FAR FROM IDEAL: TALKING ABOUT WEIGBT WITH MOTHERS AND DAUGaTERS FROM , SOUTHERN MANITOBA AND A FIRST NATIONS COMMUNITY

A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of Graduate Studies in Partial Fulfilment of the Requirements for the Degree of

DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

Department of Comrnunity Health Sciences Faculty of Medicine University of Manitoba Winnipeg, Manitoba

(c) 2001 National Library Bibliothèque nationale 1+1 of Canada du Canada Acquisitions and Acquisitions et Bibliographie Services services bibliographiques 395 Wellington Street 395. rue Weüingtocr Oîtawa ON K1A ON4 Ottawa ON K1A Canada Canads

The author has granted a non- L'auteur a accordé une licence non exclusive licence allowïng the exclusive permettant a la National Lhrary of Canada to Bibliothèqge nationale du Canada de reproduce, loan, distribute or sell reproduire, prêter, distribuer ou copies of this thesis in microform, vendre des copies de cette thèse sous paper or electronic formats. la forme de microfiche/film, de reproduction sur papier ou sur format électronique.

The author retains ownership of the L'auteur conserve la propriété du copyright in this thesis. Neither the droit d'auteur qui protège cette thèse. thesis nor substantial extracts fiom it Ni la thèse ni des extraits substantiels may be printed or otherwise de celle-ci ne doivent être imprimés reproduced without the author's ou autrement reproduits sans son permission . autorisation. THE UNWERSïN OF MANITOBA

FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES ***** COPYRIGHT PERMISSION PAGE

Far From Ideai: Talkiag abatWeight with Mothers and Daughters hmWinnipeg,

Southern Manitoba and a First Nations Community

A Thesis/Practicum submitted ta the Facnlty of Graduate Studies of The University

of Manitoba in partial falflllment of the reqairements of the degree

of

GAIL DM. MARCHESSAULT 02001

Permission has been granted ta the Library of The University of Manitoba to lend or seU copies of this thesidpmcticum, to the National Wrary of Cam& to microfilm tbis thdsand to lend or sell copies of the mm, and to Dissertations Abstncts Internitiod to pubosh an abstract of this thesis/practlcum.

The author reserves other pubiiution rights, and ncither this thesidptllcticum nor extensive extracts from it may be printed or otherwise reproduced without the author's written permission. 1dedicate this thesis to Morley and Lexi

who encouraged me to begin it

and

encouraged me to finish it. ABSTRACT

This research compared body weight perceptions of Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal girls and their mothers. Three interview formats were used: in-depth qualitative interviews; a stnictured questionnaire analysed with cultural consensus theory; and three standardized questionnaires. Families were randomly selected based on the class lists of Grade 8 students from two urban and two rural schools. The sample consisted of 80 mother- daughter pairs, interviewed separately. The response rate was 71.3%. The non-Aboriginal women almost unanimously discussed strong societal pressures that made weight more important to women than to men. Most of the Aboriginal women, especially in the First

Nations community (FNC),emphasized physiology and health, speaking in a way that suggested weight was not central to female identity. Despite this, some Aboriginal women related a history of risky eating behaviours. Analysis of the 24 weight-related values statements indicated consensus, except in the FNC women. Both agreement and disagreement were highlighted. Across locations, approximately half of the girls accuratety repeated their mothers' advice to them regarding weight. There were no significant associations between mother-daughter pairs in the analysis of the consensus statements, reports of dieting, general advice to others, the Restraint Scale, the Eating

Attitudes Test (EAT-26), or body dissatisfaction as measured by the Body Shape

Drawings Questionnaire. A pst-hoc analysis of 21 participants who most supported accepting one's body size was the only mother-daughter association that achieved statistical significance (kappa =û.51). Body dissatisfaction was higher in Aboriginal than non-Aboriginal women (83% and 62%) and girls (66% and 36%). Responses on the Restraint Scale suggested more dieting in the FNC. More Aboriginal (17.5%)than non-

Aboriginal (2.5%) girls had EAT-26 scores suggestive of risk for an eating disorder.

These results suggest that (a) girls are not necessady emulating their mothers' weight concems but may leam to challenge weight obsession fiom hem; (b) both weight and weight preoccupation need to be considered in health messages to Aboriginal girls and women living in or close to an wban centre. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

Thank you to the mothers and daughters who shared the perspectives and experiences

that fom the basis fur this sturty.

To Dr. Linda Garro, Dr. Pat Kaufert, Dr. Sharon Macdonald and Dr. Manly

Spigelman, a heartfelt thank you for an informative, chdenging and worthwhile graduate education. 1 feel both lucky and honoured to have studied with such an exceptional team of dedicated scholars and able teachers. 1 am especially grateful to my advisor, Dr. Linda

Garro, for her expert guidance and consistently excellent advice given generously, fmt here and then from the University of California, Los Angeles.

1 thank the professors and staff of the Department of Community Health Sciences for a rich and supportive learning environment. 1especially thank Drs. Tom Hassard, Pat

Martens and Bob Tate for statistical guidance rendered in plain English, Dawn Stewart for helpful cornputer advice, Joe Kaufert for filling the hallways with enthusiastic encouragement, and Camille Guenette and Theresa Kennedy for talcing care of my needs as a graduate student.

1 could not have completed this process without fiends and family. 1 thmk

Morley and Lexi, my family and my husband's family for their enduring love, understanding, tolerance and assistance. A special thank you to my sister Tina, for her many hundreds of hours devoted to transcription. 1thank my friends, fellow graduate students and colleagues for many different kinds of support: Pat Allen, Frances Berg,

Jocelyn Bruyere, Joan Butcher, Marian Campbell, Ruth Diamant, Chris Egan, Lyn Ferguson, Marcy Finlayson, Brian and Cathy Hallamore, Derek Lucian and Lillian Smith,

Kathleen Harrison, Nancy Hughes and Gerry Kaplan, Pat Martens, Catherine McNeil,

Donna Rodgers, Ted Rosenberg and Liza Zacharias, David Rosner, Sheelagh Smith,

Moneca Sinclaire, Jan Sprout, Bob Tate, and Bev Watts. And, although 1 cannot name her, a special thanks to my host in the First Nations community, whose fiiendship 1value, and whose help was invaluable.

Thanks also to the Winnipeg School Board Division No. 1, the ndschool boards and the Chief and Council of the First Nations community for granting permission to conduct the research in their district schools. 1appreciate the efforts of the principals and school personnel who kindly helped with the implementation of the project.

This research was made possible in part by Health Canada through a National

Health Research and Development Program (NHRDP)Research Training Award. I also acknowledge with gratitude financial assistance from the Manitoba Health Research

Council, the Children's Hospital Research Foundation, the Canadian Home Economics

Association, the Women's Health Research Foundation of Canada, and the Dietitians of

Canada. And 1 thank the Department of Foods and Nutrition in the Faculty of Human

Ecology for allowing the first few months of my position at the University of Manitoba to be the last few months of polishing the thesis. Table of Contents

Abstract ...... i ... Acknowledgements ...... 111

List of Figures ...... ix

Chapter0ne:Introduction ...... 1 Statement of the Problem ...... 1 Objectives and Research Questions ...... -5 Rationale for Group Identification ...... 6 Reflections on the Evolution of the Research ...... 10 Title and Outline of the Thesis ...... 16

Chapter Two: Far from Ideal ...... 19 Aboriginal People's Weight Status. Perceptions and Behaviours ...... 24 BodyIrnage ...... 33 Sociocultural Context for Perceptions of Weight ...... 35 Family ...... 39 Mothers and Daughters ...... 42

Chapter Three: The Research Process ...... 50 Research Design and Analysis ...... -50 Serni-Stnictured Interviews ...... -52 Fixed-Response Interviews ...... -54 Questionnaires ...... 56 Background Information ...... -63 Triangulating Methods ...... 65 Ethical Considerations ...... 67 The Research Sites ...... -68 Implementation ...... 70 Sample Selection ...... -75 The Research Participants ...... 76 Demographic Characteristics ...... 76 Weight Staîus ...... 82 Representativeness ...... -84 Identifying Participants ...... 86 Chapter Four: On Women. Weight and Identity ...... -87 On the Importance of Weight to Women ...... -88 Non-Abonginai Women's Responses ...... -88 Aboriginal Women's Responses ...... -92 Tdking about Weight and Judgment ...... 100 Weight in the Context of Women's Lives ...... -101 Surnrnary and Discussion ...... 104

Chapter Five: Weight Dissatisfaction ...... 111 Low Rates of Dieting and High Rates of Weight Dissatisfaction ...... 111 The Meaning of Diet ...... 114 Risk for an Eating Disorder ...... 117 Abonginal Participants' Stories ...... -119 Sam. DavidandLade ...... 120 Tansyandlily ...... 125 Bhata ...... 129 Surnmary and Discussion ...... 133

Chapter Six: A Consensus Analysis of Weight-Related Values Statements ...... 137 Cultural Consensus Theory and Method ...... 138 ExplonngConsensus ...... 142 DifferencesBetweenLocations ...... 145 Comparing Generations ...... 154 Comparing Mother-Daughter Pairs ...... 157 Summary and Discussion ...... 158

Chapter Seven: Mothers' Advice to Their Daughters ...... 162 WhodoGirlsTalkTo? ...... 163 Heavier than Average Girls ...... 164 Weight Control Advice ...... 166 Health Advice ...... 170 Descriptions of Daughters ...... 172 Do Dieting Mothers Have Dieting Daughters? ...... 174 GirlsAt-risk ...... 176 Lighter than Average Girls ...... 177 Just for Good Hedth ...... 179 WorriedMothers ...... 180 Descriptions of Daughters ...... 182 Average Weight Girls ...... 183 Talking about Mothers' Weight ...... 185 Perceptionsofhfluence ...... 187 Summary and Discussion ...... 190 A Parallel Discussion in the Health Literature ...... 193 Chapter Eight: Participants' Advice to Other People ...... 198 The Struggle for Body Acceptance ...... -201 Mother-Daughter Pairs ...... -202 Size Issues ...... -204 Self-Acceptance md Health Issues ...... -205 Control Issues by Orientation ...... -210 Summary and Discussion ...... -217

Chapter Nine: Analysis of Three Questionnaires ...... -220 Sarnple Size and Power Considerations ...... 221 Data Analysis ...... -222 Restraint Scale ...... 223 Eating Attitudes Test ...... -224 Mother-Daughter Comparisons ...... -224 Body Shape Drawings Questionnaire ...... 225 ResuIts ...... 226 Restraint Scaie ...... -226 EatingAttitudesTest ...... 228 Mother-Daughter Cornparisons ...... 230 Body Shape Drawings Questionnaire ...... -231 Surnrnary and Discussion ...... 242 Generation ...... 242 Mother-Daughter Compaxisons ...... -244 Ethnicity, Region and Location ...... 245

Chapter Ten: Teaching Ophelia to Swim ...... -251 Limitations and Strengths of the Study ...... 252 Summary of Research Findings ...... -256 The AboriginaVNon-Aboriginal Cornparison ...... 256 The Mothermaughter Cornparison ...... 262 Policy Implications ...... -266 Recomrnendations for Further Research ...... 270

References ...... 273

Appendices A-1 Descriptions of Questionnaires ...... 302 A-2 Studies of Mother and Daughter Associations in Weight-Related Attitudes and Self-Reported Behaviours ...... -306 B Inteniew Rotocol and Questions ...... 311 C-1 Approval from University of Manitoba Facul ty Committee on the Use of Human Subjects in Research ...... 323 C-2 Approval from The Winnipeg School Division No . 1 Research,

vii Planning and Technology. Research Advisory Cornmittee ...... -324 Consent to Participate in Study ...... -325 Mother's Consent for Daughter's Participation ...... 326 LnfomationabouttheStudy ...... 327 Suriunary of Findings for Participants ...... 329 Introductory Letter to Parent ...... 333 Detectable Effect for Group Comparisons of Different Sample Sizes ...... -334 Detectable Effect for Paired Cornparisons of Different Sample Sizes ...... -334 Correlations Between Weight and Weight-Related Scales for Girls ...... 335 Correlations Between Weight and Weight-Related Scales for Women ...... 335 Summary and Discussion of Correlations Between Weight and Weight-Related Scales ...... 336 Descriptive Statistics for the Modifed Restraint Scale ...... 337 Univariate Analyses of Modified Restraint Scale for 75 Paired Participants . . 338 Split Unit Analysis of Variance Table for Modified Restraint Scale for 75 Mother-Daughter Pairs ...... -339 Tukey-Kramer Multiple-Cornparison Test ...... 339 Desaiptive Statistics for the Restraint Scale ...... -340 Univariate Analyses of Restraint Scale for Paired Participants ...... 341 Split Unit Analysis of Variance Table for Restraint for Mother-Daughter Pairs ...... 342 Tukey-Kramer Multiple-Cornparison Test ...... 342 Descriptive Statistics for the Eating Attitudes Test-26 ...... 343 Univariate Analyses of Eating Attitudes Test-26 for 75 Paired Participants ...344 Mother-Daughter Comlations for Restraint Scale. Modified Restraint Scale and Eating Attitudes Test (EAT-26)Scores for Ali Families and by Location. Ethnicity and Region ...... 345 Comparing Median Responses to Body Shape Drawings Questionnaire byGeneration ...... 346 Comparing Girls' Body Shape Drawing Responses by Ethnicity and Region . 347 Comparing Women's Body Shape Drawing Responses by Ethnicity andRegion ...... 347 Comparing Girls' Median Responses to Body Shape Drawings Questions bylocation ...... 348 Comparing Women's Median Responses Body Shape Drawings Questions bylocation ...... 348 Comparing Girls' and Women' s Median Responses to Body Shape Drawings Questionnaire at Each Location ...... 349 Participant Satisfaction Rating of Body Shape by Generation, Ethnicity andRegion ...... 350 Comparing Desired and Attractive Body Shape Selections ...... 351

viii LIST OF FIGURES

Influences on body dissatisfaction. weight preoccupation and disordered eating.38 Distribution of Body Mass Index by location for girls and women ...... -83 Reported fkquency of dieting for girls and women by location ...... 112 The structure matrix to test for clifferences between Suburban. Rural. Urban Aboriginal and First Nations girls ...... 149 Mean scores for Modified Restraint Scale ...... -227 Median scores of Eating Attitudes Test-26 by generation and location ..... -229 Median responses to Body Shape Drawings Questionnaire by generation .... 232 Median responses to Body Shape Drawings Questionnaire by self-identified ethnicity for women ...... 235 Median responses to Body Shape Drawings Questionnaire by location and self-identified ethnicity ...... -236 Proportion of participants' selecting a larger perceived than desired body shape by ethnicity and generation ...... 239 LIST OF TABLES

Studies of Body Dissatisfaction and Weight Control Practices of AboriginalPeople ...... 28 Mother-Daughter Studies of Weight-Related Attitudes and Self-Reported Behaviours ...... 45 Time-line for the Approval Pmcess ...... 71 Sample Selection Process and Response Rates by Location ...... 75 Characteristics of the Research Participants by Location ...... -77 Employment Characteristics of the Research Participants ...... 80 Participants' Rating of Adequacy of Income by Location ...... 82 "1s Weight More Important to Women than to Men?" Women's Responsesbyhation ...... 88 Desire for Weight Change by Generation and Location ...... 113 Number of Participants with High Eating Attitudes Test-26 Scores or Reporting Vomiting for Weight Control ...... 117 Consensus Factor Loadings for Values Statements ...... 143 Proportion of Agreement with Consensus Position for Values Statements Meeting the Binomial Critenon by Generation at Each Location ...... 146 Proportion of Agreement with Consensus Position for Values Statements not Meeting the Binomial Criterion by Generation at Each Location ...... -147 Statements Varying from Proportion Giving the Consensus Response ...... 151 Correlations and Matching Responses for Pairs of Girls, Mothers and Daughters, and Unrelated Women and Girls ...... 157 Body Mass Index Range for Girls by Location ...... 163 Mother-Daughter Dieting Reports by Daughters' Body Mass Index ...... 175 Girls' Reported Dieting by Mothers' Dieting andor Advice in Highest Quintile of Body Mass Index ...... -176 Girls' Reported Satisfaction with their Looks by Body Mass index ...... 183 Advice About Weight to Others from Selected Mother-Daughter Pairs ...... 199 Participants' Advice About Weight to Others by Generation and Location ...200 Perspectives on Control from Participants with Different Orientations ...... 211 Responses of Girls and Women to the Body Shape Drawings Questionnaire . -231 Summary of Findings for the Body Shape Drawings Questionnaire, Modified Restraint Scale and the Eating Attitudes Test-26 by Location, Region, Ethnicity and Generation ...... 241 CHAPTER ONE

Introduction

"When we talk among ourselves, al1 we get back are echoes. But when we talk with others of a different mind, we are made to think. And it is in thinking that we learn, and in learning that we grow" (Govemor General Romeo LeBlanc c.f. Greaves, 1997, p. D10).

Statement of the Problem

Weight and weight control are compelling concems for numerous rniddletlass girls and wornen in Canada (Dairy Bureau of Canada, 1993; Health Canada, 2000;

Walters, Lenton, & Mckeary, 1995). It is not known to what extent these concerns are shared by Aboriginal girls and women. And although it is often assumed that girls learn to fear fat and value thinness from their mothers, there is little investigation of the sirnilarity of weight-related views of teen-aged girls and their mothers. This study addresses these two gaps in Our knowledge by comparing Abonginal and non-Aboriginal girls' and their mothers' perceptions about body weight.

There is little published research examining how Aboriginal people perceive weight issues. Do Aboriginal girls and women share the well-documented middle-class perspectives on weight? 1s weight as important to Aboriginal girls and women? And if it is, is it important in the same ways? The health hazards of obesity, especially the increased prevalence of diabetes amongst Aboriginal people, and the competing pressures and health hazards of weight preoccupation make these questions cntical for understanding appropriate health education approaches. Education with Aboriginal people to prevent diabetes is oiten predicated on the assumption that there is a need to increase the motivation for weight control. This approach stems from research that focusses on documenting the prevalence of obesity in

Canadian and Amencan Aboriginal people and exploring its association with chronic diseases (Harrison & Ritenbaugh, 1992; Story, Strauss, Zephier, & Broussard, 1998;

Waldram, Hemng, & Young, 1995; Welty, 1991; Young & Sevenhuysen, 1989).

A number of studies in the United States have iwussed on weight-related behaviours. Along with the higher weight noms, Native Arnerican girls and young women have been reported to use potentiaily hazardous weight control methods (Crago,

Shisslak, & Estes, 1996; Neumark-Sztainer, Jeffery, & French, 1997; Rosen et al-, 1988a;

Smith & Krejci, 1991; Snow & Hamis, 1989; Story, French, Resnick, & Blum, 1995;

Story et al., 1994). This research, which is exclusively survey-based, suggests the need for more information about the way Aboriginal people perceive their weight. Additionally, because al1 of these studies were Amencan, there is a need to document if similar behaviours are present in Canadian Aboriginal girls and women. Consequentiy, the first research question focuses on a comparison of Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal perspectives about weight.

The second major comparison of this research concerns the transmission of perceptions and concem about weight to girls. It is frequentiy assumed that mothers pass on their own desire for thinness and stigmatization of fat to their daughters (Brigman,

1994; Johnson, 1995; Parker et al., 1995; Rabinor, 1994; Waterhouse, 1997). Current research raises questions regarding this assumption (Attie & Brooks-Gunn, 1989; Byely, Archibald, Graber, & Brooks-Gunn, 2000; Keel, Heatherton, Hamden, & Hornig, 1997;

Ogden & Steward, 2000; Pike & Rodin, 1991). Studying weight perceptions of mothers and daughters in diverse situations offered potential to cIarïQ the similarities and differences between generations and within pairs of mothers and daughters. This, too, could provide useful data in support of educational approaches to help girls cope with contradictory cultural presswes concerning weight.

Rural communities are another under-studied segment of our society (Leichner,

Arnett, Rallo, Snkarneswm, & Vulcano, 1986; Reeder et al., 1997). 1am aware of oniy one study that examines the distribution of body weight of urban and nual Canadian women (Reeder et al., 1997). This study reported no significant differences in mean Body

Mass Tndex @MI) or prevalence of obesity between urtran and rural women in the national sarnple. Western Canadian rural women, however, were more likely than their urban counterparts to be heavier and more often reported trying to lose weight. An earlier study also found that rural youth in Manitoba were heavier. This study did not measure dieting, but reported fewer rural youth were at risk of an eating disorder (Leichner et ai.,

1986). Studying the perspectives of rural girls and women is therefore of interest.

Studying a rural community alongside a rural First Nations community is also of theoretical interest in delineating small town effects. Impression management, and therefore appearance and weight, may be more important where precise evaluation of individual achievement is difficult. (Featherstone, 1982). This may be more relevant to large, anonymous cities than to small, rural communities. If so, then First Nations communities may have more relaxeci attitudes to weight because they know much more about each other than city dwellers do. If this is the case, then a similar sized rural community should also have more relaxed attitudes to physical appearance.

In contrast to the paucity of research in the above areas, there is a large volume of research on North American fernales' attitudes to weight, most of it using swey rnethodology. This research has established that weight and weight control are important concens for a majorïty of girls and women in Canada @airy Bureau of Canada, 1993;

Health Canada, 2000; Walters et al., 1995). This research has largely focussed on White, middle-class students.

There is a growing body of interview-based qualitative research. However, participants in these studies were selected for specific weight problems, such as eating disorders, weight control or obesity (Allon, 1979; Brown, 1987; Bruch, 1978; Honeycutt,

1999; MïIlman, 1980; Spitzack, 1990). Several of these studies have focussed on working cIass women (AIlan, 1989), Black women (Thompson, 1992) or older men and women

(Colvin & Olson, 1983; Joanisse & Synnott, 1999).

Recently, qualitative researchers have explored a fuller range of attitudes to weight by interviewing adult volunteers from the general population (Chapman, 1999;

Grogan, 1999; Hesse-Biber, 1996; Wakewich, 20).There are surprisingly few qualitative studies with adolescents that focus on weight (Chapman & Maclean, 1993;

Marchessault, 1993a; Nichter, 2000; Parker et al., 1995; Robertson, 1991). To my knowledge, other than the work for my master's research, this is the first qualitative study that compares what mothers and daughters Say about weight. In my master's research, 1 also inteniewed Grade 8 girls and their mothers, but the families lived in the inner city. Obiecti ves and Research Ouestions

The pnmary objective of this research was to examine the extent to which

Canadian mothers and daughters of diverse backgrounds share undetstandings about weight. The main question addressed was: What are the participants' concerns and perceptions about body weight ? This was addressed in Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal participants, Grade 8 girls and their mothers, and urban and rural participants. Families were recniited through four schools, two located in Winnipeg (a suburb and the inner city), and two in southern Manitoba (a rural community and a First Nations community).

Consistency and variation in participants' understandings about weight were explored within the mother-daughter pairs; between the two generations; between

Aboriginal and non- Aboriginal families; between urban and mral families; and between the four locations studied.

Participants' understandings were explored using different interview formats to develop a more comprehensive understanding of how people perceive weight than is possible using a single approach. Ethnographie interview approaches emphasize the exploration of participant perspectives, a fundamental aspect of this research. I used two ethnographic interview formats: an openended semi-structured interview and a more stnictured fixed-response interview based on statements made by mothers and daughters who participated in the previous study. 1 also used three standardized and widely administered questionnaires to document and compare weight concerns. Rationale for gr ou^ Identification

Al1 of the families interviewed in this study were of either Aboriginal or European

ancestry. When asked about their ethnicity, participants of European heritage usually

named their country of origin, or said "Canadian." The Aboriginal participants named

their Nation, or said "Native, " "'Aboriginol." or ''Meris*" or in the case of the girls,

"Zndian" or occasionaily "treaty " or c'stntus."'

Both Native and Aboriginal seemed acceptable as inclusive names to the people 1

interviewed. 1chose to use Aboriginal throughout this report except when citing a

reference that uses other terminology. Aboriginal political leaders currently favour this

term, as demonstrated by its use by the Aboriginal Justice Inquiry of Manitoba (Hamilton

& Sinclair, 1991), and the Royal Commission on Aboriginal Peoples (1996). The Royal

Commission used Aboriginal to refer to Metis and First Nations people (and Inuit as well,

but there were no Inuit people in this study). Metis people are defined as those who self-

identify as having rnixed Aboriginal-European ancestry. The Commission proposed First

Nations to replace Indian and Native American. First Nations has been highlighted as the

only non-racist attribution (Monture, 1993). However, no one in my study refemed to

their heritage this way. People more often used First Nations to refer to reserves, the land

set aside for band members' use. 1 use First Nations communiîy to refer to the reserve that

was the site of one of the four schwls where 1 did the research.

'"Status" or "ueaty" Indians are Aboriginal people registered as Indian under the terms of the Indian Act. In the eariy 1990's, there were an estimated 750,000 Aboriginal people with "treaty" status, 250,000 without "treaty" status, 160,000 to 250,000 Metis people and 36,000 Inuit people living in Canada (ïookenay, 1996). When comparisons were based on ethnicity, 1 refer to the sample with European hentage as non-Aboriginal, largely for convenience. Caucasian. Euro-Canadian and of

European Ancestry were dso considered, but they are rarely used and therefore seemed awkward. White is used more often but impiied a uniformity that did not exist.

Self-identified ethnicity is used here to draw the Aboriginal and non-Abonginal comparisons. There is a diverse array of experience inherent within each group.

Continuity and context are important considerations in the interpretation of identity.

Sorne participants portrayed their ethnicity as an integral part of their identity dating to childhood. Others suggested that ethnicity had become relevant to them only recently.

The Abonginal women in particular related a large variety of circumstances from growing up in a non-Abonginal family, to having always lived in a First Nations comxnunity. And yet, the personal devance of identity was difficult to predict based on such facts. This was demonstrated by one set of interviews with two sisters. Both described their hentage with great pride. One described herself as Metis; the other as

French Canadian. Their daughters followed suit, suggesting that their mothen were consistent in the different meaning they drew from growing up in similar circumstances.

Daughters did not always share the their mothers' connection to their ethnic heritage. The daughters of some Urban-Aboriginal women living in the suburbs might have been the only Aboriginal girl in their class. Other Urban Aboriginal families had moved from the suburbs to the inner city specifically to live in an Aboriginal neighbourhood.

The UrbmAboriginal sample resulted from the addition of a snowball sample to complete that drawn fiom the inner city schwl, and the group name for these families refers to location plus self-identz5ed ethnicity, the dimensions by which they were selected-

1 use Suburban to refer to the older, well-established city neighbourfiood that is the site of one of the Winnipeg schwIs. This neighbourhood is situated between the imer city and the perimeter of the city and does not meet the dictionary definition of suburban as "an outlying district of a city" (Davis & Webster, 1998, p. 830). According to the three urban experts with whom 1spoke there is no word for this type of neighbourhood. Dr.

Andy Lockery, the Coordinator of Environmentai Studies and Urban Studies at the

University of Winnipeg, explained that Winnipeg's development as a transportation centre in the 1920s prompted these neighbourhoods to be built as "suburban systems" prior to suburban trends elsewhere. As the city grew, the old suburbs were nnged by new ones, but "once a suburb, always a suburb" (personal communication, February 25,

1999).'

1 refer to the girls and women at the four locations as if they were coherent groups, or "natural cornmunities," as Emily Martin (1987) phrased it. However, people do not often define themselves this way, and the= is much diversity within these groups as well as between them.

A person's body size is a relevant factor in understanding their weight-related concerns, but the words we use to describe body weight are dso unsatisfactory,

2~r.Chris Leo, Professor of Political Science, specializing in urban politics, and Dr. Tom Carter, Professor of Geography, both at the University of Winnipeg, also referred to these neighbourhoods as older suburbs (personal communication, February 26,1999). particularly at the heavier end of the scale (Berg, 1998; Pittaway, 1998; Robison, 1998).

People often use overweight interchangeably with obesity, but these words have different meanings (National Institutes of Health National Heart Lung and Blood Institute, 1998;

World Health Organization, 1998). Technically overweight refers to an excess amount of total body weight inclusive of al1 body tissues, while obesity refers to excessive body fat relative to lean body mass (Dalton, 1997, p. 2). It is possible, by these definitions, to be overweight without having excessive body fat (for example, body builders), or to be obese or overfat without king ovenveight (for example, sedentary graduate students).

Ternis such as undenveight and overweight suggest there is an ideal weight that people should aim for, implying a value judgement that is irrelevant in the context of this project.

Weight preoccupation is used throughout this thesis to mean an intense concem or obsession with weight issues, usually expressed as a desire to achieve or maintain weight loss, slenderness or a lean and muscular body. People with eating disorders are, by definition, weight preoccupied? As show in the literature, weight preoccupation is the nom for Canadian girls and women, and often leads to an unhealthy relationship with

"nie major eating disorders are anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Anorexia is diagnosed when there is a significant and unexplained loss of body weight to less than 85% of expected, a disturbed body image, an intense fear of becoming fat despite king severely underweight, and arnenorrhea. Most individuals with bulimia are within a normal weight range, but their self-evaluation hinges on their body shape and weight. Bulimia is charactenzed by bingeeating (the consumption of large amounts of food in a short time) accompanied by a sense of lack of control over eating and inappropriate methods to prevent weight gain, such as fasting, self-induced vomiting, excessive exercise, or the misuse of diuretics, laxatives or other medications. Bulimia is diagnosed after these behaviours have occurred at least twice weekly for three months. The category "Eating Disorders Not Otherwise Specifieà" includes individuals who exhibit soxne, but not al1 of the behaviours specified for anorexia or bulimia (Amencan Psychiatric Association, f 994). food. This relationship has been called disordered or dysfinctional eating. It has been defined as "eating in irregular and chaotic ways" and includes dieting, fasting, bingeing,

skipping meals, eating by rigid rules, or eating for inappropriate reasons, such as anxiety, stress or boredom (Berg, 2001, p. 51-55). Disordered eating ofien results in eating more

or less than is needed in a way that fails to satisfy hunger or nourish the body appropriately.

The focus in this dissertation is on the way people talk about weight, and therefore

authors' (or speakers') terminology is accepted as given. 1generaily use the euphemistic

categories of thin and heuvy. These concepts are not parallel, but people do not speak of

light women, and although faz is politically prefemd within the fat acceptance

movement, it remains unacceptable to most people.

Reflections on the Evolution of the Research

My interest in Aboriginal participants and rnother-daughter pairs, the two major

dimensions of this research, grew out of findings from my master's research with a

sample of 20 primarily lower income families living in Winnipeg's inner city

(Marchessault, 1993a). The rural focus developed as a result of the way the research

evolved.

In my first study, the two generations had dmost identical mean scores on the

Eating Attitudes Test (EAT-40), a scale that measures risk for an eating disorder. The

mothers' mean score was 16.05 (standard deviation [SD]of 10.34). The daughters' mean

score was 16.55 (SD of 14.34). Despite this, there was no significant association between a girl's score and her mother's (Spearman's r = .2, N.S .). This was dso the case for a sub- scale that measured dieting and the desire for thinness. (Mean for the mothers was 4.65,

SD of 5.98; mean for the daughters was 4.85, SD of 6.43, r = -.01.)

The interviews suggested that aimost al1 the women had moderate concems about their weight, while concerns in the girls were more variable, ranging from very Little concem to clear distress and alarming food-related behaviours consistent with nsk for an eating disorder. Several of the most concerned girls had mothers who had grown up in countries that did not share our culture's focus on weight, and these mothers expressed little concern about their own weight. There were no families where both mother and daughter showed high levels of concern about weight. This raised questions about the frequently expressed like-mother, like-daughter explanation of how girls leam their attitudes to weight. However, the results that 1obtained could have been an artifact of small sarnple size and the measure used, selected for its ability to assess nsk for an eating disorder. Consequently, one of the objectives of this research was to examine the sirnilarity of mothers' and daughters' concem about weight with a larger and more demographically diverse sarnple using a greater variety of mesures.

A subsequent analysis of the same data, prepared for the Third International

Conference on Diabetes and Indigenous Peoples held in Winnipeg in May, 1995, found that the nine Aboriginal women in the sample scored significantly higher on the EAT-40 than the non-Aboriginal women (means of 21.4 and 11.3, respectively). This was consistent with the greater concern about weight expressed by the Aboriginal women in the open-ended portion of the interviews. The mean scores of the Aboriginal girls were not significantly different from those of the non-Aboriginal girls (20.3 and 14.2, respectively). Girls in both groups used potentially damaging weight loss behaviours

(Marchessault, 1998). The current research was designed to explore perspectives on weight of a larger urban Aboriginal sample, and to determine if Aboriginal women and girls living in a First Nations community were similarly concerned.

