ensure linearity in the models. Examination of residuals ensured that all assumptions of univariate parametric statistics were met (Scheiner,

128

2001). Statistical significance was determined at the 0.05 Type I error level, and untransformed data are presented in the figures as the mean ± SE.

Results Corticosterone

Extraction efficiency was 73 ± 2.5 % for corticosterone (n = 4), which are comparable to corticosterone hormone extraction techniques by Schwabl (1993). In E. macquarii, corticosterone concentrations in the yolk of embryos were not significantly different between asynchronous and synchronous treatment groups (F1,88 = 1.6, p = 0.2093; Fig. 2). There was a significant difference in age of embryo and concentration (F8,88 = 10.39, p = <0.0001), but not until the final week of incubation where concentration increased significantly in both asynchronous and synchronous groups, and there was no interaction effect of treatment and age (F8,88 = 0.82, p = 0.5873). Corticosterone concentrations ranged from 521 to 1261 pg/g of yolk in asynchronous groups and 418-887 pg/g in synchronous groups within the first 31 days of incubation. Corticosterone then gradually increased to concentrations of 1305-2109 pg/g and 1275-2145 pg/g respectively from day 38 to 52 before reaching a post hatching high of

7058 pg/g in asynchronous clutches and 5400 pg/g in synchronous clutches. Corticosterone concentrations of asynchronous and synchronous clutches at day 57 were significantly higher than at day 52 of incubation (asynchronous; t88 = -7.99, p < 0.0001; synchronous; t88 = -8.99, p < 0.0001).

129

b

8000 6

7000

3 6000

5000 a 4000

3000 6 7 6 8 5 7 6 Corticosterone (pg/g) Corticosterone 2000 5 4 5 7 7

6 6 6 1000 7

0 0 8 16 23 31 38 45 52 57 Day

Fig. 2: LSMeans of corticosterone concentrations (pg/g) of embryonic Emydura macquarii embryos throughout incubation in asynchronous (dark grey) and synchronous (light grey) clutches. Letters indicate significant pairwise differences among treatments within each measurement (asynchronous; t70 = -8.05, p < 0.0001; synchronous; t70 = -5.32, p < 0.0001) ± SE. Numbers above columns represents sample size, N = 107.

Triiodothyronine (T3)

Extraction efficiency was 86 ± 3.1 % (mean and SE) for T3 (n = 6). These recoveries are comparable to thyroid hormone extraction techniques by Wilson and McNabb (1997).

Triiodithyronine concentrations in the yolk of developing embryos did not differ between asynchronous and synchronous groups throughout incubation, ranging from 4-10 pg/g (F1,88 =

0.38, p = 0.5406; Fig. 3). Age of embryo did not affect concentration (F8,88 = 1.66, p =

0.1189) and there was no interaction effect of treatment and age (F8,88 = 0.6, p = 0.7760).

There was a slight increase from 6-12 pg/g in the yolk of eggs at day 52 and 57 of incubation but this increase was only significant in asynchronous groups from day 45 to 52 (t70 = -2.24,

130

p = 0.0278), and not in synchronous groups (t70 = -0.72, p = 0.4705). There was no significant correlation between corticosterone and T3 concentrations throughout the incubation period

2 (F1,67 = 0.2, p = 0.6565, R = 0.003).

b 14 6 6 3

12

10 6 6 5 7 a 7 6 5 7 8 6 7 6 7 5 5 8 6

4 Triiodothyronine (pg/g) Triiodothyronine 2

0 0 8 16 23 31 38 45 52 57 Day

Fig. 3: LSMeans of triiodothyronine concentrations (pg/g) of embryonic Emydura macquarii embryos throughout incubation in asynchronous (dark grey) and synchronous (light grey) clutches. Letters indicate significant pairwise differences within each measurement

(asynchronous; t70 = -2.24, p = 0.0278) ± SE. Numbers above columns represents sample size, N = 108.

Incubation period

The incubation period of asynchronous- more advanced eggs and synchronous- not advanced eggs was different. Asynchronous- more advanced eggs pipped 4 d earlier than the synchronous- not advanced eggs (F1,15 = 4.54, p < 0.0001; Fig. 4).

131

63 b

62

61

60

59 a 58

57 Incubation Period (days) Period Incubation 56

55 Asynchronous Synchronous

Fig. 4: Mean incubation period of asynchronous- more advanced eggs and synchronous- not advanced eggs, ± SE, N = 67. Letters indicate significant pairwise differences between asynchronous and synchronous groups (F1,15 = 4.54, p = < 0.0001).

Yolk and embryo mass

Yolk sac mass varied from 0.4 - 5.0 g and embryo wet mass ranged from 0.005 - 6 g, depending on stage of development when frozen. Treatment group did not affect yolk mass

(F1,56 = 1.77, p = 0.1889; Fig. 5a & b) or embryo mass (F1,42 = 0.02, p = 0.8845; Fig. 5a & b).

Yolk mass was not affected by egg mass (F1,56 = 1.8, p = 0.1854) and there was no interaction effect between treatment and egg mass (F1,56 = 1.34, p = 0.2523). The age of the embryo did affect yolk mass size (F8,56 = 6.64, p = <0.0001), but there was no interaction effect of egg mass and age (F8,56 = 1.08, p = 0.3935), or age and treatment (F8,56 = 0.51, p = 0.8442), nor was there an effect of egg mass, age and treatment (F8,56 = 0.5, p = 0.8493).

Embryo mass was affected by egg mass (F1,42 = 19.91, p = <0.0001) as well as age of the embryo (F6,42 = 29.27, p = <0.0001) and there was an interaction effect of egg mass and age

132

(F6,42 = 4.27, p = 0.0019). There was no interaction effect between egg mass and treatment

(F1,42 = 0.05, p = 0.8282), age and treatment (F6,42 = 0.14, p = 0.9907) and no significant interaction between egg mass, age and treatment (F6,42 = 0.17, p = 0.9844).

a 60

50

40

30 Mass (%) Mass 20

10

0

b 60

50

40

30 Mass % Mass 20

10

0 0 8 16 23 31 38 45 52 57 Day

Fig. 5: LSMeans of yolk mass (dark grey) and embryo mass (light grey) ratio throughout incubation a) asynchronous b) synchronous. Yolk; N = 110, embryo: N = 86.

133

Discussion

T3 and corticosterone concentrations increased in the yolk of the developing embryos in both the asynchronous and synchronous clutches towards the end of incubation. The peak in concentration in both hormones coincided with hatching. Corticosterone significantly increased by day 57 of incubation (pipping commencing from day 60), reaching a peak concentration prior to hatching, a pattern also seen in chickens (Kalliecharan and Hall, 1974).

The peak is thought to result from an increased number of secretary cells in the adrenal cortex mid-way through incubation. The adrenal axis begins to synthesise and secrete corticosterone into the plasma of chicken embryos 40-50% of the way through incubation (Pedernera, 1972;

Kalliecharan and Hall, 1974) and becomes the primary secretor of steroids 70% of the way through incubation (Kalliecharan and Hall, 1974). An increase in concentration is correlated with increased stress (Wingfield and Ramenofsky, 1999) and is linked with physiological and behavioural responses that help regulate energy storage and utilisation (Dallman and

Bhatnagar, 2010). This also coincides with the secondary stage of development in reptiles, when there is a high requirement for corticosterone for tissue growth and they are preparing to breath (Medler and Lance, 1998), which involves the lungs to change from non- functioning fluid-filled organs to being able to eliminate fluid and exchange gas (Thompson,

2007; Mortola, 2009). Treatment with corticosterone also increases O2 in lizards (Miles et al., 2007; DuRant et al., 2008; Meylan et al., 2010), so it is probable that the increase in corticosterone correlates with the change in respiration method.

There was no difference in yolk T3 concentrations between asynchronous and synchronous groups throughout incubation, but there was an increase in concentration at days 52 and 57.

The increase was significant in asynchronous groups from day 45 to 52 and suggests that the increase in hormone might not have any biological significance for metabolic compensation,

134

but might be important for hatching (Chapter 4). The spike in T3 in the final 2 weeks of incubation in E. macquarii indicates an age effect on hormone concentration. A similar spike in thyroid hormones happens late in development, prior to hatching in crocodiles

(Shepherdley et al., 2002). The increase is timed with the final stages of development, and the period where metabolic compensation occurs, but might only be important for developmental processes leading up to hatching, and hatching itself.

The vitelline vasculature is responsible for the transfer of hormones and minerals from the yolk to the embryo (Richards, 1991; Richards, 1997; Moore and Johnston, 2008), and there is potential for a bidirectional transfer (Richards, 1997). The yolk sac membrane is thought to be an active barrier between the embryo and its yolk stores, and has the ability to concentrate, store, and transfer trace minerals from the yolk. Transfer back to the yolk is also active, as in the bidirectional transfer of calcium (Simkiss, 1991; Packard and Clark, 1996). The sudden increase in corticosterone and T3 hormone concentrations in the yolk of E. macquarii, prior to hatching, could be due to the embryos own production of hormones (Jennings et al., 2000).

Thus, maternal hormones stored in the yolk are not the sole source of hormones influencing the embryo. The embryo is producing its own hormones and the increase in the yolk could be due to transfer of embryonic hormones into the yolk via the vitelline circulation late in development (Richards, 1991; Richards, 1997; Moore and Johnston, 2008).

The mechanisms that underlie metabolic compensation are unknown, and it appears that corticosterone and thyroid hormones are not the triggers. There was no difference between asynchronous and synchronous groups throughout incubation suggesting both hormones are not the mechanism for metabolic compensation. There was a similar trend in both corticosterone and T3 concentrations at the end of incubation. During development, release of

135

corticosterone is one of the primary mechanisms used to re-establish homeostasis during stressful events (Scott et al., 1981; Tona et al., 2003; Spencer et al., 2005). Embryonic stress responses trigger changes in corticosterone concentrations, and can increase circulating thyroid hormone triiodothyronine (T3) because it is linked to the conversion of thyroxine (T4) to T3 in avian embryos (Decuypere et al., 1983; Meeuwis et al., 1989). Similar patterns of increased corticosterone and T3 concentrations occur during development in anurans, with both hormones having supporting roles in metamorphosis (Kikuyama et al., 1993; Becker et al., 1997; Brown, 1997; Hayes, 1997; Fort et al., 2007), for example, during limb development (Kikuyama et al., 1993; Hayes, 1997). Corticosterone and T3 have very similar biological effect on metabolism and neuromuscular development. Both hormones, which increase in embryonic circulation late in development, are required for yolk absorption, egg emergence and post hatching performance and survival (Dimond, 1954; Wikelski et al., 1999;

Miles et al., 2007).

There was no significant difference in the hormone concentrations between asynchronous and synchronous groups throughout incubation, and there was a correlation between increased hormone concentrations during the last 14 days of incubation and hatching time. It is likely that these hormones are responsible for synchronous hatching in freshwater turtles due to their strong physiological effects during stressful environments and their influence on development and hatching time (Kalliecharan and Hall, 1974; McNabb et al., 1981;

Wentworth and Hussein, 1985; McNichols and McNabb, 1988; McNabb, 1992 ; McNabb and

King, 1993a; Medler and Lance, 1998; Wikelski et al., 1999; Shepherdley et al., 2002; Weiss et al., 2007). It is not clear when this increase begins and identifying when it occurs would require more regular yolk analysis. Both thyroid and corticosterone hormones are necessary for hatching and increase towards the end of incubation as part of their function for preparing

136

the embryo for life outside of the egg (Dimond, 1954; Hylka and Doneen, 1983; Wentworth and Hussein, 1985; Medler and Lance, 1998; Shepherdley et al., 2002).

Acknowledgements Research was supported by a UWS SSH Equipment Fund Grant and was conducted under UWS

ACEC no. A9908, NSW NPWS Permit SL101065. Fiona Loudon, Jessica Dormer, Heidi Stricker helped capture turtles and collect eggs. We thank the Webb family for their time, help, and use of their property. Thank you to Mark Emanuel, Julie Svanbeg and BriAnne Addison for their support, knowledge and guidance. Research was conducted in the Evolution and Developmental

Laboratory and K1 Aquatic Facility at the University of Western Sydney.

