<<

Changes in labels in a contemporary cohort of emerging adult women: patterns, prevalence and a typology

Sexual attraction, behaviour and identity are subject to change across the life course for

some individuals, and certain developmental periods such as emerging adulthood

appear particularly conducive to this. However, the evidence documenting these

phenomena comes overwhelmingly from data collected 10-20 years ago. In the brief

interlude since, the socio-political context has changed markedly and increasing

numbers of women are reporting non-heterosexuality. Drawing on contemporary data

from the Australian Longitudinal Study on Women’s Health (n = 16,870), we provide

up to date evidence on changes in sexual identity labels among emerging adult women.

We find that 19% of women changed their sexual identity label from one survey wave

to the next, and 30.6% changed their identity label at least once across the four waves.

Mostly heterosexual and bisexual labels were both more common and more stable in

our sample than in previous studies. We propose a new typology of sexual identity

sequences and fit this to our data, providing a blueprint for researchers looking to

define sexual minority status longitudinally. Findings suggest that the ways women

perceive and label their should be treated as dynamic phenomena

situated within the nested temporalities of biographical and historical time.

Introduction

Sexual minority status, whether measured via an individual’s sexual identity label, attractions

and/or behavior, is a well-established risk factor for poor health outcomes (Wolff, Wells,

Ventura-Dipersia, Renson, & Grov, 2017). However, this evidence is based overwhelmingly

on cross-sectional analyses (Institute of Medicine, 2011), which fail to account for the

potentially dynamic nature of sexual orientation over the life course. Within-individual

changes in sexual identity label, attraction and behavior are known to take place over

1

biographical time for reasons including developmental processes (Morgan & Thompson,

2006 & 2011; Rosario, Schrimshaw & Hunter, 2008 & 2011), situation-dependent flexibility in sexual desire and responsiveness (Diamond, 2008 & 2016), and changes to the

“biographical, interpersonal, social, cultural, political and historical context” in which sexual identities are actively constructed (Gordon & Silva, 2015: p.506). In light on this, scholars have argued for the importance of “defining sexual minority status longitudinally over critical developmental periods” (Kaestle, 2019: p.811) to better understand the roots of health disparities based on sexual orientation.

A handful of pioneering studies have generated evidence on within-person changes in sexual minority status over time (e.g., Diamond, 2008; Kaestle, 2019; Ott, Corliss, Wypij,

Rosario, & Austin, 2011; Savin-Williams, Joyner, & Rieger, 2012). However, much of this evidence is based on data collected 10-20 years ago in a single country (the U.S.). Since then,

the socio-politic context surrounding non-heterosexuality in industrialized Western countries

such as Australia, the U.S., Canada and the U.K. has undergone rapid and significant change.

Public attitudes have become increasingly supportive of same- sexuality and the rights of

same-sex couples (de Visser et al., 2014; Mercer et al., 2013; Perales & Campbell, 2018;

Twenge, Sherman, & Wells, 2016). This attitudinal shift has been punctuated by significant

legislative shifts, such as the passing of laws to allow same-sex and expunge

convictions related to historical anti- laws. Over the same period, studies using

repeated cross-sectional data have documented increases in non-heterosexual identification,

particularly amongst women (Chandra, Mosher, Copen, & Sionean, 2011; Copen, Chandra,

& Febo-Vazquez, 2016; Fish, Watson, Porta, Russell, & Saewyc, 2017; Richters et al., 2014).

In this paper, we argue that socio-political contexts have the potential to not only

affect the sexual identity labels used by women at a given point in time, but also how sexual

identity develops and changes within women. Thus, the patterns and prevalence of within-

2 person changes in self-reported sexual identity label (hereon referred to as changes in sexual identity labels) may be quite different amongst young women today compared to earlier cohorts. The goal of this study is to generate novel evidence on changes in sexual identity labels amongst a contemporary cohort of Australian women during emerging adulthood. In doing so, we develop a typology of sexual identity trajectories using sequence analysis. The latter is an innovative technique well-suited for this purpose but used little in the past, one which we propose as a useful blueprint for researchers wishing to identify the longitudinal patterning of individuals’ sexualities.

Changes in sexual identity labels: what does it mean?

The focus of our study is on sexual identity labels, also referred to as sexual orientation labels or sexual orientation identities (Savin-Williams, 2011). Sexual identity labels are a measure of how an individual perceives, describes and categorizes their sexual orientation. When we talk about changes in sexual identity labels, we refer to within-person changes in sexual identity label over the course of a longitudinal study—for example, when a selects “bisexual” to describe sexual orientation in one study wave, and “mostly ” in a subsequent wave. As outlined before, current theory and evidence point to three reasons why an individual might change the way in which they label their sexual orientation: developmental processes, , and shifting context. There is nevertheless some overlap between these processes. For example, sexual fluidity may coincide with sexual identity development, and contexts are important for both (Gordon & Silva, 2015; Katz-Wise

& Hyde, 2017).

Sexual identity development

One reason why an individual might change the way in which they label their sexual orientation is that their sexual identity is still developing. Current models of sexual identity

3 development (see e.g., Dillon, Worthington & Moradi, 2011; Rosario et al., 2011) have their roots in the pioneering work of Erikson (1968) and Marcia (1987), and share some common features. First, these models feature processes whereby an individual becomes aware of, explores and comes to understand their sexual orientation. Such processes are known variously as identity formation (Rosario et al., 2011) and active exploration (Dillon et al.,

2011). While considered a universal experience in the development of a lesbian, or bisexual identity, the development of a heterosexual identity can also involve such a period of active questioning and experimentation (Dillon et al., 2011; Morgan & Thompson, 2011;

Morgan, 2012). As this process of identity development unfolds, some changes in sexual identity labelling are expected (Dillon et al., 2011). Second, these models do not assume linearity, but rather, “there are opportunities for circularity and revisiting of statuses throughout the lifespan for a given individual” (Dillon et al., 2011, p. 658). Thus, a wide range of developmental trajectories are possible, creating potential for a wide range of sexual identity label trajectories. Notably, these trajectories may be shaped by individuals’ social and interpersonal contexts, with different environments being more or less facilitative of the processes involved in sexual identity development (Katz-Wise & Hyde, 2017; Rosario et al.,

2008).

Sexual fluidity

Sexual fluidity, described in the seminal work of Lisa Diamond (2008), is another plausible reason why a person might change their sexual identity label. Diamond (2016, p.

249) defines sexual fluidity as “a capacity for situation dependent flexibility in sexual responsiveness, which allows individuals to experience changes in same-sex or other-sex desire, over both short-term and long-term time periods”. According to Diamond (2008 &

2016), the existence of sexual fluidity does not imply that sexual orientation does not exist, and nor does it “necessarily disprove biological influences on sexual orientation” (Diamond,

4

2008, p.18). Rather, it suggests that a person’s sexual orientation does not necessarily predict each and every desire experienced over the lifespan (Diamond, 2016). Diamond (2008) describes sexual fluidity as an additional component of sexuality that “operates in concert with sexual orientation” (p. 24) and introduces variability around a person’s “core sexual predisposition” (p.256). Diamond (2016) proposes that capacities for sexual fluidity vary widely across individuals: some people have very stable patterns of across their lives, while others experience periodic variations.

In certain contexts—for example, a close same-sex friendship—sexually fluid individuals may experience novel and unexpected sexual attractions, potentially triggering a change to their sexual identity label (Diamond, 2008). In a partial replication of Diamond’s

(2008) study, more than half of participants said they had experienced fluidity in their sexual attractions in the past, with approximately half of these women and one-third of these men subsequently changing their sexual identity label as a result (Katz-Wise, 2015). Interestingly, sexual fluidity was not associated with the age at which sexual identity milestones were attained, suggesting that the two processes may be distinct (Katz-Wise, 2015).

Shifting contexts

A person’s sexual experiences (i.e., their attractions, fantasies, and behavior) are important sources of information to draw on when adopting a sexual identity label. However, non-sexual social factors—such as social networks, ideological beliefs, normative practices, and levels of stigma and support—can also play a role (Diamond, 2008; Preciado,

2013; Silva; 2019). According to Preciado’s (2013) motivated cognition model of sexual orientation, self-perceived sexual orientation may be “contingent on what people, values and institutions are presently salient” (p. 22). Thus, as individuals’ social, interpersonal and cultural contexts shift across the course of their life, they may re-label their sexual orientation accordingly—even if their sexual attractions and behavior remain unchanged (Gordon &

5

Silva, 2015; Preciado, 2013). Context may be particularly influential when there is some flexibility or ambiguity in the interpretation of an individual’s sexual experiences: for example, when their sexuality is oriented towards both men and women, in sometimes variable ways (Diamond, 2008; Preciado, 2013).

