Pupil Dilation Reflects Perceptual Selection and Predicts Subsequent Stability in Perceptual Rivalry

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Pupil Dilation Reflects Perceptual Selection and Predicts Subsequent Stability in Perceptual Rivalry Pupil dilation reflects perceptual selection and predicts subsequent stability in perceptual rivalry Wolfgang Einha¨ user*†, James Stout*, Christof Koch*, and Olivia Carter‡§¶ *Division of Biology, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA 91125; †Institute of Computational Science, ETH Zentrum, CAB G 81 Universita¨tstrasse, CH-8092 Zurich, Switzerland; ‡Vision Sciences Laboratory, Harvard University, 33 Kirkland Street, Cambridge, MA 02138; and §Brain Research Institute, Waterdale Road Heidelberg West, Victoria 3081, Australia Edited by Dale Purves, Duke University Medical Center, Durham, NC, and approved December 18, 2007 (received for review August 16, 2007) During sustained viewing of an ambiguous stimulus, an individu- In each individual, pupil diameter varies considerably during al’s perceptual experience will generally switch between the dif- constant presentation of an ambiguous visual stimulus (Fig. 1A). ferent possible alternatives rather than stay fixed on one inter- However, after aligning pupil diameter traces to the time of pretation (perceptual rivalry). Here, we measured pupil diameter reported perceptual switches and pooling the data, one observes while subjects viewed different ambiguous visual and auditory a sharp increase around the time of perceptual switching. This stimuli. For all stimuli tested, pupil diameter increased just before time course was qualitatively similar for all rivalry stimuli tested the reported perceptual switch and the relative amount of dilation (Fig. 1 B–E), and for all individuals [supporting information (SI) before this switch was a significant predictor of the subsequent Fig. 4]. A t test was used to compare pupil diameter to the 5-min duration of perceptual stability. These results could not be ex- mean (0 z score) at each of the 6,001 time points spanning Ϯ 3s plained by blink or eye-movement effects, the motor response or from the reported switch. To correct for multiple comparisons, stimulus driven changes in retinal input. Because pupil dilation significance is asserted only for time points with a P value below reflects levels of norepinephrine (NE) released from the locus that corresponding to an expected false discovery rate (FDR) of coeruleus (LC), we interpret these results as suggestive that the 0.05. Under this criterion, pupil diameter is significantly in- LC–NE complex may play the same role in perceptual selection as creased around the time of the perceptual switch in three of four in behavioral decision making. stimuli (black indicators in Fig. 1 B–E) and for five of six subjects (SI Fig. 4 and SI Table 1). When pooling over all subjects and attention ͉ norepinephrine ͉ vision ͉ decision making stimuli (Fig. 1F), the period of significance for the dilation response extends from 244 ms before to 1,552 ms after the reported switch (peaking at ϩ 602 ms: P ϭ 1.5 ϫ 10Ϫ18). ne pervading mystery in neuroscience is how the brain can Surrogate analysis rules out statistical artifacts (uncorrected P Ͼ Ogenerate an ‘‘internal’’ perceptual experience from the 0.12 for all time points). These findings provide evidence of a available ‘‘external’’ sensory information. Ambiguous stimuli, relationship between pupil dilation and perceptual switch events. like the Necker cube, offer a unique means to investigate this Because the pupil response spans nearly 2 s, pupil dilation at process because observers generally experience changes between any time point may be affected by either the preceding or the multiple perceptual states without corresponding changes in the following switch. Indeed, if we minimize the influence of in- stimulus (1). This phenomenon (‘‘perceptual rivalry’’) has been creased dilation stemming from the previous switch by excluding suggested to reflect a general strategy that balances a need for all short (Ͻ3 s) preswitch durations, the calculated preswitch a decisive stable percept for action planning (2), against the need diameter is systematically reduced, although the overall pattern for rapid reinterpretation of sensory information that is often of pupil dilation is conserved (Fig. 1F, green). Because the ambiguous or impoverished (3, 4). Neuroimaging and electro- dominance durations exhibit large variability within and across physiological studies are beginning to tease apart the different subjects (means ranging from 2–155 s, SI Table 2), we reanalyzed aspects of neural activity that correlate with the perception or pupil dilation with respect to a normalized time frame (9). This