References Becker, K. B., Stephens, K. C., Davey, J. C., Schneider, M. J. & Galton, V. A. 1997. the type 2 and type 3 iodothyronine deiodinases play important roles in coordinating development in Rana catesbeiana tadpoles. Endocrinology, 138, 2989-2997.

Bernal, J. & Nunez, J. 1995. Thyroid hormones and brain development. Eur. J. Endocrinol., 133, 390-398.

Booth, D. T. 1998. Incubation of turtle eggs at different temperatures: do embryos compensate for temperature during development? Physiol. Zool., 71, 23-26.

Bradley, D., Towle, H. & Young, W. 1992. Spatial and temporal expression of alpha- and beta-thyroid hormone receptor mRNAs, including the beta 2-subtype, in the developing mammalian nervous system. J. Neurosci., 12, 2288-2302.

Brown, D. D. 1997. The role of thyroid hormone in zebrafish and axolotl development. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci., 94, 13011-13016.

Burch, W. M. & Lebovitz, H. E. 1982. Triiodothyronine stimulation of in vitro growth and maturation of embryonic chick cartilage. Endocrinology, 111, 462-468.

Colbert, P. L., Spencer, R.-J. & Janzen, F. J. 2010. Mechanism and cost of synchronous hatching. Funct. Ecol., 24, 112-121.

137

Dallman, M. F. & Bhatnagar, S. 2010. Chronic stress and energy balance: role of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis. Compreh. Physiol.: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Decuypere, E., Nouwen, E. J., Kühn, E. R., Geers, R. & Michels, H. 1979. lodohormones in the serum of chick embryos and post-hatching chickens as influenced by incubation temperature. Relationship with the hatching process and thermogenesis. Ann. Biol. Anim. Bioch. Biophys., 19, 1713-1723.

Decuypere, E., Scanes, C. G. & Kühn, E. R. 1983. Effects of glucocorticoids on circulating concentrations of thyroxine (T4) and triiodothyronine (T3) and on peripheral monodeiodination in pre- and post-hatching chickens. Hormone Metab. Res., 15, 233- 236.

Deeming, D. C. & Ferguson, M. W. J. 1991. Egg Incubation: Its Effects on Embryonic Development in Birds and Reptiles, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.

Dimond, S. M. T. 1954. The reactions of developing snapping turtles, Chelydra serpentina serpentina, to thiourea. J. Exp. Zool., 127, 93-115.

Doody, J. S. 2011. Environmentally cued hatching in reptiles. Integr. Comp. Biol., 51, 49-61.

Doody, J. S., Georges, A., Young, J. E., Pauza, M. D., Pepper, A. L., Alderman, R. L. & Welsh, M. A. 2001. Embryonic aestivation and emergence behaviour in the pig-nosed turtle, Carettochelys insculpta. Can. J. Zool., 79, 1062-1072.

DuRant, S. E., Romero, L. M., Talent, L. G. & Hopkins, W. A. 2008. Effect of exogenous corticosterone on respiration in a reptile. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol., 156, 126-133.

Escobar, G. M. d., Obregón, M. a. J. & Rey, F. E. d. 2004. Maternal thyroid hormones early in pregnancy and fetal brain development. Best Pract. Res. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab., 18, 225-248.

Escobar, G. M. D., Obregon, M. J., Oña, C. R. D. & Rey, F. E. D. 1988. Transfer of thyroxine from the mother to the rat fetus near term: effects on brain 3,5,3'- triiodothyronine deficiency. Endocrinology, 122, 1521-1531.

Escobar, G. M. D., Pastor, R., Obregon, M. J. & Rey, F. E. D. 1985. Effects of maternal hypothyroidism on the weight and thyroid hormone content of rat embryonic tissues, before and after onset of fetal thyroid function. Endocrinology, 117, 1890-1900.

Forrest, D., Sjöberg, M. & Vennström, B. 1990. Contrasting developmental and tissue- specific expression of alpha and beta thyroid hormone receptor genes. EMBO J., 9, 1519-1528.

138

Fort, D. J., Degitz, S., Tietge, J. & Touart, L. W. 2007. The hypothalamic-pituitary-thyroid (hpt) axis in frogs and its role in frog development and reproduction. Crit. Rev. Toxicol., 37, 117-161.

Freeman, B. M. 1974. Hormones in development In: Freeman, B. M. & Vince, M. V. (eds.) Development of the Avian Embryo. London: Chapman and Hall.

Gorbman, A., Dickhoff, W. W., Vigna, S. R., Clark, N. B. & Ralph, C. L. 1983. The Thyroid Gland. Comparative Endocrinology. New York: Wiley.

Haidar, M. A., Dube, S. & Sarkar, P. K. 1983. Thyroid hormone receptors of developing chick brain are predominantly in the neurons. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun., 112, 221-227.

Hanke, W. & Kloas, W. 1995. Comparative aspects of regulation and function of the adrenal complex in different groups of vertebrates. Hormone Metab. Res., 27, 389-397.

Hayes, T. B. 1997. Steroids as potential modulators of thyroid hormone activity in anuran metamorphosis. Am. Zool., 37, 185-194.

Hylka, V. W. & Doneen, B. A. 1983. Ontogeny of embryonic chicken lung: effects of pituitary gland, corticosterone, and other hormones upon pulmonary growth and synthesis of surfactant phospholipids. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol., 52, 108-120.

Jennings, D. H., Weiss, S. L. & Moore, M. C. 2000. Ontogenetic changes in embryonic yolk steroid content in tree lizards: transfer of hormones from the developing embryo to the yolk? . Am. Zool., 40, 1075-1076.

Kalliecharan, R. & Hall, B. K. 1974. A developmental study of the levels of progesterone, corticosterone, cortisol, and cortisone circulating in plasma of chick embryos. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 24, 364-372.

Karnofsky, D. A., Ridgway, L. P. & Patterson, P. A. 1951. Growth-inhibiting effect of cortisone acetate on the chick embryo. Endocrinology, 48, 596-616.

Kikuyama, S., Kawamura, K., Tanaka, S. & Yamamoto, K. 1993. Aspects of amphibian metamorphosis: hormonal control. Int. Rev. Cytol.

Klein, I. 1990. Thyroid hormone and the cardiovascular system. Am. J. Med., 88, 631-637.

Lin, H., Decuypere, E. & Buyse, J. 2004. Oxidative stress induced by corticosterone administration in broiler chickens (Gallus gallus domesticus): Short-term effect. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. B: Biochem. Mol. Biol., 139, 745-751.

Little, A. G. & Seebacher, F. 2014. Thyroid hormone regulates cardiac performance during cold acclimation in zebrafish (Danio rerio). J. Exp. Biol., 217, 718-725.

139

McEwen, B. S., Biron, C. A., Brunson, K. W., Bulloch, K., Chambers, W. H., Dhabhar, F. S., Goldfarb, R. H., Kitson, R. P., Miller, A. H., Spencer, R. L. & Weiss, J. M. 1997. The role of adrenocorticoids as modulators of immune function in health and disease: neural, endocrine and immune interactions. Brain Res. Rev., 23, 79-133.

McGlashan, J. K., Loudon, F. K., Thompson, M. B. & Spencer, R.-J. 2015. Hatching behaviour of eastern long-necked turtles (Chelodina longicollis): the influence of asynchronous environments on embryonic heart rate and phenotype. Comp. Biochem. Physiol., A: Mol. Integr. Physiol., 188, 58-64.

McGlashan, J. K., Spencer, R.-J. & Old, J. M. 2012. Embryonic communication in the nest: metabolic responses of reptilian embryos to developmental rates of siblings. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci., 279, 1709-1715.

McNabb, A. F. M. & King, D. B. 1993a. Thyroid hormone effects on growth, development, and metabolism. In: Schreibman, M. P., Scanes, C. G. & Pang, P. K. T. (eds.) The Endocrinology of Growth, Development, and Metabolism in Vertebrates. San Diego: Academic Press.

McNabb, A. F. M. & Wilson, C. M. 1997. Thyroid hormone deposition in avian eggs and effects on embryonic development. Am. Zool., 37, 553-560.

McNabb, F. M. A. 1992 Thyroid Hormones, New Jersey, Prentice Hall.

McNabb, F. M. A. & King, D. B. 1993b. Thyroid hormone effects on growth, development, and metabolism. The Endocrinology of Growth, Development, and Metabolism in Vertebrates. New York: Academic Press.

McNabb, F. M. A., Weirich, R. T. & McNabb, R. A. 1981. Thyroid function in embryonic and perinatal Japanese quail. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol., 43, 218-226.

McNichols, M. J. & McNabb, F. M. A. 1988. Development of thyroid function and its pituitary control in embryonic and hatchling precocial Japanese quail and altricial ring doves. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol., 70, 109-118.

Medler, K. F. & Lance, V. A. 1998. Sex differences in plasma corticosterone levels in alligator (Alligator mississippiensis) embryos. J. Exp. Zool., 280, 238-244.

Meeuwis, R., Michielsen, R., Decuypere, E. & Kühn, E. R. 1989. Thyrotropic activity of the ovine corticotropin-releasing factor in the chick embryo. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol., 76, 357-363.

Meylan, S., Haussy, C. & Voituron, Y. 2010. Physiological actions of corticosterone and its modulation by an immune challenge in reptiles. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol., 169, 158- 166.

140

Miles, D. B., Calsbeek, R. & Sinervo, B. 2007. Corticosterone, locomotor performance, and metabolism in side-blotched lizards (Uta stansburiana). Horm. Behav., 51, 548-554.

Monaghan, P. 2008. Early growth conditions, phenotypic development and environmental change. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., Ser. B: Biol. Sci., 363, 1635-1645.

Moore, M. C. & Johnston, G. I. H. 2008. Toward a dynamic model of deposition and utilization of yolk steroids. Integr. Comp. Biol., 48, 411-418.

Morkin, E., Flink, I. L. & Goldman, S. 1983. Biochemical and physiologic effects of thyroid hormone on cardiac performance. Prog. in Cardiovasc. Dis., 25, 435-464.

Morreale de Escobar, G., Obregon, M. & Escobar del Rey, F. 2004. Role of thyroid hormone during early brain development. Eur. J. Endocrinol., 151, U25-U37.

Mortola, J. P. 2009. Gas exchange in avian embryos and hatchlings. Comp. Biochem. Physiol., A: Mol. Integr. Physiol., 153, 359-377.

Packard, G., Packard, M., Miller, K. & Boardman, T. 1988. Effects of temperature and moisture during incubation on carcass composition of hatchling snapping turtles (Chelydra serpentina). J. Comp. Physiol., B, 158, 117-125.

Packard, G. C., Packard, M. J., Boardman, T. J., Morris, K. A. & Shuman, R. D. 1983. Influence of water exchanges by flexible-shelled eggs of painted turtles Chrysemys picta on metabolism and growth of embryos. Physiol. Zool., 56, 217-230.

Packard, M. J. & Clark, N. B. 1996. Aspects of calcium regulation in embryonic lepidosaurians and chelonians and a review of calcium regulation in embryonic archosaurians. Physiol. Zool., 69, 435-466.

Packard, M. J., Packard, G. C. & Boardman, T. J. 1982. Structure of eggshells and water relations of reptilian eggs. Herpetologica, 38, 136-155.

Pedernera, E. A. 1972. Adrenocorticotropic activity in vitro of the chick embryo pituitary gland. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol., 19, 589-591.

Perez-Castillo, A., Bernal, J., Ferreiro, B. & Pans, T. 1985. The early ontogenesis of thyroid hormone receptor in the rat fetus. Endocrinology, 117, 2457-2461.

Prati, M., Calvo, R., Morreale, G. & Escobar, G. M. d. 1992. L-thyroxine and 3,5,3'- triiodothyronine concentrations in the chicken egg and in the embryo before and after the onset of thyroid function. Endocrinology, 130, 2651-2659.

Richards, M. P. 1991. Mineral metabolism in the developing turkey embryo—II. The role of the yolk sac. Comp. Biochem. Physiol., A: Mol. Integr. Physiol., 100, 1017-1023.

141

Richards, M. P. 1997. Trace mineral metabolism in the avian embryo. Poult. Sci., 76, 152- 164.