The master's research study documented fairly high levels of concern about weight in lower income families, but it was not lcnown how these concems compared to more middle-class participants. Therefore, this research was designed to include a higher income sample as well. The plan was to conduct interviews with families drawn from schools in a higher income neighbourhood and a rurai First Nations community. The inner city neighbourhood school was selected to yield both lower income Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal samptes. There were two inner city schools with an enrollment meeting the criteria, and the principals of both agreed to participate. The method used to contact parents, however, resulted in an insufficient response rate to proceed. The research plan was altered to do the interviews at an inner-city school serving the Aboriginal cornmunity and a school in a rural community chosen to match the First Nations community as closely as possible. The study was implemented at the Suburban and First Nations community schools as planned.

The change in research plan allowed a comparison of a First Nations community with a similar-sized Rural community. This allows an assessrnent of the impact of a small town environment on the importance of weight in people's daily inter-actions and assists in interpreting the findings in the First Nations community. Exploration of weight perceptions wi th rurai girls and women is also of interest because, as already noted, there is virtually no information about rural people's weight perceptions.

There were fewer eligible families than anticipated in the Urban Aboriginai school, and a non-random snowball sample was added. This was obtained by asking the interviewees and others to recommend Aboriginal families that 1could contact. The snowball sample resulted in an uiban Abonginal sample with a greater variety of income levels than the random sarnple. The wide range of income levels in the wban setting and the First Nations cornmunity provide additional information that adds to the understanding of Aboriginal perspectives.

In this thesis, 1wish to examine how girls learn their attitudes to fatness and thinness with specific attention to the mothers' alleged influence on their daughters. 1 am interested in exploring these dynamics in Aboriginai and non-Aboriginal families living in different settings. Like the proverbial Gordian hot, the strands of influence may be so tightly interwoven they cannot be untangled. But unlike Alexander the Great, we cannot slash through this riddle with a sharp sword. Picking a strand or two to examine the subtleties more closely can offer partial answers.

These partial answers highlight the complexity of weight problems for Canadian families. The results of this analysis do not lend t!!emselves to a simple summary statement that non-Aboriginal participants think one way, and Aboriginal participants think another way, that mothers will do just fine if they follow some identified script on weight. The results are more complex and also more interesting. The results suggest the importance of broadening the scope for examining weight problems and potential ways of handling them.

This is particularly clear in examining the mother's dilemma in sorting through the cornpeting pressures on her daughter. We live in a society that promotes promiscuous eating and contrives ever-more ingenious labour-saving devices and enforced or tempting sedentary activities. People often equate the provision of food, especially treats, with love. We expect families to withstand these day-to-day pressures. We expect mothers to counter the culture in other ways as well. Mothers must not only prevent ovenveight and obesity in their children, they must help their daughters stay thin without becoming weight pre-occupied.

Multiple pressures push the thin ideal in our society, affecting girls and women alike. Given the results of my master's research, 1couldn't help but wonder if mothers were being unfairly blamed for our culture's creation of and focus on weight problems.

Although 1believe that parents have an influence on their children, I am less sanguine about what that influence is, and how it is transmitted. 1wanted to know more about how mothers were coping with al1 these cornpeting pressures, and how their daughters reacted.

1enjoyed these interviews. Both the girls and their mothers intuitively broadened the topic beyond weight issues to discuss weight in the context of their lives, and this was always absorbing. Weight, and the issues involved with it, intertwine with other needs and values. It was obvious that mothers loved and cared for their daughters and wanted to help them successfully navigate the teen years to grow into healthy, effective and happy young women. They considered more than their daughters' physical needs, and sometimes supported their daughters even when they disagreed with their goals. The girls' energy, youth and potential were captivating, and their honesty and directness were

helpful and much appreciated,

My views on weight have changed since 1first began to study weight issues. My

initial plan was to interview people who had lost weight and successfully maintained that

loss for several years. My goal was to share their successful approaches with others.

Fortunateiy, other researchers had already investigated the topic. My views now lean

more toward the Vitality approach. Canada's Vitality program suggests we focus on

people's behaviours (active living and healthy eating), and increasing self-acceptance.

This approach seems sensible in preventing and treating both overweight and weight

preoccupation problems and is least Iikely to cause harxn to people. It lends itself to

promoting joie de vivre and quality of life for families.

After my interviews with First Nations community women, 1turned to the

wntings of First Nations women and this increased the complexity of my feminist

interpretation of weight issues. A qualitative approach is sufficiently flexible to allow this tjrpe of learning to occur. I have made an effort to represent participants' orientations and the reasons for their actions as fairly as 1could. As is true of al1 research, my interests naturally influence my choice of research question, methods, themes to arialyse and interprétation of results. Title and Outline of the Thesis

The Farfiom Ideal in the title of this thesis refers to problems with weight on multiple levels and implies at least one solution. It is meant to refer to: (a) deparmes from healthy weights which may signal lifestyle-related health nsks; (b) the widely acknowledged cultural imperative of extreme, emaciated thinness as the ideal body shape for women; and (c) girls' and women's consequent widespread dissatisfaction with their bodies and the actions they take to resolve that dissatisfaction. These situations, which permeated participants' discussions, are fa.from ideai.

More positively, this title is meant to convey the stmggle of some girls and women to accept their own bodies, bodies that are uniquely desirable and worthy of respect and care, regardless of sorne extemally defined unifonn standard of beauty. Their stniggle implies that king far from the ideal is acceptable because weight preoccupation is not. This theme did not permeate the interviews, but, surprisingly, was one of the few areas w here mothers ' influence on their daughters was apparent.

Finally, Farfrom Ideal is no? meant to refer to the mother-daughter relationship, but rather to the dilemma culture creates for mothers. To focus on the mother-daughter relationship is fiaught with danger, hardly fended off by a declaration that my intent is not to blame mothers for their daughters' concem about weight. I wish to re-emphasize that 1 began this exploration in reaction to the widespread, and seemingly cornmon-sense, assumption that mothers were the major influence on their daughters. In particular, would the well-established sirnilarities between groups of women and girls play out within individual pairs of mothers and daughters? The next chapter presents a review of the relevant literature. The literature review is divided into three parts. The fmt part presents a bnef overview of general weight issues for women in the North Amencan context, This overview sketches a picture of simultaneously increasing rates of obesity and weight preoccupation and outlines the impact on girls and women. The second part of the literature review looks at weight status and concems of Abonginal girls and women, 1end by discussing a conceptual framework for the potential influences on girls' and women's perceptions of weight issues. Within this framework, 1 discuss the studies that focus on the influence of families, concentrating especially on those that examine mothers' influence on their daughters.

In Chapter 3,1 describe the research design and analysis and explain the rationale for using interview techniques drawn from ethnographie and survey methodology, a somewhat unonhodox combination of methods. 1 also describe how the research was implemented and present selected charactenstics of the researc h participants.

Chapters 4 and 5 focus on the comparison of the Abonginal and non-Abonginal perspectives. In one of my first questions about weight, I asked each participant if weight was more important to women than to men. Abonginal and non-Abonginal women responded differently. The non-Aboriginal women spoke in a way that endorsed weight as a central construct of female identity. The Aboriginal participants, especially those in the

First Nations community, did not. Despite this difference, the Abonginal participants seemed at least as concemed about their weight, and this is explored in Chapter 5.

Chapter 6 extends this comparison to the four locations with an analysis of the responses to a set of statements on weight-related values. These statements were denved from comments made by participants during the 1993 interviews and are intended to reflect the participants' framework for understanding weight. The overall results suggested both sharing and differences across generations and across the four locations.

The results highlighted the First Nations community women and, to a !esser extent, the girls as somewhat different than the other women.

Chapters 7 and 8 focus on the mother-daughter relationship and the advice they give to each other and other people. Chapter 7 structures the discussion by girls' BMI, rather than by location or self-identified ethnicity, as this seemed to be more relevant.

Chapter 8 is structured by the content of the advice given, either to promote weight control or body acceptance. Both these chapters examine similarities and differences wi thin the mother-daughter pairs. As in Chapters 4 and S. these chapters are based largely on what participants said in response to the open-ended interview questions.

The last results chapter presents the analysis of three cornmonly used questionnaires about weight. The questionnaires reflect the interests of practitioners and researchers, and are aimed at quantifying the participants' level of concem about weight.

Cornparisons are made between locations, generations, AboriginalInon-Aboriginal participants, urban-rural regions and mother-daughter pairs.

1 conclude by sumrnarizing and integrating these results and discussing the policy implications for health professionals, school personnel, parents, and women and girls themselves. Far from Ideal

"The prevalence of overweight among boys increased from 15% in 1981 to 28.8% in 1996 and among girls from 15% to 23.6%. The prevalence of obesity in children more than doubled over that period, from 5% to 13.5% for boys and 11 3% for girls" uremblay & Willms, 2000, p. 1429).

"Fifty percent of females between the ages of 11 and 13 see themselves as overweight, and by the age of 13,8096 have attempted to lose weight, with 10% reporting the use of self-induced vomiting" (Costin, 1997, p. 1).

As we begin the twenty-fit century, weight issues continue to be problematic for girls and women. The quotes cited above juxtapose weight and weight preoccupation as widespread problems. Both aspects of this juxtaposition are explored in the first part of this chapter for girls and women generally, followed by specific attention to studies of

Aboriginal girls and women. A sociocultwal mode1 for influences affecting weight is presented next. This framework sets the stage for exarnining studies of family influences on girls' weight-related attitudes and behaviours, focussing particularly on the similarities between mothers and their daughters.

The recently declared "global epidemic" of obesity is the most visible weight problem (World Health Organization, 1998). In Canada, approximately half of adults and one-quarter of children are now considered to be overweigh t (Canadian Task Force on the

Periodic Health Examination, 1994; Health and Welfare Canada, 1988a; Limbert,

Crawford, & McCargar, 1994; Macdonald, Reeder, Chen, Despres, & The Canadian

Heart Health Surveys Research Group, 1997; Statistics Canada, 1995a; Tremblay & Willrns, 2000)Similar results have recently ken reported for Manitoba (Bromeil,

Kozyrskyj, Fergusson, Lerfald, 2OOl).' Annual health care costs directly associated with obesity have been estimated at $1.8 billion in Canada, and, including indirect costs, at

$99 billion in the United States (Birmingham, Muller, Palepu, Spinelli, & Anis, 1999;

Wolf & Colditz, 1998). The public's concem is reflected in estimated expenditures of $30 billion annually on over 17,000 diet plans, products and programs in the United States alone (Hesse-Biber, 1996, p. 39).

While the average weight of women under age 30 has been increasing, it seems that the ideal womn has become increasingly slender. David Garner and his colleagues' classic study (1980) documented decreasing weights of Playboy centerfold models and

Miss Arnerica Pageant contestants from 1959 to 1978. This decline in weight continued until 1992, when the majority's body measurements were consistent with a diagnosis of anorexia nervosa (Wiseman, Gray, Mosimann, & Ahrens, 1992). The average mode1 is now 5'10" and weighs 110 pounds (Hesse-Biber, 1996). Images of the thin ideal, so pervasive in visual media that they cannot be avoided, portray the dominant paradigm for what is considered central to fernininity and cntical to success for women (Bordo, 1993;

*Rates Vary depending on the methodology and cnteria used to specify problems. Common cutoffs are based on BMIs of 25,27 and 30, yielding prevalence rates of overweight or obesity of 57%, 35% and 13% for men, and 48%, 27% and 14%for women (Macdonald et al., 1997). Estimates for children vary fiom 5% to 43% (Canadian Task Force on the feriodic Health Examination, 1994; Health and Welfare Canada, 1988a; Limbert et al., 1994).

'~lrnost29% of Manitoba children (aged 6 to 19) were assessed as obese or at-risk for obesity based on self-reported data colIected for the 1996 National Population Health Survey. Risk was defined as having a BMI at or above the 85" prcentile of the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 1. Brumberg, 1989; Fallon, Katzman, & Wmley, 1994; Stice, 1994). Even though

unrealistic and unachievable for dl but a minuscule few, these images create the context

by which females in Western civilization are judged. Bodies that depart fiom the ideaI are

constmcted as flawed and in need of remedy Darling-Wolf, 2000).

Girls and wornen continue to see their own bodies as far from the depicted ided, as indicated by the high rates of body dissatisfaction reported for females of al1 ages. For example, 55% of elementary school girls in one survey said they wanted to be thinner

(Edmunds & Hill, 1999). In another, 61% of teens reported having tried to lose weight within the last year (Nichter, 2000). Sixty-seven percent of women over the age of 30 Say they are unhappy with their weight. Although the importance of weight may lessen with age (Garner, 1997), dissatisfaction with weight continues to be reported by women in their 60s and 70s (Lafrance, Zivian, & Myers, 2000; Pliner, Chaiken, & Flett, 1990). The association of weight gain with aging (into the 50s) is well documented (Evers, 1987;

Health and Welfare Canada, 1988a; Macdonald et al., 1997; Moore, Stunkard, & Srole,

1962) and may partially account for the continued high levels of dissatisfaction. Body dissatisfaction is reportedly on the rise for men, but rernains lower and split between desires to be Iarger as well as smaller (Andersen, Cohn, & Holbrook., 2000; Garner,

1997; Grogan, 1999).

The degree of weight dissatisfaction can be glimpsed in the results of a

Psychology Today survey (Garner, 1997). A quarter of the aimost 3,500 women who responded said they would sacrifice three years of their lives to Pe at their desired weight.

This wishful thinking illustrates that weight continues to be an issue for girls and women as we enter the new millennium. It is still rare for a girl or woman to even grudgingly

acknowledge that her body is acceptable, while discussion of flaws or self-improvement plans are ubiquitous.

Al1 this body dissatisfaction is not benign. Forty percent of women and 27% of

girls aged 12 to 14 reported trying to lose weight in 1994-95 (Health Canada, 2000).

Many do not need to, as indicated by results of surveys finding 3040% of healthy weight

and 10% of underweight Canadian women trying to lose weight (Green et al., 1997;

HeaIth Canada, 2000).~

Dieting efforts are implicated in teen-aged girls' and women's increased nsk of deficient intakes of multiple nutrients (Amencan Dietetic Association & Dietitians of

Canada, 2000a; Berg, 1997; Lambert-Lagace, 1989; Neumark-Sztainer, Story, Resnick, &

Blum, 1998). Risks of dieting include retardation of physical growth. imtability, poor concentration, chronic fatigue, hypertension, heart irregularities, anaemia, haîr loss, gallstones, diarrhea and depression (Ashley & Kannel, 1974; Health and Welfare Canada,

1988a; 1988b; Lifshitz & Moses, 1988; McCargar & Yeung, 1991; Polivy & Herman,

1986; Story et al., 1994).

Widespread efforts to control or change weight andfor shape are often misguided and more likely than not to be unsuccessful (Canadian Task Force on the Periodic Health

Ex amination, 1994; Douketis, Feightner, A ttia, Feldman, & Canadi an Task Force on

Preventive Health Care, 1999). Dieting has been associated with bingeing or compulsive

6Although mean weights are increasing among youth (Limbert et al., 1994), young women remain twice as likely to be classified as "underweight" as "overweight" (Statistics Canada, 1995a). eating and depressed metabolic rates that ironically may lead to weight gain when normal eating is resumed (Garner & Wooley, 1991; McCargar & Yeung, 1991; Rothblum, 1990).

Subclinical eating problems are the best predictors of the subsequent development of eating disorders (Graber, Archibald, & Brooks-Gunn, 1999). While obesity carries lifetime health risks, eating disorders can be fatal to young people in a relatively short period of time (Berg, ~oOO).' Disordered eating does not usually lead to an eating disorder, but our culture's fascination with slenderness and homor of fatness is the backdrop against which eating disorders occur.

Because of the way weight is frarned in our culture, with image designed to cornmunicate identity, feelings about the body can be seen as a proxy for feelings about the self (Friedman, 1997). Consequently, undermining girls' confidence in their bodies increases their vulnerabilities, not just to food and health-related problems, but spills over to problems linked with low self-esteem. The frequent failure to lose weight can cause psychological distress (Health and Welfare Canada, 1991a). Girls stress how they feel unattractive and undesirable even when the topic is teen pregnancy, violence, school achievement or the place of feminism in their lives (Artz, 1998; Bnunberg, 1997;

Glickman, 1993; Orenstein, 1994; Pipher, 1995; Robertson, 1991).

This focus on weight and appearance is not vanity but a logical reaction to cultural imperatives for women. Consistent messages implying that thinness is equivalent to

'~hefatali ty rate for anorexia is over 101(Amencan Psychiatrie Association, 1994). The prevalence of anorexia and bulimia in Canadian adolescents is estimated at 1- 2%, and 2-34, respectively (Health Canada, 2000, p. 24). Similar rates of anorexia (1%) and slightly higher rates of bulimia (6.6%) have ken estimated for Manitoba youth based on body weight and a scale measuring risk for eating disorders (Leichner et al., 1986). happiness, health and success, and that fatness signifies disease, premature death, iailure

and certain misery, have created a cultural preoccupation with weight (Marchessault,

1993b). There is evidence that these cultural messages have had an effect on Abonginal

people as well. This will be addressed next, starting with a consideration of Aboriginal people's weight status.

Aboriginal People's Weieht Status. Perce~tionsand Behavioursg

Aboriginal people's Iives and Iifestyles have changed drastically in living memory.

These historical changes have affected health status, as reflected in a shift from infectious to chronic diseases, sometimes referred to as the "epiderniologic transition" (Young,

1994). The major hedth burden has shifted to diseases such as cancer, ischemic heart disease, hypertension, stroke and diabetes. Weight is associated with many of these diseases, and it is this association that has led to researchers' interest in the weight status of Aboriginal people. Genetic susceptibility and lifestyle changes in activity and dietary patterns, food availability, social welfare, and stress, among other factors, are important topics related to the cause of weight problems. The focus here is on how people talk about their weight concerns. Therefore, this review concentrates on the weight-related attitudes and self-reported behaviours that reveal concem about weight. Weight noms provide essential background for these concems.

'~ortionsof this section of the literature review have ken adapted from Marc hessault, G. (1999). Weight perceptions and prac tices in Native youth. Healthy Weighr Journal, 13(5), 71-73,79. The heavier weight noms of Aboriginal people are a relatively recent phenornenon (Harrison & Ritenbaugh, 1992; Mohs, Leonard, & Watson, 1988; Story et al., 1998; Waldram et al., 1995; Young, 1994). Prior to the 1940s, starvation was the major problem (Moore, Kxuse, Tisdall, & Comgan, 1946; Vivian et al., 1948). Reports of a higher prevalence of obesity began to appear in the mid-1970s (Health and Welfare

Canada, 1975; West, 1974). Available comparative data indicates that BMI or other measures of adiposity have increased for Aboriginal children and adults, just as they have for non-Aboriginal populations (Berg, 2000; 2001; Garner et al., 1980; Hall, Hickey, &

Young, 1993; Harrison & Ritenbaugh, 1992; Limbert et al., 1994; Mohs et al., 1988;

Mokdad et al., 1999; 20;Sugarman, White, & Gilbert, 1990; Tremblay & Willms,

2000; Will, Denny, Serdula, & Muneta, 1999). The Aboriginal increases in weight are much greater, given that mean weights were once below population means, and now they are consistently found to be higher.

Despite using variable measwes, most researchers find 30-40% of Aboriginal girls are overweight (Story et al., 1999). The prevalence of reported overweight and obesity varies from 25% to 78%, and from 6.1% to 51.8%, respectively (Broussard et al., 1991;

Davis, Gomez, Lambert, & Skipper, 1993; Jackson, 1993; Story, Tornpkins, Bass, &

Wakefield, 1986; Sugman et al., 1990). The prevalence is higher for Aboriginal women

(Broussard et al., 1991; Harnack et al., 1999; Story et al., 1999; Teny & Bass, 1984).

Research in Canadian First Nations communities is more limited, but findings are similar (Bernard, Lavallee, Gray-Donald, & Delisle, 1995; Hanley et al., 2000;

Katzmarzyk & Malina, 1998; McIntyre & Shah, 1986; Potvin et al., 1999). The Nutrition Canada Survey of 1970, while dated, is the only nation-wide study that reports body weight for Aboriginal ? It found they generally had higher weight-for-age than other Canadians (Health and Welfare Canada, 1975). Over 60% of youth aged 4 to 19 in a remote Ojibwa-Cree cornmunity in northem Manitoba had a BMi that exceeded the 85& percentile of the American National Center for Health Statistics, and 40% of girls had

BMIs over the 95" percentiie (Young, Dean, Flett, & Wood-Steiman, 2000). Young and

Sevenhuysen (1989) classified close to 90% of Cree and Ojibway women (aged 45 to 54) from northern Ontario and Manitoba as at nsk for health problems due to excessive weight, defined as a BMI at or above 26. Gittelsohn and his colleagues (1996) reported that approximately 30% of those aged 10-19,50% of those aged 20-29, and 70-80% of those over age 30 had a BMI over 27. The consistent pattern of strikingly high prevalence rates of obesity, in combination with the emergence of chronic diseases associated with obesity (Waldram et al., 1995; Welty, 1991; Young, 1994), has led to concem about weight control. Some researchers recommend prevention programs start in elementary school (Delisle, Mavrikakis, & Strychar, 1995; Macaulay et al., 1996; Perez, 1995; Story et al., 1999; Wilson, Graham, Booth, & Gohdes, 1993). Undertaking these interventions requires a clear understanding of the views and practices of Abonginal people regarding weight. This topic has been studied much less extensively.

Several investigators focussing on middle-aged and older Abonginai people with chronic health problems such as diabetes have reported varying perceptions of weight

g~elf-reportedheight and weight was collected for a national sample in the Aboriginal Peoples Survey of 1991, but these data have not been analysed. (Garro, 1995; 1996; Joe, 1993; Lang, 1989). Some Aboriginal people have indicated a preference for heaviness as a sign of health and as the ideal of attractiveness (Garro &

Lang, 1993; Hickey & Carter, 1993). Spielmann (1993) titled his article "You're So Far, " using his wife's hurt reaction to an elder's intended compliment as a prototypical example of the vast cuItural differences between Abonginal and White ways of Iooking at the world. In their study of Ojibway-Cree people living in northern Ontario, Gittelsohn and his colleagues (1996) concluded that the personally desired shapes of participants (aged

10 and up) were larger than those reported in the literature (usually American college students, which they recognized as a problematic compatison). In a national survey of

American Indian high school students, one-third of the girls with a BMI in the heaviest quartile perceived themselves as "normal" weight (Story et al., 1994). That "normal" does not mean "acceptable" was apparent given high rates of weight dissatisfaction (25%). long-tem dieting (31%) and vomiting for weight toss (43.5%)in the girls who rated themselves as normal weight. Whether Aboriginal girls and women have different ideals for attractive and healthy body shapes remains unclear.

Native American girls and women have consistently been found to be at least as concemed about their weight as other North Amencans. Table 2.1 summarizes this work.

Most of these are smdl studies in the rural United States (Davis et al., 1993; Hamack et al., 1999; Rosen et al., 1988a; Smith & Krejci, 1991; Snow & Harris, 1989; Stevens et al.,

1999; Story et al., 1986; 1995; Terry & Bass, 1984). There is also one national survey in the United States (Neumark-Sztainer et al., 1997; Story et al., 1994) and one Canadian study (Gittelsohn et al., 1996). Table 2.1 Studies of Body Dissatisfaction and Weight Control Practices of Aboriginal People

Subjects Reported Tried Self- Used a Used a Fasting, feeling to induced laxative diiiretic exlreme OW UWa lose vomiting dietingb Author, year Tribe, location Agetgrade n Sex 96 % % % % % %

Stevens et Arizona, New Grade 4 164 F 48 22 38 al., lW9 Mexico, South 140 M 34 15 38 Dakota MtF

Davis et al., Navajo, Pueblo, Grade 5 1,543 MtF 34 15 59 1993 New Mexico

Story et al., National U.S. 13,454 1994 Grade 7-9 F 34 42 28 0.6 1.2 6 M 20 30 II 1.4 0.9 3

Grade 10-12 F 50 56 26 0.7 1.2 8 M 23 3 1 13 0.8 0.5 4

Story et al., Minnesota Grade 7- 12 291 F 51 8 19 2.1 2.7 11 1995 269 M 27 12 1O 0.7 0.7 2

Story et al., Cherokee, North Grade 8- IO 138 F 73 1986 Carolina 139 M 36 Continucd ncxt pap Table 2.1 continucd Studies of Body Dissatisfaction and Weight Control Practices of Aboriginal People

Subjects Reported Tried Self- Used a Used a Fasting, feeling: ta induced laxative diuretic extreme OW UWa !ose vomiting dietingb Author, year Tribe, location Age/grade n Sex % % % % % % %

Smith and Southwest U.S. High school Krejci, 1991

Snow and Pueblo, High school Harris, 1989 Southwest U.S.

Gittelsohn et Ojibway-Cree, Age 10 - 60+ al., 19% Ontario

Rosen et al., Chippewaya, Age 12-55 1988 Michigan

Hamack et Lakota, South Age 19-47 al., 1999 Dakota

Terry and Cherokee, North Age 18-87 Bass. 1984 Carolina

'ûW; UW = Feeling overweight or undenveight, and includes reports of wishing to weigh less or more; desiring to be slimmer or heavier. bVariously defineâ as missing more than three mals in a row, not eating al1 day, or dieting more than 10 times in the past year. 'In this study, proportion using laxatives and diuretics were combined. % this study, vomiting and Iaxativeldiuretic use was combined. 'Estimates were reported only for the entire sample, although there were significant differences by age and gender. Fewer 10-19 year old children and more 30-49 year old adults would like to be slimmer. Only a small proportion of 30-49 year old adults indicated a desire for a fuller body shape. '~urrenteffort only. The studies cited in Table 2.1 demonstrate a pattern of concem about weight in girls and women. Wherever measured, at least a third of Aboriginal females thought they were overweight or wanted to weigh less. As many as 59% of Grade 4 and 5 girls, anci up to 82% of older Abonginal girls, reported feeling overweight. Lower proportions of boys reported dissatisfaction, with the discontent split more evenly between feeling overweight and feeling underweight. msis important to consider when findings are cornbined). The few studies of Abonginal adults' perceptions of their weight suggest even higher levels of dissatisfaction.

Given the high levels of discontent, it is not surprising that many Abonginai people have tried, or are currently trying to lose weight. Rates as high as 74% and 75% have been reported for girls and women (Rosen et al., 1988a; Story et al., 1986; Terry &

Bass, 1984). Surveys of Grades 4 and 5 students demonstrate that the concept of dieting is well-known even to young children (Davis et al., 1993; Stevens et al., 1999). Children, adolescents and adults suggest exercising, eating smaller portions and eating more healthfully as weight-loss strategies they used or recomrnended (Davis et al., 1993;

Hamack et ai., 1999; Rosen et al., 1988a; Stevens et al., 1999). However, some

Aboriginal people have reported using potentially health darnaging weight-loss techniques.

In a 1988-90 swvey of 13,454 American Indian-Native Alaska youth, Story and her colleagues (1994) found that unhealthy weight-control practices were common among

Grade 7 to 12 students living on or near reservations throughout the United States.

Almost half of the gids (48%) mported they had dieted in the past year, and 7% reported they dieted more than 10 times a year. Twenty-seven percent of the girls reported inducing vomiting, more than double the rate reported by other rural youth. Eleven percent said they used diet pills, and a small proportion used laxatives and diuretics (0.6% and 1.5%, respectively). Twelve percent of boys reported vomiting to lose weight, indicating the importance of this problem for boys as well.

In an exploration of ethnic differences in dieting behaviours of Grade 7 to 12 students in Minnesota, Story and her colleagues (1995) found Arnerican Indian students had the lowest prevalence of weight satisfaction compared to White, Black, Hispanic and

Asian youth. They also had among the highest rates of deliberate vomiting. Other weight- related behaviours were comparable. hwerestimates of self-induced vomiting have been reported elsewhere, but these occur when findings are aggregated for both sexes (Smith &

Krejci, 199 l), or for youth and adults (Rosen et al., l988a). Women have also reported using undesirable weight-loss methods, as shown by a study of Nota women from two communities in South Dakota. They reported not eating for a day (54%), increasing their smoking (16%), taking diet pills (14%), vomiting (7.5%) and taking diuretics (5.6%) or laxatives (1.9%) to control their weight (Harnack et al., 1999).

Based on self-reported height and weight, Neumark-Sztainer and colleagues

(1997) classified 25% of close to 1,200 adolescents from the above-noted Amencan

Indian-Native Alaska survey as overweight. Even the youth who were not classified as overweight were dissatisfied with their bodies (60%). The overweight youth reported slightly lower rates of health-promoting behaviows, such as regularly eating fruits and vegetables and being active. Lower rates of healthy behaviours in heavier children were also reported in two Cree communities in Quebec (Bernard et al., 1995). Rates of frequent dieting, binge eating, diet pi11 use and vorniting increased with increasing BMI, as was the case in earlier studies (Rosen et aI., 1988a; Snow & Hams, 1989).

These large studies confiearlier, smaller studies of eating disturbances and unhealthy weight-reducing methods among Aboriginal youth. Snow and Hamis (1989) found 11% of 51 Pueblo Indian girls reported eating habits consistent with a diagnosis of bulimia. Smith and Krejci (1991) found the Native Amencan students consistently scored higher than White and Hispanic students on al1 disordered eating items on the Eating

Disorder Inventory and the Bulimia Test, with similar proportions of each group meeting strict critena for binge eating. The prevalence of eating disorders in Aboriginal girls is not known, but these reports suggest nsk might be hi@.

With the possible exception of Terry and Bass (1984), who asked Cherokee women for their primary reasons for wanting to change their weight, al1 of the studies noted above used survey methodology to collect data on perceptions and practices. There is a need for research to combine surveys with other methods in exploring Aboriginal and other ethnic groups' ideas about weight. Parker and her colleagues (1995) found African-

American girls'responses on a survey to be inconsistent with their responses in ethnographie interviews. Individual interviews and focus groups suggested that African-

American high school girls had flexible and individualized concepts of beauty more related to style and expressiveness than to physical appearance. This was contrasted with the White students' stereotypical "Barbie" do11 images of beauty. Despite this, more

Afiican-Arnerican girls reported trying to lose weight (54%) than Caucasian girls (44%). Responses were different when a culturally sensitive survey based on ethnographie interviews and administered by an African-Amencan researcher was used.

There are few reports about weight based on open-ended interviews with

Aboriginal people. It is possible that the above surveys misrepresent Aboriginal people's concerns about weight. The in-depth interviews that 1 conducted for my first study were consistent with the general findings reported above, but they were conducted in an urban setting with only eight Native families (Marchessault, 1998). 1am aware of only one other published qualitative study of Aboriginal peoples' views of the body. This study, with Yurok women in northem Califomia, widened the context to their attribution of problems associated with the body to community and personal histones of violence, brutality and exploitation (Ferreira, 1998).

Despite great diversity in the lives of Canadian and American Abonginal peoples, the consistency of these findings suggests potential relevance for many communities, and therefore the necessity of a better understanding of these concems.

Bodv Image

The previous discussion of weight perceptions, dissatisfaction with body weight and weight-related behaviours suggests a need to define body image. The concept of body image grew out of studies in the early 1900s of distorted body perceptions related to brain darnage (Grogan, 1999). Discussions of body image retain this connotation of distortion, and books on body image are frequently illustrated with a thin young woman looking in a mirror, "seeing" a much heavier image (for an example, see Cash, 1997). Paul Schilder, the first to study perceptions and experiences of the body more broadly in the 1920s, defined body image as the mental picture we have of Our own bodies, a definition still in use, and the one that 1prefer (Gingras, 1998; Grogan, 1999; Health Canada, 2000). Body image has been defined as "a person's perceptions, thoughts and feelings about his or her body" (Grogan, 1999, p. 1). Cash (1999), who has written prolifically on body image, also includes related behaviours. This global definition is used often. However, the individual components of the construct are not always congruent, and it is sometimes useful to examine them separately.

As documented above, body dissatisfaction, defined as a negative evaluation of the appearance of one's body, or some aspect of it, is cornmon. The impkation is that if dissatisfied, a person will feel unhappy and will attempt to change the body. However, this is not the only possibility. One cm objectively evaluate one's body as falling short of one's ideal but still be accepting of it. If the appearance of the body is relatively unimportant, then a negative self-evatuation will not lead to more global dissatisfaction or unhappiness. People can also consciously work towards changing their attitude to their body rather than trying to change the body itself, the usual objective of workbooks on body image (Cash, 1997). 1refer to this aspect of body image as body acceptance to differentiate it from body satisfaction.

People cm be both dissatisfied with and accepting of or striving to accept their body, but they cannot be accepting of the body and trying to change it at the sarne time.

For exarnple, as women get older, they typically become heavier and more dissatisfied with their body shape, but they may also view being thin as less important or less achievable, and adjust their expectations accordingly. They may more realistically focus

on improving lifestyle habits and trying to accept the body shape that results than on

trying to achieve a specific weight. To take another example, a girl may be dissatisfied

with her body, but assess her desire to weigh 110 pounds as unrealistic for her 5'10"

frame. Acceptance of her body frame may corne easily, with a struggle, or, far too often,

not at dl. However, if unrealistic desires are seen as such, accepted and viewed as

relatively unimportant, then body acceptance or striving for body acceptance can coexist

with some degree of dissatisfaction. This distinction between satisfaction and acceptance

has implications for the actions individuals take with regards to resolving negative body

image, as will be seen in Chapters 7 and 8 when mothers' and daughters' advice about

weight is exarnined.

Sociocultural Context for Perce~tionsof Weieht

Many individual and societal factors can affect susceptibility to weight pressures.

Some factors that have been investigated include body weight (Attie & Brooks-Gunn,

1989; Birch & Fisher, 2000; Stice, 1994), the timing of physical maturation (Attie &

Brooks-Gunn, 1989; Graber et al., 1999), self esteem (Gnlo, Wilfiey, Brownell, & Rodin,

1994; Hill & Pallin, 1998; Stice, 1994), the normal stresses of adolescence (Levine &

Smolak, 1998), coping skills and social support (Stice, 1994), family relationships or functioning (Haudek, Rorty, & Henker, 1999; Ogden & Steward, 2000), and trauma, including poverty and racism (Health Canada, 2000; Thompson, 1996). The consistently strong association of weight and socioeconomic statu suggests it has an often-overlooked but integral role in the evolution of weight concems (Evers, 1987; Gam, 1981; Goldblatt,

Moore, & Stunkard, 1965; Leichner et al-, 1986; Sobal, 1991; Sobal & Stunkard, 1989).

The media, peers and faxdy are often discussed as major influences on how girls cope with weight issues (Hill, In press; Stice, 1994; Striegel-Moore, Silberstein, & Rodin,

1986; Tucker, 2000). Published reviews of the literature provide modest support for an association between eac h of these factors and weight-related attitudes. However, the relative contribution of each to causation, maintenance and exacerbation of concems remains unclear (Berndt & Hestenes, 1996; Galuska, Serdula, Parmuk, Siegel, & Byers,

1996; Hesse-Biber, 1996; Levine & Smolak, 1998; Thompson & Heinberg, 1999). In one study, 44% of respondents said teasing by others shaped their body image when they were young (Garner, 1997, p. 36). This was followed by their mother's and father's attitudes to their body (3 1%and 23%, respectively), and movie and TV celebrities (22%) or fashion and magazine models (23%). However, a study of Grade 10 girls in Austraiïa suggested the reverse order (Paxton, Schutz, Wertheim, & Muir, 1999).

Eric Stice (1994), after a comprehensive review of the literature, hypothesized a sociocultural mode1 for factors promoting bulimia nervosa in girls. He included family, peers and the media as carriers of the key sociocultural pressures--the thin ideal, the importance of appearance for success, and the centraiity of appearance to fernales. Stice further proposed that body dissatisfaction, restrained eating and negative affect might account for the relationship between sociocultural pressures and bulunia. He also suggested that self-esteem, identity confusion, weight, coping skius, impulsivity and famil y, peer and media modelling of attitudes, behaviours and ideals moderated the direction and strength of the impact of these pressures.

In adapting Stice's model to apply to girls' and women's normative weight

concerns (see Figure 2. l),1 drew on my master's interviews and research addressing more

general weight concems. 1 added the stigmatization of fat as an additional socioculturaI

pressure because 20 of 20 girls and 19 of 21 women spontaneously raised weight-related

harassment or "teasing" to explain why weight was an issue to girls and women. This has

been addressed in the literature as well (Allon, 1973; Dalton, 1997; Joanisse & Synnott,

1999). The participants also talked about the media and pers as influences, although girls

tended to emphasize "frïends and boyfriends," while women spoke more about the media.

In contrast to the emphasis in the research literature, participants seldom raised farnily

influences. This discrepancy is of major interest. Although the girls raised health issues, it

was usually discussed in only a cursory fashion. The women raised health and fitness

concerns repeatedy, and so this was added to the model as a fourth influence carrier.

Others have also reported that middle-aged women suggest health concems or a

physician's advice as partial motivation for dieting or watching their weight (Galuska et

al., 1996; Green et al., 1997; Hesse-Biber, 1996; Koslow, 1988; Teny & Bass, 1984),

although some have concluded the underlying motivation remains weight or shape-related

(Chapman, 1999; Colvin & Olson, 1983). Women's discussion of weight and health may

serve to normalize and legitimize weight concems in daily life.

Stice (1994) emphasized modelling of attitudes and behaviows in his model. The literature and my own research suggested two additional mechanisms for transmitting weight concems: (a) social approval expresscd through compliments, positive attention, Centrali ty (Far] w'=-ce Cfor ïnaiegen~ lnnuencecarriers: iM-aJ

Mechanisms: *Social modelling (to protect against or *Social approvai promote dissatisfaction) *Social disapproval

Personalfictor.g Other factor$ Gender Socioeconomic status Age I Race & racism Timing of physical maturation Trouma/inequality,/poveq Body weight Weight nomof peer group Self esteem Body dissatisfaction Family fwlctioning Coping skills Social & community support Values 1 Importance of

to individual

Disordercd eating

Figure 2.1. Influences on body dissatisfaction, weight preoccupation and disordered eating. (Adapted from Stice, 1994. p. 646. Additions are italicized.) 38 friendships, dating; and (b) social disapproval expressed as discrimination, teasing or

harassment, or, less blatantly, weight control advice (Health Canada, 20;Pike & Rodin,

1991; Stice, 1994). Each of these rhree mechanisms cut across the four major carriers of

influence. Aithough usually perceived to heighten concern about weight, participants

indicated they can aileviate weight concems as well (Marchessault, 1993a). The personal

and other factors, discussed above, are show affecting the internalization of the

pressures at the point where they may produce body dissatisfaction. 1 have positioned the

importance of appearance to the individual after body dissatisfaction to indicate it is key

in the subsequent development of weight preoccupation and disordered eating.

This framework demonstrates that the acquisition of weight concerns involves an

immense complexity. The role of mothers is but one of many influences, including

historical and cultural factors, as well as more individual biological and psychological

factors. This review will focus on family influences next and then examine the studies

that compare mothers and daughters.

Fami ly

Certainly, in retrospective accounts people with poor body image remember

various family members cnticising their weight or making hurtfùl comments (Hesse-

Biber, 1996; Joanisse & Synnott, 1999; Schwartz, Phares, Tantleff-Dunn, & Thompson,

1999). Almost one-third of the girls in a qualitative study said that a parent had advised them to lose weight (Nichter, 2000). Based on her interviews with women, Carole

Spitzack (1988, p. 83) summed up her interviewees' recollections of childhood interactions with their parents over appearance with the heading, "mother criticizes, father compliments." She concluded that both serve as powerful influences in making weight a salient issue.

Parents themselves, both mothers and fathers, report helping their children, boys as well as girls, lose weight. However, overall, they report king content with their children's appearance, with a predilection to praise girls about their appearance more than boys. Only a minority reported criticising their children, although they were most likely to criticize and least likely to praise adolescents (Striegel-Moore & Kearney-Cooke, 1994).

Contrary to Spitzack's conclusion, mothers in this study, reported complimenting their children more often than fathers, with no difference in their reporting of cntical comments. Parents reported making such comments more often than girls reported their parents making them (Thelen & Cormier, 1995). The association between parental comment and girls' attitudes was reported as stronger for parental self-reports in one study

(Thelen & Cormier, 1995), but stronger for girls' reports of parental behaviour in another

(Keel et al., 1997).

Examining reports of association between parental commentary and various indices of concern about weight produces mixed evidence (Keel et al., 1997; Smolak &

Levine, 1996; Thelen & Cormier, 1995). Thelen and Cormier (1995), after controlling for body weight, found that only the fathers' reported encouragement for weight control correlated with dieting. Smolak and hvine (1996) found that only the mothers' comments about daughters' weight were significantly correlated with weight concem and weight loss attempts. When both parents commented, children made more attempts to Iose weight, but the lack of control for body weight is problematic in this cornparison.

Keel and her colleagues (1997) found the mothers' comments more closely related to

daughters' dieting (restraint scores), while the fathers' comments were related to

daughters' weight dissatisfaction-

There are few studies that examine the effect of the fathers' role modelling of

concern and behaviour. One such study found a daughter's weight dissatisfaction was

associated with her father's dissatisfaction, but weight was not controlled (Keel et al.,

1997). Other studies have reported no association of father variables with daughters'

dieting tendencies (Smolak, Levine & Schemer, 1999; Steiger, Stotland, Trottier, &

Ghadirian, 1996).

Although sibling teasing is mentioned frequently, it does not appear to have been

systematically examined. In one snidy, girls with restrictive attitudes towards eating were

twice as likely to have brothers (Hill & Franklin, 1998). Siblings' non-weight-related

values such as competitiveness and the valuation of non-traditional roles for women have

been associated with weight concerns (Ogden & Chanana, 1998).

Other aspects of family relationships are repeatedly found to be important (Attie

& Brooks-Gunn, 1989; Bruch, 1978; Haudek et al., 1999; Hill & Franklin, 1998; Ogden

& Steward, 2000; Pike & Rodin, 1991; Silverstein & Perlick, 1995; Striegel-Moore et al.,

1986; Thompson, 1996). Swan and Richards (1996) found that young adolescents'

reports of more time spent with parents correlated with healthier eating habits two years

later and offset some of the negative effects of early puberty. Graber, Archibaid and

Brooks-Gunn (1999) suggest the ability to handle conflict while maintaining wann relationships may be cntical. Nichter (2000) also pointed out that the quality of the relationship made a difference to teen-aged girls' reactions to their mothers' weight- related advice. If given sensitively within an overall positive relationship, the advice seemed to be appreciated. In conflicted relationships, girls sometimes resisted the advice as a way of establishing their autonomy.

Family hinctioning itself must be contextualized within the structures that support or fail to support families. Social and living conditions such as poverty, personal safety, inequality and racisrn have direct and indirect impact on parents' abilities to care for their children (Health Canada, 2000; Thompson, 1996).

Mothers and Daunhters

It is perhaps not surprising that the research focus within the farnily context is on the mother-daughter relationship. Weight continues to be viewed as a woman's issue, although this may be changing (Andersen, Cohn, & Holbrook, 2000; Grogan, 1999).

Mothers typically remain most involved with child rearing; they spend more time nurturing and socializing their children than fathers. There is the added aspect of shared gender between mothers and daughters with nch possibilities for role modelling andor rebellion. The notion that rnothers have a major influence on their daughters' weight concems is widespread both in the popular press and. in academic writing (Brigman,

1994; Johnson, 1995; Parker et al., 1995; Striegel-Moore et al., 1986; Waterhouse, 1997).

Rabinor (1994) has documented numerous examples of researc h articles that assume an association between mothers' and daughters' concem with weight. Debra Waterhouse (1997) acknowledges these assumptions in the title of her recent book, Like Mother, Like Duughter: How Women are Znfluenced by Their Mothers'

Relationship with Food--and How to Break the Pattern. Waterhouse, a dietitian with 15 years experience counselling eating disordered women, opens her book with the statement :

Weight-loss behaviours are so firmly integrated into our daily lives that mothers diet without thinking about it and pass dong their weight preoccupations to their daughters without realizing it. By modelling these behaviours and setting the standard for female identity: A mother's dieting history becomes her daughter's dieting future; a mother's eating habits are passed on to her daughter; a mother's poor body image is mirrored by her daughter. (p. xv)

Waterhouse notes that daughters lem to fear weight gain and how to diet from many sources, but she clearly views mothers as the most important influence. She writes,

"Daughters learn them [dieting behaviours] from other women, peers, mentors, and the media--but especiallyfrom their morhers" (Emphasis in original, p. 2). She provides the following evidence:

88% of al1 mothers have dieted, and 80% of their adolescent daughters have already embarked on their first diet. 75% of al1 mothers "feel" too fat, and 81% of their teenage daughters suffer from this same feeling of weight preoccupation. An estimated 60% of al1 mothers have practised severe weight-loss attempts through repeated dieting, skipping meds, fasting; laxatives, diuretics, vomiting, and/or excessive exercise. And 80% of their daughters are following in their unhealthy footsteps. (p. xvi)

However, these results are drawn from separate studies of unrelated women and adolescents. They provide evidence that weight is an issue to the majority of females of ail ages in North America. They do no? provide evidence that mothers influence their daughters. This is, of course, possible. It is equally plausible that something else could be infiuencing both girls and women.

Waterhouse's book is well-referenced (and provides good advice). At the time she wrote it, few studies in nonclinical settings had exarnined how mothers' own body image and eating behaviours influenced their daughters. These studîes reported confiicting results (Attie & Brwks-Gunn, 1989; Hill, Weaver, & Blundell, 1990; Pike & Rodin,

1991; Steiger et al., 1996). Since then, additional studies imply less similarity and greater complexity than the above "facts" suggest. Table 2.2 lists studies that have surveyed both mothers and daughters to test for association between their attitudes to weight and dieting behaviours (Attie & Brooks-Gunn, 1989; Byely et al., 2000; Hill & Franklin, 1998; Hill et al ., 1990; Keel et al., 1997; Marchessault, 1993a; Ogden & Chanana, 1998; Ogden &

Elder, 1998; Pike & Rodin, 1991; Ruther & Richman, 1993; Smolak et ai., 1999; Thelen

& Cormier, 1995).L0Further details about these and several other mother-daughter studies addressing related questions are given in Appendix A-1 and A-2. As stated earlier, 1am not aware of any qualitative snidies that interview both a girl and her mother about weight other than rny own preliminary work in this area.

Do dieting mothers have dieting daughters? When assessed with a direct question about dieting or a questionnaire designed to measure dietary restraint, positive associations were found in four of eleven studies (see Table 2.2, Hill et al., 1990; Pike &

Rodin, 1991; Ruther & Richman, 1993; Smolak et al., 1999). With one exception, the positive findings were in elementary school-aged children (Pike & Rodin, 1991). When

'%s list excluded studies that surveyed just children, just parents. and one siudy that surveyed both, but did not match pairs in the analysis (Rozin & Fallon, 1988). Table 2.2 Mother-Daughter Snidies of Weight-Related Attitudes and Self-Reported Behaviours

Author. Body Dietary Year Description of sample Measures used image Diet restraint

Hill, et 20 pairs; mean age 10.1 y'; Restraint Scale; EAT; Eating Patterns Yes - R al., 1990 Inner-city, Leeds- Questionnaire. NO - EAT

Ruther & 44 pairs (26 girls; 18boys); Ratraint Scale; understanding of Yes - R Richman grade 4; hi& socio- dieting; desire to lose or gain weight, 1993 economic status.

Thelen & 70 pairs (35 girls; 35 boys); Desire to be rhimer; diet frequency; Cormier. grade 4; aged 9-10-5 y; EAT-26 or ch(EAT); BMI. 1995b rnostly Caucasian. Smolak. 131 pairs (boys; girls); grade Frequency of dieting or weight Ioss Yes Yes et ai., 4-5; White, working- middle attempts; Body Esteem Scale; weight 1999 class; nirai. concems.

Hill & 40 pairs; mean age 1 1.75 y; Dietary Restraint Scale of DEBQ; Franklin, mostly Caucasian; Iower body shapes; body esteem; dieting 1998 middle class; urban. history and self-perception; BMI.

Byely et 77 pairs; grade 6-7, mean Body image scale hmSIQYA; NO - EAT d., 2ooob age = 12.2 y at Time 1; 55 EAT; M & D asscssment of girls' subscale pairs at Tirne 2; White. relative weight; dieting history and urban, middle-to upper- encouragement; BMI. middie class, well educated. Marches- 20 pairs; grade 8; aged 13- EAT-40; frequency of dieting; weight sault 14 years; low income; satisfaction; qualitative inte~ew; 1993 ethnically divene; urban. BMI-

Attie & 193 pairs; grade 7 - 1O & EAT-26; Body image Scale hm No NO - EAT Brooks- mean age = 13.9 y at Time SIQYA; Eating Disorders Inventory; Gunn, 1; 16.1 y at Time 2; White; daughter's height & weight, 1989 middle-class.

Keel. et 51 pairs; mean age 14.8 y Restraint Scde; disordered eating No No NO-R al., 1997 (12-1 8); Caucasian; middle- behaviours; weight perception and class; suburban. satisfaction; dieting history; BMI.

Pike & 77 pairs; grade 9 to 12; Eating Disorders Inventory; weight; Yes - EDI Rodin. mean age =16 y; White; ideal weight; dieting history; ratïngs 1991 rniddle-class; urban & of atûactiveness; BMI. suburban.

Ogden & 30 pairs; mean age 17.1 y Restrained eating section of DEBQ; NO No - R Steward, (16-19); majority White & Body Shape Questionnaire; BMI. 2000 upper class. Ogden & 50 pairs (25 Asian; 25 Restrained eating section of DEBQ; No NO - R Eider, White); medical school; Body Shape Questionnaire; BMI. 1998 mean age 20 y (18-26).

Note. Studies of youngest girls are Iisted first- See Appendix A-1 and A-2 for descriptions of questionnaires and details of these and other studies. j. = years; EAT = Eating Aninides Test; ch(EAT) = children's EAT; R = Restraint; BMI = Body Mass Index; DEBQ = Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire; SIQYA = Self-Image Questionnaire for Adolescents; M = Mother; D = Daughtcr. bBody weight was conWed in these studies. the basis for comparison was concern about weight or a desire to be thinner, five of six studies found no significant association between rnothers' and daughters' responses (Attie

& Brooks-Gunn, 1989; Keel et al., 1997; Ogden & Elder, 1998; Ogden & Steward, 2000;

Thelen & Cormier, 1995). Again, the positive results were found in elementary school- aged girls (Smolak et al., 1999).

In the studies examining adolescents in middle school and beyond, only Pike and

Rodin (1991) found a significant association in mother-daughter dieting tendencies. In this early and influentid study, the investigators used the Eating Disorders Inventory to screen 350 White, rniddle-class, Grade 9 to 12 girls for high and low degrees of disordered eating. They found that the mothers of girls who were having problems with disordered eating wanted their daughters to be considerabl y thinner (daughters* weight was controlled for in this comparison); had started dieting at a younger age themselves; rated their daughters as less attractive than the daughters rated themselves; were more critical of their daughters than they were of themselves; and were more eating disordered themselves. There were no significant differences between the two groups of rnothers in terrns of proportion who were dieting. their cument body weight, the amount they wished to iose, or the largest amount of weight they had ever lost by dieting. Pike and Rodin (p.

203) concluded that "some of the mother's own dieting and eating behaviours, and especially her concerns about her daughter's weight and appearance, pose a significant risk that a daughter will be disordered in her eating." They suggested this might operate through modelling these types of behaviours for their daughters as well as placing pressure on them to be thinner. In a similar study with 12-year-old British girls using the Restra.int Scale, Hill and

Franklin (1998) found no significant differences between the mothers of high- and low-

scoring girls in current BMI, current dieting, age of fust diet, maximum amount of weight

lost by dieting or restra.int scores. Both groups of mothers saw themselves as slightly

overweight and viewed their daughters' current weight as "just nght." Both groups of

mothers thought their daughters were slightly more attractive than other girls of the same

age, although the rnothers of the high-restraint girls rated their daughters' attractiveness

lower. These mothers also showed some minor differences in eating style-they snacked

more, and a slightly higher proportion had fasted. They also reported a greater range of

past weights and more dissatisfaction with some aspects of family functioning. The mean

weight of the high-restraint girls was heavier, although the investigators did not control

for weight other than to note that there were heavy and light girls in both groups.

In one of the few longitudinal studies of girls' eating attitudes to also survey

mothers, Attie and Brooks-Gunn (1989) concluded that mothers' body image and degree

of compulsive eating did not contribute significantly to the prediction of compulsive

eating in their daughters. Nearly 200 White, middle-class girls in Grades 7-10 (initially)

and their mothers filled out the EAT-26. The relationship between the girls' and the

mothers' scores was not significant. Physique was the most important factor in early

adolescence, and body image was the strongest predictor of compulsive eating two years

later. Mothers' (but not the daughters') reports of family functioning were also related to problem eating at the second testing. The EAT-26 is a measure of relatively severe eating disturbances, but three other studies found no association between mothers' and

47 daughters' dieting or dieting tendencies as measured by restraint scales (Keel et al., 1997;

Ogden & Elder, 1998; Ogden & Steward, 2000). Neither were there any significant

associations between mothers' and daughters' body image in these four studies. In one

study, there was a strong association between mothers and their two daughters' eating

concerns (Steiger et al., 1996). This seems noteworthy, especidly considenng that there

was no association between fathers' and daughters' scores. However, scales measunng

eating concems, dietary restra.int and body image were combined, and body weight was

not controlled, making results difficult to interpret. (See Appendix A-2 for details).

While most of the direct comparisons of dieting and body image measures for

older girls are non-significant, three of the four studies of elementary school children

report positive associations between mothers and daughters-two of two for restraint

scores (IIill et al., 1990; Ruther & Richman, 1993), one of two for dieting (Smolak et al.,

1999), and one of two for weight dissatisfaction (Smolak et al., 1999). Thelen aiid

Cormier (1995), the only one of these four studies to control for body weight, reported

non-significant results in their comparisons of mothers' and daughters' self-reported diet

frequency, desire to be thinner (measured by body silhouettes), and EAT scores. This suggests that a girI's body shape, or factors associated with it, is more influentid in shaping her attitudes and dieting behaviours than her mothers' weight-related concerns.

There is also evidence of matemd influences on weight-related behaviours and body weight in children under five years of age (see Appendur A-2). In several studies, mothers who scored high on dietary restraint, restricted their preschoolers' eating (Birch

& Fisher, 2000; Ciitting, Fisher, Grimm-Thomas, & Birch, 1999). This was associated with the children's lessened ability to self-regulate their food intake, demonstrated by greater consumption of palatable snack foods in the absence of hunger. These children were also heavier. In a Bn tish study of 12-year-olds, those with higher restraint scores reported greater parental control of their eating. Although they were heavier, their dieting status was linked more closely to parental control (Edmunds & Hill, 1999). Longitudinal studies are required to determine the direction of the effects in these studies. Did parental intervention intended to avoid eating problems cause hem, or were parents reacting to existing differences in their child?

In sumrnary, the evidence for the mothers' role in modelling weight-related attitudes and behaviours appears to be stronger in elementary school-aged children than in older girls. What mothers and fathers Say to their teen-aged daughters may be more influential than role modelling attitudes and behaviours, although the rack of control for weight is a problem in interpreting positive associations. The findings are inconsistent, and there are more negative findings than positive within most studies, even the larger ones (see Table 2.2 and Appendix A-2). The associations, with a few exceptions, are found for attitudes rather than behaviours, are generally weak, and sometimes disappear in the occasional study that controls for body weight. This very variability suggests the need for a more exploratory approach combining methods in order to understand the issue in greater depth. The Research Process

"National surveys are like satellite photos, giving a broad overview of public opinion. Anthropological research corresponds to exploration on the ground, chaning details of the features glimpsed by the national surveys" (Kempton, Boster, & Hartley, 1995, p. 18).

Research Design and Analysis

Ethnographie methods were developed to snidy foreign cultures, although their use in one's own culture is becoming more common (Kempton et al., 1995). The use of these methods is based on the assumption that participants have learned some cornmon ways of perceiving the world. Garro (1988) suggests that while people make statements based on individual knowledge, much of this knowledge cm be accounted for by cultural sharing. She refers to this shared knowledge as culrural understandings. Discovering people's cultural understandings within a given domain can provide a sense of what people attend to and how they might interpret it. The pattern of agreement and disagreement in understanding may Vary across different social groups and can be measured through the use of specific methods such as cultural consensus analysis (Gamo,

1995; 1996; Hubert & Schultz, 1976; Kempton et al., 1995; Weller, Pachter, Trotter II, Br

Baer, 1993).

This research is a cross-sectional descriptive study, using three formats in face-to- face interviews to develop a more comprehensive understanding of how people perceive weight than would be possible through the use of only one approach. The interview formats were: an open-ended, semi-stnictured interview; a more structured fixed-response

interview; and the administration of three standardized research instruments.

The first two interview formats are ethnographie interview methods used in

cognitive anthropology. They are appropriate methods for attempting to understand the

meaning of weight to the people king interviewed, a major goal of this research. The

strength of the semi-structured in-depth interview is its potential for facilitating insight

and deepening understanding of issues (Bernard, 1994/1988; Kempton et al., 1995;

Silverman, 1993). Adding the fixed-response interview and using cultural consensus

analysis allow statisticaily valid conclusions to be made regarding the extent of cultural sharing within and between the populations sarnpled (Garro, 1996). These fixed-response statements were constructed in a way intended to preserve the focus on the participants' understandings of the topic.

Survey methods are used to address issues as identified by experts in the field

(Kempton et al., 1995). The questionnaires provide standardized data on key issues of interest to health professionals and health researchers. The use of the questionnaires provides easy comparability to other research using the sarne instruments, and thus provides additional context for situating the participants' perspectives.

For each part of the interview, 1 provide an ovewiew of the method and analytic strategy used, and the advantages and limitations of the method as used in this study. 1 then describe the standard procedure for interviews, how the projet was implemented, and the characteristics of the research participants. (See Appendix B for the interview protocol and questions, worded slightly differently for mothers and daughters.) Semi-Stnictured Interviews

Qualitative in-depth interviews were used because a major goal was to understand the meaning of weight to mothers and daughters, and how this meaning varied within the context of people's experience. Semi-stmctwed interviews use an interview guide to encourage participants to share their views on the topic and to elaborate what they feel is relevant. It is standard procedure to use probes and additional questions as needed to ensure responses are interpreted as fully and as correctly as possible.

Each participant was asked open-ended questions to elicit their understandings of weight. The openended interviews focussed on participants' perceptions of 1) the causes and consequences of people's weight problems and concem about weight, 2) appropriate ways to deal with weight issues, and 3) the role of weight issues in their own lives. The analysis focuses on the latter two issues because consensus was not confmed for most samples for the statements addressing causes and consequences. Most of the questions were drawn from the previously mentioned research (Marchessault, 1993a; Cath, 1988;

Havens, 197 1 - 1996; Nichter & Vuckovic, 1994).

The analysis of the interviews consisted of reading and reflecting on the interview transcripts to generate insight into the meaning of the issue to the participants. Chapman and Maclean (1990, p. 133) refer to this as essentially an "intuitive and creative process."

This process is stimulated by organizing the data through systematic coding to generate categories, themes and patterns. Connections are clarified through writing notes about this coding, sometimes referred to as memos, returning to the data to check the accuracy of the observations, to test emerging hypotheses, and to search for alternative explanations, and in this way, writing the report (Marshall & Rossman, 1989). In addition to post- interview review and reffection for each interview, relevant portions of transcripts were reviewed three times: fmt, to detennine themes and code broadly, then to code more finely, and third to check that coding was correct and complete. This was accomplished using a program from Qualitative Solutions and Research (1997) titled Non-numerical

Unstmctured Data Indexing Searching and Theorizing (NUD*IST). The unstnictured interview data was explored in conjunction with the stnictured demographic and other quantitative data, which was imported into NUD*IST. Case studies and data sumaries are used to illustrate important findings regarding shared and divergent understandings.

Credibility and transferablility have been well-received as quality criteria for qualitative research (Chapman & Maclean, 1990; Guba & Lincoln, 1994; Hall & Stevens,

1991; Krefting, 1990; Marshall & Rossman, 1989). The goal of credibility is to ensure the accurate identification and description of the subject of the inquiry (Marshall & Rossman,

1989). If achieved, credibility is generally regarded as the major advantage of this interview method (Krefting, 1990; Marshall & Rossman, 1989). Transferability refers to the ability to generalize findings from the sarnple to the population from which it was drawn and to other contexts. The burden of judgment regarding generalization of findings to other contexts falls to the person wishing to do the generalizing. The researcher's responsibility is to present sufficient information about the research so that there is a basis for these judgrnents to be made. The in-depth interview allows exploration of what is important to participants. It is sufficiently flexible that unanticipated aspects of the topic can be accomrnodated. Fixed-Res~onseinterviews

After the open-ended interview questions, each participant was asked to agree or disagree to a series of statements intended to reflect understandings from the participants' perspective. Asking participants to respond to fixed-response statements gives them the opportunity to discuss items they know about but may not have raised in the open-ended interviews (Garro, 1996). The consensus statements followed the open-ended questions, to avoid influencing responses, and were read to each participant.

The fixed form questionnaire was constructed from cornrnents participants made during semi-stmctured interviews on the same topic from my master's research

(Marchessault, 1993a). As discussed, this sample consisted of 40 participants, similar in age to the cument sarnple. Almost half of the farnilies were Aboriginal, and al1 lived in the inner city of Winnipeg. Transcripts of mothers and daughters were reviewed for key ideas, paying particular attention to frequently or strongly stressed themes and contradictions wi thin or between transcri pts, Quotes were paraphrased into questions, preserving as much of the original wording, context and bias as possible. Questions were formulated to reflect the diveaity of opinions in participant perspectives. For example, rnany participants in this study said that weight would help them to achieve other goals that they found desirable, such as attracting friends, boys and getting a job. The following quotes are examples of these opinions:

Girl: Z think so many girls have becorne obsessed with looking good mostly because of boys, around my age. Because like those in my school won t go out with anybody who S noticeably fat. Girl: Because girls, they won 't like you [if you 're fat]. I mean people won 'r like you. Woman: Women have ?O be nice-Zooking tu ger jobs. Woman: Whereas women always get their image that if rhey Ze nice and slim, you know, they 're going ro be rewarded by being loved. Or by being acceped.

The following statement was built from these quotes: "A woman should womy

about her weight because other people will be more Iikely to like her, she wiil have an

easier time attracting a boyhiend, and it will help her to get a job. Basically, dl other

things being equal, she will do better in life if she is slim." As noted by Kempton, Boster

and Hartley (1995), this type of statement does not reflect standard survey procedure. It

may test several things at once and does not aim for neutrality. The intent is to see if

people subscribe to the whole argument and to deterrnine the range of agreement (The

statements are itemized in the Interview Guide, Appendix B, and are listed in Chapter 6.)

The analysis was modelled on Garrots work (1988; 1996). and that of Kempton,

Boster and Hartley (1995). Both the open-ended interviews and the consensus analysis demonstrate the extent to which cultural understandings were shared within the samples of mothers and daughters at each location and between the locations. Consensus analysis indicates if it is appropnate to aggregate and compare responses. The analysis highlights consensus on particular statements, and participants' comments elaborate meaning and provide broader context for understanding the statements. Cultural consensus analysis gives reliable results witb very few respondents if consensus exists (Romney, Weller, &

Batchelder, 1986).

Cultural consensus analysis (Romney et al., 1986) and the quadratic assignment program (Hubert & Schultz, 1976) were used to analyse the responses to the statements on values. ANTHROPAC 4.0 (Borgatti, 1996) was used to cany out the analysis. The consensus analysis was perfomed in two main steps. In the first step, agreement and disagreement on the content of the consensus responses was analysed. The focus in the second stage was on the patterns in participants' responses to the consensus questions.

Consensus analysis is relatively new, and so the theory and method is elaborated in

Chapter 6.

Ouestionnaires

In the 1stpart of the interview, participants were asked to respond to three standardized questionnaires. This was intended to understand the participants' concern about weight relative to the existing research on girls' and women's attitudes to weight, to test specific hypotheses and to screen for senous problems. Although each scaie has been used with an Aboriginal sample, combining their use with the open-ended portion of this study provides an opportunity to consider their appropnateness with this population.

Each questionnaire assessed a different aspect of weight. The Restraint scale measures tendency to diet (Herman & Polivy, 1980). The Eating Attitudes Test (EAT) measures nsk for an eating disorder (Garner, Olmsted, Bohr, & Garfinkel, 1982). And, the Body

Shape Drawings Questionnaire (BSDQ) was based on Stunkard, Sorensen, and

Schulsinger 's (1980) silhouettes, which have ken used to assess people's perceptions, preferences and satisfaction with their body shape. The questionnaires were administered at the end of the interview, again to protect the integrity of the open-ended portion of the interview by avoiding suggesting responses to the participants. (See the interview guide in Appendix B.) A brief description of each scale, and its use in this study foliows. Restraint Scale. The Restraint Scale was originally developed by Herman and

Mack (1975) to identify the relative effect of chronic dieting compared to body weight on

eating behaviours. Herman and Polivy (1975) made significant changes to the scale to

acknowledge the cornmon pattern of alternating restraint and lapses of restraint in the

average dieter. The 10-item Restraint Scale (Hennan & Polivy, 1980) was selected for

this project because it was designed to measure the intent to restrict food intake for the

purpose of weight control or loss, regardless of success. Other popular scales, (such as the

Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire, Stunkard & Messick, 1985, or the Dutch Eating

Behavior Questionnaire, Van Strien, Fnjters, Bergers, & Defares, 1985) measure

restrained eating, disinhibition and hunger as distinct eating styles.

The Restraint Scale has adequate interna1 consistency for normal weight samples,

with reported alphas of -75 and .86 (Herman & Polivy, 1975; Ruderman, 1986). Validity

is based on its relationship to theoretical models of restraint and disinhibition, and its predictive power (Heatherton, Herman, Polivy, King, & McGree, 1988; Ruderman,

1986). Each question on the Restraint Scale is scored from O to 3 or 4 for a maximum of

35 points. Means of 11 and 12 have ken reported for 12 to 18-year-old girls and their mothers, respectively (Keel et al., 1997) Scores above 16 have been used to distinguish groups of dieters from nondieters (Ciliska, 1990; Herman & Polivy, 1980).

Ruderman (1986) has noted low intemal consistency (alpha of .SI) and systematic overestimation of restraint when the scale is administered to heavier people. Four items on the Restraint Scale assess weight fluctuation (Heatherton et al., 1988). Heavier people may score higher on these questions, regardless of intent to diet. Two factors (weight fluctuation and concern with dieting) were identified when the scale was introduced and

have repeatedly emerged in subsequent factor analyses (Heatherton et al., 1988; Herman

& Polivy, 1975). Heatherton and colleagues point out that each factor has successfully

predicted behaviour. They recommend using the entire scale because it is not known

which factor is more important.

The full Restraint Scale was administered in this study, and the analysis was

perfomed and is given for both the Restraint Scale and a modified version. (Without the

four questions, the maximum score is 19.) Because several of the groups in this research

(women and girls, Abonginal and non-Abonginal participants) had different mean

weights, and different slopes precluded controlling for body weight, the results focus on

the Modified Restra.int Scale. This name was chosen, rather than the subscale moniker, to

emphasize its association with the original scale.

Eating Attitudes Test. The EAT was developed by Garner and Garfinkel(1979). A

short version, the EAT-26, was used in this project to identiQ participants at nsk for an eating disorder for referral as well as for analytical puiposes. Garner, Olmsted, Bohr and

Garfinkel (1982) have demonstrated that the EAT-26 was a reliable and valid, yet economic, instrument for assessing eating disorder syrnptoms. The EAT-26 had a reliability coefficient of .Wand .83 for anorexic patients and female controls, respectively, and its use correctly predicted 83.696 of cases. It has 26 items compared to

40 items in the original EAT (Garner & Garfinkel, 1979) and 64 items in the Eating

Disorder Inventory (designed by Garner, Olmsted, & Polivy, 1983, to measure personality characteristics of eating disordered patients). The EAT-26 correlated highly with the original EAT (r = .98; Garner et al., 1982). Both scales correlated sirnilarly with other clinical and psychomeaic variables (Berland, Thompson & Linton, 1986; Garner et al.,

1982).

The EAT-26 questions measure preoccupation with thinness and food, pathological avoidance of "fattening" foods, bulimic behaviours such as binging and

vomiting, self control around food and perceived social pressure to gain weight. The scale is a forced-choice six-point Likert scale. The most extreme response to each question is

scored three points; adjacent alternatives are scored two points and one point, respectively; and rernaining responses are scored zero, for a maximum of 78 points

(Garner & Garfinkel, 1979; Garner et al., 1982). Scores above 20 suggest nsk for an

eating disorder and further assessment is advised. This cutoff elirninated false negatives

but gave a false positive for risk of an eating disorder approximately 13% of the time

(Garner & G&nkel, 1979).

The EAT has ken widely used to describe food preoccupation and body image

concenis in nonclinical sarnples (Aronson, Fredrnan, & Gabriel, 1990; Smead & Richert,

1990; Wood, Waller, Miller, & Slade, 1992). Noms have been established for the EAT-

26 (Rosen, Silberg, & Gross, 1988b) with a reported mean of 11.9 (SD of 10.8) for Grade

9 to 12 students. They reported no significant variation by socio-economic status or race

(primarily White and Black). Others have reported no differences by age for teen-aged

girls (Gralen, Levine, Smolak, & Murnen, 1990; Leichner et al., 1986; Rosen et al.,

l988b). Close to onequarter of teens in Manitoba (23.7% of those aged 13 to 15) scored

above the cutoff for risk of an eating disorder (Leichner et al., 1986), slightly more than double the rates reported for similar agea girls in the United Kingdom (Wood et al.,

1992), and undergraduate students in the United States (Smead & Richert, 1990). The

EAT has been used cross-culturally with Afiican-Arnerican, Hispanie, Asian and

Aboriginal youth (Crago et al., 1996; Marchessault, 1998; McCourt & Waller, 1995;

Rosen et al, 1988b). Although some of the reported studies were small, high EAT scores have been noted in each sample.

Bodv Sha~eDrawines Ouestionnaire. Stunkard, Sorenson and Schulsinger (1980) created a series of nine drawings of male and fernale figures as an ingenious strategy for their respondents to estimate the weight of their aging or deceased parents in the Danish

Adoption study of obesity and thinness. Validity was tested in a 20-year health study in

Tecumseh, Michigan, where the weights of the aging parents were known, and was reported as "surprisingly accurate." Correlations between ratings of Mexican-Amencan adults ' body mass and silhouettes ranged from .65 to .84 (Mueller, Joos, & Schull, 1985).

Cohn and colleagues (1987) reported a significant correlation of .71 with body weight and

.75 with BMI for Grades 6 to 8 girls of rnixed ethnicity.

The silhouettes were used in this study, anticipating that some participants would not know their weight. In addition to providing a visual demonstration of body shape assessment, the Body Shape Drawings (BSDs) or similar drawings have been used to indicate body shape preference and dissatisfaction (Cohn et al., 1987; Fallon & Rozin,

1985; Fumham & Alibhai, 1983; Gralen et al., 1990; Rozin & Fallon, 1988; Silberstein,

Striegel-Moore, Tirnko, & Rodin, 1988; Wright, 1994). Questions were adapted from these sources to mate the Body Shape Drawings Questionnaire (BSDQ).The figures were presented from thin to heavy as an ordinal scale. Participants were asked to select the drawing that looked most like their body shape berceived) and the drawing that they would most like to look like (desired). They were then asked to select the shape they thought was most aitractive for women,most atîractive for giris, healthiest for women, and healthiestfor girls. A seventh question concerned the shape they thought guys would find mos t attractive (guys prefer). Body dissatisfaction was measured by the discrepancy be t ween desired and perceived shapes.

A large number of researchers have used the silhouettes, testimony to their face validity. Cohn and colleagues, in the study mentioned above, found that 94% of boys and girls who selected a current shape larger than their ideal shape also responded that they wished to be thinner. However, the body shapes are not valid as an estimate of the proportion of children desiring to be larger. Only 54% of the children selecting larger shapes as their ideal said they wanted to be heavier.

The figures have ken used with children as young as eight (Hill & Palin, 1998), with Aboriginal people (Gittelsohn et al., 1996), and with White, Black, Asian and

Hispanic people (Cohn et ai., 1987). Similar figures have been used with Kenyans living in Asia and Britain (Furnham & Alibhai, 1983) and adaptations have ken used with fourth grade Aboriginal children (Stevens et al., 1999). A varying proportion of fernales reported a desire to be smailer in these studies, ranging from 26% of Grade 4 Aboriginal girls to 75% of Yale undergraduates (Silberstein et ai., 1988; Stevens et al., 1999).

Gittelsohn et al. (19%) pretested Stunkard, Sorensen, and Schulsinger's (1980) figures with Ojibway-Cree people in northem Ontario, reporting that no one found them inappropriate. They suggested caution in interpreting their findings for the 10-to-19-year- olds. The original figures were used in this study because the children's shapes were inappropnate for use with teens, and the adult figures have worked well in a variety of settings with different ethnic and age groups. This permitted comparability with previous research and for the different age and ethnic gmups within the study.

Analvsis of Ouestionnaires. Al1 three questionnaires were used to test for differences in responses between generations, Abonginai and non-Aboriginal participants, urban and rural participants, and between the participants at the four locations. They were also used to test for association between mothers' and daughters' responses. As in the study by Hill, Weaver and Blundell(1990), the girls with the 10 highest and 10 lowest Restraint scores were identified, and tests were run to see if the mothers of the high-scoring girls scored higher on the Restraint Scale and the EAT-26 than mothers of the low-scoring girls. Association between the girls' and their mothers' scores in these 20 families was dso tested.

Data were analysed using the Number Cnincher Statistical Systems for Windows

(abbreviated NCSS, Hintze, 1997). Descriptive statistics were used to characterize the sample and to allow cornparisons with the psychological literature. Analysis of Variance

(ANOVA), r-tests, and their non-parametic equivaients were used to test for differences between generations, ethnicity, regions and locations. Paired tests, such as the split unit

ANOVA and the Witcoxon Signed Rank Sum test, were used to test the significance of the mother-daughter relationship, and correlation coefficients were calculated for test scores of mothers and daughters. Logistic regression and chi squares were used to test follow-up questions. Sample size was driven by the qualitative portion of the study. There was reasonable power for the comparisons of larger groups, but non-significant resuIts in comparisons between locations may not be meaningful. Further details of analysis, including an assessrnent of power, are given in Chapter 9.

Backmound Information

Standard background information was requested of mothers. Information was collected on occupation, income, education, religion, ethnicity and the personal importance of ethnicity, al1 of which have been identified as potentially relevant for predicting health concepts, attitudes and concems (Harwood, 198 1; Schneider, 1986).

These data have ken used to descnbe the samples, and in the case studies as markers of identity. NCSS was used to compare the samples on the basis of mothers' age, daughters' age, number of children, women's marital status, level of education, employment status, and the number of wage earners in the household. Income is reported and discussed.

Normally distributed interval data were tested with multi-way ANOVA with region and ethnicity entered as factors. A Kruskal-Wallis test was used to test ordinal data for differences between the locations.

Wei~htstatus. Height and weight were not measured for fear of conveying inappropriate messages about the purpose of the study. Weighing girls might have seemed to contradict the explanation that sample selection was independent of their weight. Participants were asked to provide their current height and weight on the BSDQ.

BMI was calculated by dividing weight in kilograms by height in metres squared (Health and Welfare Canada, 1988a). The BMI was proposed as a method to assess health risks

associated with adiposity for adults between the ages of 20 and 65. It is used hem, dong

with the self-assessed visual representation on the BSDQ, for descriptive and comparative

purposes and to provide context for people's weight concerns.

The Canadian guidelines cite a BMI between 20 and 25 as a healthy weight for

most people; between 25 and 27 as a cautionary zone; over 27 as associated with

increasing risk of developing health problems due to overweight; and a BMI of less than

20 as associated with health problems due to underweight."

There are no generally accepted weight standards for children.12 Since the

purpose of using the BMI is to pennit comparisons, the same cutoffs were used as for the

mothers. BMI noms of 18.21 and 19.3 have been reported for 13- and 14-year-olds,

respectively (Beumont, Al-Alami, & Touyz, 1988). Rolland-Cachera et al. (1982)

reported that the BMI was a valid indicator of adiposity among children, despite some

imprecision due to the different stages of growth at any given age. A low or high BMI

does not automatically signal risk of health problems. For adults, and especially children,

long-term weight patterns are needed to assess health risk against constitution (Lifshitz &

''The World Health Organization (1998, p. 9) and the National Institutes of Health (1998) classify BMIs below 18.5 as "underweight," between 18.5 and 24.9 as within the "normal range," and over 25 as "overweight." BMIs of 25 to 29.9 are noted as "pre-obese."

12~he85'" and 95' percentiles of the United States National Center for Health Statistics growth charts are comrnonly used. New standards have recently been published (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2000). Age- and sex-specific cutoffs that pass through the adult BMIs of 25 and 30 have recently been detennined (Cole, Bellini, Flegal, & Dietz, 2000). The values for 13 year-old girls are 22.6 and and 27.8, respectively; for 14 year-old girls, they are 23.3 and 28.6. Moses, 1988).

On average, women, including people with less education, adolescents, and very heavy people, have been found to underestimate their weight by 3.1 pounds (Stewart,

1982; Stunkard & Albaum, 198 1). The BMIs are based on self-reported data and should be considered approximations.

Trianmlatin~Methods

Research questions should determine the methods used in any research project

(Denzin & Lincoln, 1994; Mechanic, 1989). The major purpose of this investigation was to explore and compare the participants' perceptions of weight issues. Participants' concems were also documented in a way that would be comparable to other research reports. Therefore, this study used semi-structured in-depth interviews, fixed structure interviews and standardized questionnaires to collect data. As already mentioned, Garro

(1988, 1996) and Kempton, Boster and Hartley (1995) have combined in-depth interviews and fixed-response questionnaires. Morse (1994) discusses the appropriate use of standardized data within a qualitative-dnven project. Using different methods, or triangulating data gathenng techniques, cm balance the strengths and weaknesses of indi viduai methods (Denzin & Lincoln, 1994; Mechanic, 1989).

The in-depth interview allows the researcher to take context and rneaning into account. It facilitates an understanding of which issues are most salient to participants. It is flexible, with potential for unanticipated aspects of the research issue to emerge and be incorporated into the analysis. This becornes more important as participant and researcher perspectives Vary. It is dificult to make direct comparisons, a difficulty that is magnified when research is camied out by different researchers in different locations with different samples.

Generalizeability is one of the strengths of questionnaires, although the data gathered are determined a priori and therefore are more limited. The fixed-form interview complements the in-depth interview by taking statements made by some participants and asking other participants if they agree or disagree. This method preserves the focus on participants' perspectives while permitting direct cornparisons within and between various groups (if the conditions of consensus are met). However, the contextual understanding provided by in-depth interviews is essential to ensure the statements are interpreted appropriately. The two methods together address issues of generalizeability, replicability and the importance of context (Garro, 1996).

The three standardized scales were used to document participants' concems about weight with reliable and valid instruments with established noms. These tools were used to assess the level of dietary restraint, body dissatisfaction and risk for an eating disorder in thi s sarnple compared to other samples. Addi tionall y, questionnaires sometimes provide an opportunity for participants to disclose sensitive personal data that they may be uncornfortable raising in an interview, and not insignificantly, facilitated accessing appropriate care.

Although there was a specific rationale behind the use of each method, using the thee methods together was intended to increase confidence in the validity of the findings.

If results are congruent, this suggests validity. If they are not, then further probing is required to understand the disjuncture. Looking at what people Say from several perspectives is not working at cross puyoses but rather working towards a deeper understanding of the issue in a way that cm be understood and used by a variety of people--from mothers to health professionals to researchers.

Ethical Considerations

This study was approved by the Faculty Cornmittee on the Use of Human Subjects at the University of Manitoba, the Winnipeg Schwl Division's Research Advisory

Cornmittee, the school boards in the First Nations community and the Rural community, and the Chief and Council's delegate in the First Nations community. Additionally, permission was obtained from the principal of each school, and infomed consent was obtained from al1 participants prior to their own interview. Girls were approached only after written parental consent was received, requested from the mother at the end of her interview. (See Appendix C for copies of ail but the community-level approvals. See

Appendices D and E for parental and participant consent forms and the written explanation of the study given to each participant.)

There was a risk that answenng weight-related questions might bring up personal problems for participants. Each participant was told they did not have to answer any questions they did not wish to, and that they could stop the interview at any point. Girls were told that if their interview indicated a potentially serious nsk to their health, as assessed by their score on the EAT, this would be discussed with them, and then with their mother or a counsellor. Other than this, and the release of transcripts to participants who requested a copy of their own interview, no one was given access to the individual

data. No narnes have been or will be used in this or any report. Potentially identifying

features of individual participants have been altered to protect confidentiality

A summary of preliminary findings was sent to each participating family, the

school, the school divisions and the Chief and CounciI in the First Nations comrnunity.

(See Appendix F). Additionally, a copy of the final report will be sent to the schools, the

school divisions and the FitNations community-

The Research Sites

1 was interested in interviewing Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal middle-aged

women and their teen-aged daughters living in an urban setting or nearby rural

cornmunities. The urban centre was Winnipeg, which has a population somewhat over

600,000 (Statistics Canada, 1994). Winnipeg is the major city and capital of Manitoba, a

prairie province in the geographic centre of Canada.

The samples were obtained by randomiy selecting female Grade 8 students from

four schools, and extrapolating the generated list to include the mothers. Grade 8 was

selected because it was assumed that body awareness would be heightened around

puberty. Randornization was used primarily to assure girls that their weight was no? the basis for their inclusion in the sample.

The four research sites were selected based on ethnicity and income data collected

for the 1991 Census (Statistics Canada, 1994; 199%; Winnipeg School Division No. 1,

1996). Only schools with more than 25 female Grade 8 students on their roster were considered.

The median income for families with children in the catchment area of the

Aboriginal heritage school was $16,000 with an 80.4% incidence of low-income families

(Winnipeg School Division No. 1, 1996).13 The median income for families with children in the catchment area for the Suburban Winnipeg school was $78,700 with a 2.8% incidence of low-income families. The most common ethnic origins for this neighbourhood were British, Jewish and German, with Aboriginal presence described as

"negligible" (Winnipeg School Division No. 1, 1996).

A rural Manitoba First Nations community within 200 kilometres of Winnipeg,

and where English is commonly spoken, was selected. This community of approximately

3,000 people is considered Ojibway. The rural non-Abonginal community was selected to

match the data already collected for the First Nations community as closely as possible,

using the then-current Statistics Canada report (1994) to estimate population size and

household income data for rural c~mmunities.~~Comrnunities with a singular ethnicity

and those affected by Manitoba's 1997 flood were not considered. The selected town's

population was approximately 2,700 and was about 60 kilometres from Winnipeg.

Although between 118 and 1/16th of the adult residents work in Winnipeg, the town is

considered rural (Local Business Development Corporation, personal communication,

November 25, 1997). The median income for families in this community in the 1991

')This is higher than the median income of $10,837 reported for Manitoba Aboriginal families with incomes living off-reserves (S tatistics Canada, 1995b).

'The median income for the First Nations community was $17,500. The median income reported for those living on-=serves was $5,916 (Statistics Canada, 1995b). Census was $36,244, with a 12.5% incidence of low-income families. Residents were

pnmaril y Ukrainian, French and Scandinavian. Students are drawn from several small

towns in the surrounding area.

To summarize, the research sites and the sampies they yielded are as follows: Non-Aboriginal mothers and daughters 1. Suburban Winnipeg school 2. Rural Manitoba school

Aboriginal mothers and daughters 3. Urban Aboriginal school in Winnipeg (supplemented with snowball sample) 4. Rural Manitoba First Nations community school.

The approval process and the timing involved in the various requests are detailed

in Table 3.1. Following receipt of pertinent consents, the data were collected from

January, 1997 to December, 1998, with al1 interviews done by the investigator. The

interviews were staggered as follows:

From January 7, 1997 until October 21, 1997 in the First Nations community. From Febniary 17, 1997 until May 29, 1997 at the Suburban location. From January 19,1998 until December 9, 1998 with Urban Abonginal families. From February 9, 1998 until May 3, 1998 in the Rural community.

Because the interview was similar to one used in a previous study, pre-testing

focussed on the added components. The consensus, restraint and body shape questionnaires were pre-tested with 10 individuals, and the entire inte~ewwas piloted

with one mother-daughter pair in the First Nations community. No substantive changes

were made to the interview, and the two pilot interviews were included in the study. Table 3.1 Time-line for the Approval mess

Agency/School Requested Received Faculty Cornmittee on the Use of Human Subjects, June 7/96 June 24/96 University of Manitoba Chief and Council, First Nations community Sep 6/96 Jan 27/97 School boards: Research Advisory Cornmittee Sep 26/96 Nov 19/96 Winnipeg School Division No. 1 1" revision May30197 June 10197 zndrevision Oct 6/97 Oct 8/97 First Nations community Sep 6/96 Sep 19/96 Rural community Nov 27/97 Dec 10197 Principals and introductory letter to parents requesting release of address: Suburban school: Initiai approach Nov 18/96 Meeting with teacher Jan 21/97 Letter to parents Jan 22/97 First Nations community school: Initial approach Sep 6/96 LRtter to parents Apr 10 & 29/97 Inner city school School#l: Initial approach Sep 3 & Nov 19/96 Meeting with Grade 8 students Dec 3/96 Letter to parents - Insufficient response Dec 3/96 School#S: Initial approaches June 26 & Sep 8/97 Letter to parents - Insufficient response Sep 18/97 School#3: Initial approach Oct 16/97 Letter to parents Nov 4/97 Rural school: Initial approach Sep 27/97 Letter to parents Jan lu98 Mothers - consent for themselves, and after their own interviews, for their daughters. Daughters -infonned consent for themselves.

71 In each school, the research project started with the principal arranging for information describing the project to be mailed home to al1 of the parents of Grade 8 girls. (See Appendix G for letter.) This mailing included information about the project and a request for mothers to cal1 the principal if they did not want their name provided to the investigator. It was stressed that this did not constitute agreement to participate, but was only for contact purposes. Families who declined at this point were considered as refusais. A random sample of 25 girls was generated at the Suburban and Rural schools.

Five additional families (selected at the time of randornization) were added to the

Suburban sample. A population sample was used in the Aboriginal schools because class size was small. The Urban Aboriginal sample was supplemented with a snowball sample, as stated earlier, by requesting participants, employees of Winnipeg Aboriginal organizations and personal contacts to recommend Aboriginal mothers Living with a daughter between the ages of 12 and 15 who might be willing to be interviewed.

Next, mothers were contacted and, if willing, interviewed. They were asked for signed consent for their own participation prior to their interview. Permission to approach their daughters was requested at the end of their interview. Signed parental consent was obtained prior to contacting students. No mother objected to her daughter king approached for an interview, although two girls chose not to participate. Informed consent was also obtained from students prior to their interview. The written explanation of the study and the consent forms were explained to each participant and a copy was left with them. (See Appendices D and E.) The entire interview was tape-recorded, inciuding comments made while participants were completing the questionnaires. There were six exceptions. Three girls and one woman asked not to be taped. Their responses to the

interview questions were wntten down as fully as possible at the time and additional

notes were added imrnediately after the interview. Equipment malfunction led to another

four interviews not king taped, initially. In two cases, the open-ended portion of the

interview was subsequently taped in a repeat interview. Compared to notes from the

earlier interview, both girls gave similar responses in the second, even with a five-month

delay between interviews in one case. A typist transcribed the interview tapes into

computer files. Consensus responses and test scores were also entered.

The interviews with mothers consisted of a request for background information,

the oral interview and the administration of the questionnaires. The interview procedure

for the daughten was similar, except background information was not requested.

The majority of the participants were interviewed in their homes (73.9%).

Interviews also were conducted at school(13%, including 1 woman). This included most

of the First Nations community girls (66.7%). Less frequently, interviews occurred at the woman's place of work (8.1%, including two girls), a restaurant (2.5%, 2 women, 2 girls) and at my or my host's residence (2.5%, 3 women, 1 girl). The girls' interviews took from

45 to 1 10 minutes, averaging 63 minutes. The women's interviews took from 50 to 240 minutes, with the non-Aboriginal women's interviews about 30 minutes longer than the

Aboriginal women's (1 14 and 84 minutes, respectively). Although these interviews were long, participants did not seem to fatigue. A more serious problem was shyness in some of the girls.

It took approximately 1,200 efforts to contact families to conduct these interviews. About one-quarter (3 14) of these were home visits. Approximately equal numbers of phone calls were made to each of the four samples. With the exception of the snowbd sample, home visits to Aboriginal mothers were disproportionately higher. 1 made four times as many home visits in the Aboriginal samples compared to the non-Abriginal samples (190 vs. 50, respectively). Often these contacts were unannounced home visits because there was no phone in the home or the phone number was unlisted. For the randomized Urban Aboriginal sample, 1had only the girls' names, which sometimes varied from the mothers' narnes. Dropping by was the method used by the schools, and seemed prefemed by most participants. Appointments were arranged if suggested by the participant and this worked well when used.

Contact with the girls was achieved with about a third of the calls on the mothers except in the snowbail sample. There, my cails were to intermediaries, who arranged the interviews with the mothers. This was a much easier proçess, although it then took more effort to contact the daughters. This sample took the longest to achieve. As anticipated, it was very difficult to find Aboriginal mothers with young teen-aged daughters who were both amenable to pdcipating in the interview.

Working through the school encouraged trust and was iikely the reason some families participated. Starting early in the school year would have ken preferable, but was not feasible given the circumstances. Although interviewing people at home means less control over noise levels, distractions and interruptions, it is more cornfortable for some women, and this is critical in a study that relies on people talking. Sam~leSelection

The goal was to have a sample size of 20 mothers and 20 daughters at each of the four locations. A sample size of 20 in each subgroup is suffkient to demonstrate diversity and consensus on common themes (Romney et al., 1986), and was considered feasible

Iogistically in ternis of fieldwork.

Details regarding sample selection are sumrnarized in Table 3.2. The sampling frame consisted of al1 female students emolled in Grade 8 at the selected schools in the acadernic yeûr sarnpled. Exclusions were made due to a recent death in the family, severe illness in the mother, developmental limitations, king a 2ndtwin, or if the mother and

Table 3.2 Sarnple Selection Process and Response Rates by Location

Urban First Nations Stage of sarnple process Suburban Rural Aboriginal community N Sampling framea 46 52 28 35 161 Narne not releasedb 5 5 - - 10

Random sample 28 25 11 25 89 Refusals -by mother 7 4 1 6 18 -by daughter 1 1 O O 2 Families in sample 20 20 10 19 69 Response rate - pairs, % 60.5 70.4 90.9 76.0 71.3

"The sampling frame consists of the girls on the Grade 8 class list at each school. bPre-randomization refusal rates were 10.9%,9.6% and 6.2% for the Suburban, Rural and Total sarnples, respectively. They were calculated using the sampling frame as a denorninator, and these refusals have been considered in the response rates. 'One girl was excluded afier being inte~ewed. daughter were not living together. Three step-mothers and one gandrnother were included in the sample. When the student no longer attended the sarnpled school, the family was excluded only if they had moved without leaving a forwarding address. Some exclusions were made prior to randornizing the sampling frame, but most conditions became hown only after attempting to contact the family. The eventual sample derïved from the Urban Abonginal school consisted of only Il families. This sample was supplemented with 11 additional families obtained by the snowball method of sampling.

Overall, 71 -3% of mother-daughter pairs from the random sample participated.

The final sample consisted of 163 interviews: 80 mother-daughter pairs and three mothers. Response rates were 90.946 and 76.0% for the Urban Aboriginal and First

Nations community samples; 60.5% and 70.4% for the Suburban and Rural samples.

Three women were included in the sample without their daughters (from the Suburban,

Rural and Urban Aboriginal locations). In two cases, the girls declined the interview. The third girl was excluded because she seemed to have difficulty understanding the majority of the questions.

The Research Partici~ants

Demogravhic- - Characteristics

Selected demographic characteristics of the participants at the four locations are summarized in Table 3.3. The mean age for the girls was 13.5 years. There were no

statistically significant differences in the girls' age. The Abonginal women were younger

(ANOVA; F = 9.39, df = 1.79, p = ,003) than the non-Aboriginal women. They had a Table 3.3 Characteristics of the Research Participants by Location

Urban First Nations Characteristic Suburban Rural Aboriginal communi ty Mothers, n 21 21 22 19 Daughters' mean age 13.3 13.4 13-5 13-9 Range 13-14 12-14 11-15 13-15 Mothers' mean age 42.1, 41 -3, 36.7, 39.4,

Living with partner, % 7 1.4, 85.7, 27.3, 84.2, Mean no. children 2.9 2.3, 3-4, 3.8, Range 2-5 1-4 1-7 1-7 Mothers' educationb < High school, % 14.3 14.3 31.8 57.9 Finished high school, 5% 14.3 28.6 9.1 5.3 Some post-secondary, % 7 1.4 57.1 59.1 36.8

Note. Cells with different subscript letters were found to be different at p 5 .OS. "One grandmother is not included in the age range to protect confidentiality. "Educational level of Suburban and Rural women differed from that of the First Nations comrnunity women. This data was tested using Sratistics Canada's categories (1993), but categories are collapsed here. Post-secondary programs include certificate, diploma and university degree programs and do not necessarily require high school graduation. greater number of children than the Rural women (Kniskal Wallis H = 11.03, df = 3, p =

.01). There were also significant differences between marital status at the four locations

(2= 21.5, df = 3, p = .00008).15As can be seen from Table 3.3, the Urban Abonginal

15Livingalone or with a partner was tested with a chi square. In testing individual pairs of locations to locate the differences, only the cornparisons between the Urban Aboriginal women and the other women were significant (with the Suburban women, ]i = 8.38, df =1, p = .004, Fisher's Exact Test p = ,006; with the Rural women, 2 = 14.9, del,p women were less likely to be living with a partner. The First Nations community women had fewer years of formal schooling than the non-Aboriginal women (Kruskal

Wallis H = 10.5, df = 3, p = .OS), although there were university-educated women at each location.

Most participants gave multiple responses when asked to identify their ethnicity or ancestry. The non-Aboriginal participants were al1 of European ancestry, Sixteen women said they were Canadian (7) or French Canadian (9). The other 26 women gave multiple origins sternming from the European continent. Most frequently mentioned were British

(43), followed by Slavic (18), central European (15), and Scandinavian (5) countries,

Russia (S), and Greece (1). Two said they were Mennonite and several said they were multi-cultural. The girls identified themselves sirnilarly, although several were born in the

United States. Four women had been born outside of Canada but al1 had Lived in Canada for over 20 years.

Aboriginal participants said they were Cree (14), Ojibway/Saulteaux (9), "Native"

(IO), "Aboriginal" (8) or "Metis" (8). Girls also used "Indian" (9) or "treaty Indian" (1) or

"status" (1). One woman said she was "Anishinaabe." No one said they were First

Nations. Despite king known as an Ojibway community, only one of 19 women identified solely as Ojibway. Two women said they were Ojibway and Cree and nine said they were Cree. They gave similar responses for their husbands' ancestry. Aboriginal participants identified European backgrounds as well, including British (8), French (7),

= .ûûûl, Fisher's Exact Test p = .OOM; and with the First Nation community women, 2 = 13.3, df = 1, p = .ûûû3, Fisher's Exact Test p = .0004). Germany (3) and Russia (1). Girls answered similady, adding Slavic countries, Jewish and White,

Three mothers in the Rural community identified themselves, but not their daughters, as partially Native. The girls did not self-identify as Aboriginal either. One woman in the First Nations community was not Aboriginal. She had lived on the reserve al1 her adult life and she described her daughter as Native. There was also one girl in the

Suburban sample who identified herself as Native, whose mother was not. These five families were excluded when running analyses for comparisons of Aboriginal and non-

Aboriginal farnilies.16 There were three other girls in the non-Aboriginal samples who reported a degree of Aboriginal ancestry ("1/8 Native" or "some Native blood six generations back"). While the girls' identification has personal meaning, it did not seem relevant to this discussion. Language is sometimes used as a marker of ethnicity. There were 10 women who spoke an Aboriginal language, usually Saulteau (Ojibway) or Cree.

Eight were in the Urban Aboriginal sarnple, The two women in the First Nations community who spoke an Aboriginal language had both grown up elsewhere.

Table 3.4 gives employment characteristics of the women. There were no significant differences between locations in whether women were working or not ($ =

4.94, df = 3, p = -18, N = 83, N.S .), or whether there were one or two wage emers in the household (2 = 7.40, df = 3, p = .06,N= 70, N.S.). Almost three-quarters of the women were employed, either full-time or part-time. Another 14.5% were homemaken and

16~unningthe analyses with and without these participants did not materially alter resul ts.

79 Table 3-4 Employment Characteristics of the Research Participants

Suburban Rural Urban First Nations Aboriginal community

Number of wage earners in household, % Two One 42.9 33-3 50.0 31.6

Women's Employrnent Status, %

Unemployed or student 14.3 4.7 18.2 21.0

14.5% were unemployed or students. Including part-time employment, there were two wage earners in 44.6% of the households, one wage earner in 39.8% and no wage eamers in 15.7% of households. in the First Nations community, there may have been more employrnent opportunity than usual due to flood-related projects. However, most of the employrnent was unrelated to the flood.

The mean household income was requested in increments of $5,000 up to a cut- off of $75,000. This under-estimated income for some families at three of the four locations. There are a number of other ways that income was likely under-estimated. In identifying the source of their income, few families mentioned the Child Tax Benefit

(qualifjmg families received up to $1,500 and $1,800 per year for each child under 7 and

18, respectively). Aboriginal people living on-reserve do not pay income tax, GST or PST, and housing is provided at no cost to Band members. Urban-rural differences in purchasing power further limit the comparability of incomes. For these reasons, the proportion of low-income families was not calculated, nor were the locations tested for differences in income level. The range and median income at each location were:"

$12,500 to 2 $75000 ($60,000) for the Suburban families. $27,500 to r $75000 ($52,500) for the Rural families. $7,500 to r $75000 ($22,599) for the Urban Aboriginal families. . $7,500 to $57,500 ($17,500) for the First Nations cornrnunity families.

Six women reported receiving social assistance, one in the First Nations cornrnunity and five in the random portion of the Urban Aboriginal sample. Reported incorne was lower in the random than the snowball portion of the Aboriginal sample,

$52,500) respectively. The snowball sample reported higher educational levels and more two wage-eamer households than the random Urban Aboriginal sample.

Anticipating there might be some difficulty obtaining and interpreting income data, women were asked how well their income met their needs (see Table 3-5). There were significant differences in how participants rated the adequacy of their income

(Kniskal Wallis test; H = 10.3, df = 3, p = .02). Urban Aboriginal women rated their incornes as least adequate; Rural women rated theirs as most adequate.

''~llbut two women from the First Nations comrnunity, and one each from the Suburban and Rural communities provided income data (N = 79). The midpoint of the range, or the stated amount when given, was used to calculate the median. Median incomes are lower for the Suburban location and higher for the other three locations than medians based on data collected for the 1991 Census (Statistics Canada, 1994; 1995b; Winnipeg School Division No. 1, 1996). Table 3.5 Participants' Rating of Adequacy of Income, by Location

1. Very 2.Adequate 3. Some 4&5. Location n Meana well % % difficulty % Not well% Suburban 21 2.6 14.3 38.1 28.6 19.0 Rural 21 1.9 23.8 61.9 14.3 0.0 Urban Aboriginal 22 2.8 4.5 3 1.8 54.5 9.1 First Nations 19 2.5 21.1 26.3 36.8 15.8

Note. Closeended responses from 1 to 5 were: very well, adequately, with some difficulry, not very well and inadeqzmtely. These last two responses were combined. "Median ratings were 2 for the Subhan and Rural communities; 3 for the Urban Aboriginal and First Nations communities.

1 also asked about home ownership as a rough estimate of family resources.

Almost everyone in the First Nations community (94.74) and the Rural community

(95.2%) said they owned their own homes; 66.7% of the Suburban families and 22.7%of the Urban Abonginal r~portedhome ownership. Car ownership might have ken a usehil indicator, especially in the First Nations community, where housing was provided, distances were greater and public transportation limited.

Weight- Status

There was a significant association between mother' s and daughter' s BMIs

(Pearson's correlation = .46, p < .0001).

Figure 3.1 shows the distribution of BMI for girls and women by location. A multi-way ANOVA pinpointed significant differences by generation (F = 40.07, df = 1, Figure 3.1. Disuibution of Body Mass Index (BMI) by location for girls and women. BMI was calculated from self-reported height and weight (weight in kilograrns divided by height in metres squared). BMls were based on sarnple sizes of 17, 19, 16 and 13 for the Suburban, Rural, Urban Abonginal (Urban Ab) and First Nations community (FNC),respectively for the girls and 2 1,21,20 and 18 for the women.

137, p c .000000) and ethnicity (F = 7.49, df = 1, 137,p = .007).18

The mean BMI for Aboriginal girls was 21.8 I0.87 SE. The mean BMI for non-

Aboriginal girls was 20.2 2 0.83 SE. The majonty of girls at al1 locations had BMIs under

25, with many under 20. There were girls with BMIs above 25 with the exception of the

'There were no significant differences by Region (F = 1 .W,df = 1,137, p = .30), or for any of the interactions. This data was incomplete for 10.3% of participants (with more missing data for girls and for Aboriginal participants). Of the girls with missing data, there were four Urban Abonginal girls, two First Nations community girls and one Suburban and Rural girl who selected a Body Shape Drawing (BSD)one shape above the median for their location. Two First Nations community girls selected a BSD one shape below the median and the remaining girls with missing data chose the median shape. Heavy BMis in the Urban Aboriginal girls were likely under-represented. The two Urban Aboriginal and one First Nations community wornen with missing data selected a BSD that was one shape heavier than the median for their location. Testing the BSDQ responses yielded similar conclusions (see Chapter 9). Suburban location. The mean BMI for Aboriginal women was 27.5 I 0.77 SE. A majority

of Aboriginal women had BMIs of 25 or more. For the non-Aboriginal women, the mean

BMI was 24.8 a 0.77 SE. A majority of the non-Aboriginal women had BMIs below 25,

although there were 30% with BMIs over 27-

These results are similar to other studies in finding that a higher proportion of

Aboriginal girls and women had high BMIs. The main purpose for using the BMI in this

project was to provide context for participants' weight-related comments.

Representativeness

The sample consisted of Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal middle-aged women with

12- and 13-year-old daughters, living in Winnipeg or a nearby rural community. The

mean age of the girls was 13.5 years. The Aboriginal women at both locations were

younger, had more children and lower income and education levels than the non-

Aboriginal women. The Urban Aboriginal sample also included a higher proportion of

single women. There was considerable diversity within each sample. For example,

besides the diversity in education mentioned earlier, there was at least $50,000 between

the highest and lowest reported incomes at each location.

The samples at three of the four locations were random samples with a good response rate. The Urban Abonginai sample consisted of a random sample fiom an inner city school and a non-random snowball sample of higher income Urban Aboriginal families. Although it cannot be assumed that this sample is representative of either population, there is a large urban Aboriginal sample in Winnipeg that includes lower, rniddle and higher-income households. The majority of Aboriginal people now live in urban centres and nual cornmunities, including 40% of "treaty" Indians (Twkenay,

1996). It remains an important population to attempt to understand.

The First Nations community from which this sample was drawn was relatively accessible to Winnipeg, and this has influenced its history. The majority of the women interviewed spoke only English. The levels of education, employment and income were higher than reported for other communities in Manitoba (Statistics Canada, 199%). As in other southem Manitoba First Nations communities, this community had in-door plumbing, electncity and year-round road access to urban centres. Residents had reasonable access to grocery stores, restaurants and infra-structure support for activity.

The enormous diversity amongst Canada's First Nations is frequently referred to by First

Nations peoples. Even communities adjacent to each other may have very different social and cultural support for health behaviours, as reported by Martens (1999, p. 78) in a study of factors affecting breastfeeding. As Martens cautioned, a large random sample may mask diversity, making individual coinmunity studies sometimes more appropriate.

Rather than generalizing, results may suggest concerns be investigated elsewhere.

The response rate was lower in the non-Aboriginal families, but more important, five women in the non-Aboriginal sarnples gave weight-related reasons for their decision not to participate in the research. Concems about weight may be under-estimated in the non-Abonginal families. No Aboriginal participant mentioned weight in her decision not to participate. The majority of participants gave no reason for declining the interview or said that Iife was twbusy or too difficult at the time, or their daughter did not want to participate. One First Nations community woman cited a desire for privacy.

As anticipated, ethnicity is confounded with a nurnber of variables, including incorne, education, age, marital status, parity and weight. Because the intent of the research was to explore how women and girls in different situauons perceived weight, not to untangle the influences of race, income or socio-economic status on those perceptions, this confounding was not considered a critical limitation. It is essential, however, to address in interpreting results.

In the case studies, 1have used a pseudonyrn, but in discussions of themes, 1refer to participants using the following coding system:

The first letter refers to the participant's location (S for Suburban, R for Rural, U for Urban Aboriginal, and F for First Nations cûmmunity. The second letter is either M for mother or D for duughter. The number that follows identifies each participant uniquely at each location.

So SD19 would be the Suburban daughter from the 19" interview set at that location. UM19S would be the mother from the 19" Urban Aboriginal family interviewed.

The final S indicates that this family is part of the snowball sample. This system allows the source of quotes to be easily identified. 1tried to select quotes from both generations at the four locations to represent the frequency and intensity of the issues as presented.

In order to improve readability, 1have deleted repetitive words, false starts, and ums and uhs from quotes unless they provide emotive context. Identifying features of participants have been altered to protect confidentiaiity. CHAPTER FOUR

On Women, Weight and Identity

We know that every woman wants to be thin. Our images of womanhood are almost synonymous with thinness (Orbach, 1981, p. 69).

The strength that Aboriginal people gain today from their traditional teachings and their cultures cornes from centuries of oral tradition and Aboriginal teachings, which emphasized the equality of man and woman and the balanced roies of both in the continuation of life. Such teachings hold promise for the future of the Aboriginal community as a whole (Hamilton & Sinclair, 1991, p. 476).

One of the initial weight questions, asked to get the interview started, was: "1s weight more important to women than to men [or to girls than to boys]?" Because 1 expected virtually everyone to say "yes," my real question came next: "Why is this? What are the influences that make weight so central in the lives of girls and women?"

The girls at al1 four locations answered as anticipated. The Suburban and Rural women answered as anticipated. There was no such consensus amongst the Aboriginal women, leading me to question the corollary assumptions that weight was a womads issue, central also to the lives of Aboriginal women and girls. Was weight the same kind of issue for Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal women?

Weight was clearly important to the majority of Aboriginal participants that 1 interviewed. Whether the measure was prevalence of dieting, promotion of weight control, level of body dissatisfaction, restraint score, or risk for an eating disorder,

Aboriginal participants, particularly those living in the First Nations comrnunity, consistently demonstrated greater concem about their weight than non-Aboriginal

participants, as will be detailed in subsequent chapters. Their weight concerns were also

expressed in their interviews.

But there were competing impressions as well that suggested weight might be a

different issue for the majority of Abonginal participants. This came across immediately

and strongly in the different responses to the opening question (see Table 4.1).

Table 4.1 "1s Weight More Important to Women than to Men?" Women's Responses by Location

-- Location n Yes % No % Other % Suburban Rural Urban Aboriginal First Nations community

On the Importance of Weipht to Women

Non-Aborieinal Women's Res~onses

Al1 but two Suburban and Rural woman said weight was more important to

women than to men, although both implied some agreement with the statement. One said she would probably agree that weight was more important to women, but she thought the point of the question was ''jhess or bodily appearance, body image. " And so she disagreed, saying these were of concem to both, and sometimes of more concem to men.

The other said, "1 think there's a higher percentage of women who are ovenveight, but Z don't think the issue is any different if it's an ovenveight man, woman. No."

Generally speaking, the Suburban and Rural women answered this question easily.

The answers came quickly, without any pause to ponder the question, and they were given with certainty. The responses consistently described strong societal pressures on women pushing them to be concemed with their appearance and weight.

Cornments about media images of perfection were ubiquitous throughout the interviews. RM19, for example, said "there's a perceived image out there of the pegect woman" that cornes from "TV, magazines, everything. " Others discussed how women are judged. SM10 said: "Women whu are perceived as being ovenveight, receive more public scrutiny and criticism than men." SM7 extended this to aging: "A man, he almost gmws old, distinguished looking. Where a womun's constantly worrying about wrinkles, and how her appearance is in fiont of the public, orfiends, or whatever the case rnqbe-

Mereus men, 'you take me as 2 am. ' "

The consequences of not measuring up to appearance standards were perceived as more serious for wornen than for men:

RMI: Ir almost seems like [sigh] it's been, through socieîy, told to us that we should be able to mn a pelfect house, look great for our husbands at al1 times. And that's just a general concept of what we're supposed to be. And ifyou don'? meet that criteria, then obviously at some point you may have failed. And a lot of wornen I know, fiends of mine, have a real problem with that. Like, "so and so isn'r going to care for me because I've put on a few extra pounds. "

RM4: Yeah seems like a lot of women who objectively don't have to worry about it [Zaughs] do worry about it. ... Z guess we do it to each other probably. A lut of talk and competition to be sort of slim andfit and al1 that. Yeah, there's kind of the peer pressure. I think there's an assumption of ovenveight being cunnected to laziness, slothfilness, not caring about one's appearance. These pressures were described in ways that indicated they were pervasive influences on many important aspects of a woman's life, consequently af5ecting her thinking, feeling, behaviours and even her identity.

SMI3: Women seem to always want to have a boyfiend, or a relationship, or get mam'ed. And Z think that a lot of women feel thut to do that, they need to be thin and good looking, sort of al1 the time. And I don 't think men feel that pressure quite as much as women do. n2ey don 't seem to have to deal with it as much as girls and women do. Zt doesn 'r seem tu matter ojkn what the man looks like.

SM15: Again 2 think it 's fashion. Zt S clothes. It 's thut we are always perceived as needing to be beautijùl and attractive and itk almost like we need that tofeel good about ourselves to attract, and Z'm not talking about the opposite sex, just attract people, attract attention.

SMI6: A lot of men, maybe think that they tegot a bit of a belly or something, but they never go on and on about it. I think more for men itS a health concem. Like "Will this aflect rny health? " as opposed to "Gee, do Z look good in these jeans?" So to me, 1 would think that most men can tolerate a bit of extra weight. ... People will come up to them and sq"Oh you're getting a little paunch. " But they would never do that to a woman. She would be ... al1 uptight or crying or something [laughs] going "Oh my God. How could he say that to me?" Maybe we're supposed to think we're al1 supposed to look like the Buywatch girl.

Women talked about weight as if it was a culturd imperative. Women needed to pay attention to this in order to feel good about themselves, to be attractive to other people, to be respected. It was part of a woman's responsibility, part of her job to look attractive. It was just part and parce1 of king a woman. There were women who responded to this question, using language that intimated these concems are incorporated into a wornan's identi ty. SM1 1 put it clearly: "He's not weight conscious. He's health conscious. Men see weight more as a tool. For women, it's who they are. " This linking of extemal pressures to be slim and attractive, and the way women see themselves as women, also carne up in the following comments: SM19: II's just something that generations and generations haue just, itS inbred in you that you have to be and look und do a certain way. Nothing thatS ove- jus? general, the way advertising is, and the way we get up every morning and put mak-up on. My kids see thut you know. It's just the way we are.

RM2: Ir's always been that way, I guess. Thatk always the way the women were looked at, Zfound. And I think rhat'll never change. It 'sjust they 're supposed to be sexy and thin and thai's just the way it goes with what women are.

These observations were often delivered with large sighs, or laughter serving to indicate disagreement with the way things were, and sornetimes women stated outright that this was not right. RM1, the nual woman who spoke about king viewed as a failure if you don? run a perfect house, ended by saying "1 donf?totally agree with the whole idea but falking with difieren? other women, it's almost ingrained in us." SM3 clarifies that it is not vanity but a reaction to the sum total of dl these daily pressures: "Idon't tlzink it 's vanity. Z think it 'sjust perceptions and peer pressure and everything else they

encortnter day-in, day-out. "

Women were not necessarily describing the impact of weight on their own lives.

They were describing the social context in which they live that makes weight more important to women than to men. There seemed to be close to unanimous agreement about these pressures and their impact: Weight is critical to women's personal identity and success in life. Consequently, it is something that girls and women cannot afford to ignore. AborMnal Women's Reswnses

In contrast to the Suburban and Rural women, half of the women in the First

Nations community (9 of 18) did not agree that weight was more important to women than to men. They said it was equally important to both. Some seemed puzzled by the question and worked out their response as we talked. The women who agreed that it was more important to women gave different reasons for their conclusion. Half discussed how women gain weight more easily, seem to have a harder time losing it and they tied weight concems to health concems. There werp only a few comments about the importance of looking good to attract a man, although this came up in other parts of the interviews.

About one-third (6 of 20) of the Urban Aboriginal women said weight issues were equally important to women and to men. These women did not discount the importance of weight. They just applied their reasoning to men as well as to women, enlarging the discussion of media, for example, to include boys. The majority of the 13 who said that it was of greater relevance to women answered similarly to the non-Aboriginal women. focussing on societal pressures to be thin or the social stigma of king heavy. There were two Urban Aboriginal women who discussed women's propensity to gain and greater difficulty in losing.

Because the Urban Aboriginal women's responses overlapped with both the First

Nations community and the non-Abonginal women's positions, this discussion will focus on how the First Nations community women answered this question. Most of the First

Nations community participants who agreed that weight was more important to women gave different explanations than the non-Aboriginal women. Although nine women in the First Nations community said that weight was more important to women than to men, only three discussed women's need to be attractive to

men. One commented that women had to look good for men and added that women didn't

look at men that way. FM8's choice of words was interesting. She said, "Well, zifthey're single and they want to try to look for a man. " FM8 has the woman doing the looking, rather than king looked at, giving a more active role for women than most of the non-

Aboriginal women's words. The third woman takes a different tack, as 1probed in an effort to understand if heavier weights were seen as more attractive:

FM12: 'Cause men really don 't cure how they look [G: Hmrn.] Well, they don% G: Why do you think that's the case? FM12: 'Cause they think they could get any woman they want in the world, ifthey wanted to. [Luughs] G: They can get women without having to look good? FM12: Yeah. G: Whereas women ? FM12: Hus to tvto look good in order to try to get a man. [Laughs]. G: Are there any other reasons rhar you think women might be more concerned about weight than men? FMl2: Well, the marned women will wony about their weight because their husband might leave them for another younger, attractive, skinnier girl. The married men, even boyfien&, end up cheating on their girljiiends. But 2 do hear a lot of men saying "The reason why 2 get fat women. this way they wouldn't leme me. " 2 have a lot of men fnénds to talk $0, and Z ask them. Just because, su the women wouldn't leave them. G: Tlzat's interesting. Ifyou're heavy, then it rnight be more attractive to a man who's lookingfor a wife? FM12: No, no, no. Because the man is so insecure they want tofind sornebody they make sure they don't Zeave him. But they could go out and do whatever, 'cause they think that woman's so big that nobody will want her.

Although size standards for fat and attractive were not clarified, she links

"attractive" and "skinnier," and clearly denies that "big, " was ideal. She implies the opposite in her explanation that insecure men like big women because "they want tofind somebody they make sure they don 't leave him. But they could go out and do whatever,

'cause they think thaf woman 's so big thtnobody will want her." This is differen t than the non-Aboriginal women's comments that some women feared that if they gained weight their partners would leave them. According to this quote, king fat would increase a woman' s risk of king trapped in a bad relationship, and so weight was important not just to attract a man in the first place, but to avoid an abusive relationship.

FM10 brought up clothing and ease of exercising as the reasons why weight was more of an issue to women: "1 have sumefiends that are heavier, and they say when they go in tu buy clothes, it's hard tofind clothes that fit. And also it's hurd for ?hem to get into exercising. " She elaborated that "when they don't lose weight ehtaway, it bothers them. They'd like to lose it, fast you know. "

Five women giving affirmative responses pondered who was trying to lose weight, or who was heavy, counted more women than men, and therefore concluded it was of more importance to women:

G: Do you think weight is mure of an issue for women than for men? FM1 3: Yes. Al1 the time. Zt's easier to put on weight than for a man. Zt 's no? too bad with men. Men can be fat and jolly. Ifs no? healthyfor either men or women.

FM19: I think so, yeah. ïîtere's not many men that Z ses that are oveweight. And Z see more women overweight thun men. Even the men say ... "Oh, it's so unfair that women have to fight so hard and restrict themselvesfrom su much foods and keep up their activity to lose the food they eat, like to burn offthe fat." And the men, 1 think because they're so muscley, you know they huve so much more muscle rnass than women. thnt they cmburn caloriesfmter than women.

FMI: Z think so yeah, because the women thar Z'nr with they always talk about Zosing weight. There was about eight of us, we were a12 going to pre-natal at the same time. That's al1 they talked about, was Tmgoing to lose this weight once 1

have this baby. " Thar was the furthest thing fiom my mind. There is a hint that the social context is different for men than women in FM13's

comment that "Men can be far and jolly. " Yet the ernphasis is on physiological and

health circumstances: Women put on weight easier, it's not healthy for either.

FM5 comrnented on saciety's impossible images for women:

FM5: 1 think women are more prone to gain weight. Itk more of a problem in it's harder for women to lose it Ifind because women store fat in different ways than men do. I read sornewhere in an article that once a woman gains weight it's harder for her to lose iî, because they have more fat srorage places. I don 'r know. [laughs] Something like that. G: Do you think it S more of an issue to women? FM5: Z think so. Well, ifyou go to most of the exercise clubs in the cities and see the health spas. the majority ofpeople in there are women. G: Why do you think that is? FM5: Because society holds a stereotype against women. Nut a stereotype, but an image. Like they set us up so; an image that women can 'rfit.

FM5 was the only First Nations community woman to refer to an ideal image in her

response, and, in contrat to many Suburban and Rural women, it was not her first

impulse. Note how she drew her conclusion based on her city expenence, rather than her

knowledge of the women in her community.

Those who did not see weight as more important to women also reflected on who

was trying to lose weight, but they concluded that it was equally important to both, or in

FM1 6's case, equally unimportant:

FM16: Z don 't know. To be honest with you, Z don 2 know because Z've never met anyone thar's wom'ed about rheir weighr. ... No. Z have friends that are ovenveight but it 's not a major problem to them. And I don 't see hem as ovenueight.

FM7: 1 think no. My husband is the same wuy roo. He watches what he eats and sruff Zn fact, he lost more than me.

FMI 1: Oh, Z think about the same. G: What are the reasons they're concemed with weight? FMIl: Well,for me when Z tn-ed to lose weight after Z had rny kïds a lot of people like insulted me and everything you know. G: 2 was getting the impression thut people were nicer to each other here in tems of weight. FMI 1: There are just certain people on this reserve thut they 'refeeling like this, but other people you can get along because there are a lot of big people on this reserve and we never say anything wrong to them.

When quened further about why people were concerned, women most frequently spoke of health, but appearances and clothing also came up. These reasons were given as concems for both men and women:

FMI 1: Some that are too ovenveight are scared ro you know have a heurt attack now.

FM16: Health wise.

FM2: I used to see rny dad, he was really an overweight man and he go? diabetes. And Z see being ovenueight you can't get rid of al1 the food that's in you, and it probably tums like to some kind of sickness. That's what Z always said, like it's nowhere to go und it jus? kind of turns to something Zike sickness, like maybe cancer. Maybe that's where cancer starts or diabetes, or even stomach problems. And I'm quite positive that al1 these sickness that the food can be mm]fast food. Zt just poisons the body.

FM9: Their appearance, I guess, ifthey're ovenveight. G: Why do you think they're thinking about rhat? FM9: Z don't know, but 2 think for myselfifl was zoo ovenveight then Z wouldn't look right ut all. Then I'd feel uncornfortablefor health reasons. You know d~zerentlittle things like that. I thinkfor healthy eh. You gotia keep your weight down too, somewhat. You know, not becorne overweight.

FM7: You feel so uncomfo~ableright now, like get back to your noml weight. You '11 feel more healrhier and you can do things more. Run up and down the stairs more. [Laughs] Short-winded. And Z still have my clothes stuck in the closet. [laughs]

FM15 They can'tfrr certain clothes when they go shopping. They have a hard time to fir into something nice. FMI 7: For appearance.

FM18: Z think itS because it could be of rheirjkiends, or the style of clothing. Could be their job orjust their health.

Aboriginal women mentioned health as a reason for concern about weight more

frequently than the non-Aboriginal women. As can be seen frorn responses already given,

the non-Abonginal women were more likely to use health to draw a contrast between men

and women's interest in weight. They pointed out that if men were interested in weight, it

was likely because of their health, but this was not the major reason for women.

Health came up for both Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal women in other parts of

the interview as well. Non-Aboriginal women raised heart problems more often than

diabetes (70.0% vs 22.5%). Aboriginal wornen raised both equally often (47.4%).

Despite shorter interviews, a higher proportion of Aboriginal women raised

diabetes as a concern. More interesting, when the First Nations community women

discussed diabetes, they talked about how diabetes had touched their Iives or the lives of

their loved ones, whereas the non-Aboriginal women largely discussed diabetes

abstractly. The Urban Aboriginal women again did both. Only one Rural and one

Suburban woman talked about peopie whom they knew had diabetes. All but one of the

First Nations wornen who raised diabetes discussed it in the context of family or friends.

For example, FM2 cornrnented, 'Y used to see my dad. he was really an ovenveight man

and he got diabetes. " Another woman said, "And the thing that bothers me, on Joe's family 's side, there was diabetes." FM10 said, " 'Cause Zfind there's a lot of sugar diabetes going around, and Z think it scares a lot of people. You know you see them, Umm, die so fast. " These conclusions were based on fmt-hand knowledge.

The non-Aboriginal women's responses usually drew on more general knowledge.

For example, RM4 said, "1 guess it [being heavyl can [aflect health]. Yeah. 1don 't think

it has much to do with mine. I guess heurt and lung problems can be associated.

Diabetes. " Listing issues in this way was a typical response style. Others referred to the authority of science to explain the connection between weight and health. For example,

RM11 said, "Zt's more hngerous to be heavier when you're older. Ztk harder on your heart, your bod[y]. That's proven it k harder on your heart. And there 's diabetes, You can get a lot more sicknesses fiom being ovenveight. Ztk not healthy. Zt's just not healthy tu

Three of the Urban Aboriginal people discussed Aboriginal peoples' greater risk for diabetes. Take, for example, UM19S's comment:

Genetically,Aboriginal women are prone to curry theirfat around their middle and tu have skinny legs. I think that S one of the reasons why they are such a hearty race and why we were able to withstand what we withstood, like generations ago. 'Cause when they tried tu knock us out, and there was always feast orfamine, 'cause of the fact that the women carned the extra fat around their waist made them able to be reproductive. Which helped them, definitely was a bonus. But now everything's su easy, and we're not in that feast and famine stage any more. Zt is more or less a hindrance now. And there 's things such as diabetes and this sort of thing.

Drawing on a scientific discussion of weight and the risk of diabetes for Aboriginal people when you are Aboriginal brings it closer to home. It does not, perhaps, make it as real as having "witnessedpeople suffering fiom orjtxing those problems, " as one participant phrased it.

To summarize, only a few of the reasons that First Nations community women gave for the importance of weight were uniquely applied to women. Two women stated that women needed to stay slim in order to attract a man. One focussed on women's desire to find clothing that fit, a reason that others raised as important to both men and women. Most of the First Nations cornrnunity women, however, talked about how women gained weight more easily than men and had more difficulty losing, and Iogically, it must be more important to women because more women were uying to lose weight. The remaining reasons were cornmon to both women and men, and included staying healthy,

meeting personal weight goals, wanting to look good or avoid "insults. "

These reasons for the importance of weight to women or people are mainly instrumental functions. Only one person brought up media influences (and this was in the context of city living). No one stated or implied that weight was part of king a woman.

There was no discussion of kingjudged as slothful or a failure, of not feeling good about oneself or not measuring up to society's standards. This may be a function of conversational style. However, the= were several other aspects of the interviews that suggested that although weight is important to a majonty of the First Nations community women, the concems varied from those of the non-Aboriginal women.lg

'%est of the girls (83%) agreed that weight was more important to girls than to boys (72.2% of First Nations, 75% of Suburban, 84.2% of Urban Aboriginal and 100% of Rural girls). The main reasons discussed at al1 locations were: (a) Girls care more about how they look, (b) to get a boyfnend, and (c) so you wont be teased. The Suburban and Rural girls raised the impact of media, advertising and wanting to be or look like a model. Only one First Nations community girl mentioned this. Several girZs commented that boys get praised for other things (e.g. sports and macho qualities) and that boys sometimes are rewarded for king large, but girls never are. Diabetes was raised by only two girls (one Suburban and one Urban Aboriginal). Talking: about Weight and Judment

Non-Abonginal women more frequently commented about feeling judged, or judging themselves than the Aboriginal women. The way the two groups of women used

the words "judge" and "judgment" was also different.

Eleven non-Aboriginal women used "judge" or a denvative in the context of

weight 30 times. Al1 used it to describe judgment around weight issues as a fact of life.

For example, RM8 said, "We look ut people's comperency ofien through their look And

sure ifs a judgment cal1 [laughs], but we still do th?." W1refexred to "The judgmental look that you get buying clothes, thut kind of stufi " SM9 commented, "rfyou

don'?show them anything else. they judge you totally on your appearance. " RMlO said,

"'Cause I always think about society and how it looks ut you. ... The majDrity do look ut

people, judge a book by its cover. " This was the tenor of these comments. Only one

woman used "judge" differently. In addition to this type of comment. she added that her

husband did not judge her. Another mral woman compared king insiderloutsider in two

rural communities. Her comment, that when you're the outsider you're discriminated

against, but as an insider you don't get judged on superficial qualities such as appearances

may have some relevance to the First Nations community.

Eight Aboriginal women used these words 13 times, and al1 but one (an Urban

Aboriginal woman) used it to reject judgment. A First Nations community woman also used the analogy to reading a book. However, unlike the rural woman quoted above, she rejected appearance-based judgment, and then went on to reject the whole concept of judgrnent: FM16: No matter how much I weigh, or how much weight I've put on, thut's not gonna change what 's inside of me. Itk just like reading a book You don 't judge it by its cover. And you don 't judge it by what you see inside because we are not here to judge one another. We are here to accept each other for what we are. 1 ~hinkwhat I want to see in a person is what 's inside, not what 3 on the outside.

W21S suggested this as an explicit value. "We talk about thut, Z mean about not judging

people. That's a big thing with us in general. As a family we don'?judge people, don't

judge the way they look" UIWJ said, "We're non-judgmental we accept everybody for

what they are and we gofi-orn there. " FM10 explicitly stated that people did not judge

you on the basis of your lmks in response to a question about how undenweight people

are treated: "Z don 't think, like around here I know that, people don 't really judge you by

the way you look "

Again, the way these words were used throughout the interviews can be seen as a

reflection of the fabric of people's lives. The absence of commentary about women king judged based on appearances, or seeing other people unfaidy treated because of their

appearance, implies that the Aboriginal women who spoke with me did not perceive this

as a large problem. It may be that other larger issues dominate consciousness, so that

weight becomes relatively rninor by cornparison. This is speculative as it did not corne up

in the interviews.

Weight in the Context of Women's Lives

"We don 't talk about weignt in this house. "This was one Urban Aboriginal woman's booming response to me, when 1explained the research to her and requested an interview. There was no mistaking her meaning. She said it loudly, laughingly, and she said it twice. She did agree to be interviewed, and her interview was consistent with her initial statement that weight was a non-issue to her. She was a large woman, and it was unusual to hear weight discussed in such neutral, disinterested terms. Few women indicated that weight was not something they ever thought about. 1wondered if she was puIling rny leg. On reflection, 1think not. This woman's interview certainly wasn't typical, but there were a small number of heavy Aboriginal women who explicitly expressed a lack of concem about weight. This was not a defiant attitude but more that weight was viewed as irrelevant to their lives. ArguabIy, this perspective could affect even those who want to lose weight, if it is obvious in daily living, by legitimizing an alternative point of view to the slim ideal. It did not, however, have this effect on this woman's daughter, who was tolerant and protective of her mother's size, but very concemed about her own.

For those who are dissatisfied with their weight, the level of distress will depend on how important weight is to the individual. Unfortunately, the few questions intended to elicit this interaction between satisfaction with and the importance of weight failed to differentiate anything but the extremes. Participants' open-ended responses to whether they were happy with their looks contradicted their close-ended responses. That is, people who sounded as if they were quite satisfied stated they were "somewhat unhappy" and people who initially responded "no" and explained why they weren't happy with their looks often ended up categorizing their reaction as "somewhat happy." Most participants responded between these moderate but uninterpretable categories.

When asked what they would like to change about themselves, weight was mentioned by about one-third of the women, except in the First Nations community where three of 18 mentioned it. Weight seemed to be an important issue to most of the

Aboriginal women intervieweci but perhaps eclipsed by other issues.

The anecdotes that came up in (or after) the Aboriginal women's interviews aiso positioned weight in a less central role. For example, after completing an interview with a woman from the snowball sample, 1went for coffee with her and her friends. The women joked about king of the wolf clan, which, they said, means you are quiet until backed into a corner, and then you rip your opponent apart. This came up because the woman 1 had just finished interviewing had been up until three in the morning helping a &end protect her family from an abusive ex-husband. Al1 of the women drew on their own experience to empathi-ze and reaffinn the wom&'s actions. Their stones made it obvious that although this trauma was not an everyday occurrence, neither was it extraordinary.

Some Aboriginal women at both locations discussed problems, such as overcorning addictions (dmgs, dcohol, gambling), escaping physical abuse, horrendous residential school experiences, and early pregnancies that foreclosed their opportunity for improving their lives. For the women who lived in poverty with its attendant issues of not having enough to eat, finding decent housing in a safe neighbourhood, and creating more opportunity for their children, weight and even health, seemed comparatively minor issues. They were emphasized only if they were creating senous problems, as attention to more irnmediate concems stressed their personai resources.

Of course, there were also Aboriginal women who did not discuss these types of problems. Non-Aboriginal women may be deaüng with equaily serious issues, but they did not discuss them with me. If discussing crises is more cornmon to Aboriginal women, then they would have greater awareness of their friends' and neighbours' life problems.

This in itself could create a context for "seeing" people that eases judgments of self and others.

Summarv and Discussion

Whether discussing body politics at the local level or the intemationai level, a

"one culture/one gender" analysis cannot adequately represent women, as Lock and

Kaufert (1998, p. 3) have observed, reminding us that class, religion, language, and ethnicity necessarily cross-cut gender. The findings of this chapter demonstrate this yet again.

To sumrnarize, non-Aboriginal women almost unanimously viewed weight as of greater importance to women than to men. Aboriginal women were as Likely to respond that weight was equally important to both genders. The reasons for agreement were also different. The non-Aboriginal women focussed on societal influences which they viewed as making weight an issue for women. The First Nations community women focussed on ph ysi ological di fferences between men and women, differences that made women fatter, and therefore made weight into an issue for more women. Media, appearance standards, and pervasive influences of feeling judged were discussed most frequently by the non-

Aboriginal women, less frequently by the Urban Abonginal women, and hardly at ali by the First Nations community women in their explmations of the imptance of weight.

Aboriginal women focussed more on health, although participants from ail locations raised al1 of these issues (with the exception of feeling judged) in other parts of their interview. Responses here relate specifically to the question of whether weight is framed as an issue of greater importance to women than to men.

The survival issues that some Aboriginal women discussed also served to downplay the centraiity of weight for the group, without necessarily making it a non-issue for individuals. The interviews with Aboriginal women were shorter, perhaps reflecting less interest in discussing weight issues, greater demands on their time and more urgent priorities, or a disinterest in the research process rather than weight as a non-issue.

Despite the impression that weight might be less important to Aboriginal women, as the next chapter will demonstrate, there were Aboriginal women and girls for whom weight was a serious, and even distressing, issue. Some of their experiences fit the theory that girls and women focus on weight issues to substitute for lack of control in other aspects of their lives (Mackenzie, 1985; Thompson, 1996). The presence of serious survival issues may draw out as well as diminish the role of weight in women's lives. The way participants handled their weight prevents a conclusion that weight was a non-issue, or simply a health issue for Abonginal participants.

As mentioned, Aboriginal women raised similar issues as the non-Abonginal women elsewhere in the interviews. However, they were not fit into a gendered analysis, and so did not corne up in discussing the relevant importance of weight to men and women. In fact, when I asked about the impact of feminism on the way women viewed weight, the Aboriginal women's responses indicated this was an inappropriate question.

As Patricia Monture, a Mohawk wornan and professor of law, has cornmented, the women's movement is imelevant to First Nations women: "The socially constructed boundaries of the ferninist movement and the lives of many First Nations women have never crossed" (Monture, 1993, p. 335).

My interviews did not probe this further. First Nations authors wrïting on racism and sexism generally have emphasized the common interests between men and women rather than an analytical framework based on gender. Despite the recognition of greater oppression of Aboriginal women, there is a frequently expressed resistance to clairning ferninism as relevant (Lachapelle, 1982; 1989; Monture, 1993).

Lachapelle (p. 257) explains there is a fear of dividing the Aboriginal community when issues of survival of that community necessarily take precedence. Other Aboriginal women writers suggest they have much in common with Aboriginal men and littie in common with non-Aboriginal women (Brundige, Fedorick, & Finn, 1993; Green, 1980;

Osennontion & Skonogan1eh:ra. 1989). The issues are well-hown and include poverty, inadequate housing, inadequate access to food, substance abuse, alcoholism and gambling addictions, high rates of violence and suicide attempts, and other indicators of poor health and premature death. Jamieson (1981, p. 140), after examining health statistics concluded, "in Iife and death Indian women and Indian men are more like each other than they are Zike others."

For some, Abonginal traditions offer women better goals (Anderson, 2000;

LaFromboise, Heyle, & Ozer, 1990; Monture-Angus, 1995; Osennonlion and

Skonoganleh:ra, 1989). For example, Osennontion and Skonogan1eh:ra (1989, p. 15) write that their goal is to revitalize the traditions where women "were treated as more than equal-where man was helper and woman was the centre of that environment, that comrnunity." Similarly, LaFromboise (1990, p. 471) writes:

It is important to recognize that retraditionalization efforts on the part of Indian women are often inconsistent with some goals of the current majority-culture women's movement. Non-Indian feminists emphasize middle-class themes of independence and androgyny whereas Indian women often sec their work in the context of their families, their nations, and Sacred Mother Earth.

These cornments go beyond survival issues and the fear of feminism as potentially divisive to a vision of a better way of living.

Some First Nations women writers locate women's greater oppression within the context of colonialism (Anderson, 2000; Stacey-Moore, 1993). Sinclaire (1997) does this when she uses an histoncal context to locate inner city Aboriginal women's difficulty in food procurernent for their families. Tsosie (1988, p. 20) emphasizes that modern gender conflict "for many Indian groups has largely resulted from patterns learned from white colonial authorities whose policies destroyed traditional egalitarian systems arnong Indian people." Monture-Angus writes:

The simple truth is feminism as an ideology remains colonial. ... The state's involvement in the histonc oppression of Aboriginal women and the denial of our individual and collective rights has not been incidental or accidental. It at times may have been without intent but that does not excuse the devastating impact it has had on our cornmunities. The state is fully implicated in the violence that exists in Indian communities today (Monture-Angus, 1995, p. 171, 178).

Within this framework, Aboriginal men are not seen as people with the power to oppress, but as victims of a broader, greater violence endorsed by the state and unquestionably accepted by feminist organizations. The negative impact of colonialization on Aboriginal men as well as women is emphasized. What does this have to do with the way Aboriginal women perceived weight?

Weight preoccupation is often based on a feminist analysis, and these influences can be seen in the interviews with non-Aboriginal women. In searching the literahm for

Aboriginal women's explanations of indisputable discrimination, 1found this colonialization discourse contrasted against and largely displacing a feminist analysis. 1 am not clairning that the wornen 1inte~ewed held these political perspectives because they didn't corne up. 1 am speculating that it is possible that the lack of a broader feminist discourse among First Nations cornmunity women has influenced the way they perceive weight issues, and may partial1 y explain why weight was not seen as more important to women than to men. Although the responses cited above were most frequently framed in a health context, some First Nations community women and girls dealt with their weight in definitely unhealthy ways. Their storïes, to be discussed in the next chapter, preclude seeing weight entirely as a health issue. Given the way girls and women discussed weight in other parts of the interview, it seems that the pressures are sirnilar but are not interpreted similarly. The difference seems to be the usage or non-usage of a gender- based analysis. The lack of discussion of weight as a central part of identity fits this interpretation.

In the socio-cultural model presented in Chapter 2, Stice placed the importance of appearance (and therefore weight) to women's success in our culture at the centre of the model (Stice, 1994). This seemed completely appropriate for the non-Aboriginal women, given the way they discussed weight. This did not seem to fit the experience of the

Aboriginal women, and therefore may not be entirely appropriate for the girls in this communi ty ei ther.

This has implications for educational work around weight issues. Rograms aimed

at preventing weight preoccupation in girls ofien take a gender-based approach (Brown &

Jasper, 1993; Diamond, 1985; Fallon et al., 1994; Friedman, 1994; 1997; Katzman &

Lee, 1997; Orbach, 1981; Piran, 1999). Although gender differences did corne up

elsewhere in the interviews, placing emphasis on these might be inappropriate. Anderson,

in her book on Native identity, has a bnef discussion of media that stresses the exclusion

of images of Native people from media representations and the stereotyping of beauty as

White. More exploratory work with Aboriginal people is needed to determine appropnate

approaches for promoting healthy attitudes to weight as well as healthy weights. The

overall health and survival of comrnunities and the individuais within are important

considerations. The Medicine Wheel has been suggested as ideologically compatible in

the sense of borrowinp each other's traditions, and may provide a basis for interventions (Anderson, 2000; Bartlett, 1995;- Postl, 1995). Bruyere and Garro (2000) found that health - professionals' recommendations about exercise and diet fit easily with Nehinaw (Cree)

frameworks, increasing their acceptability. Most of the Nehinaw interviewed reported

I 1 doing some exercise, and dmost dlsaid theyhad lost weight. Basing initiatives on

cornmunity-specific history and understandings holds promise, not just for helping people

with diabetes but also for preventing future cases. Given that teens may have a variety of

perspectives on the role of tradition in their lives, it would be critical to involve them (and

other community members) in developing this approach pnor to implementing and

evaluating it. Without this discussion of weight as a women's issue, it might appear that the

Aboriginal participants were more weight pre-occupied than the non-Abonginal participants, but this would misrepresent the entirety of the interviews. As remarked upon by Kaufert and 07Neil(1993), the similarities in the northern and southeni discussion cm obscure qualitative differences unique to the northern setting. Although the circumstances are different, but their remarks are appropriate within this cultural setting as well. The findings of this chapter need to be considered in interpreting the concern about weight reported in the next and subsequent chapters. A cornparison of participants' reports of their weight dissatisfaction and dieting efforts is discussed next, followed by a more in- depth Iook at the role of weight in the lives of some of the Aboriginal participants. Weight Dissatisfaction

"If shame could cure obesity, there wouldnt be a fat woman in the world Dr. Susan Wooley.

"lfyou are Aboriginal, you have to try 10 rimes harder than anybody else to be successfil in Ive, " according to one of the Urban Aboriginal woman 1interviewed. She applied this to weight. Reflecting on her own teen-aged years, she said, "I did everything jkom popping amphetamines to throwing up to starving myself: . . . 1noticed if1 drank alcohol that 1 never ate. And Z'd get skinnier. "

This woman's experience was not typical, but neither was it unique. Such stories came up too frequently to be ignored in the 40 Aboriginal families that 1 interviewed.

These stories indicate that the different emphasis that Aboriginal women put on weight as a woman's issue did not necessady reflect less concern about weight. This chapter addresses participants' weight dissatisfaction and their reported dieting efforts. Because these results are discrepant, the meaning of the word diet to participants is explored in some detail. The chapter ends with a look at nsk for an eating disorder and three

Aboriginal participants' stories.

Law Rates of Dietinn and High- Rates of Weight Dissatisfaction

It has been over a decade since Judith Rodin, Lisa Silberstein and Ruth Striegel-

Moore coined the phrase normative discontent to describe women's relationship with weight (1985). Studies continue to find high rates of dissatisfaction and dieting (Berg, 2000; 2001; Garner, 1997; Grogan, 1999). Consequently, it was surprising to find that the majority of women and girls in this study said they did not diet.

At the end of the oral interview, participants answered a close-ended question on the Restraint Scale that asked how fkequently they dieted. Approximately, threequarters of the participants said they never or rarely diet. Only 24.3% of the girls and 33.7% of the women said they sometimes, uszïally or always diet. (See Figure 5.1 for a breakdown by location and generation.)

Figure 5.1. Reported frequency of dieting for girls and women by location. There were no significant differences in reports of dieting between locations, generations, regions or between Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal participants?

2%xations were tested with the Kruskal Wallis test, comcted for ties (H= 1.29, df = 3, p = .73, N.S . for women; H = 6.59, df = 3, p = .û9, N.S.for girls). Generations were tested with a Mann-Whitney U-test (z = 0.59, df = 161, p = .55, N.S.).Generations were combined to test for hanand rural differences (z = 0.18, df = 161, p = .86, N.S.) and for Abonginal and non-Abonginal differences (z = 1.9 1, df = 161, p = -06, N.S.).Figure 5.1 shows combined categones, but tests were carried out on the full range of responses. This finding of lower rates of dieting does not mean that participants were satisfied with their body shape and weight (see Table 5.1). More than threequarters of participants indicated a dispaity between their current weight and what they would like to weigh (8 1.8% of the girls; 92.4% of the women). The majority wanted to weigh less:

63.6% of the girls wanted to weigh an average of 18.9 pounds less (ranging from 1 to 100

Table 5.1 Desire for Weight Change by Generation and Location

- - Generation and location n Desiring loss % Desking gain % Satisfied % Women 79 84.8 7.6 7.6

Suburban 20 95.0 0.0 5.0

Rural community 20 70.0 10.0 20.0

Urban Aboriginal 20 90.0 5.0 5.0

Fust Nations community 19 84.2 15.8 0.0

Girls 66 63.6 18.2 18.2

Suburban 17 64.7 11.8 23.5

Rural cornmunity 18 50.0 27.8 22.2

Urban Aboriginal 14 85.7 14.3 0.0

First Nations cornmunity 17 58.8 17.7 23.5

Note. Desired weight change was calculated by subtracting desired weight from self- reported weight, using the midpoint if a range was given. There were significant differences between the generations, none for location, region or ethnicity?

*lLocations were tested with chi square tests (2 = 5.44, df = 3, p = .14, N.S. for women; J = 4.57, df = 3, p = .20, N.S.for girls). Next, generations were tested O(- = 8.64, df = 1, p = .003, Fisher's Exact Test was significant at p = .MM). This was the only significant test. Because it was significant, ethnic and regional difierences were tested for each generation separately. The results for ethnicity were: 2 = .02, df = 1.0, Fisher's Exact Test p = 1.0, N.S. for women; 2 = .26, df = 1, Fisher's Exact Test p = 30, N.S. for girls. The results for Region wem: 2 = 3.72, df =1, Fisher's Exact Testp = .07, N.S. for women; 2 = 2.82, df = 1, Fisher's Exact Test p = -13, N.S. for girls. pounds); 84.8% of the women wanted to weigh an average of 26.5 pounds less (from 1.5 to 82 pounds). The mean desired weight loss for the FitNations community girls was more than double that of the other locations (34.9 pounds compared to 16.4, 15.8 and 9.8 pounds for the Rural, Urban Abonginal and Suburban girls, respectively). There was less of a discrepancy between the women. The Urban Aboriginal and First Nations community women desired mean weight losses of 33.5 and 26.4 pounds compared to

22.7 and 23.0 pounds for the Suburban and Rural women, respectively.

The low rates of dieting and high rates of dissatisfaction with weight were found at every location for both girls and women, a finding that is explored in more detail in

Chapter 9. We tum now to examine the way the participants defined diet in an effort to unders tand this discrepancy.

The Meaning of Diet

When asked what came to rnind when they heard the word "diet," participants' definitions spanned a continuum from "What you usually eut" to "healthy eating " to

'yust watching " to ryuust exercising " to "losing weight " to '*starvingyourself: "

A minority of participants (7.3%) gave the general definition of what you eat on a day-to-day basis, although some added that this is not what most people really mean.

Another 16% centred on healthy eating. Some of their responses fofussed on a particular eating habit which seemed to be code for a healthy diet, such as eating more fnùts and vegetables, or eating low-fat food, or not eating a lot of junk food.

Not surpnsingly, however, the majonty of participants (61.6%)either linked diet with losing weight or implied this by giving methods or examples of ways to lose weight.

Some participants referred to "watching what you ea?" in the sarne breath as "cuunting calories" or some other indication that weight control was the pnmary purpose of the watching. Exercise was discussed less often than food. More girls (25%) than women

(8.4%) mentioned exercise. Eight girls and one woman mentioned exercise first. Food was not even mentioned in several definitions, demonstrating the extent to which diet is synonymous with weight loss.

Some definitions suggest that healthy eating may also be code for weight loss eflorts. For example, one woman said, "It means eating healthy. Years back a doctor had

said, 'Ifyou're going to diet, don't eut out foods, because you need them. Just cut back ' "

This conflation seemed implied in other responses also, perhaps because for some, attempting weight loss entails a change to a healthier Iifestyle. Several women explicitly rejected the concept of dieting for weight loss, and substituted the concept of healthy eating or healthy lifestyle. It was not always clear if the weight loss goal was dmpped along with the terminology. One woman very clearly stated that she no longer dieted, but had changed her diet and exercise pattern in order to lose weight. Her words suggested that efforts to lose weight, involving food are no longer labelled dieting.

The word, die?,often had negative connotations. Forty-nine people (32.5%)

responded, sometimes with more than one answer, saying that diet meant "starving" (7 women, 4 girls), ''jifods" (7 women, 6 girls), 'failure " (2 women, 2 girls) or they made a disparaging comment (18 women, 8 girls) suggesting diet was "ludicrous,"

"threatening," "evil" or "bad." Some of the people who made these comments emphasized lifestyle changes, as discussed above. (Elsewhere in their interviews, some participants were explicit that their attempts to improve their health did not include weight loss efforts. These comments are discussed in Chapter 8.)

Participants' contradictory reactions in defining diet suggest one possible explanation for the findings of low rates of dieting coupled with high rates of weight dissatisfaction. The positive responses suggest dieting is socially acceptable for many, at least given the opportunity to explain their focus on lifestyle and health, in addition to their weight loss goal. Strong negative reactions indicate that dieting is not an acceptable concept for about one-third of the participants. Some people described the same motivation and behaviour but rejected the label diet. Negative connotations and preference for alternatives to the word diet have been previously noted (Chapman, 1999;

Marc hessault, 199 1; Nichter, Ritenbaugh, Nichter, Vuckovic, & Aickin, 1995; Spitzack,

1990; Wakewich, 2000).

There is a need to interpret simple questions about dieting cautiously. The dieting question captured only some of the participants who were trying to lose weight. Nor can it be assumed to predict actual lifestyle behaviours. Nichter and colleagues (1995) reported that although 63% of girls reported dieting, one-third of them reported a duration of less than a week, and only 14% of their food records reflected decreased food intalces

(although nutrient intakes were compromised for these dieters).

Although the rates of dieting were low in this study, an admission of frequent dieting often fore-shadowed more serious problems in the Aboriginal girls. These are discussed in the next section which examines the responses to the EAT-26. Risk for an Eatin~Disorder

There were nine EAT-26 scores above the cutoff for risk of an eating disorder.

Eight were in the girls, a prevalence of 10.3% for the girls and 1.3% for the women. One of 40 of the non-Aboriginal girls (2.5%) had a high score (Suburban location). Seven of

40 Aboriginal girls (17.5%) had high scores (four in the First Nations community and three in the city). The one woman who scored above the cut-off lived in the First Nations community. There were an additionai two girls and four women who did not score above the cut-off for nsk, but indicated they sometimes or very ofren used vomiting as a method of weight control (see Table 5.2). Weight was not a predictor of a risk score or use of vorniting for weight control. The BMIs of the 10 girls noted above ranged from low to high. One was under 20, six were between 20 and 22, and two were 25 or higher. One was not known. Four of the five women had BMIs of 25 or more.

In follow-up interviews to assess the need for referral, these participants al1

Table 5.2 Number of Participants with High Eating Attitudes Test-26 (EAT-26) Scores or Reporting Vomiting for Weight Control

Girls Women High Reported High Reported Location n EAT-26 Vomiting n EAT-26 Vomiting Suburban 20 1 O 21 O 1 Rural 20 O O 21 O O Urban Aboriginal 21 3 1 22 O 1 First Nations cornmunity 19 4 1 19 1 2 Overall 80 8 2 83 1 4 affirmed weight control intentions, discussing either severe food restriction or vomiting towards this end. These concerns appeared real, and 1discussed them first with the participant, and then in the case of the girls, with their mothers, andor arranged a meeting wi th an appropriate counsellor.

Some of the participants did not discuss vomiting for weight loss or indicate distress around weight issues in the oral interview, but they did check them off on the

EAT-26 and confirmed them at our foIIow-up meeting. Although there was no confirmation of an eating disorder, the EAT-26 seemed to have good face validity for highlighting participants who were extremely concemed about their weight, especially if the questions conceming vomiting habits were examined as wel as the aggregate score.

Examining just the EAT-26 questions about vomiting, about onequarter of the girls living in the First Nations community said they had vomited for weight controi.

Another four girls indicated they sometimes had the impulse to vomit. This is difficult to interpret, but raises questions about normative attitudes if a majority of girls in this community have considered vomiting as a way of controlling their weight. 1s there general acceptance of vomiting as a method of weight loss? Are girls aware of the risks involved in frequent vomiting? At what age does this become an issue? Are boys and older girls similarly concerned? These questions require further investigation.

The prevalence of at-risk scores in the Aboriginal girls seemed comparable to or higher than the rates of 9 to 24% reported by other researchers. Wichner et ai., 1986;

Wood et al., 1992). As mentioned earlier, the low prevalence in the non-Aboriginal sarnple may be due to sampling bias. Five non-Abonginai families declined interviews, giving their own or their daughters' prior and painful weight experiences as the reason. It is possible that these issues came up at a younger age or were viewed more negatively in these families. Their omission from the sample leaves these questions unanswered.

Girls were not promised confidentiality if an eating disorder was suspected and this may have prevented some from disclosing problems. Senous eating problems may be under-estimated. Nevertheless, these results suggest that Aboriginal girls living in and within driving distance of an urban centre are vulnerable to weight pressures.

As rnentioned earlier, Parker and her colleagues (1995) reponed a discrepancy between survey and ethnographic results for Afncan-American high school students.

Unlike this study, the survey and interview data here were either consistent, or follow-up confirmed problems raised on the EAT-26. It seemed difficult for girls to volunteer that they were throwing up, but once raised, problems could be discussed. The mothers' concem about their daughters' behaviours provided additional information useful in interpreting the girls' interviews, and in some cases vice versa.

In the next section, 1present excerpts from several participants' interviews to complement the above survey results. The details of people's lives are unique, but the gist of their stories seemed representative of concems expressed by a significant minonty of

Aboriginal participants, both in the city and in the FitNations community.

Abonerinal Partici~ants'Stones

1 have selected excerpts from three interviews to convey the role weight has played in some Abonginal participants' lives. There were other such stories in the First Nations community and in the Urban Aboriginal sample (both the random and the snowball portions). The late Sir Austin Bradford Hi11 remarked that health statistics are people with the tears wiped off (c.f. White, n.d.). These case studies are presented as a way to cornmunicate the importance of these experier.ces in the context of people's lives.

They validate the need for a better understanding of the nuances of weight. They provide evidence that weight has been of great concern to some Aboriginal girls and women. 1 have changed details of al1 three stones to preserve confidentiality, although the core of the interview, the participant's description of her relationship with her weight, is unaltered. The women's stories were past tense and they are not included in the above statistics.

Sam. David and Laune

It was a crisp fall morning as 1 walked up to Sam's house. I'd started this interview the previous May, when Sam said she wanted to tell me something, but couldn't talk right then because she was expecting her daughter home from school shortly. She said both her daughters had lost weight by throwing up, and that she had done the same thing when she was younger. She said she tried to use her own experience to help them stop. That was as far as we got.

It was hard to re-connect with Sam because opportunities for her to work came up unexpectedly and frequently, making appointments unpredictable. This particular moming, the interview had to be completed between the time her daughter Ieft for school, and the time she left for work, less than one and a half hours. This was usually more than adequate, but if Sam had a lot to Say, as it seemed she might, 1was &raid it would affect the telling.

Sam's husband, David, was aiso home and he joined us for the interview. He took care to keep refilling Our coffee cups, while expressing incredulity that anyone would go to this much trouble to ask questions about weight. David also contributed information and opinions generously, both about weight and about the family 's Living circumstances.

For exampie, near the end of the interview, when 1 asked about their level of income,

David showed me their couch and dining room suite, commenting with pride that he'd got these for his wife last Christmas. Sam added, "We 're struggling, but we provide for our kids. '"

When 1 asked Sam how she would descnbe herself, she replied "Z'm pretïy open. I don 't beat around the bush. Ifind it easy to socialize. Z'm not shy. " When 1 asked what she would change about herself if she could, she replied "'My age. " She said she wished

she could return to a time when she weighed less, adding "I got maded and I got fat. "

Sam started her story by recalling her mother's side of a phone call: "1 was 14 and

I hit the scales at 175.1 heard my mom say, 'No, she 's not pregnant. ' So I starved rnyse&

I went down 10 pounds a week Z went down to 135. And people thought I went away and had a baby. " She used the word "bulimic" to describe herself. She said she kept the weight down until she married.

Then she noticed her older daughter was "losing weight like crazy, while eating

like craq. I discovered she was making herselfthrow up. " She said she told her daughter about the acid eating away her teeth, and her daughter stopped. She thought this daughter, now married, was doing okay. Sam said she advised her daughters that they could have three meals a day, as long as they didn't overdo it, rneaning their weight would be fine if they followed her advice. "1just try to control her eating- She eats and eats and eats. She can gain weight easily and loses weight easily too. Z try to get her to control her eating. I

say 'You can eut, but don 't overdo it. ' Z always have snacks around. "

Sam mentioned other ways of trying to lose weight, ways that she had tried and one of her daughters had tried, too. "Look at the time Z went on SlimFast. My oldest went on it toa--secretly. She was getting skinrty, sickly--going to the washroom al1 the time. Ir did me good. Z was Zosing weight. It was just like a laxative. Around three months afier I

quit, 1 gained it al1 back "

When 1asked how women should deal with their weight, she recommended exercise, focussing on walking, even though she was not seeing any results for herself.

She said they walked a lot. "1 discovered al1 this walking we 've been doing--we 're not losing weight. Could be building muscles, though. We used to huffand pufi Z used to hate walking. "

She rated herself as neither happy nor unhappy with her looks, and said she wasn't

afraid of getiing fat "because Z'm mamed. " She added that she has "put on lots of weight in the last six months. " It seemed as if weight continued to be an issue for her, although not to the same extent as when she was younger. She rated her heaith as only fair, explaining "because you don 'tfeel good about your weight. Fat, 1 guess. You want to be

skinny. You admire other women who are nice and skinny. And here you are--fat. "

Despite her overall emphasis on the dangers of risky weight loss behaviours, her explanation of her rating of her health went very quickly from health to admiration for women who were thin.

Sam and David highlighted different causes for underweight. Her husband answered first, explaining that people were underweight because there was nothing to eat

in the fridge because the money had gone to "the dots, " a reference to the video display terminais easily accessible to gamblers throughout Manitoba. Sam's focus was on eating disorders: "The really skinny unes are the people that have lors of prublems. I had a friend who had anorenia. Thin ro this day. Now she knows she S sick She had su many problems. "

David closed the interview by telling me that his grandfather used to Say that you've got to follow your heart, or you will get into trouble. We talked about how girls get into trouble trying to please other people, and what that meant for weight.

When 1 spoke with Laurie later that day at school, I met a confident, outgoing 13- year-old girl. Laurie had short black hair, plucked eyebrows and burgundy nails. She was wearing blue jeans and a buttoned-up black t-shirt with an unbuttoned grey plaid shirt overtop. She talked about enjoying sports, especially volleyball, and was proud of how she was doing at school. She said she's leaming Cree from her granny. She liked

"goofing around" with her friends, and they liked her because she made them laugh.

Weight came up irnmediately as the thing she would change if she could. Laurie saw herself as fat. She said, "1just wish Z was skinny, tkt's all. ... 'Cause I've been fat all my life just about. '* She replied that she was afraid of getting fat "becnuse, I don? know, Z just don't want to be fat. " She said she was "very unhappy" with her weight. She was being teased about it and "itfeels uwjùl. "

She said she talked about weight with her granny, her mother, her sister and her

friends. The advice they gave her? "Theyjust tell me not to eut lots--1ike don? eut as

much. " When she told her friends "I was going to try to go on a diet this coming winter,

... they said okay and Z bet you ten buckr. ... IfZ get skinny, they give me ten bucks, and if

Z don't I give them ?en buckx "

Laurie saw being heavy as potentially causing problems. Besides king teased "a

lot, " she said a girl "wouldn 't get a boyfiend" and "ifyou get too fat you '11 probably,

umm. have a hemartack " She said that king underweight would not affect a girl at dl,

noting that underweight girls "don 1 get teased. They Ze popular. "

So, there was a great deal of pressure from famil y, friends and from Laurïe herself

to lose weight. She saw this as do-able. When Laune replied that people can control their

weight, 1 asked if this was dways the case. She said, "Probably, ifthey watch what

they're earing. " This was what she would advise others. But she herself had not had much

success. She explained her own efforts, "1 tried to go on a diet but--it didnft work ... Z just tried tu watch what Z was eating, but it didn't wurk Z just gained more weight. " Next,

she said, "1 tried to starve myself[laughs]. Umm. Z think that was a couple of weeks ago.

... Zt never worked either. When I'm in school here, 1jus? dont eat lunch or anything,

when I go home Z just end up--pigging myselfout. ... Zt's not helping eirher, 'cause Zjus?

get more hungry when I starve myself:" She said she usually skips breakfast and lunch,

but eats a lot at night. She eats the lunch her mother made her when she gets home hm

school, and then her dad makes her eat supper. Her mother had also said that Laurie usually rushes out of the house without breakfast, as she'd done that moming. When 1 offered to take the Iunch to school, Sam said not to bother, Laurie wouldn't eat it anyway.

Laurie said she smokes, but did not relate this to her weight. She seemed hesitant to discuss smoking so I did not ask further direct questions. Nor did Laune discuss throwing up in order to Iose weight during the interview, but she indicated this was a problem afterwards.

Laurïe was slightly heavier than the mean for Aboriginal girls. Her goal weight was 95 pounds @MI of 16). Despite her determination to lose weight, Lawie said 1could talk to her mother about her interview, and she thought she'd like to talk to a counsellor.

She seemed ready to try a diHerent approach.

Tansv and Lily

Tansy called me after I'd interviewed her mother. She said she spent a lot of time hanging around at her friend's house, and I was unlikely to catch her at home. Tansy was the only teen to cal1 me to set up an interview, although some teens called to re-schedule.

She, and her fnend and classrnate Lily were eager to be interviewed, a likely hint of their approach to life. 1 had already interviewed Tansy's mother. Lily's mother was also willing to participate. The focus here will be on the girls' interviews, although it may be helpful to know that both girls' mothers wanted to lose weight, although neither were cumntly trying. One said she had resumed smoking to deal with her undesired increasing weight.

The two girls were supporting and encouraging each other in what appeared to be extremely focussed efforts to lose weight. Each said she and her fiend had lost 30 to 40 pounds in the last month. They were pleased with the weight loss but concemed with some of the unanticipated physical side effects (such as hair loss and feeling awful). 1 focus on Tansy, because she was enthusiastic about telling her story. Her fnend had lost more weight, and seemed more vulnerable, perhaps because of her shyness.

Obviously, Tansy was not shy. She was a tall, strong and healthy-looking girl. She had her long, dark hair pulled back away from her face. She was wearing a sweatshirt and b!ue jeans on this cool spring evening and she looked like she was prepared to enjoy the interview. She sank into the couch and started to talk immediately and with confidence.

She scooped up the youngest baby, and jigged him on her knee throughout the interview.

There were other young children in the room also, and they were kept amused by my "kid kit," a bag of toys brought dong for this purpose.

When 1asked Tansy to tell me a bit about herself, she said she liked to roller skate and play pool when she wasn't busy baby sitting. Then she said she had had "bone" problems for years. Her condition sometimes caused her a lot of pain. She took pills to control the pain, but it still prevented her from being active. "[The pain is] everywhere in my body. Sornetimes if could go in my han&, or sometimes it just goes into my knee. My knee's been bugging me a lot lately. Sornetimes, it'll just go mvay. And then other times

if11 just corne nghr back. where Z can 't, like in gym, when Z run and that. 2 can't do tha?. "

Tansy came back to this three or four times during her interview. It was an important

concern, one that related to weight. When 1 asked her what she would change about

herself, she replied "If2 could change something, Z'd probubfy change the fact thm my

bones hurr. Or my waist line. " Afier a short pause, she said her waistline was the more important.

1 asked her if girls were thinking about different things than boys and Tansy

responded with a laugh, "WelZ, Z don't think about boys al1 the time. Z look ut them, think

they're cute. " And then she described some of the pressures she was dealing with:

T: And then there's my fnends, like they used to be my fnends, they pressure them [theirjiiends] to do drugs or something. But Z don't do drugs or anything like that. Z smoke. That's it. G: Why do you smoke? T: Z don 't know. [Laughs.] That 's addictive. Z started offwith my fnends and then after, when Z tried to quit, I just couldnk G: How old were you when you srarted? T: Eleven. Yeah and then afier when Z tried quimiig a couple of months later it was, Z couldn % 'Cause of al1 the cherniculs they put in there and everything. G: So you got hooked prerty fast? T: Yeah I got hooked pretty fast- And then I had a couple offiends who tried to get me to do dnrgs, and then Z dropped them right away because 1know a whole bunch of people who do drugs, and Z look ut them and Z donlt want to tum out like them. Like my mom. She's on welfare and everything. She had me when she was sixteen, and she dropped out of school, and 2 just don't want to muke the same mistakes as her.

Some of these concerns came up again when she described what she'd iike to see in a boy: "The fact like he doesn 't do drugs or he just doesn't want um--he doesn't want sex like, something like that. And who would look at you, not for your looks but for what's inside." She said "Z went out with a guy before. He was like that bu? then Z'm only thirteen and he did want sa, so Z dumped him. " She added, that she tw would go for what's inside when choosing a boyîriend.

Tansy listed her background as Christian and "more Zndian thun anything else. "

She said she knew that she was smart, but she was in danger of failing her grade. She reported missing more days than she had attended so far. She was pleased with herself because she'd recently arranged to be suspended if she skipped classes. This allowed her to refuse her mother's requests for her babysitting services.

Despite al1 this, or mayk because of it, weight concems were central to Tansy. 1 asked her if weight was a problem for girls. She said, "Lots of girls I know, it's jus? like

'I'm so fat. ' And they '11 be skinny but it's in their heads thut they're fat.'%en 1asked why this was the case, she replied, "they want to impress guys probably. " She said weight is important to girls "because they want to feel good about themselves, how they look. "

Tansy said she was 5 foot, 9 inches and 120 pounds, and "very unhappy" with her looks. She said, "Everyone says Z'm skinny but I know Z'm fat. ... They say 'you're skinny, '

Z'm like 'No. I'm nut. Z'm fat. Look ut all rny fat. "' She wanted to weigh 100 pounds and she said she dealt with this by not eating. Here is our conversation after 1 asked ber if she had ever done anything to try to change her weight:

T: 1 don't eut. G: You don't eut. Can you tell me a bit about that? T: Well, like, sometinzes like I have my, what I like. I, I never eut breakfast. I don? know why, Z never eut. I would never eat breavast. Then Z wouldn't eat lunch, and Z just drink water al1 day, so it kind offilled me up and then, I wouldn't eut anything al1 day. And then the next day only, I'd only eut, like lunch or something and then I ivouldn 't eut anything. Stufllike that. G: And how about the evenings? What do you do at night? T: Umm, uh, what Z do ut nigh? is just I sit around and like eut crackers and water and thut, trying to fill myself up. ... And then I would like 2, Z tïy ncnning, exercise and thut. But because of my knee it. 1can't run very far. ... One of my cousins, she never eats either. Like once in awhile we eut a big meal but that's about it. We don't eut much. G: Hmm. How's it working? T: I lost weight. Z weigh a hundred and twenty. G: And what did you weigh before? T: One fifi. G: How long did thttake you? T: About a month. G: That's a lot to lose isn 't it? Hmm. And how do you feel? T: Zfeel okay Z guess but I think 1 still fat.

Later, she said, "Irmafraid of getring fat al1 the time. Just the fact thar when I eat

I feel like Z'm gonna gain like ten pounds if1 eut this and that." She was not afraid of getting too thin: "'Cause Z think Z'm fat. ... To get too thin would be acîually, I'd be happy if1 was skinnier. "Tansy said she didn't talk about weight with most of her fnends, "Just my fnend [Lily]. Yeah. She's like 'Z'm fat. ' And she's like 'No,you 're no?. You 're skinny. '

'I'mfat. ' She goes like that. " She continued to describe her friend's concems, which were sirnilar to her own:

'Cause Lily used to be really chubby, you see how skinny she is. You could tell in her face, that it's jus? been thinner. 'Cause she used to have a fat face and if's skinny now. And she Zost about srjrsr pounds. Not eating. She used to weigh like one sîrry nine. Well, not sixty, about forty pounds. She used tu weigh one seventy, and now she only weighs one thirty.

Barbara

Barbara called me after hearing about the study in one of her classes at university.

She said she wanted to share her experience in the hopes of helping others. As mentioned in the introductory comrnents, there is a great diversity of experience captured when people identify themselves as Abonginal. Barbara, like many Urban Aboriginal people, had grown up in a non-Aboriginal family. She was candid about her dificult, early life experiences. The details of her story are, of course, unique, but the experience of growing up in foster homes, or with adoptive parents is relevant, as much as any unique experience can beyto others who share a similar background.

At the time of the interview, Barbara was a 38-year-old mother of three children.

There were two girls who met the eligibility criteria. 1 used the time-tested randomization procedure of flipping a coin and ended up interviewing the younger daughter. Barbara's interview took approximately three hows. There were a number of interruptions, including making coffee, chatting with her older daughter and moving from the living room to the porch of her third floor walk-up apartment. It poured while we taiked, which was refreshing on a hot day, and the sound of the falling rain was soothing.

Barbara planned to work in the North with Aboriginal people after finishing her degree. She described her early years as "scatrered"and said she didn't know much about her life before age three, other than she was taken from her Ojibway birth mother and placed in a number of foster homes before getting settled into the "White,

Caucasian " working class fami1y that raised her.

Pnor to retuming to school, Barbara had worked in an office and had been a dance instmctor. She said her ex-husband was Caucasian and college-educated. She pegged her annud household income as "rotally inadequate, " but was expecting it to improve because she had just been awarded a prestigious scholarship.

Barbara identified with her Aboriginal heritage, saying it was very important to her. She described herself as "an honesr person. Also cmpassionate " and was proud of her children. She said her youngest two were "more Aboriginal. " The older daughter, she said, looked White, even before she dyed her hair blond. She cornmented that her older daughter was sensitive to Aboriginal issues, but had rarely experienced discrimination. Barbara said she was proud of her children, and she was proud of having survived.

Barbara inherited the gene for story telling. Her story spilled out in one more-or-less continuous Stream of consciousness:

Zt 's really fUnny because I didn 't realize when Z was in Grade 9, like my [older] daughterk in Grade 9 right now, I didn 'r realize that the more Z ate, the tighter my jeans would get. Z didn 'r realize rhere was a connection there. [hughs.] And Z had ulcers since I was thifleen. I was hospitalized for ulcers quite ofien. I have the type of ulcer that when you ate, it relieved the pain. I can 'r remember which one that is, I think if'sa stomach ulcer. But anyways, I never made that connection. And then al1 of a sudden, one day, me and my girlfiends, it was spn'ng time and we jus? stmed walking to McDonalds evety day for lunch fiom school, which was quite a walk for us. ... We did it for about hvo weeks, and al1 of a sudden my girlfiend's mother said to me, "Barbara, it looks like you're losing weight. You were getting quite fat there for awhile. " ... Then al1 of sudden my one girlfiend who was older, told me she was eating these diet candies. They were called Ayds at the time. Do you remember them? And she was taking those and no? earing. And so she says, "Oh you know you can get skinny on these things. " Then Z started doing thai. 1was losing weight and then rny otherfnends, like I don't know we jus? starred drinking and partying and I noticed if1 drank alcohol that Z never ate and I'd get skïnnier. [hughs.] You know we were taking bennies and stufllike this and Z met more people through purtying and everything and these girls would tell me that ifyou eut bennies and you don? eut food [you] get really skinny. So I staned doing that. And uh and then everything just kind ,7f like followed afier that. The vomiting and trying to starve myselfand that sort of thing.

G: So why do you think it had such a hold for those years?

B: It was like--hey I can control this thing. I can starve myseK and Z see results. I Iose weight. It was sort of Zike instant gratification more or less. You see results right away. You starve yourself for about three days. You see results. And then it was also like everybody was trying to always lose weight. And everybody, we al1 said, "Wow, you're so skinny. You're so lucky. " And stuff like this. So it was compliments that I received. And you go out and look for clothes and everything. I guess clorhes initiallyfit me beîter but then it go? to the point where I looked horrible.

G: How did you switch into the exercising and dropping al1 the other ways? B: Well, it wasn 't like an ovemight decision or anything. Z don't know it was [laughs1--this is a really deep--1 guess like Z had to be hospitalized several rimes for overdoses and everything. Z'd had quite a bit of problerns with doing drugs when Z was younger. One dqZ was over at one of my @endsJ apartments and Z realized that "Whoa what a dunip you Zive in." [Laughs.J She was on her own, and she was one of my bestfiends. We had gone through al1 this wtogether. And 2just thought, "Oh I don't want to end up Iike you. " Z don't know what made me al1 of sudden think this one day. We were just sining over ut her apartment, and I realized I could never live like this. I think we were about maybe seventeen at the time and I just couldn 't imagine myselfliving like thut- You know, like that's al1 there was to life--this dumpy apartmenr and jusr going out and gerring high. I justjigured there's got to be something else. And Z just dislocated myselffiorn my fiiends and Z stopped associating with them. And uh, actually 2 started reading the Bible. Z think 2 was about sixteen. But then 2 still kept doing drugs ut that time. Umm,just reading the Bible and trying tofigure out what it meunt. Z guess it was a fonn of meditution. And my parents had a cottuge out near Victoria Beach. When Z was a teenager 1 hated going out there. And Z just al1 of a sudden bved going out there, and I would go out by myselfearly in the moming and go for a walk and just have nature all around me, and like sit on a rock and jus? listen to the wind going through the trees and watching the birds. ... Watching ants build the nest and stuff like this. [Laughs.] And I just got away fjom that type of behaviour. And then one day Z was standing downtown by myselfand Z saw this guy and this girl walking down the Street, and Z looked ut the woman and Z thought, "Like wow, has she ever got a gorgeous body! Like, you must be bom that way or

something, right. " [Laughs.] And they walkd into a gym or some type of workout place. Z didn't understand yet about your muscles. Z didn 't even actually know what a muscle was. And Z noticed that they had gone into this place, and there was this book advenised on the wall. Zt was Lisa Lyon 'S. She was a body builder. She was the first woman body builder. ... Z bought the book, and Z started reading about it. And then this other girlfnend told me that she had joined the Spa. She

sqs, "You know you should corne down there. " ... And so Z ended up joining and that's actually how it started. Something definitely happened. Z don 't know what it was but 2 was on a big down hill slide to nowhere. And Z don't know what it was that brought me up.

Weight was sti11 an issue for Barbara, although again, not to the extent it was when she was younger. She said she was not happy with the way she looked. Like the majority of women, she'd like to lose 10 pounds. In response to rny question about whether she was afraid of getting fat, she said, "2 don't want to get fat. Z don? know ifh afrad of it though. " In her explanation, she talked about how she was better able to get

her way if she was at a cornfortable weight, mediated partly through king more

attractive, since she worked mostly with men, and partly through feeling more confident

because she felt attractive. She said she controlled her weight now by woriung out and

eating healthfully.

Barbara said that while weight was not more of an issue for girls than for women,

it could be "more traumatizing " to a younger person. She said she talked to her daughters

about weight. It was not an issue for her older daughter, who eats nutritiously and was

active. She said that her younger daughter likes more mats and was not athletic and

consequently was "a lirrle chubby" and starting to get "very conscious" about it. She

advised her daughter that she was not fat, didn't need to diet, but should stop eating junk

food and start eating nutritiously and exercising more.

I interviewed Barbara's younger, and extremeiy busy, daughter about three

months later. She was a petite girl. She said she'd gained weight when her parents got

divorced and had pu~posefullythinned out last year by king active. She seemed only

moderately concerned about weight.

Summarv and Discussion

Weight dissatisfaction was almost universal for the gids and women in this study,

reaffirming the appropnateness of the nonnative discontenr label (Rodin et al., 1985).

Most of the dissatisfaction was oriented to desires to weigh less (63.61 of girls; 85.1% of

women). The frequency of participants who reported dieting sometimes to alwuys was much lower (24.3% for girls and 33.7% for women). This may be because many girls and

women did not label their weight loss efforts dieting, but talked about "just wa~ching" or

'yust exercising. " For some, healthy eating may be code for weighr loss efforts. The majonty defined "diet" as efforts to lose weight, or in terms that implied weight loss.

Some viewed this as a favourable activity, but others responded to the concept of "diet" extremely negatively. Motivations behind healthy eating are difficult to interpret given the ambiguous social acceptability of weight loss efforts, in combination with most women's continued desire to lose weight.

A strong interest in weight issues clearly exists in the Aboriginal sample, both in the city and in the First Nations community. Moreover, as the women's mernories of their own nsky behaviours illustrate, a high level of personal dissatisfaction with weight has existed for at least a generation, at Ieast in these two southem Manitoba locales. More

Aboriginal women and girls related stories of distress around their weight than non-

Aboriginal participants, likely because of the sampling bias noted earlier. Extreme behaviours were rarely reported by the non-Aboriginal girls that 1interviewed, although some mothers spoke of their own past dificulties around eating, often a continuing struggle. The non-Aboriginal women who talked to me had received counselling. They were extremely hesitant to talk with me, and some did not. By contrast, none of the

Aboriginal girls or women for whom this was an issue had received counselling. Perhaps knowing that Aboriginal girls' probtems with weight were not king addressed was part of their motivation for participating in the interview.

Aboriginal women's experiences with disordered eating may be under-recognized. In "The Health of Manitoba's Children, " eating disorders such as anorexia or bulimia, are noted as one of the few conditions for which "Status" children are less at risk than other Manitobans (Postl, 1995, p. 27). No further data has been reported since 1995

(personal communication P. Martens, July 24,2000; L. Elliott, July 17,2000). The findings of risk for an eating disorder, as assessed by the EAT-26, although not corroborated, suggest risks may be under-estimated, at least in or close to urban communities. It is unlikely that anorexia nervosa would be overlooked, However, it seems possible that refeml biases may be operating for bulimia.

These interviews demonstrate that some Aboriginal girls in and near the city have been senously concemed about their weight for at least a generation. Several of the girls exhibiting the behaviours discussed in this chapter had BMIs above 25, and none were extrernely thin. The women exhibiting these behaviours were mostly heavier, and none were extremely thin. Some women in the sample, and this is especially meof Aboriginal women, would be constmed as king at risk of health problems due to overweight. These women, and girls who may be headed in this direction, also need help. Admonishments to lose weight, however, do not seem to be helpful, given that almost everyone already desires to lose weight. Almost al1 had tried, using a multitude of methods, most of which were unsuccessful. Some were unhealthy. There is greater potential to deal holistically with the various environmental influences leading to these concems in small cornmunities. Obesity prevention is the classic situation requinng a broad-based public health perspective with CO-operationacross multiple sectors. The public health approach, as outlined by Ritenbaugh and colleagues (1999), focusses on the structural complexity underlying individual behaviours. Education, transportation, health and food policies for the community CO-ordinatedin the work, school and home environments need to be addressed to foster healthier individual living patterns.

Despite the findings of the last chapter, where a substantial number of Aboriginal women, especially from the First Nations community, did not perceive weight as more important to women than to men, this chapter makes it clear that some of the Aboriginal girls' and women were extremely concemed about weight. The next analysis extends this discussion by examining consensus withi-n and between the four locations on a set of values statements related to weight. The îindings support an overall consensus amongst al1 participants except the First Nations community wornen. Results again point to the

First Nations comrnunity as king somewhat different from the other locations. The consensus analysis is also used to examine the agreement between mothen and daughtea in their responses to the statements. c- c- SIX

A Consensus Analysis of Weight-Related Values Statements

Tultural consensus analysis maps out patterns of agreement and helps to corroborate claims about shared cultural understandings, while pointing out areas where there is less agreement" (Grno, 2000, p. 312).

This chapter presents the findings of an analysis of weight-related values statements constructed in a way intended to preserve the focus on the participants' framework for understanding weight issues. Responses have been analyseci with cultural consensus theory to determine the extent of sharing of concepts within and between the generations across the four locations. The analyses were modelled on Gm's work (1986;

1988; 1995), particularly her examination of agreement and diversity of cultural understandings in causal accounts of diabetes in thme Ojibway communities (1996). This research also drew on the work of Kernpton, Boster and Hartley (1995), whose study,

Environmental Values in American Culture, demonstrated the appropriateness of using these techniques with diverse groups within their own culture.

Cultural consensus analysis is relatively new, and so a brief overview of the theory and method is provided prior to discussing its application in this project. The technical details of the mathematical mode1 are given in Romney, Weller and Batchelder

(1986). More general explanations are provided in Weller and Romney (1988) and

Borgatti (1994). Cultural Consensus Theorv and Method

The use of cultural consensus analysis is based on the assumption that people share perceptions about the world around them (Garro, 1988). It is fkther assumed that agreement and disagreement in such understandings is patterned and cm be measured.

Cultural consensus analysis and the quadratic assignment program are specific methods designed to assess the pattern of sirnilarities and differences within and between different social groups (Gano,1995; 1996; 2000; Hubert & Schultz, 1976; Kempton et al., 1995;

Weller et al., 1993).

In the following explanation of consensus theory and method, the term knowledge is used to help convey the basic concepts underlying the method. Consensus analysis is derived from and is analogous to test theory, but there are some differences. References to knowledge are intended to facilitate understanding the parallels between the methods. In discussing the application of consensus analysis to this project, I use the tems shured understandings or cultural agreement to emphasize that my objective is to assess if there is a pattern of culturally accepted responses to the questions.

Weller and Romney (1988, p.75) explain the central concept of the theory behind cultural consensus as "the use of the pattern of agreement or consensus arnong informants to make inferences about their knowledge of the answers to the questions." Their method allows cultural howledge to be measured and its distribution in a group of participants to be described mathematically (Romney et al., 1986, p. 313).

The mathematicai mode1 was derived from reliability testing theory but is applied differently. In reliability testing, test scores are used to describe something about individuals. For example, a score on a spelling test predicts a person's ability to spell. In cultural consensus theory, people's answers are used to predict the correct spelIing of words (Weller & Romney, 1988). More practically, it has been used to determine the folk names used for the plant manioc (Boster, 1987), or the cornmon features of the disease empacho (Weller et al., 1993). These examples illustrate how it is used in situations where there is no answer key known to the researcher (Batchelder & Romney, 1988).

In cultural consensus analysis, it is assumed that the "comespondence between any two informants is a function of the extent to which each has knowledge of the culturally correct answers" (Weller & Romney, 1988, p.75). Knowledgeable respondents are expected to give a higher proportion of such answers, and therefore respond more similarly to each other than those with less knowledge about the domain under investigation. The technique developed by Romney, Weller and Batchelder (1986) estimates how much each respondent "knows" (or, to use their terminology, measures competency in a cultural domain), weights each respondent's input and uses the aggregate to estimate the most likely answers. Consensus analysis, therefore, estimates the reliability of the information content of an interview (Weller & Romney, 1988). This is important in the discovery of the (unknown) culturally appropriate responses to the questions.

The assumptions underlying the mode1 are that (a) there is a single culturdly acceptable answer for each question, which if known, is always given; (b) each question is answered independently of every other question, and respondents answer independently of each other; and (c) al1 questions are drawn from the same cultural domain (Borgatti, 1994, p.275; Romney et al., 1986). Over-reliance on individual questions is cautioned against, given the modelTsinability to test individual questions for independence or difficulty level.

Analysis involves calculating a matxix of response matches among participants.

This matnx is corrected for guessing and then factor analysed. The fmt factor represents cultural competence. Subsequent factors account for the remaining variability (Kempton et al., 1995). If the assumption of a shared cultural domain is met, the first factor will account for the structure of the matrix. The amount of variance accounted for by the fmt factor should be several times larger than the second with al1 other factors king relatively mal1 and positive (Romney et al., 1986, p. 333). If these conditions are not met, there could be more than one set of answers, and a common culture with respect to the domain being studied cannot be assumed. Under these circumstances, systematic subgroup differences could produce a patteming of responses, and therefore it would not be meaningful to aggregate responses.

The reliability for a set of responses is derived from the number of respondents and the extent of agreement between respondents and is the basis for estimating the validity of aggregated responses. Validity is estimated by taking the square root of the reliability coefficient (Weller & Romney, 1988, p.76). The average inter-correlation among respondents (comcted for guessing) gives the proportion of answers reflecting shed cultural understandings. This is referred to as the mean consensusfactor, or the mean conzpetency of respondents (Weller & Romney, 1988; Garro, 1996). A mean competency of less than .3 is considered to represent too little sharing to meet the assumptions of the mode1 (Romney et al., 1986, p. 333). Consensus for individual questions with associated level of confidence can be estimated (Romney, Batchelder, &

Weller, 1987, for an example, see Garro, 1995).

Since cultural consensus theory is used to predict cultural knowledge of a domain wi th a single answer for each question, well known to most of the respondents, only a small number of informants is needed to reiiably predict the cukurally accepted responses. For example, a small number of hockey professionals would give accurate information about the rules of the garne. Their answers would be more reliable than even a large random sample of grocery shoppers. Romney, Weller and Batchelder (1986) illustrate this principle by using the responses of four infonnants (two each with high and iow cultural competence) to correctly predict the answers to 27 of 30 questions on a general information test. Weller and Romney (1988, p.74) conclude that the technique provides "as much assurance as if we had the actual answers." For example, a mean competency of .6 based on interviews with 20 respondents would indicate that the aggregated responses should correlate approximately .99 with the culnirally appropnate responses (c.f. table in Weller & Romney, 1988, p.77). While systematically collected data provide a statistically valid way to assess the prevalence of expressed sentiments,

Weller and colleagues (1993) caution that such cornparisons are only valid when ethnographically informed.

The analysis for this project was based on cultural consensus analysis, the

Quadratic Assignment Program (QAP)and a binomial test using Anthropac 4.0 (Borgatti,

1996) and Number Cmncher Statistical System (Hintze, 1997). First, the extent of agreement was determined for the responses to the set of values statements. To do this, consensus was detennined for girls and women separately at each location, and if appropnate, groups were aggregated to assess the extent of consensus (al1 the girls, al1 the women, the mothers and daughters at each location, and the overall sarnple). Next, the consensus responses for each statement were exarnined, Even with overall consensus, there are usuall y some differences within and between research sites. QAP was used to explore this with particular reference to comparing locations, generations and mother- daughter pairs. In contrast to consensus analysis, which is based on a sumrnary measure across al1 questions, QAP examines pattemed differences based on subgroup responses to the yes-no statements (Garro, 2000, p. 295).

Exploring Consensus

As stated, the first step in cultural consensus analysis involves determining the extent to which responses to the 24 true-fdse values statements were shared. Table 6.1 shows the results for the women and the girls separately at each site and for al1 combinations of groups which met the conditions of consensus.

The assumptions of consensus were met for al1 but the First Nations community women. Although the average competency for the First Nations community women was high (-58, SD of .lg), the ratio of the first to second eigenvalues was only 2.46. This combination of results suggests that although there is much agreement, there is insuficient patteming to produce consensus. This could result from a subgroup giving systematically different responses. Altematively, the First Nations community women Table 6.1 Consensus Factor Loadings for Values Statements

Generation and location Consensus factof' Eigenvalue ratiob

Girls Suburban Rural Urban Aboriginal First Nations community (FNC) Al1 Women Suburban Rural Urban Aboriginal First Nations community Al1 except First Nations women Mothers and Daughters Suburban Rural Urban Abonginal Al1 participants except First Nations cornrnunitv women

"Themean consensus factor is the average inter-correlation among respondents and is an estimate of shared responses (corrected for guessing). 'The eigenvalue ratio is used to test the assumption of a shared cultural domain. The fint eigenvalue should be at lest three times the second with al1 subsequent factors relatively small and positive. could have a di fferent framework for weight than the other groups. Either way, their statements should not be included in the aggregated analyses. The responses of the First Nations comrnunity women wilI be discussed subsequent to examining the sirnilarity and difference in the other participants. There was considerable agreement overall, and the extent of consensus wiil be exarnined next.

For the analyses of the seven other groups, the ratio for the First to second eigenvalues was higher than three (3.46 to 6-69), mean consensus factors ranged from 56 to -73, and al1 the other factors were positive? As can be seen from Table 6.1, the assumptions of consensus were aiso met for the aggregated samples show. The results suggest, that with the stated exception, participants shared cultural understandings of weight for each generation and across both generations at each research location. The overall mean consensus factor of .64 indicates that 64% of responses reflect shared understandings. flhis would be approximately 82% without the correction for g~essing.)~~These results demonstrate high levels of consensus and low variance (based on criteria cited in Garro, 1996) and provide evidence of general sharing of weight-related values amongst middle-aged women and teen-aged girls at these different locations.

Cultural consensus andysis is also a technique for discovering the (unknown) culturally shared responses to questions. The consensus responses to individual statements across the four locations and the two generations will be compared next. As expected, given the overall consensus findings, there was a high level of congruence in

%ighty-three per cent of the individual values were above .S. One individual factor loading was below zero in a First Nations cornmuni ty girl. This girl described a history of stmggling to overcome her anorexic and bulimic behaviours.

23~ssumingthat individuals will guess in agreement with the culturally shared response for half of the questions over the overall mean value of -64, this estimate is calculated as follows: (642 + [l - .642/2] = .821). the responses given. Table 6.2 gives the statements for which the analyses indicated

congruent responses at dl four locations and for both generations. Agreement (or

disagreement) is indicated by a Y for Yes/ime (or an N for No&ialse), followed by the

proportion agreeing (or disagreeing) to the statement. Statements are listed from a high to

a low level of consensus, with statements where agreement was the consensus response

listed first. Each of these 11 statements attained significance at p < .O1 by the binomial

test, which indicates that the response differed significantly hman even split of

agree/disagree responses.

Differences Between Locations

Table 6.3 lists the 13 statements that elicited somewhat more variable responses,

although even here there is much agreement. Small letters for yes and no indicate responses given by the consensus program that had Bayesian vaiues at the confidence level of p < .O1 but were not classified as significant by the binomial test. Statements that did not meet the Bayesian criterion are indicated with a dash preceding the consensus response. These responses cannot be considered reliable. However, the consensus response indicated by the program is given to enable interpretation of the proportion agreeing or disagreeing with this response. For example, -y = .57 means that there was no reliable consensus response even though 57% of the group agreed with the statement.

Eight statements did not meet the Bayesian criterion in at least one of the eight groups (see Table 6.3, statements 2,3,4,7,9, 16, 19,21). Looking at each generation separately, the proportion of matching statements for each pair of communities was over Table 6.2 Proportion of Agreement with Consensus Position for Values Statements Meeting the Binomial Criterion by Generation at Each Location

------Urban Fit S tatement Generation Subufban Rural Aboriginal Nations

Proportion agreeing with staternent 5. It is wrong to judge another person on Wornen 1-00 1.O0 .95 the basis of their weight- Girls 1.OO 1.00 -95 22. A fat woman can be beautiful. Women 1.00 -95 1.00 Girls 1-00 1.00 1-00 23. The rnedia should show women of Women 1.00 1.00 1.00 different shapes and sizes. Girls 1.00 1.00 1.00 13. If people felt good about Wornen -90 .8 1 .86 themselves, they would accept their Girls -85 1.00 1.00 body as it is, whatever their size and shape. 15. Women ought to be more concemed Women -95 .9S .91 .89 about being fit than about their weight. Girls -90 .85 -95 .84

6. Girls pay more attention to what thei. Women 1.OO 1-00 -81 -83 friends Say than what their mothers Say Girls .90 .95 -90 -79 about how they look. 20. Mothers should help their daughters Women -86 .95 -82 -68 lose weight if they need to lose. Girls -85 .80 -76 .95

Proportion disagreeing with staternent 1. Teasing a friend is a good way to get Wornen 1.O0 1.00 -95 -95 her to realize she needs to lose weight. Girls -85 1.00 -90 .84

18. A heavy woman should not Wear a Woxnen -90 .90 .86 .74 bathing suit or do things that show too Girk 1-00 1 .O0 -90 .84 much body. 11. If a cute guy tells a girl she's too fat, Wornen .90 -90 .95 -89 and if she likes him she should try to Girls .90 .95 1.O0 .68 lose weight. 10. If you had the rnoney, it would be Women .8 1 .86 -86 1.00 worth spending it on plastic surgery or Girls -90 -95 -81 -95 Iiposuction to improve your appearance.

Note. AI1 statements met criterion for consensus anaiysis and significance for the binomial test at the level of p < .O1 for al1 groups tested. Statements are listed fiom high to low consensus, numbered according to the order in which they were asked. Table 6.3 Proportion of Agreement with Consensus Position for Values Statenients no? Meeting the Binomial Cnterion by Generation at Each Location

Statement Generation Suburban Rd Aboriginal Nations 14. A girl or woman should worry about her Womcn n -71 N 36 N -86 n -78 weight because more people wiU Lilrc her. she will have an easier time attracting a boyfiend N N -95 N -86 n .63 and getting a job. AU other things king equal, she will do better in life if she is slim. 8. Models are thinner than they should be, but Womcn n -71 N -95 N -77 n -63 they look great so it's okay. Girls N -90 N 1.00 N -95 n .63 9. People should not weigh thcmselvcs. Women -y -48 N -76 n -73 N -84 Girls n -75 N 1.00 N -81 N -95 2. Weight is not so important- People Women Y .86 Y -81 Y -81 -y .58 shouldn't spend so much time thinking about it. Girls 3. People should refuse to buy a product Women -n .60 n -57 -n -55 N -74 that uses extremely thin models in its advertising. Girls 21. If a woman is very concemed about Women Y -95 Y -86 Y .86 y -74 her omweight. then it's Iikely that her daughter wiil be too. Girls 7. Some people are meant to be big and it Women y .67 y .67 Y .86 y .59 wouId not be heaithy to try to change their -n weight. Girls .58 y .68 Y .76 n .63 24. There should be a special tax on junk Women food and the money should be spent to Girls build bike paths and swimrning pools and to teach people about healthy living. 17. In general, people are not bom fat; Women they let thernselves get fat- Girls 12. Dieting is unhealthy. Women Girls 16. Mothers should tell their daughters not Women to worry about their weight no matter what they weigh. Girls 19. Women should always watch what Women n -60 n .57 n -64 -n -50 they eat because it's eas& to stay thïn than to Iose weight. Girls 4. if people felt good about thernselves, Women -y -52 n .62 n .59 y -68 they would take the tirne and energy needed to lose weight. Girls n .55 -y .53 y -62 y -68

Note. The letters y and n indicate consensus responses of yes and no, respectively. Capital letters indicate significance for the binomial test at the level of p s .01. A dash means the responses cannot be classified as yes or no using Bayesian values at the confidence level of p s .01. Statements are listed fiom high to low consensus, numbered according to the order in which they were asked. 75%. Most of the variability was due to unclassifiable rather than contrasting responses.

Within each generation, there were contrasting differences between communities at the level of the consensus response for only four of the 24 statements. In the three communities where consensus indicated aggregated responses were meaningful, the praportion of matching responses was over 87.5%. These results show that the women in the First Nations cornrnunity often gave the sarne answers as the other women even though a pattern of cultwally accepted answers cannot be assurned for this group.

Despite these high levels of agreement in the responses to the staiements , there is some disagreement as well. QAP was used to test for significant differences between locations (as used by Garro, 1986, 1996). The analyses were conducted on women and girls separately. A proximity matrix of inter-respondent comlations at al1 the locations was created and compared with a structure matrix created by placing 1s where agreement is expected and Os elsewhere. That is, 1s were placed to correspond to correlations within the same location and Os were placed elsewhere (see Figure 6.1). QAP correlates these two matrices cell-by-cell, then compares this observed correlation to a distribution of correlations generated by correlating hundreds of randornly pennutated matrices. This can be interpreted similarly to apvalue. (Borgatti, 1994, p. 273; Garro, 1996). The level for rejecting the nul1 hypothesis of no difference between subgroups was set at p < .05, considered appropnate given the exploratory nature of the study. The alternative hypothesis king tested is that correlations within locations will be higher than elsewhere.

QAP indicated significant correlations for both the women and girls (p = .O2 and

-002, respectively). This means that correlations between participants within a community SSSSSSSSSSSSSSSSSSSRRRRRRRRRRRRRRUCTUUUUUFFFFFFFFFFFFFFFFFF

Figure 6.1. The structure matrix to test for differences between Suburban (S), Rural (R),Urban Aboriginal CU) and First Nations (F) girls. The 1s and Os indicate the expected pattern of agreement and disagree~~~~nt for conditions where there are significant ciifferences bctween locations. were higher than correlations with participants in other communities. These results indicate significant differences between communities for both women and girls.

QAP was then used to see if differences between the four sites could be more specifically determined. Because a comparison of pairs of sites suggested that the significant differences were between the First Nations cornrnunity and the other locations, the above analysis was repeated without the First Nations community participants. This time the nul1 hypothesis was supported, suggesting no significant differences between the other three locations, and confimiing that the differences were between the First Nations comrnunity and the other communities for both women and girls."

QAP does not indicate the source of the differences between communities but this can be deduced by comparing the individual responses with the consensus response for the appropriate comparison group (not shown). There were three statements where the

First Nations community women gave responses that contrasted with the consensus response for the other women (compared with two, one and none for the Rural, Suburban and Urban Abonginal women, respectively). The girls at each location gave one response that contrasted with the consensus response for dl girls.

Most of the diflerences were not due to contrasting statements, but rather to varying strength of agreement. Differences were diffused throughout the statements for both generations as shown by the number of statements that were at least 15% above or

*mep value was .31 for the women and -71for the girls. Using the same method, no significant differences were found for self-identi fied ethnicity or current dan-rural residential status in either the women or the girls. below the proportion of participants giving the consensus response (see Table 6.4)?

There were ten such statements for First Nations community women, compared to a maximum of three for women at the other locations. There were five for the First Nations community girls, compared to none or two for the girls at the other locations.

Responses to the statements may reveal strong and consistent patterns in the data.

Compared to the overall consensus responses, more First Nations community participants agreed with staternents suggesting pater concem about weight, and fewer disagreed w i th statements that might have mi tigated such concem. These differences wiU be examined more closely, starting with the girls where the pattern seemed clearer.

Table 6.4 Statements Varying from Proportion Giving the Consensus Response

Generation and location More than 15% More than 20% Women Suburban 9, 17,24 - Rural 12,24 - Urban Aboriginal 17 - FirstNationscommunity 3,8,9,10,16,18,20,21 2,4 Girls Suburban - - Rural 16, 19 - Urban Aboriginal 2,7 -- First Nations community 3,7 8, 11, 14

ZSKempton,Boster and Hadey (1995) used a similar strategy. The cut-off is arbitrary, but seemed reasonable given the pattern of results. The First Nations community girls' responses chat varied more than 15% from the aggregated response for the girls were:

7. Some people are meant to be big and it would not be healthy to try to change their weight (19% more First Nations community girls disagreed. This negative response contrasted with the consensus response for al1 girls.) 8. Models are thinner than they should be, but they look great so it's okay (25% more agreed). Il. If a cute guy tells a girl she's too fat, and if she likes him she should try to lose weight (21% more agreed). 14. A girl or woman should worry about her weight because more people wiil like her, she will have an easier time attracting a boyfriend, and getting a job. AI1 other things king equal, she will do better in life if she is slim (21% more agreed).

These aH fit the pattern of greater concem about weight, but there was one that did not.

Seventeen percent more First Nations community girls agreed that people shouid refuse to buy products that use emaciated models in their adveriising. There may have been a problern with this statement. Some girls did not seem to understand it.

The First Nations community women' s contrasting responses were:

4. If people felt good about themselves, they would take the time and energy needed to lose weight (24% more First Nations community women agreed). 17. In general, people are not bom fat; they let themselves get fat (12% more agreed) . 12. Dieting is unhealthy (14% more disagreed).

Three other statemenis with more than a 15%discrepancy in response from the other women add to the impression of support for the importance of weight to the First Nations comrnunity women. These were:

9. People should not weigh themselves (18% more disagreed). 2. Weight is not so important. People shouldn't spend so much time thinking about it (25% more disagreed). 18. A heavy woman should not Wear a bathing suit or do things that show too much body (15% more agreed). The First Nations community women's responses to some statements ran contrary

to support for weight control. For example, although al1 groups opposed liposuction or

surgery to improve one's appearance (statement IO), the First Nations community women

were the only group that was unanimously opposed (with their daughters close behind).

Some other statements where their responses did not fit the pattem of greater support for

weight control were:

8. Models are thinner than they should be, but they look great so it's okay (18% more agreed). 3. People should refuse to buy a product that uses extremely thin models in its advertising (17% more disagreed). 16. Mothers should tell their daughters not to worry about their weight no matter what they weigh (19% more agreed). 20. Mothers should help their daughters lose weight if they need to lose (19% more disagreed). 21. If a woman is very concemed about her own weight, then it's likely that her daughter will be too (15% more disagreed).

It is plausible that other values, perhaps the desire to cornmunicate love and acceptance to

their daughters, a disbelief in criticizing others, the value of non-interference, respect for

the integrity of the body may explain these statements and take precedence over the

importance of weight. Consequently, the pattern of greater concem about weight in the

First Nations community is more complex for the women than for the girls. Some

statements imply greater support for weight control, but others seeming to be based on competing values.

Given the lack of consensus for the First Nations comrnunity women, their results were examined for evidence of a subgroup who might be responding differently. The variables tested (income, level of education, BMI or current body shape, and residentiai provided no strong evidence of pattemed responses. Comparing those who had always lived in the First Nations cornrnunity with those who had lived in a city or town

(residential pattern) was not significant (p c .06, N.S.). This cornparison grouped any residence off the reserve as living away, whether it was six nionths or 10 yem, in a large city or a small town, as an adult or whiIe growing up. Although there is not enough data to explore this relationship further, the bluntness of the measure, and its theoretical probability suggest further exploration dong these Iines may be fniitful. We Nm now to a discussion of the similarities and differences between mothers and daughters by age group, and then paired within families.

CornparinE Generations

The proportion of matching statements between women and girls was 79%. As with the differences between locations, there were some differences between the age groups, within the context of this high level of agreement. Again, their significance was tested with QAP. The data for the generations at each location was compared with a structure matrix that had 1s for girl-to-girl comparisons, 1s for woman-to-woman cornparisons, and Os for girl-to-woman comparisons. The alternative hypothesis king tested was that correlations within age groups would be higher than elsewhere.

The resulting correlations were significant for the Suburban and Urban

Z6Q~Pwas used to test for systematic subgroup differences on the basis of income (using the median income to divide the sample into two groups), education (those with Iess than a high school education compared to those witb high school graduates andor some college or university), current BMI (using the median [26.2] to divide the women into two groups), and self-assigned body shape drawing (smallest, median and largest). Aboriginal participants @ < .ûûû and -02, respectively). At these two locations, the girls' responses correlated more with other girls' responses than with the women's responses.

Likewise, the women's responses were more similar to the other women's. At the other two locations, the correlated responses within age groups were not significant so the nul1 hypothesis of no differences cannot be rejected @ = -11, N.S. for the Rural community; p

= -10,N.S. for the First Nations community).

In other words, the age groups were more similar in the rural communities than in the city. Plausibly, there may be fewer competing influences on girls anaor women in rural locations and therefore the effect is one of overall conpence of messages. Families may spend more time together, or girls may spend less time with their peers. Or it's possible that there is an influence acting on both age groups in the country, or on only one in the city. There is insufficient data to address this question.

An examination of Table 6.3 allows a cornparison of responses between the generations at each location. There were few differences in the number of statements where the generations responded differently at the consensus level (four each in the

Suburban, Wrban Aboriginal and First Nations community; one in the Rural community).

Specific contrasting statements varied across locations. The direction of the difference, however, was generdy consistent. There were two questions where women's and girls' responses varied by more than 15% at al1 locations (p s .O0007 when tested with a chi square test). Both questions concemed mothers' influence and treatment of daughters.

A large majority of women (86%) agreed with question 21, "If a mother is very concemed about her own weight, it is likely that her daughter will be concemed too." Far fewer girls perceived mothers as having such influence. Only half of the girls (50%) agreed with this statement.

The responses to statement 16 were even more diarnetrically opposed. Two-thirds of the women (67%) disagreed, whereas almost two-thirds of the girls (65%) agreed that

"mothers should tell their daughters not to worry about their weight no matter what they weigh." When participants disagreed, they usually gave explanations based on health. A few of those who agreed talked about love. One Rural girl (RD13) said, "Z'd probably have to agree with them because it jus? kind of tells them, 'Well, we'll love you no matter what you weigh, [no matter] what you look like, you 're still our kid. ' " One of the Urban

Aboriginal mothers (LJMS) said, "Z'd love my daughter no rnatter how big she was. As long as she doesn 't have a bad hearî, rhen Z guess it 'd be al1 right because Z wuuldn't want her to die. " Even though only a few participants explicitly raised similar thoughts, the implications are enomous. They suggest the intertwining of weight and worthiness.

Despite the high proportion of girls who said that mothers should tell girls not to worry about their weight no matter what they weigh, 84% of the girls agreed that mothers should help their daughters lose weight if they needed to lose. This contradiction would seem to place mothers in a bind. The next two chapters examine mothers' and daughters' views on appropriate actions to take around weight, looking specificaily at what mothers and daughters Say about advice given and received.

This cornparison has looked at the statements where mothers and daughters (as groups) gave different responses. Next, the degree of matching responses within mother- daughter pairs is examined. Corn~aringMother-Dauehter Pairs

Comparing mother-daughter pairs is similar to a test-retest situation (except girls'

responses are compared to their mothers' responses rather than to their own upon re-

testing). It is important to evaiuate similarïty and variation within mother-daughter pairs

within the context of al1 variation (Garro, 1983). To determine if the responses a girl gave

were more similar to her own mother's than to her classrnates' or the other mothers'

responses, the mean correlations and mean matches for individual mother-daughter pairs at each location were calculated and compared to those for girl-to-girl pairs and unrelated pairs of women and girls?' The results are presented in Table 6.5.

The correlations between related mother-daughter pairs at each location varied from .32 to .53 in the First Nations and Rural cornmunities, respectively. Similarly the

Table 6.5 Correlations and Matching Responses for Pairs of Girls @-D), Mothers and Daughters (M-D) and Unrelated Women and Girls (W-G)

Location n Mean co~relations Proportion of matches D-D M-D W-G D-D M-D W-G Suburban Rural Urban-Abonginal First Nations

"The matrix of agreddisagree responses for the girls at each location were entered into similarity (comelations or matches respectively), and then into univariate statistics (without the diagonal) to get the mean comlation or matching statistic for the girls. This was done for the mother and daughter responses at each location, but ody the results on the diagonal were averaged to give the means for the related mother-daughter responses. proportion of matching respoiises given by related mother-daughter pairs is high, from .66 to -76 in the First Nations and Rural comrnunities, respectively.

B y itself, this would seem to support the common sense notion that girls leam these concepts from their mothers. However, the agreement must be evaluated in the context of how the other participants responded. There was a similar high congruence in the average correlations and matching responses between pairs of girls, and among al1 unrelated women and girls. Despite evidence of similarity between mothers and daughters, girls were not more sirnilar to their mothers than to their peers. The findings of similar associations between related mother-daughter pairs and within the group of girls and within the group of unrelated women and girls suggest the high degree of similarity is no more than would be expected given the level of consensus overall. There was no evidence in this data to suggest that girls were more sirnilar to their mothers or their pers than to the broader community.

Summarv and Discussion

There was consensus in the overall sample of participants except for the First

Nations community women. Even in this sample, there was fairly high agreement as indicated by the high cornpetence value (.58), and the matching of over 75% of responses wi th women at the other locations, There was agreement at the binomial level for al1 groups for 11 of the 24 statements. Despite this similarity, the lower consensus ratings for the First Nations community women indicated a lack of patterning in their responses to the questions. Analyses with QAP did not find any significant differences between the generations in the First Nations community. QAP highlighted both the girls and the women in the First Nations community as responding differently than the participants at the other locations. Differences in their responses were largely due to varying level of support for statements rather than to contrasting responses. An examination of the responses suggested a pattern of greater concern about weight in the First Nations community girls. There was greater variability in the women's responses, including both greater acceptance and greater rejection of statements about the importance of weight.

There are a number of possible explanations for this finding.

It is possible that there was a sub-group within the community who systematically responded differently to the questions. However, there were no significant differences within the First Nations community women when comparisons were made on the basis of income, education, BMI, self-rated body shape, or residential pattern. A more finely delineated cornparison of those who have lived outside the community with those who have always lived in the cornrnunity may be helpfd in detemiinhg if this could account for differing responses, given the results of this analysis.

Another possibility is that the First Nations community women may have a somewhat different frarnework for viewing weight issues. The First Nations community women may be aware of the culturally dominant responses, but have an underlying perspective that departs from this. Their responses suggest they may be pulled in several directions simultaneously. If the First Nations community participants have a different frarnework for understanding weight issues, then using a questionnaire based on statements made by inner city families would not be expected to correspond with their

perspective. The lack of consensus suggests this is the case. Although appropriate for

studying comparability of responses at a number of locations, this questionnaire cannot

do more than highlight the lack of consensus in the community. The open-ended

interview data from the First Nations community can now be used to constmct a fixed-

structure questionnaire to clarify an alternative framework within this community.

The results demonstrated that the First Nations community girls share

understandings about weight with their mothers and also with the other girls. The pattern

of their results is compatible with the hypothesis that the girls may be intermediate

between cultures with respect to learning about weight issues: Consensus was lowest for

the First Nations comunity girls; they were the only girls highlighted as having some

differences from the overall sarnple of girls; and the mean correlations and proportion of

matching between mothers and daughters were lowest for the first Nations cornmunity.

Studying a more isolated First Nations community would be interesting in that it could

clarify whether less exposure to the broader culture would result in pater differences

from girls in other locations and more sirnilarity with the older generation in their

community. Alternative ways of understanding weight would likely be more evident in a

more isolated community.

Comparing Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal participants, or urban and rural

participants did not reveal any differences pater than would be expected by chance except for the different inter-generational patterns.

The women and girls living in the city responded more similarly to their own age group. These differences also appeared to be due to greater or lesser proportions agreeing to statements rather than to contrasting responses. There was no evidence of diffenng responses between the generations at the two rural locations, although the general direction of their responses seemed similar to the wban locales. Finally, mother-daughter pairs did not have higher correlations or matching responses than non-related pairs.

In conclusion, there are some demonstrated differences between the four locations and between the younger and older generations. Despite this, there is much similarity in the way participants responded to the values statements. The next chapter continues to examine similarities and differences, turning to the responses to open-ended questions about weight management within families. CHAPTER SEVEN

Mothers' Advice to Their Daughters

"Let us put our minds together and see what Efe we cm make for ow children" (words of Lakota Sioux leader Sitting Bull in 1877, cf Story et al., 1998, p. 175).

How do mothers cope with a culture that presents girls with damaging models of womanhood? What do they tell their daughters? How do girls take their mothers' advice?

Can we take what mothers and daughters Say about each other's attitudes and advice at face value? In particular, do girls hear their mothers' comments accurately? Do mothers w ho diet have daughters who diet? Does it make a difference if the girl is lighter or heavier than average? Should mothers discuss weight issues witb their daughters, and if so, how should they approach it?

1 attempted to obtain at Ieast partial answers to these questions by asking each rnother: "Do you talk to your daughter about weight? Do you give her any advice? Does she give you any advice?" asked girls similar questions about their parents. When asked what they talked about (an intentionally open question), 13 participants who initially answered "no" went on to give examples of what they talked about. These responses were coded affirmatively. Hoping to obtain perceptions of longer-tem influences, 1also asked each woman if she thought her feelings about her weight affected her daughter's attitudes.

A girl's weight status may be a major factor in eliciting concems about weight

(her own, her parents' and other people's). Consequently, this analysis was stratified by girls' weight. Girls at each location were divided into quintiles of BMI. This chapter focuses on the interviews of mother-daughter pairs for the girls with the highest, median and lowest BMIs. The range of BMIs for each category is given in Table 7.1?

Table 7.1 Body Mass Index (BMI) Range for Girls by Location

BMI range for quintile Highest Median Lowest Location n= 18 n= 16 n=16 Suburban 2 1.5-23.4 18.7-19.8 ~18.0 Rural >25.0 19.8-20.6 48.0 Urban Abonginal >25.0 20.3-21.8 49.0 First Nations community >25.0 20.1-2 1.5 49.0

Who do Girls Talk To?

When asked if they talked to theirparenrs about weight, 68.4% of girls said they did. Forty-three girls specifically said they talked to their mothers. Seven mentioned their fathers. More mothers (88.5%) said they discussed weight or related issues with their daughters. Mother-daughter responses matched in 70.5% of the families, with both usually agreeing that they discussed the issues." Mothers explained the conversations

"~henheight and weight were not available, the girl's body shape selection was used. One Urban Aboriginal girl was not considered due to lack of data. There were more girls in the highest quintile due to the inclusion of ties.

2%espite seemingly high agreement, the concordance between mothers' and daughters' responses was non-significant, as indicated by a kappa value of .34. This test measures inter-rater agreement for categoncal scales, correcting for chanceexpected agreement. Rather than having two people rate many items. many mother-daughter pairs rated the same item. Kappas greater than -75 represent excellent agreement beyond chance, values between .4 and .75 suggest faV to good agreement that is diable, and values below more extensively than their daughtea. Daughters were expen at succinctly summing up their mothers' main points.

Participants mentioned numerous weight-related interactions with other people, including advice or teasing from fathers, siblings, grandmothers, other relatives, fiiends and acquaintances, especially boys, and especially at school. Professionals were also mentioned, including teachers, coaches and health professionals.

When asked whom they should talk to if they were having problems with their weight, girls said parents (SO), other family members (Io), a variety of professionais @O), and friends (29). Seventeen girls raised their mothers. Three girls also said fathers, but each added that dads rnight not listen or understand the issues as well.

Heavier than Average Girls

Knowing how to cope can be particularly perplexing when girls are heavier than their peers. The 18 sets of interviews in this category show how mothers see the dificult issues that corne up for their daughters, how they attempt to resolve them, and how their daughters react. Girls' BMIs were at or above 25 except at the Suburban location, where the highest BMIs fell between 21.5 and 23.4. Although these girls' BMIs faIl within healthy weight guidelines (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2000; Cole et al.,

2000), they were above BMI noms of 18.2 and 19.3 for 13 and 14 year-olds, respectively

(Beumont, Al-Alami, & Touyz, 1988), and they were heavier than the other girls at their

.4 represent poor agreement (Fleiss, 1981, p. 218).

164 location. They are included here for these reasons, but because they are lighter than the other girls, they are also examined separately at the end of this section. Mothers are the other half of the equation, and therefore their weights are also relevant. Two-thirds of the girls' mothers (11) had BMs over 30. There were five with BMIs under 25, and one was under 20. Al1 but two girls and two women wished to be smaller or to lose weight? Half of the participants (10 women; 8 girls) indicated on the Restraint Scale that they were dieting.

Two major themes came up in this discussion. Ten mothers' accounts of the advice they gave their daughters were either explicitly focussed on weight control or the context suggested this was part of the motivation. Their advice often focussed on nutrition and exercise. It was always moderate and sometimes cautioned against extreme behaviours based on the recognition of their daughters' desire to lose weight. Advice was usually given in a context of concem for health. Seven of the girls echoed their mothers' advice, two said their mothers didn't give them advice and one repeated only her mother's cautionary advice, "She told me not to szarve myselfto death. "

The second theme also focussed on nutrition or exercise, but five mothers explicitly stated that their advice was given to improve health rather than for weight control. Al1 of their daughters repeated this advice. Of the three rernaining mothers, one did not give any advice, and two said they told their daughters they were fine. Four other

30 Their desired weight was less than their current weight, or their desired body shape was smaller than their current perceived shape on the BSDQ (see Chapter 9). The exceptions included one Suburban woman, one First Nations community woman, one Rural girl and one Suburban girl (who reported she was always dieting). mothers also said this in the course of their response. None of the girls repeated these complimentary rernarks.

Despite the relative ease of dassifying responses into these broad categories, each ans wer was unique1y embedded wi thin a rnother-daughter relationship, and a family and social context. Mothers often brought in a multitude of factors as they deliberated the reasons for the advice they considered most appropnate for their daughters. Mothers' explanations and their daughters' reactions are examined next.

Wei~htControl Advice

One of the Rural mothers was very concerned about her daughter's eating and activity habits, explaining, "She's not a big eater. When it comes to eating, she eats ofien.

She'll eat in between meals and then, of course, when it comes to eating your main meal, she's no? hungry, and so she picks. And then, afier that she's going for a bug of chips. If she could leave her snacks and pop alone, she'd do okay. " And she added, "She's not involved in anything--she doesn 'Z get any exercise. "

RM2 said she talked and talked, but it seemed to go in one ear and out the other.

She said her daughter worked part-time wi th a group of slightly older girls, who were slim and frequently talked about dieting. After these exposures, her daughter would talk about starting a diet, tw, but it never lasted more than a day or two. The mother speculated:

RM2: Z'm hoping she'll grow a litrle bit and lose some of it but what can you sq? ... She doesn't really cure. Zt doesn't bother her. ... She has lots offiendi, and she gets along with everybody. Even ifsomebody makes fin of her, it doesn't seem to bother her. ... Z guess lin glad that it doesn 'î bother her because if it did there could be problems. But in the same tokn, I wish it did bother her a little bit so that she 'd try and do something about it.

RM2's reference to "problems" was an allusion to an earlier comment she' d made about the suicidal feelings of another girl, who was the same height and weight as her daughter.

She didn't want a suicidal daughter, but she wanted one who was motivated to lose weight. She ended by commenting "Put it this way, ifshe's not changed any by Grade 10, because then you're mature enough to know how you wanna look, ifnothing's changed by rhen, then she 's probably not going to change. "

The daughter undentood and agreed with her mother's advice, "My mom always tells me to cut down on the ice cream and sometimes pop and start eating more healthy,

like eating more salads. .. . but it 's hard. " Both said they dieted, dthough the mother' s efforts seemed to be more effective. The frustration expressed by this mother also came up in other mothers' interviews. Several participants cornmented that their weight-related conversations typically shaped up as arguments.

Some weight control advice seemed to be given in acknowledgement of their daughters' stated intent to lose weight, and seemed intended to prevent extreme measures.

For example, a First Nations community mother said:

FMI 7: Like my daughter, she 's always worried about her weight. She gets mad with herse& She's trying to lose her weight or she's trying to be slim. ... I say. "You gona walk and get enough exercise. " For sure, cut out al1 her junk food ... Z said, "Give it time."

Her daughter said her mother's advice was " 'Ifyou think you're too big, ' she says, 'well start exercising. "' 1t would seem that the daughter got the mother's message, although following it was more difficult. When asked how her daughter took this advice, FM17 repIied, "She says, 'Okay.' Nexî thing you know, she's eating it. "

Mothers also wonied about discussing weight with their daughters. One of the

Suburban mothers was wonied about her daughter because there was a family history of weight problems. And she womed that king loved might make it tw easy to gain weight:

SMI: Z guess ifyou grew up, ifyou grow up in a household where people are overweight, you know you love that person and they're okq. Maybe it wouldn't be so bad, ifyou kept eating and did al1 the wrong things and gained weight too. Zr's afier, when you know you can 'ido anything about it, or it's hard for you to do sornething about it. By then it's roo late.

SM1 seemed to be reflecting on her own past experience to predict how king loved and accepted might influence her daughter. She talked about weight and love together several times in her interview. She discussed a similar hesitation in approaching her mother with her concerns about her weight increasing her risk for a heart attack. She noted that thin mothers must be selfish women because time spent on personal groorning and exercise was time not devoted to their famiIy. But if loving and being loved result in unwanted and unhealthy weight gain, there's an obvious dilemma for mothers who love their daughters and feel responsible for ensuring a healthy weight. When 1 asked SM1 if she discussed weight with her daughter, she replied:

SMI: 2 haven 'r really sat down and told her why I think she should get into better eating habits. Of course Z've said, "I don 't want you to gain weight, or keep gaining weight, or get heavy like me. " ... It's jus?, "Stop eating that. You 're gonna gain weight. " That's about the extent of our conversation about weight. ... I would advise her, "lf you gain 10 pounds or 20 pounds do sornething about it then, you know. Don'? wait 'til it gets to 40,50." [G: Have you told her this?] No, that's the thing. I didn't think it would be a good idea yet to tell her. Ifshe notices her jeans are getting too tight and that she has to buy new jeans every four months, then I think it S time to take a look

Her daughter's response was short and direct: "My mom jus? goes. 'We should lose some weight or something. ' " Elsewhere she added, they never did, although she indicated serious efforts on her own. Given the mother's ambivalence about discussing weight control with her daughter, the suggestion of mother-daughter tandem dieting may have been a way of suggesting weight loss without comprornising the relationship.

An Urban Aboriginal mother spoke about her admiration for her daughter's

"incredibly neat " and "reallyprogressive " feminist perspectives, which encompassed weight and were different fiom her own. She also expressed scepticism and caution:

UM17S: Z try and do it [talk about weight] more front an exercise perspective, rather than a food consumption perspective. ... She's kind of got a strong ferninist perspective thlooks mean nothing, but Z don 't know if if's a defense mechanism. But ulso, she's 14. You have to remember rhat [laughs]. They don k do well with moms ut that age. ... Hmm fine, but Z mean she 's ovenveight and Z just want to encourage her to exercise 'cause she doesn't do anything. ... But she'll have a difieren?perspective on it, she'll think that Z care al1 about her weight. Zt's typical of the uge. They over-dramatize things. Like Z mention it once in six months, and shelZZjust [say], 'wu always want me to be thin. " ... So I stay away fiom it.

UM17's rationale hints at wanting to respect her daughter's autonomy. Despite the

warning that her daughter's perspective may not reflect what she actually said, her daughter fai thfully played back her advice, saying The'11 look at the treadmill and she's like 'Oh you should work out on that. " Or she'll go, ''Here you should try these bwer fat recipes]. We should eat differently. " When asked how she reacted, she said, "Oh, Z just let her do whatever shs wants. IfZ want to do it, then I'll do it. IfZ don't then Z won%"

The phrase junk food came up frequently in these discussions. For example, one of the Urban Aboriginal mothers (UM4) said she advised her daughter "Just not to eut so much. Well, she doesn't really eut that much but Z just tell her nof to eu1 so much junk food or stufllike that. " A First Nations community mother (FM4) said, "2 guess it'sjus? the eating habits. ... My hands are tied with that because when they're out and it's junk food, you know, they go for it. " The first girl said her mother did not discuss weight with

her. The second girl echoed her mother's words when 1asked her what her mother

advised her, "Just try and cut down on the junk food. "

Junk food was used in 42% of interviews across al1 of the participants."

Participants also talked about no treats, or cutting out specific foods, such as chips and pop. This advice was not limited to discussing weight control, but was also given in the context of healthy eating. Chapman (1993) noted that junk food was contrasted to healthy eating in her interviews with girls aged 11 to 18. She comrnented on, among other things, how junk food was used in the adolescent search for autonomy. Girls associated healthy eating with weight loss, parents and king at home. Junk food was associated with friends, pleasure, weight gain and guilt.

Five mothers discussed nutrition and exercise, making these recommendations for health reasons rather than weight loss. Al1 of their daughters indicated they understood this advice, although two girls indicated their goal was weight loss. One of the S uburban

3'This phrase has reached the Canadian Arctic. Egan (1999, p. 235) has noted references to junk food in her interviews with Inuit women in Coral Harbour, Nunavut. mothers, for example, said they didn't specifically talk about weight:

SMD: We talk sometimes about, oh, I don 'r know, swing in truck because it 's a good way tu keep in shape and things Iike that. But no, not about losing weight or gaining weight. ... She restnkts herseifa Little bit, like at night tirne ifwete having potato chips she won 't indulge in them. She doesn 't restrict her portions but you see her making choices that are good for her, or heafthy ones and 2 can see that 's a conscious eflortfor her.

SM19 recognizes that her daughter's goal (weight loss) differed from her goal for her (to keep in shape). She did not confront her daughter on her goal or interfere with it, but neither did skie support it. She said her daughters were larger than she was, and she wanted to prevent this from creating problems for them.

Her daughter echoed her advice quite clearly, saying her mother talked "about eating healthily, but not that Z shoufd lose weight, or Z shouldn 2. " And although she was concerned about her weight and said she dieted, she also said she rnostly did it by "eating more healthy, but no1 like going on a strict diet or anything. ... Just to get more in shpe

and exercise and stufl " Her mother's approach did not change her goal, but seerns to have infl'ienced her methods.

One Urban Aboriginal mother was dissatisfied with her own weight, but focussed on eating well and king active. She stated that health was her goal but she hoped she would lose weight too. She said she talked about weight a lot with her daughter but focussed on health. She xeferred to her own experience of pressure from her family while growing up. It seemed that sharing her story was intended to be instructive, perhaps to acknowledge that she knew how it felt, that peopIe's cornments shouldn't be taken personally. She explained her philosophy: UM22S: Z'rn trying not to make comments on whether she looks heavy or thin,

trying not to put too much emphasis on ht,or "you could lose weight. " ,.. It's really awing she gets îhat pressure fi-om people, that she's overweight and people tell her she is and she gets it mom] my family. My siszer. My mom. ... One of her cousins, they're quite vocal about her weight, and again, use it as a put down or oppression. ... I can't really make it go away or make it hun less. But then we just keep talking about it.... Z've been very conscious of not wanting her to diet and lose weight and no? eut, and tht kind of stufi

UM22S said she found it difficult not to focus on weight and emphasized how hard she tried to stop herself from pressuring her daughter about her weight. Her daughter's emphasis was somewhat different:

Once in awhile she 71 ask me ifpeople tease me. One time she jus? came out and asked me ifpeople teased me about my weight, and 1said, "No." ... [Z] never have like conversations but once in awhile she'll be, "1 wish [Z] wasn't fat." And then Z just go, "Ah. " And then we jus? don't really talk about if.... It's kind of uncornfortable 'cause Z don? really know what to say, so Z usually don't say anything. [G: Does she ever give you any advice?] She always tells me to eut healrhier, and once in awhile she'll tell me to exercise more.

AIthough she rnay be refemng to different conversations, she seemed to interpret her rnother's sharing her own experience as seeking reassurance from her. This conversation made her uncomfortabie, perhaps because she felt unable to give her mother the reassurance that she wanted. This sarne discornfort was apparent in the mothers' talk about their daughters' size and shape.

Descriptions of Dauehters

At least half a dozen mothers discussed praising their daughters specifically about

their looks, weight or shape, commenting that they were "justfine, " 'yust ràght, "

"pegect. " Tellingly, not one girl said her mother made these kinds of comments to her. FM1 1 answered my question with "1 told her she was nice the way she is. She doesn't need to lose weight. But she said she'd like to try it, like to lose a Zittle more." Her daughter said they did not discuss weight. RM13 said she did not have to give her daughter any advice "because she's a healthy eater. " She described a typical interaction,

"ifshe says that shek chubby, then Z'll Say, Well, no. You're not fat, you know. You're just right. ' I will compliment her on her weight. " This daughter also said she didn't discuss weight with her mother, except to support her mother's efforts to watch her own weight. SM7 also said she complimented her daughter:

SM7: Not constuntly but Z have because as Z said before, with her being in these teen years, every now and [then] she might corne down and say ... "I look too fat in this. " Z go, "No. You're jus? pegect the way you are. " ... To me she 's perjiect and she'd be pevect if she was fat, or extra skinny.

Her daughter commented, "7'hey don'? really give me any advice, except for when we see on TV like, 'It's no? worth making yourselfsick, or it's not worth making yourselfhave health problems because you want tu look a ceriain way. You can look that way without having any of those problems. '" This comment was consistent with her mother's emphasis, although again she did not repeat her mother's praise. The mothers' compliments were almost always prompted by a question, a negative comment or other indication of concern from their daughters.

Some spoke of complimenting their daughters, not on their appearance, but on their handling of appearance-related issues or on other aspects of character. For example,

UMl7S's admiration for her daughter's ferninist analysis, which she applied to weight issues. RM5 said, "1 think she's go? a really healthy am0tude.She's thefirst person to tell you, you have to accept me the way Z am. And uh. ... She's she S overweight. but not

exîrernely. ... She's just very comfortable with herse& IJnd. Her own person." Another

said, "She's [a] pretty on the bal1 kid. "

Some mothers were direct about their daughters' "overweight" as in some of the

already-cited references. Other examples are: "She's not thaî fat, but you know shek only

I3. She's bigger than Z am." "She gained too much for her age. " "She's uh she's pretîy

big--for her, you know for her age. Ifînd her big. Big bone size. " "She's built bigger. "

These statements were punctuated by long pauses, nervous laughter and sometimes stuttering. Only a few volunteered that they would tell their daughter that she was overweight. Mothers tried hard to be tactful in describing their daughters' body shape.

Some stated they intentionally avoided commenting on weight to their daughters.

One mother said, Yf1 notice her getting chunky, Z'll tell her, 'You shouldn't eat before you go to bed. You should eut your supper and then thaz's it. And tyyou get hungry in the evening have a @it. ' I dont say anything like 'You'refat' or 'YOUmight get fat.' I say that ta her, but Z don 't say 'You're fat. '" S he noted, however, that her daughter reacted

testily anyway, "She gels kind of umm-touchy. " The daughter's reaction suggests that promoting the need for weight loss or weight control implies the unstated concerns.

Do Dieting Mothers Have Dietinn Dauehters?

Similarity in dieting habits would iikely be most evident in this quintile, which contained nine dieters, half of the 19 girls in the overall sample who reported dieting sometimes. usually or always. As the discussion of the meaning of the word "diet" indicated, not everyone who is trying to lose weight said they were dieting. Self-reports of dieting should be interpreted cautiously, more as a statement of desire and intent to lose or control weight, rather than flagging particular behaviours. Self-reported dieting of mother-daughter pairs for the entire sample is given in Table 7.2.

Table 7.2 Mother-Daughter Dieting Reports by Daughters' Body Mass Index (BMI)

BMI Pairs Both diet Neither diets Only mother Only daughter n n n diets, n diets, n Highest 18 4 3 6 5

Lowest 16 O 9

Total 78 6.4% 53.9% 21.8% 18 .O%

There were five families in the overall sample where both mother and daughter said they dieted. In this quintile there were four families where both dieted and three families where neither dieted, six with a "dieting mother" and five with a "dieting daughter." This is not convincing evidence of mothers and daughters dieting together?

Perhaps a mother's advice is more important than whether or not she diets. Of the

10 mothers who gave advice in a weight control context, five of their daughters said they dieted, and five said they rarely or never dieted. Of the five mothers who said they did not advise weight control, one of their daughters said she dieted. The two mothers who

32 Kappa statistics did not indicate any significant agreement for mother-daughter dieting in the overall sample (-.02) or in the heaviest BMI quintile (-.22). said they gave compliments, not advice, to their daughters both had daughters who dieted.

Examining a mother's dieting status and advice to her daughter together did not seem to produce any consistent outcome either. (See Table 7.3.)

The stratification of girls by BMI somewhat controls for body weight. The

Suburban girls, however, had BMIs within healthy weight guidelines for their age group and weighed less than the other girls. Nevertheless thRe of the four Suburban girls said they dieted. The one girl who said she didn't diet had a mother who said she dieted and gave her daughter weight control advice. Two of the dieting girls' mothers did not diet or promote weight loss. The last girl, a dieter, had a mother who dieted and advised similarly. These results are consistent with the overall lack of a pattern.

Table 7.3 Girls' Reported Dieting by Mothers' Dieting andor Advice in Highest Quintile of Body Mas Index (BMI)

Behaviours of mothers Mothers, n Dieting daughters, n Diets and advises weight control 7 4 Diets only 3 O Advice only 3 I Nei ther diets nor advises weight control 5 4

Girls At-risk

Some health professionals would consider al1 girls with BMIs over 25 as at-risk of long-term health problems. Some would consider ail teen dieting to be problematic. Two girls in this gmup had high EAT-26 scores, suggesting a potentially serious risk. