“Showing” Gesture in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

“Showing” Gesture in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Beyond Pointing: Development of the “Showing” Gesture in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Caitlin Clements & Katarzyna Chawarska YALE UNIVERSITY While research on early indicators of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) has focused predominantly on infants’ inability to point, other social gestures such as showing may also help to distinguish ASD from non- autistic language delays in high-risk populations. Parents of infants at high-risk (HR) for ASD ( n = 28) and typically developing infants at low-risk (TD-LR; n = 12) reported their child’s gesture use at nine and twelve months. Infants were followed until 24 months, at which time 8 high-risk children showed symptoms of ASD (ASD-HR), 14 showed symptoms of language delay (LD-HR), and 6 were developing typically (TD-HR). Results indicate a lack of pointing in both ASD-HR and LD- HR groups at twelve months, but decreased frequency of showing on- ly in the ASD-HR group. These results suggest that the showing ges- ture might be a better indicator of ASD than pointing among infant siblings of children with ASD. Implications for early identification are discussed. 46 INTRODUCTION early recognition and diagnosis must rely on indicators of nonverbal communication Autism Spectrum Disorder skills that typically emerge before twelve Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a group months of age. of neurodevelopmental disorders character- ized by profound deficits in communication, Nonverbal communication social functioning, and the presence of re- Nonverbal communication skills develop petitive and stereotyped behaviors or inter- before verbal language and therefore can ests (American Psychiatric Association, play an important role in identification of 2004). It affects 1 in 110 children in the US ASD during the first year of life (Baron- (CDC, 2009) and in most cases impairs the Cohen, Cox, Baird, Sweettenham, & Night- child’s ability to function independently ingale, 1996; Chawarska et al., 2007; Lord, (Howlin, 2005). Because there is currently 1995; Mundy, Sigman, Ungerer, & Sherman, no known cure, to date the best means of 1986; Paul, 2007; Stone, Ousley, Yoder, Ho- mitigating the negative effects of ASD are gan, & Hepburn, 1997; Wetherby, 1986). early identification. This way, children can Nonverbal communication behaviors such begin an early behavioral intervention pro- as pointing, waving goodbye, and smiling gram (Klin, Chawarska, Rubin, & Volkmar, convey a message to another person without 2004). language (Tager-Flusberg et al., 2005). Nonverbal communication is particularly ASD and nonverbal communication relevant to early ASD diagnosis because Parents and clinicians use early indicators, children use hand gestures such as reach- such as delayed speech and language, to ing, pointing, and giving to express their recognize ASD during the second year of life desires before they can do so with verbal (Chawarska et al., 2007). Though commu- language. In typical development, gestures nication is often impaired in ASD (American emerge around eight to ten months and are Psychiatric Association [ DSM-IV-TR ], 2004), used frequently until around 18 months, identification during the first year of life when their use begins to fade as verbal lan- cannot depend on verbal language alone. guage develops to replace them (Bates & Even in typical development, children do not Dick, 2002; Capone & McGregor, 2004). speak their first words until around twelve months of age and do not combine words Pointing into phrases until around 18 months (Tager- The vast majority of studies on nonverbal Flusberg, Paul, & Lord, 2005). Therefore, communication in ASD have focused on The authors would like to give special thanks to Jessie Bradshaw, Suzanne Macari, and Fred Shic. The first author can be reached at [email protected] or at Massachusetts General Hospital, Simches Research Bldg., 6th floor, 185 Cambridge St., Boston, MA 02114. 