Sapolsky, R. M., Romero, L. M. & Munck, A. U. 2000. How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocr. Rev., 21, 55-89.

Scott, T. R., Johnson, W. A., Satterlee, D. G. & Gildersleeve, R. P. 1981. Circulating levels of corticosterone in the serum of developing chick embryos and newly hatched chicks. Poult. Sci., 60, 1314-1320.

Shepherdley, C. A., Daniels, C. B., Orgeig, S., Richardson, S. J., Evans, B. K. & Darras, V. M. 2002. Glucocorticoids, thyroid hormones, and iodothyronine deiodinases in embryonic saltwater crocodiles. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol., 283, 1155-1163.

Simkiss, K. 1991. Fluxes During Embryogenesis. Egg Incubation: Its Effects on Embryonic Development in Birds and Reptiles. D. C. Deeming & M.W.J. Ferguson, M. W. J. ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Spencer, R.-J., Thompson, M. B. & Banks, P. B. 2001. Hatch or wait? A dilemma in reptilian incubation. Oikos, 93, 401-406.

Spencer, S. J., Heida, J. G. & Pittman, Q. J. 2005. Early life immune challenge: Effects on behavioural indices of adult rat fear and anxiety. Behav. Brain Res., 164, 231-238.

Thommes, R. C. 1987. Ontogenesis of thyroid function and regulation in the developing chick embryo. J. Exp. Zool., 1, 273-279.

Thommes, R. C. & Hylka, V. W. 1977. Plasma lodothyronines in the embryonic and immediate post-hatch chick. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol., 32, 417-422.

Thommes, R. C., Vieth, R. L. & Levasseur, S. 1977. The effects of hypophysectomy by means of surgical decapitation on thyroid function in the developing chick embryo. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol., 31, 29-36.

Thompson, M. B. 1988. Nest temperatures in the pleurodiran turtle, Emydura macquarii. Copeia, 1988, 996-1000.

Thompson, M. B. 1997. Egg Physiology and Biology. The Biology, Husbandry And Healthcare of Reptile. New Jersey: TFH Publications Inc.

Thompson, M. B. 2007. Comparison of the respiratory transition at birth or hatching in viviparous and oviparous amniote vertebrates. Comp. Biochem. Physiol., A: Mol. Integr. Physiol., 148, 755-760.

142

Tona, K., Malheiros, R., Bamelis, F., Careghi, C., Moraes, V., Onagbesan, O., Decuypere, E. & Bruggeman, V. 2003. Effects of storage time on incubating egg gas pressure, thyroid hormones, and corticosterone levels in embryos and on their hatching parameters. Poult. Sci., 82, 840-845.

Tullett, S. G. 1990. Science and the art of incubation. Poult. Sci., 69, 1-15.

Vulsma, T., Gons, M. H. & de Vijlder, J. J. M. 1989. Maternal-fetal transfer of thyroxine in congenital hypothyroidism due to a total organification defect or thyroid agenesis. New Engl. J. Med., 321, 13-16.

Webb, G. J. W., Choquenot, D. & Whitehead, P. J. 1986. Nests, eggs, and embryonic development of Carettochelys insculpta (Chelonia: Carettochelidae) from Northern Australia. J. Zool., 1, 521-550.

Weiss, S. L., Johnston, G. & Moore, M. C. 2007. Corticosterone stimulates hatching of late- term tree lizard embryos. Comp. Biochem. Physiol., A: Mol. Integr. Physiol., 146, 360-365.

Wentworth, B. C. & Hussein, M. O. 1985. Serum corticosterone levels in embryos, newly hatched, and young turkey poults. Poult. Sci., 64, 2195-2201.

Wikelski, M., Lynn, S., Breuner, J. C., Wingfield, J. C. & Kenagy, G. J. 1999. Energy metabolism, testosterone and corticosterone in white-crowned sparrows. J. Comp. Physiol., A, 185, 463-470.

Wilson, C. M. & McNabb, F. M. A. 1997. Maternal thyroid hormones in Japanese quail eggs and their influence on embryonic development. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol., 107, 153- 165.

Wingfield, J. C. & Ramenofsky, M. 1999. Hormones and the behavioral ecology of stress. Stress Physiology in Animals. P. H. M. Balm ed. Sheffield: Sheffield Academic Press.

Wise, P. M. & Frye, B. E. 1973. Functional development of the hypothalamo-hypophyseal- adrenal cortex axis in the chick embryo, Gallus domesticus. J. Exp. Zool., 185, 277- 291.

Wourms, J. P. 1981. Viviparity: the maternal-fetal relationship in fishes. Am. Zool., 21, 473- 515.

143

CHAPTER 6

GENERAL DISCUSSION

Environmentally cued hatching has been reported in up to 41 species of reptiles, some showing multiple mechanisms (Doody, 2011). The range of mechanisms already known is diverse among species, as is the current understanding of the physiological and evolutionary causes. Three current mechanisms of synchronous hatching in freshwaters turtles are observed, delayed hatching, early hatching and metabolic compensation. This study focused on two of the mechanisms, early hatching and metabolic compensation. Temperature induced developmental differences between clutch-mates during embryogenesis alter developmental rate and incubation periods of embryos (Thompson, 1988; Deeming and Ferguson, 1991;

Thompson, 1997), but the Murray short-necked turtle (Emydura macquarii) and the painted turtle (Chrysemys picta) still hatch synchronously. The mechanism utilised by E. macquarii was already understood but not that of C. picta. I aimed to identify the mechanisms of synchronous hatching in two geographically different species of freshwater turtles, the eastern long-necked turtle (Chelodina longicollis) and C. picta, as populations of both species often overwinter in the nest, and compare the findings to what is already known in E. macquarii to further support the evolutionary perspective of synchronous hatching.

144

Mechanisms and evolutionary significance of synchronous hatching

The mechanisms of synchronous hatching in C. longicollis are similar to those of E. macquarii. Chelodina longicollis hatches synchronously through metabolic compensation, evident through diel heart rates of embryos and residual yolk sac size of hatchings

(McGlashan et al., 2015; chapter 2). Although some populations may overwinter in the nest

(Roe and Georges, 2007), embryos adjust their heart rates during development and hatch without any phenotypic disadvantage in size or performance, which is important for immediate emergence from the nest. Metabolic compensation is correlated with reduced residual yolk reserves, as seen in E. macquarii (McGlashan et al., 2012). Synchronous hatching through metabolic compensation favours early emergence from the nest, but if overwintering occurs in C. longicollis, reduced yolk reserves could be detrimental to their energy supply until emergence in spring and ultimately survival. To understand the evolutionary significance of a particular trait, more must be known about the life history, ecology and behaviour of the species and unfortunately not enough is known about overwintering in Australian turtles, but observations have only been recorded in the cool, southern areas of the country (Parmenter, 1985; Kennett, 1999).

Synchronous hatching via early hatching occurs in C. picta as there is no evidence of metabolic compensation (Chapter 3). Many populations of C. picta in North America overwinter in the maternal nest (Gibbons, 2013). Overwintering ensures a better chance of survival from the environmental and physiological risks that come with emerging prior to winter. Winter is a critical time for hatchling survival in C. picta as nest temperatures can drop below -10 °C (Packard and Packard, 2004). Hatching early ensures an optimal overwintering position in the nest to reduce the chance of mortality from freezing, dehydration and starvation from loss of energy reserves (Gibbons and Nelson, 1978; Packard,

145

1997; Costanzo et al., 1999; Spencer and Janzen, 2014). Synchronous hatching in C. picta is a result of hatching early (Colbert et al. 2010). Chrysemys picta is not affected by hatching early (Chapter 3), but previous studies show poor performance ability immediately after hatching and several months later (Colbert et al., 2010). Seasonal nest conditions and maternal nest site choice can have different impacts on developmental rates of embryos

(Thompson, 1988) and could result in greater asynchrony between clutch-mates. The level of asynchronous development that was induced in my study (Chapter 3) and Colbert et al.

(2010) differed and may have caused the difference in hatchling performance post hatching.

This could indicate that the earlier in development that the embryo is triggered to hatch in relation to its more advanced clutch-mates, the more likely the hatchling will suffer performance costs. Temperature variations are inevitable and ideally, less variation in a nest is better for neonates, but regardless of any asynchrony in developmental rates, hatching synchrony will still occur in these species.

Although metabolic compensation is a benefit to species that regularly display group emergence, it comes at a cost to the individual. Emydura macquarii embryos that are stimulated to hatch synchronously catabolise more yolk reserves during incubation, resulting in smaller residual yolk sacs at hatching but no difference in hatchling size (McGlashan et al.,

2012). Chelodina longicollis hatchlings that metabolically compensate during incubation hatch with significantly smaller yolk sacs (McGlashan et al., 2015; Chapter 2). Yolk reserves are necessary for post hatching growth and development (Radder et al., 2002; Radder et al.,

2004; McGlashan et al., 2012) but, for E. macquarii and C. longicollis, optimal performance for group emergence requires a tradeoff of reduced yolk reserves. This same strategy would not be favoured in C. picta because hatchlings typically remain in the nest overwinter, and utilising yolk reserves prior to hatching could be detrimental to their survival in the nest

146

(Willette et al., 2005). Although temperatures can drop below freezing, embryos still metabolise yolk in the nest (Willette et al., 2005; Spencer and Janzen, 2014). Overwintering of C. picta favours cooler conditions for males and warmer conditions for females for metabolising yolk reserves. If the overwintering condition is atypical to the preferred condition, then embryos can use up to 50% more of their yolk stores (Spencer and Janzen,

2014). Thus, it is more viable for C. picta to hatch early and less developed to gain an optimal position in the nest than it is to metabolise valuable energy stores and hatch fully developed.

Circadian rhythms in heart rates are evident in embryonic E. macquarii (Loudon et al., 2013),

C. longicollis (McGlashan et al., 2015; Chapter 2) and C. picta (Chapter 3). Heart rates during embryonic development show periods of peaked and reduced activity that deviate from the mean heart rate over a 24 h period. Deviations from the mean indicate metabolic compensation through either positive or negative compensatory changes in embryo metabolism. Circadian rhythms in C. longicollis embryos show that positive metabolic compensation occurs within asynchronous clutches (McGlashan et al.,2015; Chapter 2), but the lack of positive compensatory changes in the heart rates of embryonic C. picta in similar conditions further supports early hatching as opposed to metabolic compensation (Chapter 3).

Heart rates are not fixed and environmental conditions merely determine the limits of heart rate and thus developmental rate (Loudon et al., 2013; McGlashan et al., 2015). Circadian rhythms within embryonic heart rates are a recent discovery in freshwater turtles and could be a potential mechanism for embryonic communication within the nest (Loudon et al., 2013;

McGlashan et al., 2015), making group environments necessary for synchronous hatching, particularly in the final 2 weeks of incubation (McGlashan et al., 2015; Chapter 2). Eggs

147

incubate in close contact with each other and heart rates could be detected and used to determine the developmental stage of clutch-mates, thus mediating synchrony of hatching.

The cues that trigger synchronous hatching vary among species and appear to be driven by different abiotic and biotic factors (Webb et al., 1986; Thompson, 1989; Doody et al., 2001;

Spencer et al., 2001; Colbert et al., 2010; Spencer and Janzen, 2011). Cues within a nest that trigger synchronous hatching might be heart rates, CO2 concentrations in the nest (Spencer et al., 2001; Spencer and Janzen, 2011) or acoustic communication through stridulation, vibration and audible clicks (Vergne et al., 2009; Ferrara et al., 2013; Ferrara et al., 2014).

Embryos of both C. longicollis and E. macquarii have greater variation in incubation period when incubated individually (Loudon, 2015; McGlashan et al., 2015; Chapter 2), demonstrating the importance of group environments during incubation and the potential for embryonic communication. Embryos are able to communicate developmental stage within a nest and either hatch early or metabolically compensate, and the crucial period for signalling and responding to cues in the nest is the final two weeks of incubation (McGlashan et al.,

2015).