suppression of alternative perceptual states (for review see refs. maps each dominance period to a unit interval, aligning the 1 and 5). However, the mechanisms driving the switch in switches at time t ϭ 0 and the midpoints of each dominance perception are less clear. To date, no physiological marker has duration at Ϯ50%. This makes it possible to measure the been identified that shows any predictive relationship to the phase-shift of pupil-modulation relative to the surrounding duration of stability between successive switch events. switches. In this time frame, pupil dilation has a trough at Ϫ20%, Here we turn to pupil diameter, a physiological measure used i.e., a fifth cycle, before the switch (or four fifth cycles after the frequently half a century ago, but generally disregarded in preceding one), and peaks at ϩ13% after (ϭϪ87% before) each modern eye-tracking and imaging studies. A number of these switch (Fig. 1G). The switch coincides with the strongest slope older studies identified differential pupillary response to flashed of pupil dilation, whereas the mid-point of a dominance period Ϯ lights in the dominant or suppressed eye during binocular rivalry ( 50%) is associated with relatively stable pupil diameters. This (6–8). These findings remain intriguing; however, they are analysis shows that the rate of pupil dilation increase is maximal tangential to our current focus on the relationship between pupil around the time of perceptual switching, and starts just before diameter and the timing of perceptual rivalry switch events. its report. Results Author contributions: W.E. and O.C. designed research; W.E., J.S., and C.K. performed Pupil diameter of the right eye in six naı¨ve observers was research; C.K. contributed new reagents/analytic tools; W.E. analyzed data; and W.E. and recorded at 1 kHz during exposure to four different types of O.C. wrote the paper. rivalry stimuli: a Necker cube, structure from motion, visual The authors declare no conflict of interest. plaid, and auditory streaming. All measurements were recorded This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. in the dark. After familiarization with the stimuli, subjects were ¶To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected]. presented each stimulus for 5 min and instructed to immediately This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/cgi/content/full/ report any perceptual switch by pressing one of two keys 0707727105/DC1. (‘‘immediate report condition’’). © 2008 by The National Academy of Sciences of the USA 1704–1709 ͉ PNAS ͉ February 5, 2008 ͉ vol. 105 ͉ no. 5 www.pnas.org͞cgi͞doi͞10.1073͞pnas.0707727105 Downloaded by guest on September 27, 2021 A MM - Necker-Cube; immediate response condition A 10-6 C0.4 9000 exp1 rivalry ]z[ exp2 rivalry 10-5 exp1 surrog. exp2 replay .te dia m eter [au] exp2 rivalry o n -4 i 10 exp2 replay c o r r e l a t -3 l d i a m p u p i l 10 0 p=.009 -2 p 10 p=.018 i 0.1 p p<.016 u p p<.016 1 -0.4 6000 -3 0 +3 -3 0 +3 200 210 220 230 240 time to report [s] time to report [s] time since stimulus onset [s] B D +4 ] 0.2 z[ p u pil dia m et. [z] t=-596ms B 0.8 C0.8 p o s p u pil dia m et. [z] p u pil dian=367 m et. [z] n=136 0 y e - pupil e -0.2 0 15% 9% pupil .cc sk nil 0 0 a all (exp1) s b p<.022 plaid 2% p<.009 -4 5% -0.4 -0.4 -1 0 +1 -3 0 +3 -3 0 +3 -3 0 +3 rel. dominance duration time to report [s] time to report [s] time to report [s] Fig. 2. Prediction of dominance durations and control conditions. (A) Sig- nificance of the correlation between relative postswitch dominance duration D0.8 E 0.8 and pupil diameter plotted for each time point Ϯ 3 s around the switch. The ] p u pil dia m et. [z] n=398 n=220 z a m e t . [ logarithmic scale indicates higher significance (lower P values) toward the top. Black, all data from experiment 1; pupil dilation offers the greatest prediction of subsequent perceptual stability 596 ms before the reported switch (r ϭ 0.13, i ϭ ϫ Ϫ5 0 p u p i l d 0 P 8.5 10 ). Blue, all rivalry data from experiment 2. Red, replay data from experiment 2. The lack of significance in the replay condition shows that the p<.014 prediction effect is not an artifact of analysis. For experiment 1, where no -0.4 -0.4 -3 0 +3 -3 0 +3 replay condition exists, this verification is done through surrogate analysis time to report [s] time to report [s] (gray). Horizontal lines indicate the P value thresholds corresponding to an expected FDR of 0.05. (B) Plot of correlation for data of experiment 1 at the time point of peak significance (t ϭϪ596 ms). Pink circles mark data from plaid F 0.4 G0.3 - r m a stimulus, which are significant on their own right. (C) Experiment 2 pooled ]z[ . ]z[ i a m e t . [ z ] n=1121 n=1121 s o e n f n over subjects and stimuli. Red, replay; blue, rivalry.
Recommended publications
  • Longitudinal Investigation of Disparity Vergence in Young Adult Convergence Insufficiency Patients
    New Jersey Institute of Technology Digital Commons @ NJIT Theses Electronic Theses and Dissertations Summer 2019 Longitudinal investigation of disparity vergence in young adult convergence insufficiency patients Patrick C. Crincoli New Jersey Institute of Technology Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.njit.edu/theses Part of the Biomedical Engineering and Bioengineering Commons Recommended Citation Crincoli, Patrick C., "Longitudinal investigation of disparity vergence in young adult convergence insufficiency patients" (2019). Theses. 1683. https://digitalcommons.njit.edu/theses/1683 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Electronic Theses and Dissertations at Digital Commons @ NJIT. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons @ NJIT. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Copyright Warning & Restrictions The copyright law of the United States (Title 17, United States Code) governs the making of photocopies or other reproductions of copyrighted material. Under certain conditions specified in the law, libraries and archives are authorized to furnish a photocopy or other reproduction. One of these specified conditions is that the photocopy or reproduction is not to be “used for any purpose other than private study, scholarship, or research.” If a, user makes a request for, or later uses, a photocopy or reproduction for purposes in excess of “fair use” that user may be liable for copyright infringement, This institution
    [Show full text]
  • The Figure Is in the Brain of the Beholder: Neural Correlates of Individual Percepts in The
    The Figure is in the Brain of the Beholder: Neural Correlates of Individual Percepts in the Bistable Face-Vase Image A Thesis Presented to The Division of Philosophy, Religion, Psychology, and Linguistics Reed College In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Bachelor of Arts Phoebe Bauer May 2015 Approved for the Division (Psychology) Michael Pitts Acknowledgments I think some people experience a degree of unease when being taken care of, so they only let certain people do it, or they feel guilty when it happens. I don’t really have that. I love being taken care of. Here is a list of people who need to be explicitly thanked because they have done it so frequently and are so good at it: Chris: thank you for being my support system across so many contexts, for spinning with me, for constantly reminding me what I’m capable of both in and out of the lab. Thank you for validating and often mirroring my emotions, and for never leaving a conflict unresolved. Rennie: thank you for being totally different from me and yet somehow understanding the depths of my opinions and thought experiments. Thank you for being able to talk about magic. Thank you for being my biggest ego boost and accepting when I internalize it. Ben: thank you for taking the most important classes with me so that I could get even more out of them by sharing. Thank you for keeping track of priorities (quality dining: yes, emotional explanations: yes, fretting about appearances: nu-uh). #AshHatchtag & Stella & Master Tran: thank you for being a ceaseless source of cheer and laughter and color and love this year.