47 THE YALE REVIEW OF UNDERGRADUATE RESEARCH IN PSYCHOLOGY pointing gestures. Pointing emerges around 3.5 years, Baron-Cohen et al. (1996) found eight to ten months in typically developing that a deficit in protodeclarative pointing children (Bates & Dick 2004; Capone & was one of the three best predictors of au- McGregor, 2004), and accounts for the ma- tism. (Gaze monitoring and pretend play jority (60%) of gestures by twelve months also predicted autism.) An early deficit in (Kita, 2003). There are two types of point- protodeclarative pointing is consistent with ing: protodeclarative and protoimperative the social impairments that characterize au- (Paul, 2007; Tomasello, 2007). Protoimpera- tism, and other highly social gestures are tive points represent desire for an object, likely to be impaired early as well. e.g., pointing to a cookie. Protodeclarative points indicate the desire to share an experi- Other protodeclarative gestures: giving and ence with another person, e.g., a child showing pointing to an airplane overhead. Protode- Though pointing is a crucial indicator of so- clarative pointing requires joint attention, or cial functioning, other gestures such as the ability to share experiences with others showing and giving also reflect social skills by attracting or following their attention by and interest. Showing refers to a person’s looking or pointing (Paul, 2007; Tomasello, arm extending toward another person’s face 2007; Loveland & Landry, 1986). Joint atten- while holding an object (Wetherby, Yonclas, tion is an early emerging social cognitive & Btyan, 1989; Stone et al., 1997). Giving re- skill and is impaired in autism (Dawson et fers to placing an object in another person’s al., 2004; Loveland & Landry, 1986). hand or pushing an object at least halfway toward another person (Wetherby et al., Pointing in Autism Spectrum Disorders 1989; Stone et al., 1997). While many in- In autism, protodeclarative pointing is im- stances of giving serve protodeclarative pur- paired while protoimperative pointing re- poses (e.g., giving an interesting object to a mains relatively intact, especially in older parent), others serve protoimperative pur- children (Baron-Cohen, 1989; Camaioni, poses (e.g., handing a jar to a parent to get it Perucchini, Muratori, & Milone, 1997; Ca- opened) or occur in the context of sharing maioni, Perucchini, Muratori, Parrinini, & (e.g., giving a cracker or a toy to another Cesari, 2003; Itoh, 2000). Because protode- person). Both showing and giving emerge clarative pointing requires social interest, its around eight to ten months in typically de- deficit is considered one of the critical ASD veloping children, concurrently with point- risk indicators on a number of screening ing (Capone & McGregor, 2004; Bates & instruments, including the Checklist for Au- Dick, 2004). tism in Toddlers (CHAT; Baron -Cohen, Al- len, & Gillberg, 1992) and Modified Check- Giving and showing in Autism Spectrum list for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT; Rob- Disorder ins, Fein, Barton, & Green, 2001). Pointing Little is known about the early development deficits have proven to be an effective flag of giving and showing in