Physiological and endocrine mechanisms

For metabolic compensation to occur during development, a physiological change within the body must occur. Hormones are likely candidates for these changes and in particular thyroid and glucocorticoid hormones. Both thyroid and glucocorticoid hormones have the potential to stimulate metabolic compensation and synchronous hatching in freshwater turtles due to their strong physiological effects and their influence on metabolism and hatching time (Dimond,

1954; Kalliecharan and Hall, 1974; Wentworth and Hussein, 1985; McNichols and McNabb,

1988; McNabb and King, 1993; McNabb and Wilson, 1997). Thyroid hormones regulate

148

embryonic development, growth, and metabolism in animals (Himms-Hagen, 1976; Dauncey,

1990; Silva, 2001; Hollenberg and Forrest, 2008) but the application of exogenous triiodotyronine (T3) during embryonic development in E. macquarii does not affect heart rate,

O2 or total metabolic expenditure during incubation. An increase in T3 does, however, have a significant effect on incubation period. Increased T3 causes neonates to hatch earlier than expected and with no developmental cost, which suggests that embryonic development is completed (Chapter 4). Triiodothyronine is an important hormone for hatching and could be important for increasing developmental rate to hatch synchronously, but does not incur a metabolic cost.

Metabolic compensation may be a due to a stress response in embryos and both thyroid and the glucocorticoid (corticosterone) hormones could stimulate this. Analysing both T3 and corticosterone during incubation where asynchronous development of eggs has been induced does not show any significant difference between less advanced and more advanced embryos and therefore does not appear to be stimulating metabolic compensation (Chapter 4 and 5).

Triiodothyronine and corticosterone concentrations increased in the yolk of the developing embryos towards the end of incubation in both treatment groups, which coincides with hatching (Chapter 5). The increase in T3 and corticosterone might not have any biological significance for metabolic compensation, but they are important for hatching. The spike in T3 and corticosterone in the final 2 weeks of incubation in E. macquarii also occurs prior to hatching in crocodiles (Shepherdley et al., 2002) and chickens (Kalliecharan and Hall, 1974).

There is a correlation between patterns of thyroid hormones and corticosterone during development in metamorphosis in anurans (Kikuyama et al., 1993; Becker et al., 1997;

Brown, 1997; Hayes, 1997; Fort et al., 2007), and corticosterone is linked to the conversion of thyroxine (T4) to T3 by deiodination in birds (Decuypere et al., 1983; Meeuwis et al.,

149

1989). The increase in both hormones prior to hatching may only be important for preparing to hatch, including yolk absorption and the change to neonatal respiration, and hatching itself, such as egg emergence and post hatching performance (Dimond, 1954; Wikelski et al., 1999;

Miles et al., 2007).

Increased concentrations of T3 does alter incubation period and development with neonates hatching earlier and more developed relative to controls (Chapter 4). Triiodothyronine concentrations significantly increase two weeks prior to hatching in embryos that are developing in asynchronous clutch environments (Chapter 5). Although the increased thyroid hormones in the embryos yolk do not stimulate metabolic compensation through increased

O2, thyroid hormones are important for embryonic development and hatching.

Concluding remarks Embryonic development is complex and the environment in which embryos develop can influence hatching behaviour. There is a level of developmental plasticity that allows embryos to hatch synchronously but this is different among species. Also different are the abiotic and biotic forces that have driven the evolution of synchronous hatching through metabolic compensation and early hatching. Metabolic compensation in E. macquarii and C. longicollis is a remarkable trait that enables synchronous hatching with no developmental or performance costs, essential for predator avoidance and the survival of hatchings when digging out of a nest and traversing to the water. There is a trade-off of reduced energy reserves required to sustain the hatchling for the first few days after leaving the nest, but this is not an immediate concern for survival, unless hatchlings overwinter in the nest. Early hatching in C. picta does not benefit from the loss of yolk reserves as overwintering in the nest requires embryos to have enough energy to sustain the metabolic requirements until

150

emergence in the following spring. Chrysemys picta hatches early, which may result in reduced performance in some cases, but as long as an optimal overwintering position to avoid freezing is gained and embryos have enough energy stores, then chances of survival for the hatchling are greatly increased. Comparing mechanisms of hatching in populations that overwinter in the nest and emerge soon after hatching in both C. longicollis and C. picta would help untangle the current confusion in the evolutionary significance of specific hatching behaviours.

Endogenous thyroid hormone and glucocorticoids in the developing embryo do not stimulate synchronous hatching in E. macquarii. Although T3 is linked to an acceleration of development that results in a reduction in incubation period, it did not appear to increase heart rate, O2 or total metabolic expenditure of embryos or result in any compensatory effects. Thyroid hormones and glucocorticords are necessary for hatching and possibly synchronous hatching in freshwater turtles but most likely only for preparation of life outside of the egg and the hatching event.

This thesis describes two different mechanisms of synchronous hatching in turtles, metabolic compensation and early hatching. There is now a greater understanding of the evolutionary significance of synchronous hatching on a geographic scale and I have demonstrated how each mechanism of synchronous hatching could relate to individual biological and ecological functions (predator avoidance and overwintering). More broadly, I have identified the importance of group environments as a mechanism of embryonic communication that could stimulate synchronous hatching. There are still many questions that can be addressed, in terms of ECH and the hatching cues within nests that stimulate metabolic compensation or early hatching. Of particular interest are the physiological mechanisms and endocrine

151

responses and how hormones might be altering development and behaviour. Although there is no difference in T3 and corticosterone concentrations during asynchronous development, there is a clear physiological response to increased T3 and a significant increase in hormone concentration in the embryos that are less advanced in a clutch. It is possible that thyroid hormones could still trigger accelerated development as seen in metabolic compensation via an alternative pathway, but further investigation into the effects on specific organs, i.e. heart and lungs, in turtles is required.

Finally, this thesis shows that the mechanisms driving synchronous hatching in E. macquarii,

C. longicollis and C. picta differ from each other and could have evolved independently.

Geographic, environmental and genetic factors unique to populations can all influence ECH.

Thermal gradients that cause asynchronous development between embryos are the norm in freshwater turtle nests. If we can determine the minimum and maximum extremes of asynchrony and the effects it has on development and hatching mechanisms, we might be able to predict the long-term behavioural, biological and ecological effects that may occur as a result of varying climatic conditions. Phenotypic plasticity means traits will be adapted to withstand changing conditions and selection of the optimal trait (e.g. early/delayed/metabolic compensation/synchronous) may be unique to ECH behaviour in reptiles. Plasticity in development may also be why we see such versatility in hatching behaviour and why some species display multiple forms of ECH behaviour in the present day.

152

References Becker, K. B., Stephens, K. C., Davey, J. C., Schneider, M. J. & Galton, V. A. 1997. the type 2 and type 3 iodothyronine deiodinases play important roles in coordinating development in Rana catesbeiana tadpoles. Endocrinology, 138, 2989-2997.

Brown, D. D. 1997. The role of thyroid hormone in zebrafish and axolotl development. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 94, 13011-13016.

Colbert, P. L., Spencer, R.-J. & Janzen, F. J. 2010. Mechanism and cost of synchronous hatching. Functional Ecology, 24, 112-121.

Costanzo, J. P., Litzgus, J. D. & Lee Jr, R. E. 1999. Behavioral responses of hatchling painted turtles (Chrysemys picta) and snapping turtles (Chelydra serpentina) at subzero temperatures. Journal of Thermal Biology, 24, 161-166.

Dauncey, M. J. 1990. Thyroid hormones and thermogenesis. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society, 49, 203-215.

Decuypere, E., Scanes, C. G. & Kühn, E. R. 1983. Effects of glucocorticoids on circulating concentrations of thyroxine (T4) and triiodothyronine (T3) and on peripheral monodeiodination in pre- and post-hatching chickens. Hormone and Metabolic Research, 15, 233-236.

Deeming, D. C. & Ferguson, M. W. J. 1991. Egg Incubation: Its Effects on Embryonic Development in Birds and Reptiles, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.

Dimond, S. M. T. 1954. The reactions of developing snapping turtles, Chelydra serpentina serpentina, to thiourea. Journal of Experimental Zoology, 127, 93-115.

Doody, J. S. 2011. Environmentally cued hatching in reptiles. Integrative and Comparative Biology, 51, 49-61.

Doody, J. S., Georges, A., Young, J. E., Pauza, M. D., Pepper, A. L., Alderman, R. L. & Welsh, M. A. 2001. Embryonic aestivation and emergence behaviour in the pig-nosed turtle, Carettochelys insculpta. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 79, 1062-1072.

Ferrara, C. R., Vogt, R. C., Harfush, M. R., Sousa-Lima, R. S., Albavera, E. & Tavera, A. 2014. First evidence of leatherback turtle (Dermochelys coriacea) embryos and hatchlings emitting sounds. Chelonian Conservation and Biology, 13, 110-114.

Ferrara, C. R., Vogt, R. C. & Sousa-Lima, R. S. 2013. Turtle vocalizations as the first evidence of posthatching parental care in chelonians. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 127, 24-32.

153

Fort, D. J., Degitz, S., Tietge, J. & Touart, L. W. 2007. The hypothalamic-pituitary-thyroid (hpt) axis in frogs and its role in frog development and reproduction. Critical Reviews in Toxicology, 37, 117-161.

Gibbons, J. W. 2013. A long-term perspective of delayed emergence (aka overwintering) in hatchling turtles: some they do and some they don't, and some you just can't tell. Journal of Herpetology, 47, 203-214.

Gibbons, J. W. & Nelson, D. H. 1978. The evolutionary significance of delayed emergence from the nest by hatchling turtles. Evolution, 32, 297-303.

Hayes, T. B. 1997. Steroids as potential modulators of thyroid hormone activity in anuran metamorphosis. American Zoologist, 37, 185-194.

Himms-Hagen, J. 1976. Cellular thermogenesis. Annual Review of Physiology, 38, 315-351.

Hollenberg, A. N. & Forrest, D. 2008. The thyroid and metabolism: the action continues. Cell Metabolism, 8, 10-12.

Kalliecharan, R. & Hall, B. K. 1974. A developmental study of the levels of progesterone, corticosterone, cortisol, and cortisone circulating in plasma of chick embryos. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 24, 364-372.

Kennett, R. 1999. Reproduction of two species of freshwater turtle, Chelodina rugosa and Elseya dentata, from the wet–dry tropics of northern Australia. Journal of Zoology, 247, 457-473.

Kikuyama, S., Kawamura, K., Tanaka, S. & Yamamoto, K. 1993. Aspects of amphibian metamorphosis: Hormonal control. International Review of Cytol.

Loudon, F. K., Spencer, R.-J., Strassmeyer, A. & Harland, K. 2013. Metabolic circadian rhythms in embryonic turtles. Integrative and Comparative Biology, 53, 175-182.

McGlashan, J. K., Loudon, F. K., Thompson, M. B. & Spencer, R.-J. 2015. Hatching behaviour of eastern long-necked turtles (Chelodina longicollis): the influence of asynchronous environments on embryonic heart rate and phenotype. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology, Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology, 188, 58- 64.

McGlashan, J. K., Spencer, R.-J. & Old, J. M. 2012. Embryonic communication in the nest: metabolic responses of reptilian embryos to developmental rates of siblings. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279, 1709-1715.

McNabb, A. F. M. & King, D. B. 1993. Thyroid hormone effects on growth, development, and metabolism. In: Schreibman, M. P., Scanes, C. G. & Pang, P. K. T. (eds.) The

154

Endocrinology of Growth, Development, and Metabolism in Vertebrates. San Diego: Academic Press.

McNabb, A. F. M. & Wilson, C. M. 1997. Thyroid hormone deposition in avian eggs and effects on embryonic development. American Zoologist, 37, 553-560.

McNichols, M. J. & McNabb, F. M. A. 1988. Development of thyroid function and its pituitary control in embryonic and hatchling precocial Japanese quail and altricial ring doves. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 70, 109-118.

Meeuwis, R., Michielsen, R., Decuypere, E. & Kühn, E. R. 1989. Thyrotropic activity of the ovine corticotropin-releasing factor in the chick embryo. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 76, 357-363.

Miles, D. B., Calsbeek, R. & Sinervo, B. 2007. Corticosterone, locomotor performance, and metabolism in side-blotched lizards (Uta stansburiana). Hormones and Behavior, 51, 548-554.

Packard, G. C. 1997. Temperatures during winter in nests with hatchling painted turtles (Chrysemys picta). Herpetologica, 53, 89-95.

Packard, G. C. & Packard, M. J. 2004. To freeze or not to freeze: adaptations for overwintering by hatchlings of the North American painted turtle. Journal of Experimental Biology, 207, 2897-2906.

Parmenter, C. J. 1985. Reproduction and survivorship of Chelodina longicollis ( Testudinata: Chelidae). The biology of Australasian frogs and reptiles, 53-61.

Radder, R. S., Shanbhag, B. A. & Saidapur, S. K. 2002. Pattern of yolk internalization by hatchlings is related to breeding timing in the garden lizard, Calotes versicolor. Current Science, 82 1484-1485.

Radder, R. S., Shanbhag, B. A. & Saidapur, S. K. 2004. Yolk partitioning in embryos of the lizard, Calotes versicolor: maximize body size or save energy for later use? Journal of Experimental Zoology, Part A: Comparative Experimental Biology, 301, 783-785.

Roe, J. H. & Georges, A. 2007. Heterogeneous wetland complexes, buffer zones, and travel corridors: Landscape management for freshwater reptiles. Biological Conservation, 135, 67-76.

Shepherdley, C. A., Daniels, C. B., Orgeig, S., Richardson, S. J., Evans, B. K. & Darras, V. M. 2002. Glucocorticoids, thyroid hormones, and iodothyronine deiodinases in embryonic saltwater crocodiles. American Journal of Physiology: Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 283, 1155-1163.

155

Silva, J. E. 2001. The multiple contributions of thyroid hormone to heat production. Journal of Clinical Investigation, 108, 35-37.

Spencer, R.-J. & Janzen, F. J. 2011. Hatching behavior in turtles. Integrative and Comparative Biology, 51, 100-110.

Spencer, R.-J., Thompson, M. B. & Banks, P. B. 2001. Hatch or wait? A dilemma in reptilian incubation. Oikos, 93, 401-406.

Spencer, R. J. & Janzen, F. J. 2014. A novel hypothesis for the adaptive maintenance of environmental sex determination in a turtle. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281, 20140831.

Thompson, M. B. 1988. Nest temperatures in the pleurodiran turtle, Emydura macquarii. Copeia, 1988, 996-1000.

Thompson, M. B. 1989. Patterns of metabolism in embryonic reptiles. Respiration Physiology, 76, 243-255.

Thompson, M. B. 1997. Egg Physiology and Biology. The Biology, Husbandry And Healthcare of Reptile. New Jersey: TFH Publications Inc.

Vergne, A. L., Pritz, M. B. & Mathevon, N. 2009. Acoustic communication in crocodilians: from behaviour to brain. Biological Reviews, 84, 391-411.

Webb, G. J. W., Choquenot, D. & Whitehead, P. J. 1986. Nests, eggs, and embryonic development of Carettochelys insculpta (Chelonia: Carettochelidae) from Northern Australia. Journal of Zoology, 1, 521-550.

Wentworth, B. C. & Hussein, M. O. 1985. Serum corticosterone levels in embryos, newly hatched, and young turkey poults. Poultry Science, 64, 2195-2201.

Wikelski, M., Lynn, S., Breuner, J. C., Wingfield, J. C. & Kenagy, G. J. 1999. Energy metabolism, testosterone and corticosterone in white-crowned sparrows. Journal of Comparative Physiology, A, 185, 463-470.

Willette, D. A. S., Tucker, J. K. & Janzen, F. J. 2005. Linking climate and physiology at the population level for a key life-history stage of turtles. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 83, 845-850.

156

APPENDIX

HATCHING BEHAVIOUR OF EASTERN LONG-NECKED TURTLES

(CHELODINA LONGICOLLIS): THE INFLUENCE OF

ASYNCHRONOUS ENVIRONMENTS ON EMBRYONIC HEART RATE

AND PHENOTYPE

A paper published in Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular &

Integrative Physiology

157

Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology, Part A 188 (2015) 58–64

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology, Part A

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cbpa

Hatching behavior of eastern long-necked turtles (Chelodina longicollis): The influence of asynchronous environments on embryonic heart rate and phenotype

Jessica K. McGlashan a,⁎, Fiona K. Loudon a, Michael B. Thompson b, Ricky-John Spencer a a School of Science and Health, University of Western Sydney, Locked Bag 1797, Penrith South DC, NSW 2751, Australia b School of Biological Sciences, The University of Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia article info abstract

Article history: Variable temperatures within a nest cause asynchronous development within clutches of freshwater turtle Received 5 February 2015 embryos, yet synchronous hatching occurs and is thought to be an important survival strategy for hatchlings. Received in revised form 19 June 2015 Metabolic compensation and circadian rhythms in heart rates of embryonic turtles indicate the potential of com- Accepted 19 June 2015 munication between embryos in a nest. Heart rates were used to identify metabolic circadian rhythms in clutches Available online 26 June 2015 of an Australian freshwater turtle (Chelodina longicollis) and determine whether embryos metabolically compen-

Keywords: sate and hatch synchronously when incubated in asynchronous environments. The effects of a group environ- fi Egg ment during incubation on egg development and incubation period were also investigated during the nal Embryo 3 weeks of development. Chelodina longicollis hatch synchronously and metabolically compensate so that less Embryonic communication advanced embryos catch up to more advanced clutch-mates. Heart rates of embryos remained stable from Embryonic development week 4–7 in asynchronous (M = 89 bpm) and synchronous (M = 92 bpm) groups and declined in the final Environmentally cued hatching 2 weeks of incubation (M = 72 and 77 bpm). Circadian rhythms were present throughout development and Turtle diel heart rates of embryos in asynchronous groups showed less deviation from the mean (M = −0.5 bpm) Incubation than synchronous groups (M = −4 bpm). Eggs incubated in groups had a significantly shorter incubation period Metabolic compensation than eggs incubated individually. Phenotypic traits including size, performance, and growth of all hatchlings Nest environment Synchronous hatching were not affected. Egg position within a turtle nest is important for coordinating development throughout incu- bation and facilitating synchronous hatching. © 2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction embryos (Packard and Packard, 1988; Du et al., 2009, 2010b). In natural nests of pleurodiran (side-necked) turtles, temperature fluctuates daily Environment affects embryonic development of an organism and al- and results in a temperature gradient that varies up to 6 °C between ters offspring fitness and survival, including variations in an individual's the top and the bottom of the nest. Eggs at the top experience greater behavior, physiology, morphology, performance, and phenotypic char- shifts in temperature and are warmer for more than 75% of the day acteristics (Deeming and Ferguson, 1991; West-Eberhard, 2003; Mona- (Thompson, 1988, 1997). Temperature determines the length of ghan, 2008). Collectively, these variations are referred to as phenotypic incubation, with warmer incubation temperatures resulting in higher plasticity (Price et al., 2003; Doody, 2011). Intrinsic and extrinsic factors, embryonic metabolic rate, including cardiac function, the number of such as timing of ovulation, nutrient acquisition, and dissimilar thermal heart beats, and faster development (Birchard and Reiber, 1996; Du environments in the nest, cause variation in development and incuba- et al., 2009) than do cooler temperatures (Packard and Packard, 1988; tion period of the embryo (Deeming and Ferguson, 1991). Environmen- Deeming and Ferguson, 1991; Monaghan, 2008). tal conditions, such as temperature, influence embryonic development, Despite the environmental influence of temperature on incubation including incubation length (Deeming and Ferguson, 1991; Booth, length, many avian and reptilian embryos are capable of altering their 1998; Monaghan, 2008), sex determination (TSD) (Yntema, 1968, development and hatching time (Hoyt, 1979; Vleck et al., 1980; 1978; Bull and Vogt, 1979; Bull, 1985), and physiological changes i.e. Thompson, 1989; Du et al., 2011; McGlashan et al., 2012). Embryos metabolic rate (developmental rate and cardiovascular function) of have the ability to alter time of hatching in relation to environment through phenotypic plasticity (Packard and Packard, 2000; Doody, 2011; Spencer and Janzen, 2011). A recent focal point for embryonic Abbreviations: TSD, Temperature sex determination; ECH, Environmentally cued studies, and in particular reptilian embryos, is environmentally cued hatching; bpm, Beats per minute; M, Mean; d, Day(s); h, Hour(s). ⁎ Corresponding author. Tel.: +61 2 4570 1195, +61 433 969 804 (mobile). hatching (ECH) (Warkentin and Calswell, 2009). ECH can increase E-mail address: [email protected] (J.K. McGlashan). an individual's chance of survival through a tradeoff between risks

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpa.2015.06.018 1095-6433/© 2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. J.K. McGlashan et al. / Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology, Part A 188 (2015) 58–64 59

(Gomez-Mestre et al., 2008; Warkentin and Calswell, 2009). Phenotypic permanent rivers (Chessman, 1988). Females may travel 500 m or more plasticity facilitates ECH where embryos can hatch early, delay hatching, to nest (Cann, 1998; Kennett, 1999; Kennett et al., 2009) and predation or hatch synchronously in response to environmental conditions. These on nests is high (Thompson, 1983; Beck, 1991). Clutch sizes vary between behaviors are not mutually exclusive and some species show multiple 6 and 23 eggs, each egg weighing between 4 and 8 g. Natural incubation forms of ECH (Warkentin and Calswell, 2009; Doody, 2011). and emergence takes from 110 to 150 d, with overwintering of hatchlings The event and timing of hatching can affect survival of offspring both in the nest in populations in southern Australia (Parmenter, 1985). immediately (risk of predation) and in the future (resource availability, Female C. longicollis were captured from the Hawkesbury region, size, and performance of individual) (Spencer and Janzen, 2011). Fully New South Wales (−33.61°S, 150.75°E) from 1 to 14 November 2011 developed pig-nosed turtle (Carretochelys insculpta) embryos delay using lobster and cathedral traps. Females were palpated and all gravid hatching until the onset of the wet season when nests become inundated females were given a subcutaneous injection of oxytocin (1 mL/kg) with water, causing spontaneous hatching of all eggs in the nest (Webb (Spencer et al., 2001) to stimulate oviposition. Females were then et al., 1986; Doody et al., 2001). Embryos hatch synchronously in response placed in water in individual containers in a quiet dark room. Eggs to hypoxia caused by flooding or vibrations from movement of hatchlings were collected from containers over a period of 5 to 24 h, weighed (Doody et al., 2012). Conversely, the Murray short-necked turtle and individually marked in order of oviposition using a HB pencil. (Emydura macquarii),andtheNorthAmericanpaintedturtle(Chrysemys Eggs were then placed in containers with moist vermiculite and kept picta) hatch synchronously by either catching up or hatching premature- cool until all eggs from the clutch were collected (Spencer et al., 2001; ly. E.macquarii embryos increase metabolic rates throughout the last third Colbert et al., 2010). A total of 20 clutches of 6–12 eggs were collected of development to catch up and hatch at a similar developmental stage to for the asynchrony experiment and 5 clutches of 7–10 eggs for the egg more advanced clutch-mates. Metabolic compensation does not affect positioning experiment. Prior to release, all females were palpated to hatchling size and performance immediately and up to 5 months post ensure all eggs were laid; three of the 20 females were injected twice hatching (Spencer et al., 2001; McGlashan et al., 2012). Less advanced to complete oviposition. embryos of C. picta also hatch prematurely in response to more advanced siblings, but metabolic compensation is not apparent, in terms of neonatal 2.2. Incubation methods performance post hatching (Colbert et al., 2010). The cardiovascular system is vital for oxygen and nutrient delivery Eggs were half buried in a mixture of vermiculite and deionized during development (Birchard and Reiber, 1996; Tazawa, 2005; water (1:1) and incubated in clear plastic containers (200 × 100 × Monaghan, 2008). Heart rate of precocial embryos is dependent on tem- 50 mm). Eggs were removed from containers weekly and vermiculite perature and stage of development (Cain et al., 1967; Tazawa, 2005) and water mixture was weighed and rehydrated to maintain water po- and is used as an indicator for metabolic rate in avian and reptilian em- tential of vermiculite. The initial weight of the container and vermiculite bryos (Ar and Tazawa, 1999; Pearson and Tazawa, 1999; Tazawa, 2005; were used to account for evaporation and net water gain from the eggs. Du et al., 2009, 2010b, 2011). Metabolic rate, or heart rate, can show cir- Containers were also rotated weekly within environmental chambers to cadian rhythm which can be endogenously driven over a 24 h period. counteract potential thermal gradients (Janzen, 1993; Colbert et al., Within a nest of buried eggs, where there are no external signals such 2010). as daylight, rhythms can be variable and lacking any temporal organiza- tion (Laposky et al., 2008). Embryonic reptiles show circadian rhythms 2.2.1. Asynchrony experiment in metabolic rates during incubation in the absence of environmental Six eggs from each of the 20 clutches were used for this experiment. cues. Circadian rhythms occur in rates of oxygen consumption in groups Asynchrony within clutches of C. longicollis was established in 10 of the of embryonic snakes (Dmi'el, 1969) and in heart rates of turtle embryos clutches at the beginning of the experiment using protocols similar to (Loudon et al., 2013). Metabolic compensation and synchronized circa- Spencer et al. (2001) and Colbert et al. (2010). To establish develop- dian rhythms could suggest that there is communication between em- mental asynchrony, two of the six eggs from each clutch were incubated bryos within a clutch during development, but how individuals at 30 °C and the remaining four eggs were incubated at 26 °C for 8 d. communicate developmental stage is unknown. For eggs that are incu- After 8 d, the four eggs incubated at 26 °C were reunited with the two bating in close contact, there is potential for individuals to detect heart more advanced eggs at 30 °C and positioned so that they were not in beats of siblings and adjust developmental rates accordingly. Prelimi- contact but were within 10 mm of each other. The two more advanced nary data suggest that eggs of E. macquarii incubated in groups have eggs were placed between the four eggs incubated at 26 °C. We refer to less variation in heart rates and incubation periods are significantly this group of eggs as the ‘asynchronous’ group as eggs will be at dif- shorter compared to eggs incubated in isolation (Loudon et al., un- ferent developmental stages (Fig. 1). The remaining 10 clutches were published data). The aim of this study was to further explore the phe- used as the ‘synchronous’ groups and were set up using the same proce- nomenon of ECH that's evidently increasing in reported occurrences. dure, with the two not advanced eggs being incubated at 26 °C for the We aim to identify, if hatching synchrony occurs in the Australian 8 d period. The remaining four eggs were also incubated at 26 °C so chelid, Chelodina longicollis, and the mechanisms utilized (metabolic that all six eggs are still at the same developmental stage and therefore compensation and circadian rhythms). If C. longicollis hatches synchro- synchronized when reunited (Spencer et al., 2001). After eggs were nously through metabolic compensation, it should be evident in the reunited into containers, containers were placed in a 30 °C incubator diel patterns of embryonic heart rates and incubation periods of embry- until development was completed. Each clutch was incubated in os incubated next to more advanced eggs. We also aim to test the effect separate containers throughout the experiment. Heart rates of em- of egg positioning within a nest during incubation. Eggs that are incubated bryos from asynchronous and synchronous groups were monitored in groups will have a shorter incubation period and hatch synchronously weekly and containers were opened every 2–3dtoallowfreshair compared to eggs incubated individually, due to the potential for exchange throughout the incubation period until the commence- embryo–embryo communication. ment of pipping/hatching.

