Mating Behavior of the Common Loon

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Mating Behavior of the Common Loon MATING BEHAVIOR OF THE COMMON LOON D. JEAN TATE AND JAMES TATE, JR. HUXLEY (1923) describescopulation and somedisplays for the Red- throated Loon (Gavia stellata), and Zedlitz (1913) gives similar informa- tion for the Arctic Loon (Gavia arctica). A variety of displayshave been described(Roberts, 1932; Munro, 1945; Yeates, 1950) for the Common Loon (Gavia iramet), but their sequenceand significanceare not clear. Other than Southern's(1961) singlereport of an apparentmidsummer copulationby two CommonLoons, no descriptionof copulationseems to have been reported for this species. In their detailed study of the Common Loon on its nesting grounds,Olson and Marshall (1952) never observedcopulation and suggestit possiblyoccurs at night. We watchedtwo CommonLoons attempt to copulateon the water of French' Farm Lake, Emmet County, Michigan, at about 17:30 on 19 April 1968. By this date the lake had been free of ice for only a week to 10 days; thus our observationof the pair was soonafter their arrival. We returned on 20 and 21 April (a weekend), but found the loons so disturbedby fishermenthat observationwas difficult. On 22 April from 16:30 to 19:00, we were able to watch the pair without interruption through a 15-60X telescope,and 8 X 40 binoculars. The birds were unawareof our presenceduring this time, and we saw them copulateon land. Initially, our attentionwas attractedon 19 April by a loon poisedup- right on the water, splashingand flappingits wingsjust beyondmarsh vegetationmore than 200 yards away from us. We then saw two loons swim out into open water, apparently fishing. Some 10 or 15 minutes later one loon swamup to the other, which had turned on its left side in a normal rolling preenposition (Olson and Marshall, 1952: 16, Figure 6) with its breast and belly exposedand head clear of th.ewater. The male (sexesassumed from copulatoryrole) swamup onto the female'sexposed belly at nearly a right angleto her. Contactwas very brief; the female submergedand the male rolled off to his left. The male faced away from the female and engagedin a wing-flappingact (cf. Johnsgard,1964), which exposedan expanseof white belly and breast visible for some distance. Each of the severalwing flaps sprayedwater, and the rapid return of his breast to the water also produceda splash. He then sub- merged in front of the female until only his head and neck were visible. Meanwhilethe femaleemerged to nearly a normallevel. Unlike this hasty encounter,about 30 minutesof relaxed swimming, •A contributionfrom the University of Michigan BiologicalStation. 125 The Auk, 87: 125-130. January 1970 126 TAt• ANDTATE [Auk, Vol. 87 c Figure1. Threeprecopulatory displays of the CommonLoon. A, bill-dipping,a mutualdisplay; B, head-rubbing;C, head-tossing,a mutual display. feeding,and a distinctbout of displayingpreceded the two attemptsat copulationon 22 April. The actionswe watchedon this date are described below in approximateorder of occurrence. PRELIMINARY DISPLAYS Bill-dipping.--Whenthe two birdsfirst cametogether, both engaged in bill-dipping (Figure 1A). Incliningtheir headsforward on stiffly archednecks, first at random then in unison,both birds raisedtheir bills out of the water, then immersedthem to eyelevel. The birdskept 1-2 feet apart and closelysynchronized their movements. They performedfrom 10-20 mutual dips during each session.The black-and-white flutedcollar flashed prominently at the backof the neckduring this display. The stiflened neck and stylized motions, as well as the mutual nature of this displaydistinguish it from peering(Olson and Marshall,1952: 16, Figure 6). Jan. 1970] CommonLoon Mating Behavior 127 Huxley (1923: 256) refers to bill-dippingin the Red-throatedLoon as the snake- ceremony,and he also notesits similarity to peering. Simultaneous-dive.--On the two occasions when this occurred, it immediately followed mutual bill-dipping. At the height of bill-dipping, as if on a signal, both birds threw their headsup, forward, and dove quickly. On one occasionthey faced in the same direction; on the other they faced each other when they dove. The flowing, gracefuldive and the perfect synchronizationof this display impressedus. Immediately upon emergencethey engagedin a bill-dipping ceremony. A period of vigorouspreening followed. Both birds preened,but the actionswere not synchronizednor were all the movementsclearly display elements.During this period,we interpretedtwo actsas displays,largely because of their stereotypedform. Head-rubbing.--Asin Figure lB, both sexespointed the bill vertically upward so that the head lay on the back betweenthe shouldersof the closedwings. Each bird rubbed its head on its back in a movement similar to that of oiling. The fluted black-and-white anterior throat patch was prominent during this display. Headqtossing.--Thethroat patch was also prominent during head-tossing(Figure 1C). The performingbird raisedits bill abovethe horizontalplane quickly,returning to horizontal more slowly. On some occasionsthis was done mutually but not synchronously;at other times it was an individual display. From three to seven head-tosseswere involved in a sequence. COPULATION The extendedperiods of preeninglasted about 5 and 7 minutes respectively.One or the other bird interruptedits preeningand began a long surfaceswim that endedat a placesuitable for copulation. We found we could differentiate between the two birds consistentlyby their body posture. One bird (male, based on later role in copulation) rode higherin the water, its neck pulled back farther with more white breast exposed,its neck more curved (exposingmore prominentlythe necklaceon th'eback of the neck), and its bill tilted abovethe horizontal (Figure 2A). The otherbird (female) with its breastdeeper in the water, straighterneck, and slightly downward-tiltedbill, seemedsubmissive (Figure2B). Its plumageappeared duller (probablyfrom sleekingof the feathers)at thesetimes. In contrast,the two were equallybright when they were fishingseparately. The differentialposturing began only after th'ebirds came together,dove simultaneously,and beganinteracting as a pair. When swimmingtoward the northeast,the male was first, but about halfway along a channelon the east shore he slowedand the female passedhim. She alternatelymoved ahead and was caughtup to by the male. About 300 yards up th'echannel she climbedout of the water onto a wet platform of aquatic vegetationon a small island and turned onto her left side with her head at least partially erect and facing anteriorly. The male followedimmediately and slid up againsther, his feet under her and his cloacapressed against hers. The entire contactlasted about 9 128 TATs At•) TATE [Auk, Vol. 87 Figure 2. A, erect swimming posture of the Common Loon; B, swimming posture of the fenhale Common Loon when sexually interacting with her mate. seconds,after which the male pivotedoff behindthe femaleand returned to the water in the same direction from which he had come. There he held his neckand head stiffly and dippedhis bill. The femalelay quietly for about 60 secondsand then joined the male. During the next 30-40 minutesthe birds remainedconstantly together, diving and swimming. They pulled plant materialsfrom shallowwater and placed them on a nest platform some distancefrom the copulation platform. They did considerablemutual bill-dipping and head-shaking, but no rolling preeningor head-tossing.Both birds spent some time in peeringwith the head partially submerged. Bill-dipping,a simultaneous-dive,head-rubbing, head-tossing, and preen- ing precededanother attempt at copulation.In this casethe female led the way to the northeastup a channelalong the far shore. We lost sight of them for about 2 minutesbehind a low hummockof vegetation.Copula- tion probably did not occurbehind this hummockbecause we would have seen the male mount the female. After coming back into view the male attempted to copulate. The female ran aground against a hummock, but was unable to climb up becauseof dense shrubs. The male approached Jan. 1970] Common Loon Mating Behavior 129 her threetimes but eachtime backedoff. Beforebacking off, he immersed his bill and threw water laterally upwardin an apparentdisplacement act. The movementwas similar to the head-shakeof waterfowl (Mc- Kinney, 1965: 135) exceptthat the bill was in the water when the head was thrown to the side. Becauseof the tangledvegetation the female did not turn on her sideand the male couldnot approachher: he grounded himselfagainst the tangleseveral feet away from her. Within lessthan a minute both birds swamoff togetherdipping their bills. It seemedas if the female missedthe copulationplatform and was unable to find a suitable place to turn on her side in solicitation. DISCUSSION Before the male attempted to copulate,the female first exposedher white belly in solicitation.In the copulationattempt on 19 April, the femalewas engagedin a normal rolling preen on the water, a common maintenancemovement, when the male attempted to mount her. In the completecopulation we watchedon land 2 dayslater, the female'ssolicita- tion act was very similarto the rollingpreen. On 19 April the threshold of the male's responseto solicitationmight have been so low that the similarrolling preen act elicitedthe copulatoryresponse. The swimmingposture of the malewhen with the female,as well as the mutualdisplays of bill-dippingand head-rubbing,appear to enhancethe prominenceof the necklaceposteriorly and anteriorly,and/or the promi- nenceof the white breast. Thus ritualized preening,comfort movements, and foragingbehavior
Recommended publications
  • Buzzle – Zoology Terms – Glossary of Biology Terms and Definitions Http
    Buzzle – Zoology Terms – Glossary of Biology Terms and Definitions http://www.buzzle.com/articles/biology-terms-glossary-of-biology-terms-and- definitions.html#ZoologyGlossary Biology is the branch of science concerned with the study of life: structure, growth, functioning and evolution of living things. This discipline of science comprises three sub-disciplines that are botany (study of plants), Zoology (study of animals) and Microbiology (study of microorganisms). This vast subject of science involves the usage of myriads of biology terms, which are essential to be comprehended correctly. People involved in the science field encounter innumerable jargons during their study, research or work. Moreover, since science is a part of everybody's life, it is something that is important to all individuals. A Abdomen: Abdomen in mammals is the portion of the body which is located below the rib cage, and in arthropods below the thorax. It is the cavity that contains stomach, intestines, etc. Abscission: Abscission is a process of shedding or separating part of an organism from the rest of it. Common examples are that of, plant parts like leaves, fruits, flowers and bark being separated from the plant. Accidental: Accidental refers to the occurrences or existence of all those species that would not be found in a particular region under normal circumstances. Acclimation: Acclimation refers to the morphological and/or physiological changes experienced by various organisms to adapt or accustom themselves to a new climate or environment. Active Transport: The movement of cellular substances like ions or molecules by traveling across the membrane, towards a higher level of concentration while consuming energy.
    [Show full text]
  • Tie-Up Cycles in Long-Term Mating. Part I: Theory
    challenges Article Tie-Up Cycles in Long-Term Mating. Part I: Theory Lorenza Lucchi Basili 1,† and Pier Luigi Sacco 2,3,*,† 1 Independent Researcher, 20 Chestnut Street, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA; [email protected] 2 Department of Romance Languages and Literatures, Harvard University, Boylston Hall, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA 3 Department of Comparative Literature and Language Sciences, IULM University, via Carlo Bo, 1, Milan 20143, Italy * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +1-617-496-0486 † These authors contributed equally to this work. Academic Editor: Palmiro Poltronieri Received: 26 February 2016; Accepted: 26 April 2016; Published: 3 May 2016 Abstract: In this paper, we propose a new approach to couple formation and dynamics that abridges findings from sexual strategies theory and attachment theory to develop a framework where the sexual and emotional aspects of mating are considered in their strategic interaction. Our approach presents several testable implications, some of which find interesting correspondences in the existing literature. Our main result is that, according to our approach, there are six typical dynamic interaction patterns that are more or less conducive to the formation of a stable couple, and that set out an interesting typology for the analysis of real (as well as fictional, as we will see in the second part of the paper) mating behaviors and dynamics. Keywords: sexual strategies; emotional attachment; mating; couple formation and dynamics; Tie-Up; Active vs. Receptive Areas; frustration and reward; Tie-Up Cycle; flow inversion 1. Introduction The process of reproductive mating is a clear example of a complex socio-biological phenomenon, of paramount evolutionary importance.
    [Show full text]
  • Breeding of the Leach's Storm-Petrel Oceanodroma Leucorhoa at Santa Catalina Island, California
    Carter et al.: Leach’s Storm-Petrel at Santa Catalina Island 83 BREEDING OF THE LEACH’S STORM-PETREL OCEANODROMA LEUCORHOA AT SANTA CATALINA ISLAND, CALIFORNIA HARRY R. CARTER1,3,4, TYLER M. DVORAK2 & DARRELL L. WHITWORTH1,3 1California Institute of Environmental Studies, 3408 Whaler Avenue, Davis, CA 95616, USA 2Catalina Island Conservancy, 125 Clarissa Avenue, Avalon, CA 90704, USA 3Humboldt State University, Department of Wildlife, 1 Harpst Street, Arcata, CA 95521, USA 4Current address: Carter Biological Consulting, 1015 Hampshire Road, Victoria, BC V8S 4S8, Canada ([email protected]) Received 4 November 2015, accepted 5 January 2016 Among the California Channel Islands (CCI) off southern California, Guadalupe Island, off central-west Baja California (Ainley 1980, the Ashy Storm-Petrel Oceanodroma homochroa (ASSP) is the Power & Ainley 1986, Ainley 2005, Pyle 2008, Howell et al. most numerous and widespread breeding storm-petrel; it is known 2009). Alternatively, these egg specimens may have been from to breed at San Miguel, Santa Cruz, Anacapa, Santa Barbara, and dark-rumped LESP, which are known to breed at the Coronado San Clemente islands (Hunt et al. 1979, 1980; Sowls et al. 1980; and San Benito islands, Baja California (Ainley 1980, Power & Carter et al. 1992, 2008; Harvey et al. 2016; Fig. 1B; Appendix 1, Ainley 1986). available on the website). Low numbers of Black Storm-Petrels O. melania (BLSP) also breed at Santa Barbara Island (Pitman Within this context, we asked the following questions: (1) Were the & Speich 1976; Hunt et al. 1979, 1980; Carter et al. 1992; 1903 egg records the first breeding records of LESP at Catalina and Appendix 1).
    [Show full text]
  • Human Mating Strategies Human Mating Strategies
    Human Mating Strategies Human Mating Strategies As descendants of a long line of successful maters, modern humans have inherited the mating strategies that led to their forebear’s success. These include long-term mating, short-term mating, and mixed mating strategies. This article presents empirical evi- dence supporting evolution-based hypotheses about the complexities of these mating strategies, which differ substantially for men and women. array of adaptations specifically dedicated to the David M. Buss, Professor, task of mating. Department of Psychology, Nowhere do people have an equal desire to mate University of Texas, Austin with all people. Everywhere, some people are pre- ferred as mates, others shunned. Desires are central to all facets of mating. They determine who we are attracted to, and who is attracted to us. They influ- ence which attraction tactics will be successful (those that fulfill desires) and which attraction tac- tics will fail (those that violate desires). Successful mate retention tactics involve continuing to provide resources that fulfill the desires of a mate. Failure to Perhaps no adaptive domain is more central to re- fulfill these desires causes breakup and divorce. At production than mating. Those in our evolutionary every step of the mating process, from mate selec- past who failed to mate failed to become ancestors. tion to mate expulsion, desires determine the Modern humans are all descendants of a long and ground rules. unbroken line of ancestors who succeeded in the complex and sometimes circuitous tasks involved in Sexual Selection and Parental Investment mating. As their descendants, modern humans have Although Charles Darwin (1859) recognized that inherited the adaptations that led to the success of survival was central to the evolutionary process, their ancestors.
    [Show full text]
  • Reproductive Aging and Mating: the Ticking of the Biological Clock in Female Cockroaches
    Reproductive aging and mating: The ticking of the biological clock in female cockroaches Patricia J. Moore* and Allen J. Moore School of Biological Sciences, University of Manchester, Oxford Road, Manchester M13 9PT, United Kingdom Edited by David B. Wake, University of California, Berkeley, CA, and approved June 5, 2001 (received for review March 30, 2001) Females are expected to have different mating preferences be- reproductive state? Few empirical studies have addressed cause of the variation in costs and benefits of mate choice both these questions. Lea et al. (15) present evidence that the between females and within individual females over a lifetime. consistency of mate preference in midwife toads, presumably Workers have begun to look for, and find, the expected variation reflecting a high motivation to mate, is greatest in ovulating among females in expressed mating preferences. However, vari- females. Kodric-Brown and Nicoletto (16) find that older ation within females caused by changes in intrinsic influences has female guppies are less choosy than when they are younger not been examined in detail. Here we show that reproductive even if still virgin. Likewise, Gray (17) demonstrated that older aging caused by delayed mating resulted in reduced choosiness by female house crickets show no significant preference for the female Nauphoeta cinerea, a cockroach that has reproductive calls of attractive males compared with young females. cycles and gives live birth. Male willingness to mate was unaf- An essential factor in considering the effect of reproductive fected by variation in female age. Females who were beyond the state on the expression of female mate choice is to show that in optimal mating age, 6 days postadult molt, required considerably fact there is variation in the costs associated with mate choice less courtship than their younger counterparts.
    [Show full text]
  • Courtship & Mating Reproduction in Insects
    Reproduction Courtship & Mating in Insects • How do the sexes find each other? – Light – Swarming (male only/ female only) – Leks (male aggregations) • Defend territory against males • Court arriving females – Pheromones What do they do once they find each other? Courtship • Close range intersexual behavior that induces sexual receptivity before and during mating. • Allows mate choice among and within species. 1 Types of Courtship • Visual displays Nuptial Gifts • Ritualized movements • 3 forms • Sound production – Cannibalization of males • Tactile stimulation – Glandular product • Nuptial gifts – Nuptial gift • Prey • Salt, nutrients Evolution of nuptial feeding Sexual Cannibalization • Female advantages • Rather extreme – Nutritional benefit • Male actually does not – Mate choice (mate with good provider) willingly give himself • Male advantages up… – Helping provision/produce his offspring – Where would its potential – Female returns sperm while feeding rather than reproductive benefit be? mating with someone else • Do females have • Male costs increased reproductive – Capturing food costs energy and incurs predation success? risk – Prey can be stolen and used by another male. 2 Glandular gifts Nuptial gifts • Often part of the spermatophore (sperm transfer unit) – Occupy female while sperm is being transferred – Parental investment by male • Generally a food item (usually prey) • Also regurgitations (some flies) • But beware the Cubic Zirconia, ladies Sexual selection Types of sexual selection • Intrasexual selection – Contest competition
    [Show full text]
  • Mating Preferences Might Evolve by Natural Selection. If Mating Mate
    A GENERAL MODEL OF SEXUAL AND NATURAL SELECTION P. O'DONALD Department of Zoology, University College of North Wales, bangor Received28.xii.66 1.INTRODUCTION FISHERin The Genetical Theory of JVatural Selection (1930) described how mating preferences might evolve by natural selection. If mating behaviour varies among different genotypes, some individuals may have an hereditary disposition to mate with others having particular characteristics. Usually of course it is the females who choose the males and their choice is determined by the likelihood that the males' display will release their mating responses. If some females prefer to mate with those males that have characteristics advantageous in natural selection, then the genotypes that determine such matings will also be selected: the offspring will carry both the advantageous geno- types and the genotypes of the mating preference. Once the mating preference is established, it will itself add to the selective advantage of the preferred genotypes: a "runaway process" as Fisher called it develops. In a paper in Heredity (1963) I described a mathematical model of this type of selection. In the simplest case two loci must be involved: one locus determines the preferred character and the other the mating preference. If there are only two alleles segregating at each locus, ten different genotypes can occur if the loci are linked and nine if they are not. If they are sex-linked, there are i possible genotypes. I derived finite difference equations giving the frequencies of the genotypes in terms of parameters describing the degree of dominance of the preferred genotypes and the recombination fractions of the loci.
    [Show full text]
  • A Synopsis of the Pre-Human Avifauna of the Mascarene Islands
    – 195 – Paleornithological Research 2013 Proceed. 8th Inter nat. Meeting Society of Avian Paleontology and Evolution Ursula B. Göhlich & Andreas Kroh (Eds) A synopsis of the pre-human avifauna of the Mascarene Islands JULIAN P. HUME Bird Group, Department of Life Sciences, The Natural History Museum, Tring, UK Abstract — The isolated Mascarene Islands of Mauritius, Réunion and Rodrigues are situated in the south- western Indian Ocean. All are volcanic in origin and have never been connected to each other or any other land mass. Despite their comparatively close proximity to each other, each island differs topographically and the islands have generally distinct avifaunas. The Mascarenes remained pristine until recently, resulting in some documentation of their ecology being made before they rapidly suffered severe degradation by humans. The first major fossil discoveries were made in 1865 on Mauritius and on Rodrigues and in the late 20th century on Réunion. However, for both Mauritius and Rodrigues, the documented fossil record initially was biased toward larger, non-passerine bird species, especially the dodo Raphus cucullatus and solitaire Pezophaps solitaria. This paper provides a synopsis of the fossil Mascarene avifauna, which demonstrates that it was more diverse than previously realised. Therefore, as the islands have suffered severe anthropogenic changes and the fossil record is far from complete, any conclusions based on present avian biogeography must be viewed with caution. Key words: Mauritius, Réunion, Rodrigues, ecological history, biogeography, extinction Introduction ily described or illustrated in ships’ logs and journals, which became the source material for The Mascarene Islands of Mauritius, Réunion popular articles and books and, along with col- and Rodrigues are situated in the south-western lected specimens, enabled monographs such as Indian Ocean (Fig.
    [Show full text]
  • Common Birds of the Estero Bay Area
    Common Birds of the Estero Bay Area Jeremy Beaulieu Lisa Andreano Michael Walgren Introduction The following is a guide to the common birds of the Estero Bay Area. Brief descriptions are provided as well as active months and status listings. Photos are primarily courtesy of Greg Smith. Species are arranged by family according to the Sibley Guide to Birds (2000). Gaviidae Red-throated Loon Gavia stellata Occurrence: Common Active Months: November-April Federal Status: None State/Audubon Status: None Description: A small loon seldom seen far from salt water. In the non-breeding season they have a grey face and red throat. They have a long slender dark bill and white speckling on their dark back. Information: These birds are winter residents to the Central Coast. Wintering Red- throated Loons can gather in large numbers in Morro Bay if food is abundant. They are common on salt water of all depths but frequently forage in shallow bays and estuaries rather than far out at sea. Because their legs are located so far back, loons have difficulty walking on land and are rarely found far from water. Most loons must paddle furiously across the surface of the water before becoming airborne, but these small loons can practically spring directly into the air from land, a useful ability on its artic tundra breeding grounds. Pacific Loon Gavia pacifica Occurrence: Common Active Months: November-April Federal Status: None State/Audubon Status: None Description: The Pacific Loon has a shorter neck than the Red-throated Loon. The bill is very straight and the head is very smoothly rounded.
    [Show full text]
  • Mate Choice | Principles of Biology from Nature Education
    contents Principles of Biology 171 Mate Choice Reproduction underlies many animal behaviors. The greater sage grouse (Centrocercus urophasianus). Female sage grouse evaluate males as sexual partners on the basis of the feather ornaments and the males' elaborate displays. Stephen J. Krasemann/Science Source. Topics Covered in this Module Mating as a Risky Behavior Major Objectives of this Module Describe factors associated with specific patterns of mating and life history strategies of specific mating patterns. Describe how genetics contributes to behavioral phenotypes such as mating. Describe the selection factors influencing behaviors like mate choice. page 882 of 989 3 pages left in this module contents Principles of Biology 171 Mate Choice Mating as a Risky Behavior Different species have different mating patterns. Different species have evolved a range of mating behaviors that vary in the number of individuals involved and the length of time over which their relationships last. The most open type of relationship is promiscuity, in which all members of a community can mate with each other. Within a promiscuous species, an animal of either gender may mate with any other male or female. No permanent relationships develop between mates, and offspring cannot be certain of the identity of their fathers. Promiscuous behavior is common in bonobos (Pan paniscus), as well as their close relatives, the chimpanzee (P. troglodytes). Bonobos also engage in sexual activity for activities other than reproduction: to greet other members of the community, to release social tensions, and to resolve conflicts. Test Yourself How might promiscuous behavior provide an evolutionary advantage for males? Submit Some animals demonstrate polygamous relationships, in which a single individual of one gender mates with multiple individuals of the opposite gender.
    [Show full text]
  • Women's Sexual Strategies: the Evolution of Long-Term Bonds and Extrapair Sex
    Women's Sexual Strategies: The Evolution of Long-Term Bonds and Extrapair Sex Elizabeth G. Pillsworth Martie G. Haselton University of California, Los Angeles Because of their heavy obligatory investment in offspring and limited off­ spring number, ancestral women faced the challenge of securing sufficient material resources for reproduction and gaining access to good genes. We review evidence indicating that selection produced two overlapping suites of psychological adaptations to address these challenges. The first set involves coupling-the formation of social partnerships for providing biparental care. The second set involves dual mating, a strategy in which women form long­ term relationships with investing partners, while surreptitiously seeking good genes from extrapair mates. The sources of evidence we review include hunter-gather studies, comparative nonhuman studies, cross-cultural stud· ies, and evidence of shifts in women's desires across the ovulatory cycle. We argue that the evidence poses a challenge to some existing theories of human mating and adds to our understanding of the subtlety of women's sexual strategies. Key Words: dual mating, evolutionary psychology, ovulation, relationships, sexual strategies. Hoggamus higgamus, men are polygamous; higgamus hoggamus, women monoga71Jous. -Attributed to various authors, including William James William James is reputed to have jotted down this aphorism in a dreamy midnight state, awaking with a feeling of satisfaction when he found it the next morning. The aphorism captures a widely accepted .fact about differences between women and men: Relative to women, men more strongly value casual sex (Baumeister, Catanese, & Vohs, 2001; Buss & Schmitt, 1993; Schmitt, 2003). James's statement, how­ ever, is dearly an oversimplification.
    [Show full text]
  • Reference Bird List
    Species R SP SU FA WI Notes:_________________________________________________ John G. and Susan H. Shrikes Laniidae Loggerhead Shrike (P) U U U U Vireo Vireonidae DuPuis, Jr. ________________________________________________________ White-eyed Vireo (P) C C C C Blue-headed Vireo (W) C Wildlife and Jays & Crows Corvidae American Crow (P) C C C C ________________________________________________________ Fish Crow (P) C C C C Environmental Blue Jay (P) C C C C Red-Cockaded Woodpeckers Swallows Hirundinidae ________________________________________________________ Purple Martin (S) U U Species R SP SU FA WI Area Northern Rough-winged Swallow (W) O O O O Parulidae Barn Swallow (P) O O O O Wood-Warblers Prothonotary Warbler (S) R R Tree Swallow (W) C ________________________________________________________ Pine Warbler (P) C C C C Wrens Troglodytidae Palm Warbler (W) C C C C Carolina Wren (P) C C C C Prairie Warbler (P) C C C C House Wren (W) U Yellow-rumped Warbler (W) C C C ________________________________________________________ Kinglets Regulidae Yellow-throated Warbler (W) C C Ruby-crowned Kinglet (W) C Blue-Gray Gnatcatcher (W) C C C C Black-throated Green Warbler (W) O We are interested in your sightings. Please share your American Redstart (W) O observations with us. Thrushes Turdidae Black and White Warbler (W) C C Eastern Bluebird (P) R R R R Enjoy your visit! American Robin (W) C Northern Parula (W) R Common Yellowthroat (P) C C C C Mockingbirds & Thrashers Mimidae Gray Catbird (W) C C Ovenbird (W) O O Additional contact information:
    [Show full text]