children with ASD, for autism as measured by the longitudinal but studies of older children with ASD have stability of CHAT results. In a study that fol- found deficits in both types of gestures. lowed 16,000 children from 18 months to Children with autism at 52 months were less BEYOND POINTING CLEMENTS & CHAWARSKA 48 likely to give, take, and show objects in re- found that of 469 Danish people diagnosed sponse to adult cues compared to develop- with a developmental language delay as mentally delayed children (Leekam & Rams- children, 10 later carried a diagnosis of ASD den, 2006). At 49 months, children with in adulthood (Mouridsen & Hauschild, ASD showed similar differences in frequen- 2009). Deficits in social gestures may pro- cy of giving (DiLavore, Lord, & Rutter, vide an extra indicator in these ambiguous 1995). Children with ASD demonstrated situations. Wetherby and Prizant (2002) fewer “gestural joint attention skills” (de- found that a deficit in showing differentiat- fined as pointing and showing) at 45 ed children with ASD from both develop- months when compared to children with mentally delayed and typically developing mental retardation matched on language, children. Protodeclarative pointing, howev- mental age, or IQ (Mundy, Sigman, & Kasa- er, did not distinguish children with ASD ri, 1990). Wetherby, Watt, Morgan, and from developmentally delayed children. A Shumway (2007) found that a strong predic- similar problem was also noted in the Bar- tor of ASD between 24 and 36 months was on-Cohen et al. (1996) study on 18-month “inventory of gesture,” which assessed the olds. These results suggest that other signs children’s use of eight gestures and includ- beyond pointing are necessary for distin- ed showing, pointing and giving. In an ear- guishing between these groups. Showing lier prospective study, the authors found and giving, which depend heavily on social that limited showing was a red flag that dif- skills, may serve this purpose. ferentiated children with ASD from both de- velopmentally delayed and typically develop- Diagnosis in high-risk populations ing children. Frequency of pointing, howev- Distinguishing ASD from similar non- er, did not differ between children with ASD autistic disorders is of particular concern and those with developmental delays. When when diagnosing children at high risk for Stone et al. (1997) measured giving and ASD. Children with an older sibling with showing within the same age
Recommended publications
  • The Meanings of the Term Mudra and a Historical Outline of "Hand
    The Meanings of the term Mudra T h e M and a Historical Outline of ae n ni "Hand gestures" g s o f ht e Dale Todaro t re m M u d 梗 概 ar a この 拙 論 は2部 に分 か れ る。 n d 第1部 は"mudra"と い う語 の最 も一 般 的 な 定 義 を 扱 う。仏 教 ・ヒ ン ドゥー 教 a H を 研 究 して い る学 者 や東 洋 の 図像 学 の専 門 家 は、 大 抵、"皿udra"の さ ま ざ まな 意 i torical Outline味 を 知 って い る。 しか し、特 に タ ン トラ にお い て 使 用 され た"mudr翫"の す べ て の 定 義 が、 どん な 参考 文 献 に も見 つ か るわ け で は な い。 従 って、 第1部 は これ ら 種 々の、 一 般 的 な"mudra"の 語 法 を集 め る よ う試 み た。 又、 イ ン ドの舞 踏 や 劇 につ いて 書 いた 人 が、"hasta"と い う語 を 使 用 す べ きで あ るの に、 専 門的 に言 え ば 誤 って"mudra"を 用 いて い る。 それ に つ いて も説 明 を試 み た。 fo " 第1部 よ りも長 い 第2部 で は、"印 契(手 印)"と い う意 味 で使 用 され た"mu- H a dra"の 歴 史 の あ らま しを、 系 統 的 に述 べ た。 印契 の歴 史 上 異 な った 使 用 と意 味 n d g は、 次 の4に お い て 顕著 にみ られ る。 即 ち、1)ヴ ェー ダ の儀 礼、2)規 格 化 され た se ut イ ン ドの舞 踏、3)イ ン ドの彫 刻(仏 教、 ヒ ン ド ゥー 教、 ジ ャイ ナ教)、4)タ ン ト r s"e ラの 成 就 法、 で あ る。 これ ら4の 分 野 は す べ て、 共 通 して、 イ ン ドで 使 用 され た 印 契 の 伝統 か ら由 来 して い る。 そ しで、 い くつか の事 例 に お いて、 イ ン ドか ら 日 本 密 教 の 伝 統 まで に わ た って、 特 定 の"mudra"が 驚 くほ ど継 続 して 使 用 され て い るこ とが、 証 明 で き る。 Introduction The goal of this short essay is twofold.
    [Show full text]
  • This List of Gestures Represents Broad Categories of Emotion: Openness
    This list of gestures represents broad categories of emotion: openness, defensiveness, expectancy, suspicion, readiness, cooperation, frustration, confidence, nervousness, boredom, and acceptance. By visualizing the movement of these gestures, you can raise your awareness of the many emotions the body expresses without words. Openness Aggressiveness Smiling Hand on hips Open hands Sitting on edge of chair Unbuttoning coats Moving in closer Defensiveness Cooperation Arms crossed on chest Sitting on edge of chair Locked ankles & clenched fists Hand on the face gestures Chair back as a shield Unbuttoned coat Crossing legs Head titled Expectancy Frustration Hand rubbing Short breaths Crossed fingers “Tsk!” Tightly clenched hands Evaluation Wringing hands Hand to cheek gestures Fist like gestures Head tilted Pointing index finger Stroking chins Palm to back of neck Gestures with glasses Kicking at ground or an imaginary object Pacing Confidence Suspicion & Secretiveness Steepling Sideways glance Hands joined at back Feet or body pointing towards the door Feet on desk Rubbing nose Elevating oneself Rubbing the eye “Cluck” sound Leaning back with hands supporting head Nervousness Clearing throat Boredom “Whew” sound Drumming on table Whistling Head in hand Fidget in chair Blank stare Tugging at ear Hands over mouth while speaking Acceptance Tugging at pants while sitting Hand to chest Jingling money in pocket Touching Moving in closer Dangerous Body Language Abroad by Matthew Link Posted Jul 26th 2010 01:00 PMUpdated Aug 10th 2010 01:17 PM at http://news.travel.aol.com/2010/07/26/dangerous-body-language-abroad/?ncid=AOLCOMMtravsharartl0001&sms_ss=digg You are in a foreign country, and don't speak the language.
    [Show full text]
  • Hand Gestures
    L2/16-308 More hand gestures To: UTC From: Peter Edberg, Emoji Subcommittee Date: 2016-10-31 Proposed characters Tier 1: Two often-requested signs (ILY, Shaka, ILY), and three to complete the finger-counting sets for 1-3 ​ (North American and European system). None of these are known to have offensive connotations. HAND SIGN SHAKA ● Shaka sign ​ ● ASL sign for letter ‘Y’ ● Can signify “Aloha spirit”, surfing, “hang loose” ● On Emojipedia top requests list, but requests have dropped off ​ ​ ● 90°-rotated version of CALL ME HAND, but EmojiXpress has received requests for SHAKA specifically, noting that CALL ME HAND does not fulfill need HAND SIGN ILY ● ASL sign for “I love you” (combines signs for I, L, Y), has moved into ​ ​ mainstream use ● On Emojipedia top requests list ​ HAND WITH THUMB AND INDEX FINGER EXTENDED ● Finger-counting 2, European style ● ASL sign for letter ‘L’ ● Sign for “loser” ● In Montenegro, sign for the Liberal party ● In Philippines, sign used by supporters of Corazon Aquino ● See Wikipedia entry ​ ​ HAND WITH THUMB AND FIRST TWO FINGERS EXTENDED ● Finger-counting 3, European style ● UAE: Win, victory, love = work ethic, success, love of nation (see separate proposal L2/16-071, which is the source of the information ​ ​ below about this gesture, and also the source of the images at left) ● Representation for Ctrl-Alt-Del on Windows systems ● Serbian “три прста” (tri prsta), symbol of Serbian identity ​ ​ ● Germanic “Schwurhand”, sign for swearing an oath ​ ​ ● Indication in sports of successful 3-point shot (basketball), 3 successive goals (soccer), etc. HAND WITH FIRST THREE FINGERS EXTENDED ● Finger-counting 3, North American style ● ASL sign for letter ‘W’ ● Scout sign (Boy/Girl Scouts) is similar, has fingers together ​ Tier 2: Complete the finger-counting sets for 4-5, plus some less-requested hand signs.
    [Show full text]
  • Pointing Gesture in Young Children Hand Preference and Language Development
    Pointing gesture in young children Hand preference and language development Hélène Cochet and Jacques Vauclair Aix-Marseille University This paper provides an overview of recent studies that have investigated the development of pointing behaviors in infants and toddlers. First, we focus on deictic gestures and their role in language development, taking into account the different hand shapes and the different functions of pointing, and examining the cognitive abilities that may or may not be associated with the production of pointing gestures. Second, we try to demonstrate that when a distinction is made between pointing gestures and manipulative activities, the study of children’s hand preference can help to highlight the development of speech-gesture links. Keywords: toddlers, gestural communication, pointing, handedness, speech- gesture system Emergence of communicative gestures: Focus on pointing gestures Pointing is a specialized gesture for indicating an object, event or location. Chil- dren start using pointing gestures at around 11 months of age (Butterworth & Morissette, 1996; Camaioni, Perucchini, Bellagamba, & Colonnesi, 2004), and this behavior opens the door to the development of intentional communication. One of the prerequisites for the production of pointing gestures is a shared experi- ence between the signaler and the recipient of the gesture, that is, a simultaneous engagement with the same external referent, usually referred to as joint attention (e.g., Carpenter, Nagell, & Tomasello, 1998). While pointing is sometimes regard- ed as a “private gesture” (Delgado, Gómez, & Sarriá, 2009), whose main role is to regulate the infant’s attention rather than to enable the latter to communicate with a recipient, a growing body of research suggests that the onset of pointing gestures reflects a newly acquired ability to actively direct the adult’s attention to outside entities in triadic interactions (e.g., Liszkowski, 2005; Tomasello, Carpenter, & Liszkowski, 2007).
    [Show full text]
  • 16 Gestures by 16 Months
    16 Gestures by 16 Months Children Should Learn at Least 16 Gestures by 16 Months Good communication development starts in the first year of life and goes far beyond learning how to talk. Communication development has its roots in social interaction with parents and other caregivers during everyday activities. Your child’s growth in social communication is important because it helps your child connect with you, learn language and play concepts, and sets the stage for learning to read and future success in school. Good com- munication skills are the best tool to prevent behavior problems and make it easier to work through moments of frustration that all infants and toddlers face. Earlier is Better Catching communication and language difficulties By observing children’s early early can prevent potential problems later with gestures, you can obtain a critical behavior, learning, reading, and social interaction. snapshot of their communication Research on brain development reminds us that “earlier IS better” when teaching young children. development. Even small lags in The most critical period for learning is during the communication milestones can first three years of a child’s life. Pathways in the add up and impact a child’s rate brain develop as infants and young children learn of learning that is difficult to from exploring and interacting with people and objects in their environment. The brain’s architec- change later. Research with young ture is developing the most rapidly during this crit- children indicates that the development of gestures from 9 to 16 ical period and is the most sensitive to experiential months predicts language ability 2 years later, which is significant learning.
    [Show full text]
  • University of Groningen a Cultural History of Gesture Bremmer, JN
    University of Groningen A Cultural History Of Gesture Bremmer, J.N.; Roodenburg, H. IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below. Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Publication date: 1991 Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database Citation for published version (APA): Bremmer, J. N., & Roodenburg, H. (1991). A Cultural History Of Gesture. s.n. Copyright Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). The publication may also be distributed here under the terms of Article 25fa of the Dutch Copyright Act, indicated by the “Taverne” license. More information can be found on the University of Groningen website: https://www.rug.nl/library/open-access/self-archiving-pure/taverne- amendment. Take-down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum. Download date: 02-10-2021 The 'hand of friendship': shaking hands and other gestures in the Dutch Republic HERMAN ROODENBURG 'I think I can see the precise and distinguishing marks of national characters more in those nonsensical minutiae than in the most important matters of state'.
    [Show full text]
  • The Use of Gesture in Architecture
    the use of gesture donald kunze • art3idea.psu.edu/boundaries/ In the design of architecture and landscape, the role of dynamics is evident not simply in accommodation of the (presumably) mobile behaviors of the occupants and their perceptions but as an indicator of a ‘signifier-ness’ (a sense of meaningfulness without any particular meaning attached). ‘Gesture’ stands for the ways in which mate- rial form can be imagined to express in ways analogous to human gesturing: pointing, opening, perching, facing, gazing, sneaking, waiting … in short, the full range of dramatic positions that an actor on the stage might take to indicate attitude, intention, emotion, or rôle. Gesture typically involves symmetrical signing. Thickening of one element is accompanied by the thinning of another. A opening directed skyward requires an opening (or closing) at a low point. These pairings can form an ‘idiotic symmetry’ (each element, though balanced by another, equally lacks an assigned function or meaning). Alternately, pairs can refer to a missing or silent third term that works to prevent or foreclose some spatial/temporal relationship. 1. gesture in general Gesture is the ‘language of the body’, a means of silent speech that can be read as intentional or unintentional. Gesture animates and re-configures the body, its parts, and their relationships. Gesture can create divisions, such as an audience and stage division. It can indicate impossibility (biting one’s elbow, an Italian gesture), difficulty (biting one’s hand), intelligence (pulling down the lower eye-lid), discovery (pointing to the head), idiocy (a screwing motion with the index finger to the cheek, an offensive gesture in Germany), and the countless obscene gestures in all cultures that are used for insult or mockery.
    [Show full text]
  • Prepose: Privacy, Security, and Reliability for Gesture-Based Programming
    Prepose: Privacy, Security, and Reliability for Gesture-Based Programming Lucas Silva Figueiredo∗, Benjamin Livshitsy, David Molnary, and Margus Veanesy Federal University of Pernambuco∗ Microsoft Researchy F Abstract|With the rise of sensors such as the Microsoft Kinect, Leap of an OS abstraction called a recognizer which enables Motion, and hand motion sensors in phones (i.e., Samsung Galaxy S6), gesture detection. Yet their approach fails to provide a gesture-based interfaces have become practical. Unfortunately, today, way to extend the system with new recognizers in a safe to recognize such gestures, applications must have access to depth and manner. SurroundWeb [27] demonstrates what a 3D web video of the user, exposing sensitive data about the user and her environ- ment. Besides these privacy concerns, there are also security threats in browser modified with new abstractions for input and sensor-based applications, such as multiple applications registering the output to protect privacy and security would look like. Yet same gesture, leading to a conflict (akin to Clickjacking on the web). it also lacks the capacity for precise automatic reasoning. We address these security and privacy threats with Prepose, a novel We are also inspired by world-drive access control [24], domain-specific language (DSL) for easily building gesture recognizers, which attempts to restrict applications from accessing combined with a system architecture that protects privacy, security, and sensitive objects in the environment. Lastly, Proton [15] reliability with untrusted applications. We run Prepose code in a trusted is an example of defining a higher-level abstraction for core, and only return specific gesture events to applications.
    [Show full text]
  • Module-04-Greetings-Etiquette-And
    Event Planner Module 04 4. Module 04- Greetings etiquette and handshake protocol Table of Contents 4. Module 04- Greetings etiquette and handshake protocol ............................................................................ 1 4.1 Managing Greetings etiquette and handshake protocol ......................................................................... 2 4.2 Handshake, greetings, and kisses ............................................................................................................ 3 4.3 Personal space ......................................................................................................................................... 5 4.4 Hand kisses ............................................................................................................................................... 5 1 | P a g e 4.1 Managing Greetings etiquette and handshake protocol Working as an event manager, you will certainly deal with people from different countries, cultures, and backgrounds. This can be fun, but also can be a little complicated. The customs and rituals involved in greeting often differ from country to country, and unfamiliar customs may be confusing. The situation becomes even more complex when different greeting gestures are required between men and women, women and women, and men and men. Luckily, the ultimate pleasant gesture is used in every country. Although occasionally, in some cultures and under certain circumstances, it may have negative connotations, this ultimate gesture is seldom misunderstood
    [Show full text]
  • Gestures: Your Body Speaks
    GESTURES: YOUR BODY SPEAKS How to Become Skilled WHERE LEADERS in Nonverbal Communication ARE MADE GESTURES: YOUR BODY SPEAKS TOASTMASTERS INTERNATIONAL P.O. Box 9052 • Mission Viejo, CA 92690 USA Phone: 949-858-8255 • Fax: 949-858-1207 www.toastmasters.org/members © 2011 Toastmasters International. All rights reserved. Toastmasters International, the Toastmasters International logo, and all other Toastmasters International trademarks and copyrights are the sole property of Toastmasters International and may be used only with permission. WHERE LEADERS Rev. 6/2011 Item 201 ARE MADE CONTENTS Gestures: Your Body Speaks............................................................................. 3 Actions Speak Louder Than Words...................................................................... 3 The Principle of Empathy ............................................................................ 4 Why Physical Action Helps........................................................................... 4 Five Ways to Make Your Body Speak Effectively........................................................ 5 Your Speaking Posture .................................................................................. 7 Gestures ................................................................................................. 8 Why Gestures? ...................................................................................... 8 Types of Gestures.................................................................................... 9 How to Gesture Effectively..........................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Emblems & Illustrators Kinesics • Body
    Chapter8, Presentation 2 1 CHAPTER 8 The Nonverbal Code Part 2: Body language - Emblems & Illustrators 2 Kinesics • Body movement, including gestures, hand/arm movement, leg movement, facial expressions, eye gaze, and stance/ posture • Categories…. – Emblems—direct literal verbal translation – Illustrators—accent/complement what is being said • Meta-communicative – Affect displays—facial expressions of emotion • Considered universal – Regulators—behaviors/actions that govern, direct, or manage conversation – Adaptors—actions that satisfy physiological or psychological needs 3 Emblems & Illustrators • Emblems are primarily hand gestures that have a direct literal verbal translation • Illustrators are typically hand and arm movements that accompany speech or function to accent or complement what is being said 4 What does it mean? 5 What does it mean? 6 What does it mean? 7 Functions of emblems & illustrators • Main uses: 1. Greetings & departures 2. To insult or to utter obscenities to others 3. To indicate fight or flight 4. To designate friendly or romantic relationships 1 • Also common: signs for homosexuals • Gestures culturally bound – Sometimes gestures can be regional in nature 8 Emblem gestures: Yes and no 9 Emblem gestures: Yes and no • Regional gestures – “Gesture frontiers” amazingly resilient, resistant to national boundaries – Historical origins – Head toss: originally Greek, used in southern Italy 10 Illustrator Example: Greetings 11 Illustrator Example: Greetings • Handshake: far from universal – “Limp fish” universally disdained?
    [Show full text]
  • Hand Gestures Recognition Using Radar Sensors for Human-Computer-Interaction: a Review
    remote sensing Review Hand Gestures Recognition Using Radar Sensors for Human-Computer-Interaction: A Review Shahzad Ahmed 1 , Karam Dad Kallu 2, Sarfaraz Ahmed 1 and Sung Ho Cho 1,* 1 Department of Electronic Engineering, Hanyang University, 222 Wangsimini-ro, Seongdong-gu, Seoul 133-791, Korea; [email protected] (S.A.); [email protected] (S.A.) 2 Robotics and Intelligent Machine Engineering (RIME), School of Mechanical and Manufacturing Engineering (SMME), National University of Science and Technology (NUST), H-12 Islamabad 44000, Pakistan; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +82-(02)-2220-4883 Abstract: Human–Computer Interfaces (HCI) deals with the study of interface between humans and computers. The use of radar and other RF sensors to develop HCI based on Hand Gesture Recognition (HGR) has gained increasing attention over the past decade. Today, devices have built-in radars for recognizing and categorizing hand movements. In this article, we present the first ever review related to HGR using radar sensors. We review the available techniques for multi-domain hand gestures data representation for different signal processing and deep-learning-based HGR algorithms. We classify the radars used for HGR as pulsed and continuous-wave radars, and both the hardware and the algorithmic details of each category is presented in detail. Quantitative and qualitative analysis of ongoing trends related to radar-based HCI, and available radar hardware and algorithms is also presented. At the end, developed devices and applications based on gesture- recognition through radar are discussed. Limitations, future aspects and research directions related to this field are also discussed.
    [Show full text]