2. Materials and methods 2.2.2. Egg position: Group vs individual incubation experiment Clutches consisting of 7–10 eggs were used in an egg positioning ex- 2.1. Study species periment. Each clutch was incubated in a single container at 26 °C for the first 9 weeks. After 9 weeks, each clutch was divided into a group Chelodina longicollis has a wide distribution in southeastern Australia. of three or four eggs and all remaining eggs were separated into individ- It occupies shallow, transient waterholes, remote water bodies, and ual containers. Grouped eggs were moved into a new container of the 60 J.K. McGlashan et al. / Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology, Part A 188 (2015) 58–64

Fig. 1. Incubation method—Eggs from each clutch were divided and incubated separately for 8 d, then returned to the same containers to continue incubation at 30 °C. Asynchronous group—two more advanced eggs (black) were incubated at 30 °C and four eggs (white) were incubated at 26 °C for 8 d. Synchronous group—two not advanced eggs (black) were incubated at 26 °C and four eggs (white) were incubated at 26 °C for 8 d. All eggs were then placed in 30 °C incubators until the completion of incubation. same size and individuals were each moved into new, smaller con- hatchlings were placed on their back (carapace) and the time taken to tainers (100 × 100 × 50 mm) to complete the final two to 3 weeks of in- right themselves was measured and used as an index of neuromuscular cubation. Containers of grouped and individual eggs were put back into development (Vleck et al., 1979, 1980; Peterson and Kruegl, 2005; 26 °C incubators to complete incubation. Colbertetal.,2010). Righting times were recorded to the nearest second using a digital stopwatch and all tests were stopped at 300 s. Hatchling 2.3. Heart rates: Asynchronous experiment growth in the asynchronous experiment was measured every month for 4 months to assess short-term costs to individuals. Heart rates were taken on the four eggs that were incubated next to the more advanced eggs in the asynchronous groups and the not ad- 2.5. Data analysis vanced eggs in the synchronous groups. All results relating to heart rates will be referring to those four eggs in both asynchronous and syn- A general linear model (GLM) was used to analyze group vs. individ- chronous groups. Heart rates, recorded as beats per minute (bpm), of ual incubation periods in the egg positioning experiment. A GLM was embryos from both asynchronous and synchronous groups were mea- also used to assess incubation periods in the asynchronous experiment sured to determine 1) heart rate profile, 2) circadian rhythm, and to test whether asynchronous or synchronous group incubation regime 3) metabolic compensation. Heart rates were used to infer metabolic affected incubation period and if the asynchronous group hatched syn- rate (Wallace and Jones, 2008; Du et al., 2009) of embryos and identify chronously, using egg mass as a covariate. We compared the incubation any difference between asynchronous and synchronous groups. One of periods of eggs within each clutch in the asynchronous group and syn- the four eggs from each clutch was selected to measure heart rate chronous group to assess if there were differences in incubation periods over 24 h at 4 time intervals (5:00, 11:00, 17:00, and 23:00 h), once a within clutches and therefore if hatching synchrony occurred. Incuba- week. Eggs were removed from the incubation container and immedi- tion period of eggs in asynchronous groups were compared to the syn- ately placed in a Buddy digital egg monitor system (Avian Biotech, En- chronous groups, to determine if asynchronous groups hatched earlier gland) in complete darkness. Buddy monitors provide a non-invasive than expected. To confirm a difference in incubation period that method to sense and amplify cardiovascular signals of an embryo would be expected from the initial asynchrony set-up, the incubation pe- using variations of infrared radiation intensity that is absorbed and riod of the two more advanced eggs in the asynchronous group were reflected by arterial vasculature (Lierz et al., 2006). Eggs were placed compared to the two not advanced eggs in the synchronous group. in monitors and three readings were taken when heart rates stabilized Heart rates of embryos were analyzed using GLM for every week to (within 2 min). Care was taken not to exceed this time due to the poten- test whether the asynchronous treatment affected metabolic rate, with tial heating effect from the Buddy system (Sartori et al., 2015). The egg mass as a covariate. Circadian rhythms were determined through mean of three readings, taken at 30 s intervals, was calculated. Eggs daily heart rate fluctuations, which were calculated as a difference were not disturbed during the recording period and any embryonic from the mean diel rate (bpm) at each time point. Peaks were used to movement could be detected and distinguished from heart rate read- align circadian rhythms with time of day in either asynchronous or ings. Heart rates were measured weekly from week 4 to 9 (earliest synchronous groups and analyzed using an ANOVA. and latest time heart rates could be detected by the monitors). Hatchling mass, morphology, and righting ability were individually analyzed using a GLM with egg mass as a covariate. A binomial test 2.4. Pipping and post hatching measurements was used to assess propensity to flip. All analyses were performed using SAS 9.2 (SAS Institute Inc.). To determine the incubation period, all eggs in both experiments were inspected for signs of pipping (initial break of egg with caruncle) 3. Results twice daily (Gutzke et al., 1984). The incubation period was measured as time in days from oviposition until pipping. 3.1. Heart rate Within 12 h of hatching, neonates were weighed, measured (cara- pace and plastron straight length and width), and performance tests Heart rate (bpm) of embryos from asynchronous and synchronous were undertaken. The residual yolk sac that had not yet been drawn groups decreased from week 4 to 9 of incubation from 93 to 73 bpm into the body through the umbilical fontanel at the time of hatching (21.5 %; t4 = 6.42, p = 0.002) and 95 to 66 bpm (30.5 %; t4 =6.8, was measured and an estimate of the size was calculated using length, p = 0.001), respectively (Fig. 2). Heart rates of asynchronous and syn- width, and height measurements (volume of an ellipsoid). Performance chronous embryos did not differ significantly throughout incubation and coordination of hatchlings were assessed through righting times. All with the exception of week 8 when synchronous group heart rates J.K. McGlashan et al. / Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology, Part A 188 (2015) 58–64 61

were 17.6 % higher than asynchronous embryos (F1,13 = 36.49, p = 6 b0.0001; Fig. 2). From weeks 4 to 6, heart rate of all embryos remained 4 relatively constant 89.5–92.1 bpm, after which a significant decline occurred in both asynchronous and synchronous groups. From week 7 2 a to week 8, asynchronous embryos heart rates dropped by 13.2 bpm b 0 (15.6 %; F1,14 = 4.6, p = 0.0001) and synchronous embryos dropped by 3.6 bpm (4 %) and continued to decline significantly by 19.8 bpm -2 a from weeks 8 to 9 (22.9 %; F1,9 = 61.28, p = 0.00003) (Fig. 2)however, asynchronous embryos increased heart rate by 2.5 bpm in weeks 8–9 -4

(3.4 %). b Circadian rhythms in heart rates were present with a clear peak in -6

Difference from mean heart rate (bpm) -12 -6 Peak 6 heart rates throughout the day (Fig. 3). Individuals showed minimum and maximum heart rates over a 24 h period, but they were not corre- Fig. 3. The difference from mean heart rate of synchronous (dark grey) and asynchronous lated with the time of day in both asynchronous and synchronous (light grey) groups over a 24 h period. Data from weeks 4 to 9 before hatching are com- groups. In general, heart rate of asynchronous groups did not vary great- bined, x-axis represents duration of time (h) away from the peak heart rate and y-axis rep- fi ly from the mean heart rate as did synchronous groups. There was no resents the difference from the mean heart rate (bpm), ±SE, n = 70. a) signi cant difference in heart rate deviation 12 h prior to the peak heart rate between synchronous significant difference in the peak heart rates between groups, suggest- andasynchronousgroups(F1,10 = 4.96, p = 0.03), b) significant difference in heart rate ing that asynchronous groups did not exceed the potential for periods deviation 6 h prior to the peak heart rate between synchronous and asynchronous groups of higher peak daily rate, relative to synchronous groups. In the 6 h lead- (F1,10 = 4.96, p = 0.04). ing up to the maximum rate, there was a greater negative deviation, or fi period of daily ‘resting’, in the synchronous group (M = −4bpm)than development. Yolk sac size differed signi cantly between neonates from the asynchronous group (M = −0.5 bpm) (F = 4.96, p = 0.04) asynchronous and synchronous groups at the time of hatching. Hatchlings 1,10 fi (Fig. 3). Heart rate deviation from the mean was also significantly differ- from the asynchronous groups hatched with signi cantly smaller residual 3 3 ent 12 h before the peak between synchronous groups (M = 0.3 bpm) yolk sacs (30 mm ) than those from synchronous groups (92 mm ) (F1,43 = 4.45, p = 0.04), but there was no difference in yolk sacs between and asynchronous groups (M = −1.6 bpm) (F1,10 = 4.96, p = 0.03), most evident in the diel heart rate profiles in week 5, 7, and 8. neonates within clutches in the asynchronous group (F1,26 = 0.01, p = 0.92) and synchronous group (F1,20 = 0.81, p = 0.38) (Fig. 6). There 3.2. Incubation period was no difference in hatchling mass (F1,43 = 0.47, p = 0.5) and carapace length (F1,43 = 0.83, p = 0.36) between neonatesfromasynchronousand Incubation period did not vary between clutch-mates in both asyn- synchronous groups at the time of hatching and at 4 months; mass chronous and synchronous groups. All eggs within a clutch in the asyn- (F1,15 = 0.87, p = 0.37) and carapace length (F1,15 = 0.10, p = 0.76). chronous groups hatched within 1 d of each other (F = 0.11, p = Performance (as assessed by righting times) was not affected by incu- 1,43 fi 0.74) and all eggs within a clutch in the synchronous group hatched bation treatment. There was no signi cant difference in performance (total time to right) of neonates from asynchronous and synchronous within 3 d of each other (F1,43 = 0.80, p = 0.38). There was a significant difference in incubation periods between eggs in asynchronous and groups at the time of hatching (F1,27 = 0.97, p = 0.33) and after 4 months of growth (F1,41 = 0.00, p = 0.96). There was also no significant difference synchronous groups (4–7d)(F1,43 = 4.00, p = 0.05) and an 8 d dif- ference in the incubation period of the two more advanced eggs from in righting times between hatchlings from asynchronous groups and all the asynchronous groups compared to the two not advanced eggs in other hatchlings from the experiment. Propensity to right was assessed and there was no significant difference between all groups at the time of the synchronous group (F1,43 = 9.62, p = 0.003) (Fig. 4). In the egg position experiment, grouped embryos hatched up to 3 d earlier than hatching (F3,44 = 1.51, p = 0.23) and at 4 months of age (F3,44 =0.24, p = 0.87). No phenotypic differences occurred between grouped em- embryos that were incubated individually (F1,33 = 4.45, p = 0.04) (Fig. 5). bryos and individual embryos in the egg positioning experiment. b 3.3. Hatching and post hatching development 80 a A total of 27 asynchronous and 21 synchronous hatchlings 78 representing all clutches were used to assess hatching and post hatching 76