Sex and gender differences

While the three processes just described are relevant to both women and men, sex and gender may have a significant impact on how they unfold. Compared to men, women are significantly more likely to report non-exclusive patterns of sexual attraction and behavior, adopt a plurisexual identity label, show longitudinal variation in their patterns of sexual attraction, behavior and identity, and reach sexual identity development milestones at later ages (Diamond, 2014; Katz-Wise, 2015). While knowledge of the mechanisms underlying these differences is limited, both biological and socio-cultural factors are likely implicated

(Diamond, 2014). For instance, there is a marked difference in levels of directed towards men versus women (Bettinsoli, Supps & Napier, 2019), as well as the sexual scripts that members of each gender are expected to follow (Wiederman, 2005). Further, many contexts thought to be facilitative of sexual identity development and sexual fluidity—such as close same-sex friendships and college attendance—play a more salient role in the same-sex experiences of women than men (e.g., Glover, Galliher & Crowell, 2015; Rupp & Taylor,

2010; Thompson, 2006). Yet this is not to suggest that women are free of heteronormative expectations. For example, heteronormative pressures and/or a desire to align with normative can increase as women plan for and transition into motherhood, with implications for the sexual identity labels that they choose to adopt (Budnick, 2016; Diamond, 2008; Ross,

Tarasoff, Goldberg, & Flanders, 2017; Silva, 2019).

In summary, selecting a label to describe one’s sexual orientation can be a complex undertaking, affected by developmental processes, experiences of sexual fluidity and

6

interpersonal and sociocultural contexts. Awareness of these complexities is important for

contextualizing evidence on the patterns and prevalence of change generated from large-

scale, longitudinal survey data. At the same time, it is important to recognize that these data

tell us little about the reasons for change, how individual’s experience change and the

meanings they attach to it (Diamond, 2014). Furthermore, there is always the possibility that

some of the changes observed in longitudinal survey data may reflect “noise”, rather than

genuine changes in how a person perceives their sexual orientation. For example, a person

might only feel comfortable reporting a non-heterosexual label after they have participated in multiple waves of the study and have come to trust that their anonymity is guaranteed. Thus, studies such as our own are important to document the patterning and prevalence of within- person changes in sexual identity label. Yet these studies must be complemented by qualitative and targeted survey research to gain a more complete understanding of these phenomena. In the following section, we review the current evidence on the patterns and prevalence of changes in sexual identity labels in women.

Changes in sexual identity label: empirical evidence

Early studies of sexual minority women

Early evidence on changes in sexual identity labels came from retrospective studies utilizing small, non-representative samples of sexual minority women (e.g., Kitzinger and

Wilkinson, 1995; Rust, 1992). Lisa Diamond’s (2008) seminal study of 89 young women was the first to use a prospective research design. The women in her sample were aged 16-23 years and all reported same-sex attraction upon recruitment. Over a ten-year period (1995-

2005), these women were interviewed five times, and asked about their sexual attraction, identity label and behavior. Changes between lesbian, bisexual, unlabeled and heterosexual labels were common: one-third of women changed their identity label between the first and second interview, and two-thirds changed it at least once over the ten-year period (Diamond,

7

2008). Similar rates of change in sexual identity labels were documented in a more recent

study of sexual minority women using data from the Chicago Health and Life Experiences of

Women survey (CHLEW: Everett, Talley, Hughes, Wilsnack, & Johnson, 2016), which

collected three waves of interview data from 306 women (18-82 years at baseline) over an

11-year period (2000-2012). All participants identified as mostly or exclusively lesbian at

recruitment. The incidence of change in identity labels across waves in CHLEW was 25%

(Everett et al., 2016), slightly lower than the 32% figure reported by Diamond (2000).

Studies of national samples

Altogether, the aforementioned studies provide preliminary evidence on the

prevalence of changes in sexual identity labels among women. However, the reliability and generalizability of these findings is limited due to the use of small samples of women recruited purposively based on their sexual minority status. In particular, these studies provide no indication of how common change is at the heterosexual end of the sexual identity continuum—where the majority of women sit. To obtain more reliable estimates of the population prevalence of changes in sexual identity labels, longitudinal data from large, representative samples are required. The number of datasets fitting this description—while steadily increasing—remains small. Findings based on two U.S. studies—the Growing Up

Today Study (GUTS) and the National Longitudinal Survey of Adolescent Health (Add

Health)—are the most informative: both studies feature large, national samples

(approximately 12,000-13,000 respondents) and measure sexual identity labels on a 5-point continuum (i.e., with mostly heterosexual and mostly lesbian/gay options). The inclusion of the mostly heterosexual/lesbian categories is important, as there is compelling evidence indicating that women selecting these identity labels differ in meaningful ways from women selecting a bisexual or exclusively lesbian/heterosexual label (Savin-Williams & Vrangalova,

2013; Thompson & Morgan, 2008; Vrangalova & Savin-Williams, 2012). In fact, “mostly

8

heterosexual” is now the second most-common sexual identity label reported by women

(after exclusively heterosexual), with more women selecting this label than all other sexual

minority identity labels combined (Vrangalova & Savin-Williams, 2012).

Analyses of GUTS and Add Health data have each arrived at somewhat diverging estimates of the incidence of changes in sexual identity labels in , and substantially lower estimates than reported in the studies of sexual minority women previously discussed.

Ott et al. (2011) analyzed four waves of GUTS data collected biennially between 1999 and

2005 and comprising 26,010 pairs of consecutive sexual identity reports from 11,109 young people. Amongst young women aged 18 years and older, 11% of consecutive reports

involved a change in sexual identity label. In contrast, using Add Health data collected from

12,287 young people in 2001/2002 (ages 18-24) and again in 2007/2009 (ages 24-32), Savin-

Williams, Joyner & Riegner (2012) found that 18% of women changed their sexual identity

label across the two waves. An exclusively heterosexual label was the most commonly

reported by a long margin in both studies, and the higher incidence of change away from this

sexual identity label amongst women in Add Health compared to GUTS largely accounts for

the differences in the overall incidence of change in sexual identity labels in these two

studies.

Patterns of change in women were otherwise similar across the two studies. For

example, the order of identity labels from most to least stable was the same: exclusively

heterosexual, exclusively lesbian, mostly heterosexual, bisexual and mostly lesbian. Thus, the

plurisexual identity labels (i.e., those oriented towards both ) were more fluid than the

exclusive identity labels (i.e., those oriented towards a single sex only). The incidence of

change from exclusively lesbian—the most stable of the sexual minority identity labels—was

approximately 34% in both studies, and the incidence of change from mostly lesbian—the

least stable of the sexual minority identity labels—was 65% in GUTS and 79% in Add

9

Health. In both studies, those selecting a mostly heterosexual or bisexual identity label were

considerably more likely to subsequently change towards a heterosexual identity labels than

towards a lesbian identity label. Women who self-labelled as mostly lesbian were just as

likely to change their identity label in either direction.

Less informative, yet still relevant, are longitudinal studies that have measured sexual identity labels with three options (heterosexual, bisexual, lesbian/gay) rather than a 5-point continuum. Between 2% and 2.6% of women changed their sexual identity label across two

waves of the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS: Mock & Eibach, 2012), Nurses Health II

(Charlton, Corliss, Spiegelman, Williams, & Austin, 2016) and Dunedin Multidisciplinary

Health & Development (Dunedin: Dickson, Roode, Cameron, & Paul, 2013) studies. These

rates of change are therefore substantially lower than those reported in GUTS and Add

Health. This is not surprising, given that most of the identity-label changes documented in

GUTS and Add Health were one step in magnitude along the 5-point sexual identity label continuum (e.g., from exclusively heterosexual to mostly heterosexual, or from mostly lesbian to bisexual). Studies using just three identity label options cannot capture these nuanced changes. Another contributing factor may be the age of participants. GUTS and Add

Health collected data during adolescence and emerging adulthood, which are life-course stages characterized by sexual exploration and identity development (Arnett, 2004; Erikson,

1968; Morgan, 2012). Changes in sexual identity labels may be more prevalent during these life stages than during middle and later adulthood, towards which the Nurses Health Study II,

MIDUS and Dunedin Study samples were skewed (~30-74 years). Despite the lower prevalence of changes in sexual identity labels in these studies, the patterns of change were very similar to those observed in GUTS and Add Health. For example, bisexual was consistently found to be the least stable of the sexual identity labels, and the vast majority of

10

women changing from a bisexual identity label changed to a heterosexual rather than a

lesbian identity label.

In summary, changes in sexual identity labels in women has been documented in a

small number of longitudinal studies using large, national samples—predominantly from the

U.S., and all involving data collected 10-20 years ago. These studies suggest that: (i) when sexual identity labels are measured on a 5-point continuum, the likelihood of young women changing their identity label across waves is between 11% and 18%; (ii) most changes are just one step along the 5-point continuum of sexual identity labels, and are not captured when sexual identity labels are measured with only three response options; (iii) the plurisexual identity labels are significantly more fluid than the exclusive identity labels; and (iv) changes from a bisexual or mostly heterosexual identity label are more often than not in a less same- sex-oriented direction (i.e., back towards a heterosexual identity label).

Typologies of sexual identity label sequences

When multiple waves of data are available, researchers can go beyond the basic description of the prevalence and patterns of sexual identity label change. Utilizing techniques such as sequence analysis and longitudinal latent class analysis, it is possible to create longitudinal definitions of sexual minority status in the form of typologies, which classify women according to their sexual identity trajectories or sequences. For example, based on the 79 women who participated in all four waves of her study, Diamond (2005) created an exploratory typology of sexual identity sequences at the lesbian end of the continuum. Women who self-labelled as lesbian in every wave were classified as “stable ” (22.8%), those who self-labelled as bisexual or unlabeled in every wave were classified as “stable non-lesbians” (45.6%), and those who alternated between a lesbian and non-lesbian (including heterosexual) identity label were classified as “fluid lesbians”

(31.6%).

11

Ott et al. (2011) were able to create a more comprehensive typology of sexual identity sequences covering the full continuum of sexual orientation, using data from participants who reported their sexual identity label in at least 3 waves of GUTS. Amongst the 3,268 young women aged 16-22 at the first report, sequences characterized by movement away from heterosexuality toward a lesbian, gay or bisexual (LGB) identity label were twice as common

(8.9%) as the opposite—i.e., sequences characterized by change from a lesbian, gay or

bisexual identity label to a heterosexual identity label (4.4%). A further 6.5% of young women had a multidirectional sequence characterized by movement both away from and back towards heterosexuality. A stable, exclusively heterosexual identity label was the most

common sequence (76.7% of women). In addition, 2.1% of women had a stable mostly

heterosexual identity label, 0.6% a stable LGB identity label, and 1.0% reported being

“unsure” of their identity label on at least one occasion.

A refined typology of sexual identity label sequences

Building on this pioneering work, we propose a refined typology of sexual identity

label sequences. In developing our typology, we considered evidence on how the 5-point

continuum of sexual identity labels maps onto the two independent continua reflecting a

person’s orientation to the same sex and other sex respectively. Vrangalova and Savin-

Williams (2012) found that the sexualities of women selecting the identity labels at the

heterosexual end of the continuum are, on average, strongly oriented towards men. Yet these

women differ in the extent to which their sexuality is oriented towards women. In their study,

women who identified as exclusively heterosexual reported low levels of same-sex attraction

on average. This increased to moderate levels amongst women who identified as mostly

heterosexual, and high levels amongst those who identified as bisexual. The proportion of

women reporting at least one sexual partner increased across the three groups in a

similar fashion, from 9% of exclusively heterosexual women to 86% of bisexual women.

12

In contrast, Vrangalova & Savin-Williams’ (2012) findings suggest that women selecting the identity labels at the lesbian end of the continuum differ in the extent to which their sexuality is oriented towards men. In their study, women who selected the exclusively lesbian label reported low levels of attraction to men on average. This increased to moderate levels amongst women who identified as mostly lesbian, and high levels amongst those who identified as bisexual. Meanwhile, all three groups reported similarly high levels of attraction to and partnering with women.

In light of this evidence, we propose that fluid sexual identity label sequences be grouped first according to which end(s) of the continuum they are located in: heterosexual, lesbian or both. We conceptualize the heterosexual end of the continuum as comprising the exclusively heterosexual and mostly heterosexual identity labels, while the lesbian end of the continuum includes the exclusively lesbian and mostly lesbian identity labels. The bisexual identity label sits at the mid-point of the continuum and straddles both ends. Within each end of the continuum, we propose that fluid sequences be further classified according to the direction of change: towards exclusivity, towards (and thus away from exclusivity), or both (multidirectional). Last, we include categories for women who cross from one end of the continuum to the other. For example, a woman who changes her sexual identity label from mostly lesbian to exclusively heterosexual is classified as having crossed from the lesbian end to the heterosexual end of the continuum. Figure 1 below visually represents the conceptual framework behind our typology.

[FIGURE 1 HERE]

The Australian context

The primary aim of this study is to generate contemporary evidence on changes in sexual identity labels given recent shifts in the socio-political context. However, it is

13

important to note that socio-political contexts vary not only across time, but also across space

(Elder Jr., Kirkpatrick Johnson, & Crosnoe, 2003). With most previous research conducted in

the U.S., our study provides evidence on sexual identity change in a novel country context

(Australia). As previously discussed, levels of structural stigma towards sexual minorities have decreased over the past two decades in many Western countries, and when viewed in global context, Australia and the U.S. are more similar than different in this regard (McGee,

2016; Mendos, 2019). Yet, the two countries differ in important ways. Protections against based on sexual orientation are weaker in the U.S. compared to Australia

(Mendos, 2019), and negative attitudes towards sexual minorities are more common (McGee,

2016; Valfort, 2017). For example, in 2013, 33% of adults in the U.S. thought that society

should not accept homosexuality compared to 18% of adults in Australia (Pew Research

Center, 2013). This may be an artefact of differences in the religious landscapes of the two

countries, with strong trends towards religious diversification and secularization in Australia

over the past few decades (Perales, Bouma, & Campbell, 2018; Stanley, 2015). Our study

will therefore provide evidence on sexual identity change in both a more liberal era and a

somewhat more liberal country than previous studies.

Methods

Participants

We use data from a contemporary, national sample of young women born 1989-1995

and taking part in the Australian Longitudinal Study on Women’s Health (ALSWH: see

Loxton et al., 2018). The ALSWH commenced in 1996 with women drawn from three age-

based cohorts. A fourth cohort of women, born 1989-1995, was added to the ALSWH in

2012/2013, when these women were aged 18–23. Composed of today’s emerging adult

women, this cohort is the focus of our study. Recruitment of the 1989-1995 cohort relied on

multiple methods, including traditional and online (social) media outlets and peer referral.

14

Australian women (citizens and permanent residents) who were born during the target years were eligible to enroll in the study. The wave-one sample for the 1989-1995 cohort comprised 17,012 women. Since 2013, data have been collected from these women annually via an online survey, with response rates ranging from 55% to 70%. Sexual identity data have been collected from the 1989-1995 cohort in four out of five waves thus far (2013, 2014,

2015 & 2017).

Our initial analytic sample comprises 44,552 observations from 16,870 women, of whom 2.4% were Indigenous Australian, 89.3% were non-Indigenous Australian-born, and

8.2% were born overseas. At wave one, 22.8% of the women in our sample had a university qualification (Bachelor’s degree or higher), 26.3% had a non-university post-school qualification, and 51% had no post-school qualification; 3% were married, 19.7% were in a de facto relationship, and 77% were single/never married; and 74.2% lived in a major

Australian city, 24.8% lived in a regional or remote area, and 1% were living overseas. To capture changes in sexual identity, pairs of consecutive observations from the same individual are required. Given that not every woman participated in every study wave, our analyses of sexual identity change were restricted to a subsample of 11,543 women, including

3,949 women who contributed one pair of consecutive observations, 1,875 who contributed two pairs, and 5,719 who contributed the maximum possible three pairs of observations. Our sample therefore contained a total of 24,856 pairs of observations.

Measure of sexual identity labels

In the ALSWH, sexual identity labels were measured on a Kinsey-type continuum comparable to that used in GUTS and Add Health. The item asked: “Which of these most closely describes your sexual orientation?”. The seven possible responses were: “I am exclusively heterosexual”, “I am mainly heterosexual”, “I am bisexual”, “I am mainly homosexual (lesbian)”, “I am exclusively homosexual (lesbian)”, “I don't know”, and “I don't

15

want to answer”. Women who did not want to answer the question were coded as missing,

and the remaining responses were categorized as a set of dummy variables denoting sexual

identity label. In study wave five an additional option (“other”) was added to the sexual identity item, being selected by 1.6% of women (n = 132). There are several reasons why a woman might select the “other” option (e.g., she might identify as asexual, or pansexual). With the available data, these are impossible to know for certain. Therefore, to avoid misclassification and facilitate comparability across waves, these observations were recoded as missing and excluded from analysis. In addition, between 1% and 3% of women did not respond to the sexual identity item in each wave, and were also coded as missing. An indicator variable of change in sexual identity label was created for each wave in which sexual identity labels were measured (other than the first), with respondents receiving a score of one if their sexual identity label had changed from the previous wave and a score of zero if it had not.

Statistical analysis

We used a number of analytic approaches to explore changes in sexual identity labels in our sample. First, we tabulated sexual identity label for each wave to show changes over time at the aggregate level. The sample for this analysis comprised the 16,870 women with at

least one valid report of sexual identity label across the four waves. Second, to examine the

patterns and prevalence of within-individual changes in sexual identity labels, we created a

transition matrix. This is a cross-tabulation of sexual identity label in a given wave with

sexual identity label in the subsequent wave. To assess the overall incidence of changes in

sexual identity labels, we tabulated the indicator variable for change in sexual identity label.

For these two analyses, the subsample of 11,543 women who contributed at least one pair of

consecutive observations of sexual identity label was used. Finally, we generated sequences

of sexual identity labels (i.e., time-ordered lists of women’s sexual identity labels) for the

16

subsample of women who responded to the item in all four waves (n = 5,719), using the SQ-

Ados sequence analysis package (Brzinsky-Fay, Kohler, & Luniak, 2006) for Stata version

14.1. These sequences were then mapped onto our previously described typology. Given the

potential for panel attrition to our results, we tested for non-random attrition by sexual

identity labels in our sample. Specifically, we estimated a random-effect logistic regression

model using the dummy variables denoting sexual identity label at wave t as predictors of

attrition from wave t+1. Sexual identity label was not a significant predictor of attrition in our

sample.

Results

Changes in sexual identity label: prevalence

Table 1 displays sample frequencies for the sexual identity label variables, pooled and

by wave. At the aggregate level, exclusively heterosexual identification decreased (from

65.5% to 60.0%) and bisexual identification increased (from 7.5% to 10.4%) across the four

waves. The proportion of women reporting a mostly heterosexual identity label increased

slightly from wave one to two (from 24.4% to 26.5%), and it remained relatively stable

thereafter. The proportions of women selecting a mostly or exclusively lesbian identity label

remained fairly stable across waves, at around 1% each. A sexual minority identity label (i.e.,

a mostly heterosexual, bisexual, mostly lesbian or exclusively lesbian identity label) was

reported on at least one occasion by 45% of the 16,870 women in our sample.

The incidence of change in sexual identity label was 19.3%. The incidence was similar between waves one and two, and between waves two and three (approximately

18.5%), but increased to 21.6% between waves three and five. Of the 11,543 women who contributed at least one pair of observations, 30.6% changed their identity label at least once.

Of the 5,719 women who participated in all four waves in which sexual identity data were

17

collected, 21.8% changed their identity label just once, 13.1% changed twice, and 3.1% changed it three times.

Changes in sexual identity label: patterns

Table 2 contains a pooled transition matrix, and gives a sense of the relative

(in)stability of each sexual identity label. The transition matrix is a cross-tabulation of sexual

identity label in a given wave with sexual identity label in the subsequent wave. There is a

row in the matrix for each label containing the probability of maintaining that identity label—

or transitioning to each of the other identity labels—in the next wave. Each row in the

transition matrix therefore adds up to 100%. Data in Table 2 come from the subsample of

11,543 women who contributed at least one pair of consecutive observations across the four

waves. In addition to the pooled transition matrix shown in Table 2, we calculated separate

transition matrices for each wave-transition (Waves 1-2, Waves 2-3, and Waves 3-5) and

these are shown in Online Appendix 1. Patterns of change within these three individual

matrices are highly consistent with the patterns seen in Table 2, which we describe below.

The exclusive identity labels in our Australian sample were the least likely to change.

The exclusively heterosexual identity label was stable in 87.9% of paired observations, and

the exclusively lesbian identity label was stable in 80.4%. Although the plurisexual identity

labels were the most likely to change, they were still relatively stable. For example, the

bisexual identity label was stable in 76.0% of paired observations, and the mostly

heterosexual identity label was stable in 68.1% of paired observations. The mostly lesbian

identity label was the most likely to change across waves, being stable in 59.1% of paired

observations in our ALSWH sample.

The transition matrix in Table 2 is also useful to identify the most common

destinations when sexual identity labels change. Of the 2,045 changes from a mostly

18

heterosexual identity label, 65.5% (n = 1,339) involved change to an exclusively heterosexual

identity label. Likewise, three-quarters of changes from a bisexual identity label (348 of 462

changes) were either to a mostly or exclusively heterosexual identity label. Changes from a

mostly lesbian identity label were somewhat more likely to be to a bisexual (54.5% of changes) than to an exclusively lesbian identity label (36.4% of changes). Thus, changes

away from the plurisexual labels were more likely to be towards the heterosexual end of the

continuum than the lesbian end. However, when the full sample were considered—and thus

changes away from an exclusively heterosexual identity label were also included—there was

more movement away from heterosexuality (n = 2,393) than towards it (n = 1,686) overall.

The most common destinations for women who changed from being unsure in our sample

were a bisexual or mostly heterosexual identity label (29.3% and 45.7% of changes,

respectively).

[TABLES 1 & 2 HERE]

Sequences of sexual identity labels: a typology

Sequences of sexual identity labels were generated for the subsample of women who

answered the relevant question in all four survey waves (n = 5,719). This resulted in 210

unique sequences; that is, time-ordered lists of women’s sexual identity labels across the four

waves. The 210 sequences were mapped onto our typology, as shown in Table 3. Women

who reported an exclusively heterosexual identity in every wave comprised almost half of the

sample (47.8%). Stable mostly heterosexual and bisexual sequences accounted for 10% and

3.3% of women respectively. Less than 1% of women had a stable mostly lesbian or

exclusively lesbian sequence. Altogether, less than two-thirds of women had a stable

sequence, meaning that sexual identity change was quite common in our Australian sample.

19

The overwhelming majority of women with a fluid sequence of sexual identity labels

were found at the heterosexual end of the continuum, and never crossed into the lesbian end

(89.3% of women with a fluid sequence, 33.9% of all women). Twice as many women had a

sequence characterized by movement away from rather than towards exclusively heterosexual

(14.4% vs. 7.0%). A further 12.4% of women had a sequence involving both of these

processes. A fluid sequence at the lesbian end of the continuum was evident in 1.4% of our

sample, with similar numbers of these women having a sequence characterized by movement

away from exclusively lesbian, back towards exclusively lesbian, or both. Cross-continuum

sequences were very rare, observed in just 40 women (0.7%). In almost three-quarters of

cases this involved moving from the heterosexual end of the continuum to the lesbian end.

Consistent with findings from the transition matrix, an ‘unsure’ response to the sexual identity item was most likely to be found in a sequence with an otherwise stable mostly heterosexual identity (1.2%), or a sequence characterized by movement away from exclusive

heterosexuality towards bisexuality (0.9%).

[TABLE 3 HERE]

Discussion

This study has generated evidence on the prevalence and patterns of changes in sexual

identity labels using a large, national sample of Australian women. As such, it stands as one of only a handful of studies to have done so internationally and, in particular, to have leveraged data from outside the U.S. The socio-political context surrounding same-sex sexuality is arguably more liberal in Australia than in the U.S., as evidenced by stronger anti- discrimination protections for sexual minorities (Mendos, 2019) and more positive community attitudes towards same-sex sexuality (McGee, 2016; Pew Research Center, 2013;

Valfort, 2017). This may be the product of Australia’s more secular and diverse religious

20

landscape (Perales, Bouma, & Campbell, 2018; Stanley, 2015). Crucially, our analyses also speak about changes in sexual identity labels amongst a very recent cohort of emerging adult women. The use of more recent data than in previous studies is an important contribution, given rapid and significant changes to the socio-political context surrounding non- heterosexuality in both Australia and the U.S. over the past two decades.

Prevalence of sexual identity labels

Substantially fewer women identified as exclusively heterosexual across waves in our contemporary Australian sample (60.0% – 65.5%) than in the earlier U.S. sample from Add

Health (80.2% – 85.6%: Savin-Williams et al., 2012). The proportion of women identifying as either mostly heterosexual or bisexual was particularly large in our sample compared to earlier studies. This trend is nevertheless consistent with evidence of increasing bisexual identification amongst women from cross-sectional studies such as such as the Australian

Study of Health and Relationships (Richters et al., 2014), the U.S. National Survey of Family

Growth (Chandra et al., 2011; Copen et al., 2016), and the British Columbia Adolescent

Health Survey (Fish et al., 2017).

Prevalence and patterns of changes in sexual identity labels

Consistent with prior evidence (e.g., Diamond, 2008; Ott et al., 2011; Savin-Williams et al., 2012), our analyses suggest that some women are indeed liable to re-label their sexual orientation over time. The incidence of change in sexual identity labels across waves in our sample was approximately 19%, similar to the estimated incidence for young women in Add

Health (18%), and higher than that for young women in GUTS (11%). Altogether, just under a third of women changed their sexual identity label at least once during the four-year observation window. Thus, sexual identity labels appear to be fluid in a substantial minority of emerging adult women in contemporary Australia.

21

The patterns of change in sexual identity labels observed in our sample showed both similarities and divergences when compared to previous analyses of U.S. data (e.g., Ott et al.,

2011; Savin-Williams et al., 2012). Consistent with prior research, we found the two exclusive identity labels to be the most stable (with the exclusively heterosexual identity label being the least likely to change), and the three plurisexual identity labels to be the most unstable (with the mostly lesbian identity label being the most likely to change). However,

there was one striking difference between our findings and those from earlier studies; namely,

the plurisexual identity labels showed far greater stability in our sample than previously

observed. For example, the bisexual identity label was stable in 76.0% of paired observations

in our sample compared to just 48.1% in GUTS (Ott et al., 2011) and 25.4% in Add Health

(Savin-Williams et al., 2012). Likewise, the mostly lesbian identity label was stable in 59.1% of paired observations in our ALSWH sample compared to 35.0% in GUTS and 21.4% in

Add Health. As a result, differences between the exclusive and plurisexual identity labels

were substantially less pronounced in our sample. In fact, a woman identifying as bisexual

was almost as likely to retain her identity label in the next wave (a probability of 76%) as a woman identifying as exclusively lesbian (80%) or a woman identifying as exclusively

heterosexual (88%).

One possible explanation for the increased stability of the plurisexual identity labels

in our contemporary sample relative to earlier samples is that the “push factors” acting on

these identities have decreased in recent times. Women whose sexualities are oriented

towards both sexes receive the lowest levels of social support and can be viewed as an “out

group” by both the heterosexual and lesbian communities (Doan Van, Mereish, Woulfe, &

Katz-Wise, 2018; Flanders, Dobinson, & Logie, 2017; Friedman et al., 2014). In the past, this

“double discrimination” may have made it psychologically challenging to maintain a plurisexual identity, thereby increasing the incidence of change away from one relative to the

22

exclusive identities. However, attitudes towards bisexual men and women are becoming less

negative (Dodge et al., 2016), and the number of women identifying as bisexual in large cross-sectional studies has increased (Chandra et al., 2011; Copen et al., 2016; Fish et al.,

2017; Richters et al., 2014). If bisexuality is becoming less stigmatized, young women today may feel more able to both claim and maintain a plurisexual identity label than they did in the past—hence the increased stability of the plurisexual identity labels in our contemporary sample.

There is currently little evidence on what it means when participants report being unsure of how to label their sexual orientation, or how these individuals identify in the future.

It is therefore interesting to note the destination identity labels when women changed from an initial report of “I don’t know”. While Ott et al. (2011) found that most participants who were ever unsure of their identity label ended up identifying as exclusively heterosexual, the most common destinations for women who changed from being unsure in our sample were a bisexual or mostly heterosexual identity label (29.3% and 45.7% of changes, respectively).

This may reflect that women who are actively questioning their sexual identity label arrive at somewhat different destinations depending on the socio-political context and/or life course stage in which this questioning occurs. Thus, questioning women from our sample—surveyed between 2013 and 2017—may have been more likely to adopt a sexual minority identity label than questioning participants from GUTS—surveyed between 1999 and 2005—because circumstances were more conducive to doing so.

Typology of sexual identity label sequences

To make sense of the complexity of young women’s sexual identities we proposed a typology of sequences of sexual identity labels, providing a blueprint for researchers attempting to define sexual orientation longitudinally. Just under half of the women in our sample reported an exclusively heterosexual identity label in every wave. This is a

23

substantially lower proportion than observed in previous studies. For example, the equivalent

figure amongst women was 76% in both GUTS (Ott et al., 2011) and Add Health (Savin-

Williams et al., 2012). Despite “exclusively heterosexual” being the most stable of all

identities, the majority of women experiencing changes to their sexual identity label were

situated at the heterosexual end of the continuum. Twice as many women had a sequence

characterized by movement away from rather than towards heterosexuality, the same ratio as

observed in GUTS (Ott et al., 2011). As such, the flow of women away from an exclusively

heterosexual label, while relatively small as a proportion of all women reporting that identity

label, outweighed the flow of women back from a bisexual or mostly heterosexual identity

label. This resulted in a steady decline in the proportion of exclusively heterosexual women

across survey waves.

Current knowledge on emerging adulthood and the development of sexual minority identities helps contextualize our findings. Scholars have argued that “compulsory heterosexuality” (Mohr, 2002; Rich, 1980)—i.e., heterosexuality that is assumed, institutionalized and imposed—is the starting point in the development of a sexual identity for most individuals (Dillon et al., 2011; Morgan & Thompson, 2006). That is, most individuals initially perceive themselves as heterosexual by default due to normative pressures. Developing a sexual minority identity therefore requires that an individual becomes aware of and begins to express their internal reality surrounding their sexual

orientation (Rosario et al., 2011), and ultimately breaks from conformity to heteronormative

expectations (Kaestle, 2019; Rust, 1992). The compulsory contexts of adolescence—such as

schools and families—are not always supportive of non-heterosexuality (Goldbach & Gibbs,

2017; Perales, Campbell & O’Flaherty, 2019), making it difficult for some youth to assume a sexual minority identity during this time (Morgan & Thompson, 2006).

24

However, the expectation to conform to often decreases during emerging adulthood, as young people’s agency increases, they encounter more liberal and diverse environments (e.g., higher education institutions), and move out of the family home.

In fact, the contexts of emerging adulthood may be particularly conducive to active exploration in identity domains such as romantic relationships and sexuality (Arnett, 2000,

2014; Morgan, 2013; Rosario, 2019). For many women, this stage of the life course will

present their first opportunity to explore, acknowledge and/or act upon same-sex attractions and adopt a non-heterosexual identity. The net flow of women away from an exclusively heterosexual identity during this time is therefore to be expected. It may not be until the late

twenties or older ages that sexual orientation stabilizes in women who are not exclusively heterosexual (Rosario, 2019). We therefore anticipate that the distribution of sexual identity sequences in our sample will be different when these same women are observed during later stages of the life course, as the ALSWH continues collecting information on their lives and sexuality. Testing whether or not this is the case is a promising avenue for future research.

Interestingly, many women exhibited multidirectional sexual identity label sequences, with approximately one-third of women who changed away from heterosexuality subsequently returning back to it. The meaning of these trajectories could be interpreted in a

number of ways, and we can only speculate about them here. Perhaps these are women who

experimented briefly with same-sex sexuality in the process of developing a heterosexual identity. Or, perhaps these are women who were and remain attracted to both sexes, but their social/interpersonal contexts have changed and they have re-labelled their sexual orientation accordingly. Another interesting finding was the rarity of cross-continuum sequences, which were evident in less than 1% of women. When these did occur, they involved movement from the heterosexual end of the continuum to the lesbian end in the majority of cases.

Implications for research, policy and practice

25

By viewing sexual identity labels through a longitudinal lens, we have revealed non-

heterosexual identification to be less of a minority experience for today’s young women than

it might have been assumed. Cross-sectional data on sexual identity labels can only tell us about the proportion of women self-labelling as a sexual minority in that single moment in time. Our findings indicate that such “point prevalence” is likely to be substantially lower than the proportion of women who have ever self-labelled as a sexual minority (lifetime prevalence) or who have done so over a given period of time (period prevalence). For example, the point prevalence of sexual minority identification amongst the young women in our sample was 34% in 2013. However, between 2013 and 2017, the period prevalence of sexual minority identification amongst these same women was substantially higher, at 45%.

This has wide-ranging implications for the delivery of sexual-health services, the design of sex and relationship education programs, and the collection of sexual-orientation data in research and clinical settings.

Importantly, our findings reinforce calls for researchers to take a longitudinal, life-

course approach when examining relationships between sexual minority status and health and

wellbeing outcomes. Life-course scholars have long recognized that our ability to explain and

predict individual outcomes at a point in time is strengthened by considering not only the

person’s current status (e.g., marital or employment status), but also their preceding sequence

of statuses and the timing of transitions between statuses (Elder Jr. & Shanahan, 2006).

Likewise, we might improve our understanding of associations between sexual minority

status and life circumstances if we account for women’s sexual identity label histories.

Preliminary research, while extremely limited in size and scope, offers tentative support for the notion that women’s sexual identity histories are important predictors of their outcomes in domains including educational attainment (Walsemann, Lindley, Gentile, & Welihindha,

2014), mental health (Everett et al., 2016), and substance use (Ott et al., 2013). A possible

26

avenue for future research would be to test the predictive power of the sexual identity label sequence types proposed here on women’s health and wellbeing outcomes.

Study limitations

Despite the uniqueness of our approach and findings, our study involved data-driven limitations that future research may be able to rectify. Our study involved the analysis of

secondary data from a large-scale health survey. Consistent with other similar surveys (e.g.,

Add Health, GUTS), the ALSWH has measured sexual orientation identity labels on a single

Kinsey-type continuum. Yet, this approach has inherent limitations. For example, scholars

have criticized such items in the past for presenting a bipolar view of sexual orientation in

which “an individual expresses one orientation at the expense of the other” (Shively &

DeCecco, 1977, p. 45). It may therefore be preferable if surveys measured individual

components of sexual orientation—whether that be physical and affectional preferences

(Shively & DeCecco, 1977), erotic fantasies (Storms, 1980) or sexual attraction and behavior

(Vrangalova & Savin-Williams, 2012)—on two independent continua: a same-sex continuum

and an other-sex continuum. Looking at how individual’s scores these continua intersect

would arguably allow researchers to create more nuanced sexual orientation groups. In

addition, participants could be asked how they identify—which need not be measured on a

continuum at all. A further limitation of the item used to measure sexual orientation identity

labels in the ALSWH is that there is no dedicated category for asexual women—who,

according to Storms’ (1980) conceptualization, are low on both the same-sex and other-sex

continua. Knowledge of the sexual identity trajectories of these women is lacking as a result.

Our study was further limited by our inability to undertake analyses assessing conjoint

changes in sexual identity labels, attraction and behavior (see Berona, Stepp, Hipwell, &

Keenan, 2018; Kaestle, 2019)—as the ALSWH has only collected data on attraction and

27 behavior in its most recent sweep. This prevented us from ascertaining whether or not changes in women’s sexual identity labels coincided with changes in their sexual attractions and/or behavior. Few studies to date have looked into these issues. A recent example is the work of Kaestle (2019), who conducted a longitudinal latent class analysis using multiple waves of data on romantic attractions, sexual and romantic partnering and identity labels from participants in Add Health. Kaestle’s findings suggest that changes in romantic attractions often, but not always, go hand-in-hand with changes in sexual identity label.

Future studies should further investigate the extent to which changes in an individual’s sexual and romantic attractions and behavior overlap with changes to their sexual identity labels.

In addition, as we discussed before, large-scale survey data offers an opportunity to document the prevalence and patterns of changes in sexual identity labels in a representative sample. Yet these data are ill-placed to answer questions of phenomenology (Diamond,

2014). Thus, we do not know what the sexual identity labels mean to the women in our sample, or what their motivations are for changing labels. We also cannot rule out the possibility that some of the changes observed represent “noise” in the data, rather than genuine changes in how a woman perceives her sexual orientation. For example, it is possible that some women perceive their orientation as falling in between mostly heterosexual and bisexual, and they oscillate between the two labels as a result. Alternatively, this oscillation may reflect that some women do not perceive a meaningful difference between the two labels. Other changes might be the result of a woman feeling more comfortable reporting a non-heterosexual label over time.

Furthermore, we were unable to examine changes in sexual identity labels in men.

Over the past two decades, non-heterosexuality has not increased to the same extent amongst men as it has amongst women (England, Mishel, & Caudillo, 2016; Phillips et al., 2019). The sexual identity label trajectories of men are also likely to differ from those of women

28

(Kaestle, 2019; Rosario, 2019). Thus, it is not possible to extrapolate our findings from a

sample of women to the experiences of men. Robust, up-to-date evidence on the patterns and

prevalence of changes in sexual identity labels in young men is sorely needed (Savin-

Williams, 2017).

Conclusions

Our findings provide a unique, contemporary picture of changes in sexual identity labels amongst emerging adult women. This is an important endeavor, given the significant changes to the socio-political context in countries such as Australia and the U.S. over recent

decades, and the progressive increase in the reporting of non-heterosexuality by women.

Although the patterns of change in sexual identity labels documented here bore many

similarities to those observed in earlier samples, there were also some striking differences—

for instance, the increased stability of the plurisexual identities. Our study highlights the need

for panel and cohort studies to collect repeated measurements of all three dimensions of

sexual orientation (identity labels, attraction and behavior), and for existing cohort studies to

continue doing so. This will open up a range of analytical possibilities for researchers,

allowing them to create both longitudinal and multidimensional definitions of sexual minority

status. Ultimately, the availability of these data can go a long way in understanding and

addressing deep-rooted inequalities by sexual orientation in contemporary societies.

29

References American Psychological Association. (2018). Emerging Adulthood. Retrieved from

https://dictionary.apa.org/emerging-adulthood.

Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from the late teens

through the twenties. American Psychologist, 55(5), 469-480. doi:10.1037/0003-

066X.55.5.469

Arnett, J. J. (2004). Adolescence and emerging adulthood. Boston, MA: Pearson.

Berona, J., Stepp, S. D., Hipwell, A. E., & Keenan, K. E. (2018). Trajectories of sexual

orientation from adolescence to young adulthood: Results from a community-based

urban sample of girls. Journal of Adolescent Health, 63(1), 57-61.

doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2018.01.015

Bettinsoli, M. L., Suppes, A., & Napier, J. L. (2019). Predictors of attitudes toward

and lesbian women in 23 countries. Social Psychological and Personality Science.

doi:10.1177/1948550619887785

Brzinsky-Fay, C., Kohler, U., & Luniak, M. (2006). Sequence analysis with Stata. Stata

Journal, 6(4), 435-460. Retrieved from http://www.stata-

journal.com/article.html?article=st0111

Budnick, J. (2016). “Straight girls kissing”?: Understanding same-gender sexuality beyond

the elite college campus. Gender and Society, 30(5), 745-768.

doi:10.1177/0891243216657511

Chandra, A., Mosher, W. D., Copen, C., & Sionean, C. (2011). Sexual behavior, sexual

attraction, and sexual identity in the United States: Data from the 2006–2008

National Survey of Family Growth (National Health Statistics Reports, No.36).

Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/nhsr/nhsr036.pdf

30

Charlton, B. M., Corliss, H. L., Spiegelman, D., Williams, K., & Austin, S. B. (2016).

Changes in reported sexual orientation following US states recognition of same-sex

couples. American Journal of Public Health, 106(12), 2202-2204.

doi:10.2105/ajph.2016.303449

Copen, C. E., Chandra, A., & Febo-Vazquez, I. (2016). Sexual Behavior, Sexual Attraction,

and Sexual Orientation Among Adults Aged 18-44 in the United States: Data From

the 2011-2013 National Survey of Family Growth (National Health Statistics Reports,

No.88). Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov/nchs//data/nhsr/nhsr088.pdf de Visser, R. O., Badcock, P. B., Simpson, J. M., Grulich, A. E., Smith, A. M. A., Richters,

J., & Rissel, C. (2014). Attitudes toward sex and relationships: the second Australian

study of health and relationships. Sexual Health, 11(5), 397-405.

doi:10.1071/SH14099

Diamond, L. (2016). Sexual fluidity in male and females. Current Sexual Health Reports,

8(4), 249-256. doi:10.1007/s11930-016-0092-z

Diamond, L. M. (2014). Gender and same-sex sexuality. In D. L. Tolman, L. M. Diamond, J.

A. Bauermeister, W. H. George, J. G. Pfaus, & L. M. Ward (Eds.), APA handbook of

sexuality and psychology, Vol. 1. Person-based approaches (p. 629–652). American

Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/14193-020

Diamond, L. M. (2005). A new view of lesbian subtypes: Stable versus fluid identity

trajectories over an 8‐year period. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29(2), 119-128.

doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2005.00174.x

Diamond, L. M. (2008). Sexual fluidity: Understanding women's love and desire. Cambridge,

MA: Harvard University Press.

31

Dickson, N., Roode, T., Cameron, C., & Paul, C. (2013). Stability and change in same-sex

attraction, experience, and identity by sex and age in a New Zealand birth cohort.

Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42(5), 753-763. doi:10.1007/s10508-012-0063-z

Dillon, F. R., Worthington, R. L., & Moradi, B. (2011). Sexual identity as a universal

process. In S. J. Schwartz, K. Luyckx, & V. L. Vignoles (Eds.), Handbook of identity

theory and research (pp. 649-670). New York, NY: Springer New York.

Doan Van, E. E., Mereish, E. H., Woulfe, J. M., & Katz-Wise, S. L. (2018). Perceived

discrimination, coping mechanisms, and effects on health in bisexual and other non-

monosexual adults. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 48(1), 159–174.

doi:10.1007/s10508-018-1254-z

Dodge, B., Herbenick, D., Friedman, M. R., Schick, V., Fu, T.-C., Bostwick, W., . . .

Sandfort, T. G. M. (2016). Attitudes toward bisexual men and women among a

nationally representative probability sample of adults in the United States. PLoS ONE,

11(10), e0164430. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0164430

Elder Jr., G. H., Kirkpatrick Johnson, M., & Crosnoe, R. (2003). The emergence and

development of life course theory. In J. Mortimer & M. Shanahan (Eds.), Handbook

of the Life Course. Boston, MA: Springer US.

Elder Jr., G. H., & Shanahan, M. J. (2006). The life course and development. In W.

Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology : Theoretical Models

of Human Development. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

England, P., Mishel, E., & Caudillo, M. (2016). Increases in sex with same-sex partners and

bisexual identity across cohorts of women (but not men). Sociological Science, 3,

951-970. doi:10.15195/v3.a42

Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. New York, NY: WW Norton & Company.

32

Everett, B., Talley, A., Hughes, T., Wilsnack, S., & Johnson, T. (2016). Sexual identity

mobility and depressive symptoms: A longitudinal analysis of moderating factors

among sexual minority women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 45(7), 1731-1744.

doi:10.1007/s10508-016-0755-x

Fish, J. N., Watson, R. J., Porta, C. M., Russell, S. T., & Saewyc, E. M. (2017). Are

alcohol‐related disparities between sexual minority and heterosexual youth

decreasing? Addiction, 112(11), 1931-1941. doi:10.1111/add.13896.

Flanders, C. E., Dobinson, C., & Logie, C. (2017). Young bisexual women’s perspectives on

the relationship between bisexual stigma, mental health, and sexual health: A

qualitative study. Critical Public Health, 27(1), 75-85.

doi:10.1080/09581596.2016.1158786

Friedman, M. R., Dodge, B., Schick, V., Herbenick, D., Hubach, R., Bowling, J., . . . Reece,

M. (2014). From bias to bisexual health disparities: Attitudes toward bisexual men

and women in the United States. LGBT health, 1(4), 309-318.

doi:10.1089/.2014.0005

Glover, J. A., Galliher, R. V., & Crowell, K. A. (2015). Young women's passionate

friendships: A qualitative analysis. Journal of , 24(1), 70-84.

doi:10.1080/09589236.2013.820131

Goldbach, J. T., & Gibbs, J. J. (2017). A developmentally informed adaptation of minority

stress for sexual minority adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 55, 36-50.

doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.12.007

Gordon, L. E., & Silva, T. J. (2015). Inhabiting the sexual landscape: Toward an interpretive

theory of the development of sexual orientation and identity. Journal of

Homosexuality, 62(4), 495-530. doi:10.1080/00918369.2014.986417

33

Institute of Medicine. (2011). The health of lesbian, gay, bisexual, and people:

Building a foundation for better understanding: Washington, DC: National

Academies Press.

Kaestle, C. E. (2019). Sexual orientation trajectories based on sexual attractions, partners, and

identity: A longitudinal investigation from adolescence through young adulthood

using a U.S. representative sample. The Journal of Sex Research, 56(7), 811-826.

doi:10.1080/00224499.2019.1577351

Katz-Wise, S. L. (2015). Sexual fluidity in young adult women and men: Associations with

sexual orientation and sexual identity development. Psychology & Sexuality, 6(2),

189-208. doi: 10.1080/19419899.2013.876445

Katz-Wise, S., & Hyde, J. (2017). Facilitative environments related to sexual orientation

development and sexual fluidity in sexual minority young adults across different

gender identities. Journal of Bisexuality, 17(2), 141-171.

doi:10.1080/15299716.2016.1259138

Kitzinger, C., & Wilkinson, S. (1995). Transitions from heterosexuality to lesbianism: The

discursive production of lesbian identities. Developmental Psychology, 31(1), 95-104.

doi:10.1037/0012-1649.31.1.95

Loxton, D., Tooth, L., Harris, M. L., Forder, P. M., Dobson, A., Powers, J., . . . Mishra, G.

(2018). Cohort profile: The Australian Longitudinal Study on Women’s Health

(ALSWH) 1989–95 cohort. International Journal of Epidemiology, 47(2), 391-392e.

doi:10.1093/ije/dyx133

Marcia, J. E. (1987). Identity in adolescence. In J. Adelson (Ed.), Handbook of adolescent

psychology. NewYork: Wiley.

McGee, R. W. (2016). In which countries is homosexuality most (and least) acceptable? A

ranking of 98 countries. Retrieved from https://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2799845

34

Mendos, L. R. (2019). State-sponsored homophobia 2019. International Lesbian, Gay,

Bisexual, Trans and Association. Retrieved from https://ilga.org/state-

sponsored-homophobia-report

Mercer, C. H., Tanton, C., Prah, P., Erens, B., Sonnenberg, P., Clifton, S., . . . Johnson, A. M.

(2013). Changes in sexual attitudes and lifestyles in Britain through the life course

and over time: findings from the National Surveys of Sexual Attitudes and Lifestyles

(NATSAL). Lancet, 382(9907), 1781-1794. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(13)62035-8

Mock, S., & Eibach, R. (2012). Stability and change in sexual orientation identity over a 10-

year period in adulthood. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41(3), 641-648.

doi:10.1007/s10508-011-9761-1

Mohr, J. J. (2002). Heterosexual identity and the heterosexual therapist: An identity

perspective on sexual orientation dynamics in psychotherapy. The Counseling

Psychologist, 30(4), 532-566.

Morgan, E. M. (2012). Not always a straight path: College students’ narratives of

heterosexual identity development. Sex Roles, 66(1-2), 79-93. doi: 10.1007/s11199-

011-0068-4

Morgan, E. M. (2013). Contemporary issues in sexual orientation and identity development in

emerging adulthood. Emerging Adulthood, 1(1), 52-66.

doi:10.1177/2167696812469187

Morgan, E. M., & Thompson, E. M. (2006). Young women's sexual experiences within same-

sex friendships: Discovering and defining bisexual and bi-curious identity. Journal of

Bisexuality, 6(3), 7-34. doi:10.1300/J159v06n03_02

Morgan, E. M., & Thompson, E. M. (2011). Processes of sexual orientation questioning

among heterosexual women. The Journal of Sex Research, 48(1), 16.

doi:10.1080/00224490903370594

35

Ott, M., Corliss, H., Wypij, D., Rosario, M., & Austin, S. (2011). Stability and change in self-

reported sexual orientation identity in young people: Application of mobility metrics.

Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40(3), 519-532. doi:10.1007/s10508-010-9691-3

Ott, M. Q., Wypij, D., Corliss, H. L., Rosario, M., Reisner, S. L., Gordon, A. R., & Austin, S.

B. (2013). Repeated changes in reported sexual orientation identity linked to

substance use behaviors in youth. Journal of Adolescent Health, 52(4), 465-472.

doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.08.004

Perales, F., Bouma, G., & Campbell, A. (2018). Religion, support of equal rights for same-

sex couples and the Australian national vote on marriage equality. Sociology of

Religion, 80(1), 107–129. doi:10.1093/socrel/sry018

Perales, F., & Campbell, A. (2018). Who supports equal rights for same‑sex couples?

Evidence from Australia. Family Matters, 100, 14. Retrieved from

https://aifs.gov.au/publications/family-matters/issue-100/who-supports-equal-rights-

same-sex-couples

Perales, F., Campbell, A., & O'Flaherty, M. (2019). Sexual orientation and adolescent time

use: How sexual minority youth spend their time. Child Development, [Epub ahead of

print]. doi:10.1111/cdev.13245

Pew Research Center. (2013). A survey of LGBT Americans: Attitudes, experiences, and

values in changing times. Retrieved from

http://www.pewsocialtrends.org/2013/06/13/a-survey-of-lgbt-americans/#

Phillips, G., Beach, L. B., Turner, B., Feinstein, B. A., Marro, R., Philbin, M. M., . . . Birkett,

M. (2019). Sexual identity and behavior among U.S. high school students, 2005–

2015. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 48(5), 1463-1479. doi:10.1007/s10508-019-1404-

y

36

Preciado, M. A. (2013). A social psychological approach to sexual orientation: Theory and

empirical evidence (Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles,

U.S.). Retrieved from https://escholarship.org/uc/item/3qz854mc

Rich, A. (1980). Compulsory heterosexuality and lesbian existence. Signs, 5(4), 631-660.

Richters, J., Altman, D., Badcock, P. B., Smith, A. M. A., de Visser, R. O., Grulich, A. E., . .

. Simpson, J. M. (2014). Sexual identity, sexual attraction and sexual experience: The

second Australian Study of Health and Relationships. Sexual Health, 11(5), 451-460.

doi:https://doi.org/10.1071/SH14117

Rosario, M., Schrimshaw, E. W., & Hunter, J. (2008). Predicting different patterns of sexual

identity development over time among lesbian, gay, and bisexual youths: A cluster

analytic approach. American Journal of Community Psychology, 42(3-4), 266-282.

doi:10.1007/s10464-008-9207-7

Rosario, M., Schrimshaw, E. W., & Hunter, J. (2011). Different patterns of sexual identity

development over time: Implications for the psychological adjustment of lesbian, gay,

and bisexual youths. The Journal of Sex Research, 48(1), 3-15.

doi:10.1080/00224490903331067.

Rosario, M. (2019). Sexual orientation development of heterosexual, bisexual, lesbian, and

gay individuals: Questions and hypotheses based on Kaestle’s (2019) research. The

Journal of Sex Research, 56(7), 827-831. doi:10.1080/00224499.2019.1590796.

Ross, L. E., Tarasoff, L. A., Goldberg, A. E., & Flanders, C. E. (2017). Pregnant plurisexual

women's sexual and relationship histories across the life span: A qualitative study.

Journal of Bisexuality, 17(3), 257-276. doi:10.1080/15299716.2017.1344177

Rupp, L. J., & Taylor, V. (2010). Straight girls kissing. Contexts, 9(3), 28–32.

doi:10.1525/ctx.2010.9.3.28

37

Rust, P. (1992). The politics of sexual identity: Sexual attraction and behavior among lesbian

and bisexual women. Social Problems, 39(4), 366. doi:10.2307/3097016

Savin-Williams, R. C. (2011). Identity development among sexual-minority youth. In S. J.

Schwartz, K. Luyckx, & V. L. Vignoles (Eds.), Handbook of identity theory and

research (pp. 671-689). New York, NY: Springer New York.

Savin-Williams, R., Joyner, K., & Rieger, G. (2012). Prevalence and stability of self-reported

sexual orientation identity during young adulthood. Archives of Sexual Behavior,

41(1), 103-110. doi:10.1007/s10508-012-9913-y

Savin-Williams, R. C. (2017). Mostly straight: Sexual fluidity among men. Cambridge,

Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.

Savin-Williams, R. C., & Vrangalova, Z. (2013). Mostly heterosexual as a distinct sexual

orientation group: A systematic review of the empirical evidence. Developmental

Review, 33(1), 58-88. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2013.01.001

Shively, M. G., & De Cecco, J. P. (1977). Components of sexual identity. Journal of

Homosexuality, 3, 41–48. doi:10.1300/J082v03n01_04

Silva, T. J. (2019). Straight identity and same-sex desire: Conservatism, homophobia, and

straight culture. Social Forces, 97(3), 1067-1094. doi:10.1093/sf/soy064

Stanley, T. (2015). Religion after Secularization in Australia. New York, NY: Palgrave

Macmillan.

Storms, M. D. (1980). Theories of sexual orientation. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 38(5), 783–792. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.38.5.783

Thompson, E. M. (2006). Girl friend or girlfriend? Same-sex friendship and bisexual images

as a context for flexible sexual identity among young women. Journal of Bisexuality,

6(3), 47-67, doi:10.1300/J159v06n03_04

38

Thompson, E. M., & Morgan, E. M. (2008). “Mostly straight” young women: Variations in

sexual behavior and identity development. Developmental Psychology, 44(1), 15-21.

doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.15

Twenge, J. M., Sherman, R. A., & Wells, B. E. (2016). Changes in American adults’ reported

same-sex sexual experiences and attitudes, 1973–2014. Archives of Sexual Behavior,

45(7), 1713-1730. doi:10.1007/s10508-016-0769-4

Valfort, M.-A. (2017). LGBTI in OECD Countries: A Review (OECD Social, Employment

and Migration Working Papers, No. 198). Paris: OECD Publishing.

doi:10.1787/d5d49711-en

Vrangalova, Z., & Savin-Williams, R. (2012). Mostly heterosexual and mostly gay/lesbian:

Evidence for new sexual orientation identities. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41(1),

85-101. doi:10.1007/s10508-012-9921-y

Walsemann, K., Lindley, L., Gentile, D., & Welihindha, S. (2014). Educational attainment by

life course sexual attraction: prevalence and correlates in a nationally representative

sample of young adults. Population Research and Policy Review, 33(4), 579-602.

doi:10.1007/s11113-013-9288-3

Wiederman, M. W. (2005). The gendered nature of sexual scripts. The Family Journal, 13(4),

496-502.

Wolff, M., Wells, B., Ventura-Dipersia, C., Renson, A., & Grov, C. (2017). Measuring sexual

orientation: A review and critique of us data collection efforts and implications for

health policy. The Journal of Sex Research, 54(4-5), 507-531.

doi:10.1080/00224499.2016.1255872

39

Table 1. Sexual identity frequencies

All Observations Year 2013 Year 2014 Year 2015 Year 2017 Ages 18-27 Ages 18-23 Ages 19-24 Ages 20-25 Ages 22-27 n % n % n % n % n % Sexual identity Exclusively heterosexual 27,810 62.4 % 10,793 65.5 % 6,881 62.1 % 5,276 61.1 % 4,860 60.0 % Mostly heterosexual 11,384 25.6 % 4,076 24.4 % 2,936 26.5 % 2,258 26.1 % 2,114 26.1 % Bisexual 3,775 8.5 % 1,256 7.5 % 879 7.9 % 798 9.2 % 842 10.4 % Mostly lesbian 497 1.1 % 174 1.0 % 124 1.1 % 101 1.2 % 98 1.2 % Exclusively lesbian 391 0.9 % 143 0.9 % 91 0.8 % 79 0.9 % 78 1.0 % I don’t know 695 1.6 % 281 1.7 % 175 1.6 % 125 1.5 % 114 1.4 % N (observations) 44,552 16,723 11,086 8,637 8,106 N (individuals) 16,870 16,723 11,086 8,637 8,106 Note. Women born 1989-1995 from the ALSWH. Waves 1, 2, 3 & 5: 2013, 2014, 2015 & 2017.

40

Table 2. Sexual identity transition matrix

Sexual identity at wave t+1 Excl. hetero Mostly hetero Bisexual Mostly Excl. lesbian Don’t know lesbian Sexual identity at wave t Excl. hetero (n = 15,667) 87.9% 11.3% 0.5% <0.1% - 0.3% 13,768 1,764 81 5 0 49

Mostly hetero (n = 6,410) 20.9% 68.1% 8.6% 0.3% 0.1% 2.1% 1,338 4,365 548 17 8 134

Bisexual (n = 1,924) 1.9% 16.2% 76.0% 3.2% 0.6% 2.0% 36 312 1,463 62 12 39

Mostly lesbian (n = 269) 0.7% 1.5% 22.3% 59.1% 14.9% 1.5% 2 4 60 159 40 4

Excl. lesbian (n = 199) 0.5% 1.0% 3.5% 14.1% 80.4% 0.5% 1 2 7 28 160 1

Don’t know (n = 387) 13.4% 29.5% 18.9% 2.1% 0.8% 35.4% 52 114 73 8 3 137 Note. Women born 1989-1995 from the ALSWH. Waves 1, 2, 3 & 5: 2013, 2014, 2015 & 2017. Total number of paired-observations = 24,856 from 11,543 women. The stability of each identity is marked in bold (i.e., the observed probability that a woman would not change from that identity in the next wave).

41

Table 3. Typology of sexual identity sequences

Location on Type Subtype n % Example continuum Sequences EH 2,732 47.8% EH-EH-EH-EH MH 570 10.0% MH-MH-MH-MH Stable B 191 3.3% B-B-B-B n = 3,548 ML 19 0.3% ML-ML-ML-ML 62.0% EL 25 0.4% EL-EL-EL-EL U 11 0.2% U-U-U-U MH & U 69 1.2% MH-MH-U-MH Stable with questioning B & U 22 0.4% U-B-B-B n = 111 EH & U 17 0.3% EH-EH-U-EH 1.9% ML/EL & U 3 <0.1% U-EL-EL-EL Heterosexual end EH  MH EH-EH-EH-MH 531 9.3% n = 1,938 EH-MH-MH-MH 33.9% Towards bisexuality MH  B MH-MH-MH-B 170 3.0% (away from exclusivity) MH-MH-B-B n = 825 EH  (MH ) B EH-EH-B-B 71 1.2% 14.4% EH-EH-MH-B Towards bisexuality with 53 0.9% EH-EH-U-MH questioning MH-U-U-B MH  EH 313 5.5% MH-MH-MH-EH MH-EH-EH-EH B  MH 59 1.0% B-B-B-MH Towards exclusivity B-B-MH-MH n = 402 B  (MH ) EH 11 0.2% B-MH-EH-EH 7.0% B-B-B-EH Towards exclusivity with 19 0.3% MH-U-U-EH questioning B-B-U-MH Away  Towards 355 6.2% EH-EH-MH-EH Combination of away MH-B-B-MH from and towards Towards  Away 235 4.1% MH-EH-B-B exclusivity B-MH-MH-B n = 711 107 1.9% MH-B-MH-B Multidirectional 12.4% MH-EH-MH-EH Multidirectional with 14 0.2% MH-EH-MH-U questioning Lesbian end Towards exclusivity B  ML 9 0.2% B-B-ML-ML n = 82 n = 31 ML EL 8 0.1% ML-EL-EL-EL 1.4% 0.5% B  (ML) EL 8 0.1% B-ML-ML-EL Towards exclusivity with 6 0.1% U-ML-ML-EL questioning Towards bisexuality ML  B 18 0.3% ML-B-B-B (away from exclusivity) EL  ML 8 0.1% EL-EL-ML-ML n = 28 EL  B 1 <0.1% EL-EL-EL-B 0.5% Towards bisexuality with 1 <0.1% ML-U-B-B questioning Combination of away from and towards exclusivity 23 0.4% B-ML-ML-B n = 23, 0.4% Cross-continuum Hetero end  Lesbian 29 0.5% EH-MH-MH-EL n = 40 end MH-MH-ML-ML 0.7% Lesbian end  Hetero 10 0.2% EL-EL-EH-EH end ML-ML-ML-EH Double-cross 1 <0.1% MH-ML-ML-MH Total 5,719 100% Note. Women born 1989-1995 from the ALSWH. Waves 1, 2, 3 & 5: 2013, 2014, 2015 & 2017. EH=Exclusively Heterosexual; MH=Mostly Heterosexual; B=Bisexual; ML=Mostly Lesbian; EL=Exclusively Lesbian; U=Unsure (“I don’t know”).

42

Figure 1. Processes of sexual identity label change: towards bisexuality, towards exclusivity and cross-continuum change

43