    [Show full text]
  • UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society
    UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society Title Voluntary versus Involuntary Perceptual Switching: Mechanistic Differences in Viewing an Ambiguous Figure Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/333348w4 Journal Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 27(27) ISSN 1069-7977 Authors Rambusch, Jana Ziemke, Tom Publication Date 2005 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California Voluntary versus Involuntary Perceptual Switching: Mechanistic Differences in Viewing an Ambiguous Figure Michelle Umali ([email protected]) Center for Neurobiology & Behavior, Columbia University 1051 Riverside Drive, New York, NY, 10032, USA Marc Pomplun ([email protected]) Department of Computer Science, University of Massachusetts at Boston 100 Morrissey Blvd., Boston, MA 02125, USA Abstract frequency, blink frequency, and pupil size, which have been robustly correlated with cognitive function (see Rayner, Here we demonstrate the mechanistic differences between 1998, for a review). Investigators utilizing this method have voluntary and involuntary switching of the perception of an examined the regions within ambiguous figures that receive ambiguous figure. In our experiment, participants viewed a attention during a specific interpretation, as well as changes 3D ambiguous figure, the Necker cube, and were asked to maintain one of two possible interpretations across four in eye movement parameters that may specify the time of different conditions of varying cognitive load. These switch. conditions differed in the instruction to freely view, make For example, Ellis and Stark (1978) reported that guided saccades, or fixate on a central cross. In the fourth prolonged fixation duration occurs at the time of perceptual condition, subjects were instructed to make guided saccades switching.
    [Show full text]
  • Frontoparietal Activity and Its Structural Connectivity in Binocular Rivalry
    Author's personal copy BRAIN RESEARCH 1305 (2009) 96– 107 available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Frontoparietal activity and its structural connectivity in binocular rivalry Juliane C. Wilckea,b,⁎, Robert P. O'Sheac,d, Richard Wattsb,e aDepartment of Psychology, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand bDepartment of Physics and Astronomy, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand cDepartment of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand dPsychology, School of Health and Human Sciences, Southern Cross University, Coffs Harbour, NSW, Australia eVan der Veer Institute for Parkinson's and Brain Research, Christchurch, New Zealand ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Article history: To understand the brain areas associated with visual awareness and their anatomical Accepted 20 September 2009 interconnections, we studied binocular rivalry with functional magnetic resonance imaging Available online 25 September 2009 (fMRI) and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI). Binocular rivalry occurs when one image is viewed by one eye and a different image by the other; it is experienced as perceptual alternations Keywords: between the two images. Our first experiment addressed problems with a popular Visual awareness comparison condition, namely permanentsuppression,bycomparingrivalrywith Conscious perception binocular fusion instead. We found an increased fMRI signal in right frontal, parietal, and Binocular rivalry occipital regions during rivalry viewing. The pattern of neural activity differed from findings Binocular fusion of permanent suppression comparisons, except for adjacent activity in the right superior fMRI parietal lobule. This location was near fMRI signal changes related to reported rivalry DTI tractography alternations in our second experiment, indicating that neighbouring areas in the right parietal cortex may be involved in different components of rivalry.
    [Show full text]
  • M Pathway and Areas 44 and 45 Are Involved in Stereoscopic Recognition Based on Binocular Disparity
    Japanese Journal of Physiology, 52, 191–198, 2002 M Pathway and Areas 44 and 45 Are Involved in Stereoscopic Recognition Based on Binocular Disparity Tsuneo NEGAWA, Shinji MIZUNO*, Tomoya HAHASHI, Hiromi KUWATA†, Mihoko TOMIDA, Hiroaki HOSHI*, Seiichi ERA, and Kazuo KUWATA Departments of Physiology, * Radiology, and † Nursing Course, Gifu University School of Medicine, Gifu, 500–8705 Japan Abstract: We characterized the visual path- was reported that these regions were inactive ways involved in the stereoscopic recognition of during the monocular stereopsis. To separate the the random dot stereogram based on the binocu- specific responses directly caused by the stereo- lar disparity employing a functional magnetic res- scopic recognition process from the nonspecific onance imaging (fMRI). The V2, V3, V4, V5, in- ones caused by the memory load or the inten- traparietal sulcus (IPS) and the superior temporal tion, we designed a novel frequency labeled sulcus (STS) were significantly activated during tasks (FLT) sequence. The functional MRI using the binocular stereopsis, but the inferotemporal the FLT indicated that the activation of areas 44 gyrus (ITG) was not activated. Thus a human M and 45 is correlated with the stereoscopic recog- pathway may be part of a network involved in the nition based on the binocular disparity but not stereoscopic processing based on the binocular with the intention artifacts, suggesting that areas disparity. It is intriguing that areas 44 (Broca’s 44 and 45 play an essential role in the binocular area) and 45 in the left hemisphere were also ac- disparity. [Japanese Journal of Physiology, 52, tive during the binocular stereopsis.
    [Show full text]
  • Symmetric Networks with Geometric Constraints As Models of Visual Illusions
    S S symmetry Article Symmetric Networks with Geometric Constraints as Models of Visual Illusions Ian Stewart 1,*,† and Martin Golubitsky 2,† 1 Mathematics Institute, University of Warwick, Coventry CV4 7AL, UK 2 Department of Mathematics, Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected] † These authors contributed equally to this work. Received: 17 May 2019; Accepted: 13 June 2019; Published: 16 June 2019 Abstract: Multistable illusions occur when the visual system interprets the same image in two different ways. We model illusions using dynamic systems based on Wilson networks, which detect combinations of levels of attributes of the image. In most examples presented here, the network has symmetry, which is vital to the analysis of the dynamics. We assume that the visual system has previously learned that certain combinations are geometrically consistent or inconsistent, and model this knowledge by adding suitable excitatory and inhibitory connections between attribute levels. We first discuss 4-node networks for the Necker cube and the rabbit/duck illusion. The main results analyze a more elaborate model for the Necker cube, a 16-node Wilson network whose nodes represent alternative orientations of specific segments of the image. Symmetric Hopf bifurcation is used to show that a small list of natural local geometric consistency conditions leads to alternation between two global percepts: cubes in two different orientations. The model also predicts brief transitional states in which the percept involves impossible rectangles analogous to the Penrose triangle. A tristable illusion generalizing the Necker cube is modelled in a similar manner.
    [Show full text]
  • The Perception of Transparent Three-Dimensional Objects (Vision͞illusion͞visual Learning͞association)
    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 94, pp. 6517–6522, June 1997 Neurobiology The perception of transparent three-dimensional objects (visionyillusionyvisual learningyassociation) DALE PURVES* AND TIMOTHY J. ANDREWS Department of Neurobiology, Box 3209, Duke University Medical Center, Durham, NC 27710 Contributed by Dale Purves, April 9, 1997 ABSTRACT When the proximal and distal elements of orientation, the cube appears to be balanced on its distal– wire-frame cubes are conflated, observers perceive illusory inferior vertex, with the surface on which it actually rests rising structures that no longer behave veridically. These phenom- from the balance point (see Figs. 1 and 2). (Illusory, in this ena suggest that what we normally see depends on visual case, means an interpretation of the stimulus that does not associations generated by experience. The necessity of such accord with the configuration of the object determined by learning may explain why the mammalian visual system is direct measurement.) In short, the observer no longer judges subject to a prolonged period of plasticity in early life, when the object to be a cube, despite the unchanged retinal image, novel circuits are made in enormous numbers. knowledge of its actual structure, and the immediately pre- ceding perception of a cube in top-down view. Information generated by the eyes is ambiguous. Everyday we A first order explanation of these phenomena follows from have to make decisions (about the size and distance of objects, the geometry of the situation. Because of their greater dis- their form, and whether they are moving) based on retinal tance, the angles subtended on the retina by the distal elements images that can have two or more meanings (1–4).
    [Show full text]
  • Brain Networks Underlying Bistable Perception
    This is a repository copy of Brain networks underlying bistable perception. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/88383/ Version: Accepted Version Article: Baker, Daniel Hart orcid.org/0000-0002-0161-443X, Karapanagiotidis, Theodoros orcid.org/0000-0002-0813-1019, Coggan, David et al. (2 more authors) (2015) Brain networks underlying bistable perception. Neuroimage. pp. 229-234. ISSN 1053-8119 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.053 Reuse Items deposited in White Rose Research Online are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved unless indicated otherwise. They may be downloaded and/or printed for private study, or other acts as permitted by national copyright laws. The publisher or other rights holders may allow further reproduction and re-use of the full text version. This is indicated by the licence information on the White Rose Research Online record for the item. Takedown If you consider content in White Rose Research Online to be in breach of UK law, please notify us by emailing [email protected] including the URL of the record and the reason for the withdrawal request. [email protected] https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/ Baker, Karapanagiotidis, Coggan, Wailes-Newson & Smallwood (2015) Neuroimage, 119: 229-234 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.053 Brain networks underlying bistable perception Daniel H. Baker, Theodoros Karapanagiotidis, David D. Coggan, Kirstie Wailes-Newson & Jonathan Smallwood Department of Psychology, University of York, YO10 5DD, UK email: [email protected] Abstract Bistable stimuli, such as the Necker Cube, demonstrate that experience can change in the absence of changes in the environment.
    [Show full text]
  • Cueing the Necker Cube: Pupil Dilation Reflects the Viewing-From-Above Constraint in Bistable Perception
    Journal of Vision (2020) 20(4):7, 1–14 1 Cueing the Necker cube: Pupil dilation reflects the viewing-from-above constraint in bistable perception Department of Computer Science and Engineering, Toyohashi University of Technology, Toyohashi, Aichi, Fumiaki Sato Japan Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway RITMO Centre for Interdisciplinary Studies in Rhythm, Bruno Laeng Time, and Motion, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Department of Computer Science and Engineering, Toyohashi University of Technology, Toyohashi, Aichi, Shigeki Nakauchi Japan Department of Computer Science and Engineering, Toyohashi University of Technology, Toyohashi, Aichi, Tetsuto Minami Japan We hypothesized that a perceptually ambiguous or bistable object (Necker cube) can be more effectively Introduction biased to assume a point of view-from-above (VFA) than from below the object by cueing attention. Participants Our visual system receives two-dimensional viewed a Necker cube in which one surface was information from each image on the retina. From temporarily shaded so as to prime a specific perspective these two-dimensional images, it restores to vision on the cube. Subsequently, the standard (wireframe) the three-dimensional shape of the object, sometimes Necker cube was viewed for 3 seconds, and participants resolving ambiguities in the visual input in an automatic reported what perspective they had seen initially and and unconscious manner. As a way of resolving whether their perception shifted to the alternative perceptual ambiguity, Nakayama and Shimojo (1992) perspective during the brief viewing. Concomitantly, proposed a theoretical framework in which the visual pupil size was monitored with an eye-tracker to obtain system tends to interpret images and surfaces as an index of cognitive effort.
    [Show full text]
  • Depth Cues, Rather Than Perceived Depth, Govern Vergence
    Exp Brain Res (2008) 184:61–70 DOI 10.1007/s00221-007-1081-2 RESEARCH ARTICLE Depth cues, rather than perceived depth, govern vergence D. A. Wismeijer · R. van Ee · C. J. Erkelens Received: 5 May 2007 / Accepted: 21 July 2007 / Published online: 24 August 2007 © Springer-Verlag 2007 Abstract We studied the inXuence of perceived surface Introduction orientation on vergence accompanying a saccade while viewing an ambiguous stimulus. We used the slant rivalry In vision, binocular fusion is facilitated by vergence. Vari- stimulus, in which perspective foreshortening and disparity ous diVerent types of horizontal vergence have been distin- speciWed opposite surface orientations. This rivalrous con- guished based on inputs used by the vergence system, e.g. Wguration induces alternations of perceived surface orienta- binocular disparity and accommodation. One of those, tion, while the slant cues remain constant. Subjects were fusional vergence, uses binocular disparity as input. And able to voluntarily control their perceptual state while view- another, proximal vergence, is associated with “knowledge ing the ambiguous stimulus. They were asked to make a of nearness” (Howard, 2002). Recently, the inXuence of saccade across the perceived slanted surface. Our data show depth perception on vergence has regained interest, because that vergence responses closely approximated the vergence of the possibility to probe perception through vergence. In response predicted by the disparity cue, irrespective of vol- several studies, various depth cues were used to induce untarily controlled perceived orientation. However, com- depth and the inXuence of perception on vergence was stud- paring the data obtained while viewing the ambiguous ied both without and with binocular disparity present.
    [Show full text]
  • Right Parietal Brain Activity Precedes Perceptual Alternation During Binocular Rivalry
    r Human Brain Mapping 32:1432–1442 (2011) r Right Parietal Brain Activity Precedes Perceptual Alternation During Binocular Rivalry Juliane Britz,1* Michael A. Pitts,2 and Christoph M. Michel2 1Department of Fundamental Neuroscience, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland 2Department of Neurosciences, University of California, San Diego, California r r Abstract: We investigated perceptual reversals for intermittently presented stimuli during binocular ri- valry and physical alternation while the ongoing EEG was recorded from 64 channels. EEG topogra- phies immediately preceding stimulus-onset were analyzed and two topographies doubly dissociated perceptual reversals from non-reversals. The estimated intracranial generators associated with these topographies were stronger in right inferior parietal cortex and weaker bilaterally in the ventral stream before perceptual reversals. No such differences were found for physical alternation of the same stim- uli. These results replicate and extend findings from a previous study with the Necker cube and sug- gest common neural mechanisms associated with perceptual reversals during binocular rivalry and ambiguous figure perception. For both types of multi-stable stimuli, the dorsal stream is more active preceding perceptual reversals. Activity in the ventral stream, however, differed for binocular rivalry compared to ambiguous figures. The results from the two studies suggest a causal role for the right in- ferior parietal cortex in generating perceptual reversals regardless of the type of multi-stable stimulus, while activity in the ventral stream appears to depend on the particular type of stimulus. Hum Brain Mapp 32:1432–1442, 2011. VC 2010 Wiley-Liss, Inc. Key words: EEG; EEG mapping; binocular rivalry; inverse solution; LORETA; right inferior parietal r r INTRODUCTION every few seconds between the two rivaling percepts.
    [Show full text]
  • Reality, Perception, and Simulation: a Plausible Theory
    WILLIAM R. POWELL REALITY, PERCEPTION, AND SIMULATION: A PLAUSIBLE THEORY Vision is such an automatic process that few people think about it. In this article, I examine some new and well-known visual phenomena and suggest a framework for understanding them. I conclude with some speculation about the nature of experienced reality and the classical philosophical problem of free will. The proposition advanced is that all perception is contained within a simulation of physical reality created by the brain and, further, that this simulated reality has at least as strong a claim to being "real" as the physical reality that is inferred on the basis of the brain's simulation and analysis. INTRODUCTION doesn't the world appear to lurch as object images jump When most people think about vision, they are usually to new locations on the retina several times each second? concerned only with how an image is focused on their Yet another puzzle: How does the brain recognize that retinas. But that part of the visual process is relatively an object that is activating a set of neurons in one small simple. Even the electrochemical process that converts part of the periphery of the retina on one fixation is the same object that was creating different neural activity in the photons of light into a two-dimensional (2D) pattern of neural activity in retinal neurons is simple in compar­ another part of the retina during the previous fixation? ison to what is required to convert those signals (retinal The problem becomes even more complex when we cell output pulses) into a visual experience of recognized recognize that the shadows of the retinal blood vessels fragment the object images into many different parts with objects in a perceived three-dimensional (3D) world.
    [Show full text]