74 120 b 72 100 a a 70 80 a 68

60 66 Incubation Period (days) 64

Heart Rate (bpm) 40 62 20 60 0 Synchronous Not advanced Asynchronous More advanced Week 4 Week 5 Week 6 Week 7 Week 8 Week 9 eggs eggs

Fig. 2. Mean (M) heart rates (bpm) of embryos in synchronous (dark grey) and asynchro- Fig. 4. Mean incubation period of eggs in synchronous and asynchronous groups, ± SE, nous (light grey) groups: synchronous, M = 90 bpm; asynchronous, M = 84 bpm (week n = 48. a) significant difference between more advanced eggs and not advanced eggs

8: F1,13 = 36.49, p = b0.0001 a) ± SE, n = 70. a) significant difference in heart rates be- (F1,43 = 9.62, p = 0.003), b) significant difference of both synchronous and not advanced tween asynchronous and synchronous groups, n is the total number of eggs used. eggs compared with asynchronous eggs (F1,43 = 4.00, p = 0.05). 62 J.K. McGlashan et al. / Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology, Part A 188 (2015) 58–64

90 the 9 weeks of incubation, similar to Spencer (2012). Metabolic com- pensation in C. longicollis may be stimulated by communication through 89 diel adjustments of heart rate. Circadian rhythms are present in devel- oping C. longicollis embryos, as they are in embryonic E. macquarii 88 (Loudon et al., 2013), despite no environmental influences such as daylight. Heart rates are elevated or ‘peak’ during the day, indicating pe- 87 riods of high developmental activity and drop, indicating periods of low developmental activity or ‘resting’. These rhythms are not synchronized 86 to the time of day among eggs of C. longicollis, and there is no indication of synchrony between individuals when compared from week to week. 85 The mean weekly heart rate can be used as an indicator for metabolic rate of embryos, and deviations from the mean rate throughout the Incubation Period (days) 84 day may indicate positive or negative compensatory changes in embryo metabolism. Asynchronous groups showed less overall deviation from the mean heart rate in relation to synchronous groups and instead 83 remained relatively constant throughout incubation (Fig. 3). In daily pe- riods of ‘rest’ where heart rate drops, asynchronous embryos showed 82 Group Individual less negative deviation, compared to synchronous groups where the drop in heart rate was up to 5 bpm (t10 = 2.99, p = 0.006). Maintaining Fig. 5. Mean incubation period of eggs in groups (dark grey) and individuals (light grey) ± a constant heart rate in these periods of resting throughout incubation is

SE, n = 35, (F1,33 = 4.45, p = 0.04). indicative of metabolic compensation. Changes in relative heart rate are more subtle in asynchronous embryos, as they remain at a constant 4. Discussion higher rate compared to synchronous groups, this is necessary to meta- bolically compensate, hence ‘catch up’ to more advanced embryos and In a natural nest, eggs of C. longicollis take 110–150 d to hatch and facilitate synchronous hatching. emerge (Parmenter, 1985), but they incubate for 62–79 d at a constant Embryonic oxygen consumption and heart rate increase in a sigmoi- temperature of 30 °C and 75–94 d at a constant temperature of 26 °C in dal pattern (having an ‘S’ shape) throughout development in many the laboratory (J. K. McGlashan, unpublished data). Much of this varia- turtles, until they reach a peak several days before hatching and then tion in incubation periods may relate primarily to complex interactions decrease steadily (Thompson, 1989; Booth, 2000; Booth and Astill, between siblings within individual nests. Here we show that eggs of 2001; Du et al., 2010b; McGlashan et al., 2012). In many precocial ani- C. longicollis hatch synchronously and embryos are able to metabolically mals, this decline following the peak is thought of as a ‘resting stage’. compensate and catch up to more advanced clutch-mates by remark- The resting stage is one where tissue growth is essentially complete ably reducing incubation periods by up to 7 d (Fig. 4). Group environ- and the sensory, neuromuscular, and thermoregulatory systems ma- ment also influences hatching times, with eggs incubated in groups ture. Referred to as secondary development, it is an important stage hatching up to 3 d earlier than eggs incubated in isolation (Fig. 5). for species with little to no parental care post hatching (Vleck et al., Hatching synchrony in reptiles can occur either by delaying hatching 1979; Peterson and Kruegl, 2005). The heart rate of embryos of (eg. C. insculpta (Webb et al., 1986; Doody et al., 2001)), hatching C. longicollis remains stable from weeks 4 to 7, a pattern that is seen in prematurely (eg. C. picta (Colbert et al., 2010)) or through metabolic semi-precocial birds (Cain et al., 1967; Tazawa et al., 1991a,b; Tazawa, compensation (eg. Eastern three-lined skink (Bassiana duperreyi)(Du 2005). In week 8 (asynchronous) and week 9 (synchronous), heart et al., 2010a) and embryonic E. macquarii (McGlashan et al., 2012)). rate declines significantly (Fig. 2), indicating a potential resting stage. Neonatal C. longicollis show a level of developmental plasticity that This marked fall in metabolic rate has also been shown in embryos of allows them to hatch synchronously, but we found no significant evi- C. insculpta (Webb et al., 1986), E. macquarii, and the American alligator dence of metabolic compensation in mean daily heart rate throughout (Alligator mississipiensis), 80–90% of the way through incubation (Thompson, 1989). The embryos of the more advanced eggs from the

120 a asynchronous group were 8 d more advanced than the embryos of the not advanced eggs of the synchronous groups. The significant decline in heart rate by the asynchronous embryos is potentially a result of em- 100 bryos responding to the developmental rates of more advanced eggs in the clutch and therefore beginning their resting stage, in this case, a ) 3 week earlier than expected, relative to synchronous groups. Both asyn- 80 a chronous and synchronous groups began their resting stage 80–85% of the way through incubation. The asynchronous group, as a result of 60 being incubated with more advanced eggs, shifted their resting stage to coincide with the more advanced eggs, ensuring enough time to com- plete secondary development with no developmental consequences. 40 Embryos of the asynchronous group hatched up to 7 d earlier than the

Yolk Sac Volume (mm synchronous group, assumed to be at the same stage, thus hatching syn- chronously with clutch-mates. 20 The cost of metabolic compensation during development may relate to increased yolk reserve utilization. Certain incubation conditions that 0 demand increased developmental rates or metabolic compensation Synchronous Not advanced Asynchronous More advanced during development often results in greater utilization of yolk reserves eggs eggs (Bhan et al., 2002; Radder et al., 2002, 2004; Van Dyke et al., 2011). Re- sidual yolk at hatching is a result of left-over yolk from incubation but is Fig. 6. Mean yolk sac size (mm3) of synchronous (dark grey) and asynchronous (light grey) hatchlings, ± SE, n = 48. a) significant difference between asynchronous and used for post hatching energy supply and growth (Tucker et al., 1998; synchronousgroups(F1,43 = 4.45, p = 0.04). Radder et al., 2004; Van Dyke et al., 2011). Evidence of metabolic J.K. McGlashan et al. / Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology, Part A 188 (2015) 58–64 63 compensation is apparent in diel heart rates of embryos (Fig. 3)andis Bhan, S., Singh, I., Pandian, V., 2002. Pattern of yolk internalization by hatchlings is related fi to breeding timing in the garden lizard, Calotes versicolor. Curr. Sci. 82, 1484. also supported by asynchronous group hatchlings having signi cantly Birchard, G., Reiber, C., 1996. Heart rate during development in the turtle embryo: effect smaller residual yolk volumes than synchronous groups (Fig. 6). Yolk of temperature. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. B Biochem. Mol. Biol. 166, 461–466. sac sizes within the asynchronous group and the synchronous group Booth, D.T., 1998. Incubation of turtle eggs at different temperatures: do embryos fi compensate for temperature during development? Physiol. Zool. 71, 23–26. are not signi cantly different between hatchlings so asynchronous em- Booth, D.T., 2000. Incubation of eggs of the Australian broad-shelled turtle, Chelodina bryos utilized the same amount of yolk during the period of increased expansa (Testudinata : Chelidae), at different temperatures: effects on pattern of demand as the more advanced eggs did at the beginning of incubation oxygen consumption and hatchling morphology. Aust. J. Zool. 48, 369–378. when asynchrony was established (Fig. 1). E. macquarii embryos catab- Booth, D.T., Astill, K., 2001. Incubation temperature, energy expenditure and hatchling size in the green turtle (Chelonia mydas), a species with temperature-sensitive sex olize yolk reserves to increase metabolism during incubation, resulting determination. Aust. J. Zool. 49, 389–396. in smaller residual yolk sacs at hatching and no difference in hatchling Bull, J.J., 1985. Sex determining mechanisms: an evolutionary perspective. Experientia 41, – size (McGlashan et al., 2012). Chelodina longicollis hatchlings that 1285 1296. Bull, J.J., Vogt, R.C., 1979. Temperature-dependent sex determination in turtles. Science hatch with smaller yolk sacs are not phenotypically disadvantaged in 206, 1186–1188. regards to size and performance at the time of hatching and up to 4 Cain, J.R., Abbott, U.K., Rogallo, V.L., 1967. Heart rate of the developing chick embryo. Proc. months post hatching, indicating that development is completed. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med. 126, 507–510. Cann, J., 1998. Australian Freshwater Turtles. Beaumont publishing, Singapore. Nests are versatile environments for developing embryos. Until re- Chessman, B.C., 1988. Habitat preferences of freshwater turtles in the Murray Valley, cently, it was believed that the microenvironment of a nest, such as tem- Victoria and New South Wales. Aust. Wildl. Res. 15, 485–491. perature and humidity, was the primary factor that determined how Colbert, P.L., Spencer, R.-J., Janzen, F.J., 2010. Mechanism and cost of synchronous hatch- ing. Funct. Ecol. 24, 112–121. reptiles and embryos grow and develop, but it is apparent that reptilian Deeming, D.C., Ferguson, M.W.J., 1991. Egg incubation: Its effects on embryonic develop- embryogenesis is far more complex than previously assumed. The ment in birds and reptiles. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. mechanisms of ECH in turtles are still poorly understood, and although Dmi'el, R., 1969. Circadian rhythm of oxygen consumption in snake embryos. Life Sci. 8, 1333–1341. temperature and nest ecology can impact on incubation length and Doody, J.S., 2011. Environmentally cued hatching in reptiles. Integr. Comp. Biol. 51, 49–61. development of ectothermic embryos, there is evidence of plasticity in Doody, J.S., Georges, A., Young, J.E., Pauza, M.D., Pepper, A.L., Alderman, R.L., Welsh, M.A., development (West-Eberhard, 1989, 2003; Deeming and Ferguson, 2001. Embryonic aestivation and emergence behaviour in the pig-nosed turtle, – 1991; Packard and Packard, 2000; Johnston and Temple, 2002; Price Carettochelys insculpta. Can. J. Zool. 79, 1062 1072. Doody, J.S., Stewart, B., Camacho, C., Christian, K., 2012. Good vibrations? Sibling embryos et al., 2003; Doody, 2011). It is not known how C. longicollis and expedite hatching in a turtle. Anim. Behav. 83, 645–651. E. macquarii accelerate development, but heart rates are not fixed and Du, W.-G., Radder, R.S., Sun, B., Shine, R., 2009. Determinants of incubation period: do rep- fi environmental conditions merely determine the limits of heart rate tilian embryos hatch after a xed total number of heart beats? J. Exp. Biol. 212, 1302–1306. and thus developmental rate (McGlashan et al., 2012; Loudon et al., Du, W.-G., Thompson, M.B., Shine, R., 2010a. Facultative cardiac responses to regional 2013). Synchronous hatching has evolved in many aquatic turtles with hypoxia in lizard embryos. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A Mol. Integr. Physiol. 156, many abiotic and biotic factors serving as the cues (Webb et al., 1986; 491–494. Du, W.-G., Zhao, B., Shine, R., 2010b. Embryos in the fast lane: high-temperature heart Thompson, 1989; Lindeman, 1991; Packard et al., 1997; Doody et al., rates of turtles decline after hatching. PLoS One 5. 2001; Spencer et al., 2001; Packard and Packard, 2004; Tucker et al., Du, W.-G., Ye, H., Zhao, B., Pizzatto, L., Ji, X., Shine, R., 2011. Patterns of interspecific 2008; Colbert et al., 2010; Spencer and Janzen, 2011). Synchronous variation in the heart rates of embryonic reptiles. PLoS One 6. – Gomez-Mestre, I., Wiens, J.J., Warkentin, K.M., 2008. Evolution of adaptive plasticity: risk- hatching is not a passive action, it is driven by embryo embryo commu- sensitive hatching in neotropical leaf-breeding treefrogs. Ecol. Monogr. 78, 205–224. nication in the nest. It is not clear how embryos are communicating, but Gutzke, W.H., Paukstis, G.L., Packard, G.C., 1984. Pipping versus hatching as indices of time it is apparent that the final 2 weeks of incubation are important for signal- of incubation in reptiles. J. Herpetol. 494–496. Hoyt, D.F., 1979. Osmoregulation by avian embryos: the allantois functions like a toad's ing and responding to cues in the nest as evident in both the asynchro- bladder. Physiol. Zool. 52, 354–362. nous and egg position experiment. Egg positioning in the nest Janzen, F.J., 1993. The influence of incubation temperature and family on eggs, embryos, potentially facilitates communication between embryos, and this may and hatchlings of the smooth softshell turtle (Apalone mutica). Physiol. Zool. 66, – be due to auditory cues from embryos or vibrations from heart beats dur- 349 373. Johnston, I.A., Temple, G.K., 2002. Thermal plasticity of skeletal muscle phenotype in ecto- ing development. thermic vertebrates and its significance for locomotory behaviour. J. Exp. Biol. 205, Thermal and phenotypic plasticity of embryos, communication be- 2305–2322. tween embryos within the nest, and most recently, embryonic circadian Kennett, R., 1999. Reproduction of two species of freshwater turtle, Chelodina rugosa and Elseya dentata, from the wet–dry tropics of northern Australia. J. Zool. 247, 457–473. rhythms have all been discovered in recent years (McGlashan et al., Kennett, R., Roe, J., Hodges, K., Georges, A., 2009. Chelodina longicollis (Shaw 1794): East- 2012; Loudon et al., 2013). The mechanism and evolutionary purpose ern long-necked turtle, common long-necked turtle, common snake-necked turtle. that stimulates and enables early/premature synchronous hatching Chelonian Res. Monogr. 5, 31.1–31.8. Laposky, A.D., Bass, J., Kohsaka, A., Turek, F.W., 2008. Sleep and circadian rhythms: key could vary among species of turtles, but it is evident that cues that components in the regulation of energy metabolism. FEBS Lett. 582, 142–151. occur within a nest are capable of altering a very important stage in Lierz, M., Gooss, O., Hafez, H.M., 2006. Noninvasive heart rate measurement using a digital the life history of these animals to allow synchronous hatching, thereby egg monitor in chicken and turkey embryos. J. Avian Med. Surg. 20, 141–146. Lindeman, P.V., 1991. Survivorship of overwintering hatchling painted turtles, Chrysemys maximizing the chance of survival for the individual. picta, in northern Idaho. Can. Field Nat. 105, 263–266. Loudon, F.K., Spencer, R.-J., Strassmeyer, A., Harland, K., 2013. Metabolic circadian Acknowledgements rhythms in embryonic turtles. Integr. Comp. Biol. 53, 175–182. McGlashan, J.K., Spencer, R.-J., Old, J.M., 2012. Embryonic communication in the nest: metabolic responses of reptilian embryos to developmental rates of siblings. Proc. Research was supported by a UWS SSH Equipment Fund Grant and R. Soc. Lond. B 279, 1709–1715. was conducted under UWS ACEC no. A9300, NSW NPWS Permit Monaghan, P., 2008. Early growth conditions, phenotypic development and environmen- – SL101065. Karen Harland helped capture turtles and collect eggs. Re- tal change. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 363, 1635 1645. Packard, G.C., Packard, M.J., 1988. The physiological ecology of reptilian eggs and embryos. search was conducted in the Evolution and Developmental Laboratory In: Gans, C., Huey, R.B. (Eds.), Biology of the Reptilia. Wiley, New York. and K1 Aquatic Facility at the University of Western Sydney. Packard, G.C., Packard, M.J., 2000. Developmental plasticity in painted turtles, Chrysemys picta. Funct. Ecol. 14, 474–483. Packard, G.C., Packard, M.J., 2004. To freeze or not to freeze: adaptations for overwintering References by hatchlings of the North American painted turtle. J. Exp. Biol. 207, 2897–2906. Packard, G.C., Fasano, S.L., Attaway, M.B., Lohmiller, L.D., Lynch, T.L., 1997. Thermal envi- Ar, A., Tazawa, H., 1999. Analysis of heart rate in developing bird embryos: effects of ronment for overwintering hatchlings of the painted turtle (Chrysemys picta). Can. developmental mode and mass. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A Mol. Integr. Physiol. J. Zool. 75, 401–406. 124, 491–500. Parmenter, C.J., 1985. Reproduction and survivorship of Chelodina longicollis (Testudinata: Beck, R., 1991. The common long-necked tortoise Chelodina longicollis (Shaw 1802) Chelidae). The biology of Australasian frogs and reptilespp. 53–61. (Testudines: Chelidae): a comparative study of the morphology and behaviour of dis- Pearson, J.T., Tazawa, H., 1999. Development of cardiac rhythms in altricial avian embryos. junct populations. S. Aust. Nat. 66, 4–21. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A Mol. Integr. Physiol. 124, 475–482. 64 J.K. McGlashan et al. / Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology, Part A 188 (2015) 58–64

Peterson, C.C., Kruegl, A., 2005. Peaked temporal pattern of embryonic metabolism in an Thompson, M.B., 1997. Egg physiology and biology. The Biology, Husbandry and emydid turtle (Chrysemys picta picta). J. Herpetol. 39, 678–681. Healthcare of Reptile. TFH Publications Inc., New Jersey. Price, T.D., Qvarnström, A., Irwin, D.E., 2003. The role of phenotypic plasticity in driving Tucker, J.K., Filoramo, N.I., Paukstis, G.L., Janzen, F.J., 1998. Residual yolk in captive and genetic evolution. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 270, 1433–1440. wild-caught hatchlings of the red-eared slider turtle (Trachemys scripta elegans). Radder, R.S., Shanbhag, B.A., Saidapur, S.K., 2002. Pattern of yolk internalization by hatch- Copeia 1998, 488–492. lings is related to breeding timing in the garden lizard, Calotes versicolor. Curr. Sci. 82, Tucker, J.K., Paukstis, G.L., Janzen, F.J., 2008. Does predator swamping promote synchronous 1484–1486. emergence of turtle hatchlings among nests? Behav. Ecol. 19, 35–40. Radder, R.S., Shanbhag, B.A., Saidapur, S.K., 2004. Yolk partitioning in embryos of the liz- Van Dyke, J.U., Plummer, M.V., Beaupre, S.J., 2011. Residual yolk energetics and postnatal ard, Calotes versicolor: maximize body size or save energy for later use? J. Exp. Zool. A shell growth in smooth softshell turtles, Apalone mutica. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A Comp. Exp. Biol. 301A, 783–785. Mol. Integr. Physiol. 158, 37–46. Sartori, M.R., Taylor, E.W., Abe, A.S., Crossley, D.A., 2015. An appraisal of the use of an in- Vleck, C.M., Hoyt, D.F., Vleck, D., 1979. Metabolism of avian embryos: patterns in altricial frared digital monitoring system for long-term measurement of heart rate in reptilian and precocial birds. Physiol. Zool. 363–377. embryos. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A Mol. Integr. Physiol. 188, 17–21. Vleck, D., Vleck, C.M., Hoyt, D.F., 1980. Metabolism of avian embryos: ontogeny of oxygen Spencer, R.J., 2012. Embryonic heart rate and hatching behavior of a solitary nesting consumption in the rhea and emu. Physiol. Zool. 53, 125–135. turtle. J. Zool. 287, 169–174. Wallace, B.P., Jones, T.T., 2008. What makes marine turtles go: a review of metabolic rates Spencer, R.-J., Janzen, F.J., 2011. Hatching behavior in turtles. Integr. Comp. Biol. 51, and their consequences. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 356, 8–24. 100–110. Warkentin, K.M., Calswell, M.S., 2009. Assessing risk: embryos, information, and escape Spencer, R.-J., Thompson, M.B., Banks, P.B., 2001. Hatch or wait? A dilemma in reptilian hatching. In: Dukas, R., Ratcliffe, J. (Eds.), Cognitive ecology II. The evolutionary ecol- incubation. Oikos 93, 401–406. ogy of learning, memory, and information use. University of Chicago Press, Chicago. Tazawa, H., 2005. Cardiac rhythms in avian embryos and hatchlings. Avian Poult. Biol. Rev. Webb, G.J.W., Choquenot, D., Whitehead, P.J., 1986. Nests, eggs, and embryonic develop- 16, 123–150. ment of Carettochelys insculpta (Chelonia: Carettochelidae) from Northern Australia. Tazawa, H., Hiraguchi, T., Kuroda, O., Tullett, S., Deeming, D., 1991a. Embryonic heart rate J. Zool. 1, 521–550. during development of domesticated birds. Physiol. Zool. 1002–1022. West-Eberhard, M.J., 1989. Phenotypic plasticity and the origins of diversity. Annu. Rev. Tazawa, H., Kuroda, O., Whittow, G.C., 1991b. Noninvasive determination of embryonic Ecol. Syst. 20, 249–278. heart rate during hatching in the brown noddy (Anous stolidus). Auk 594–601. West-Eberhard, M.J., 2003. Developmental Plasticity and Evolution. Oxford University Thompson, M., 1983. Populations of the Murray River tortoise, Emydura (Chelodina): the Press, Oxford. effect of egg predation by the red fox, Vulpes vulpes. Wildl. Res. 10, 363–371. Yntema, C.L., 1968. A series of stages in the embryonic development of Chelydra Thompson, M.B., 1988. Nest temperatures in the pleurodiran turtle, Emydura macquarii. serpentina. J. Morphol. 125, 219–251. Copeia 1988, 996–1000. Yntema, C.L., 1978. Incubation times for eggs of the turtle Chelydra serpentina (Testudines: Thompson, M.B., 1989. Patterns of metabolism in embryonic reptiles. Respir. Physiol. 76, Chelydridae) at various temperatures. Herpetologica 34, 274–277. 243–255.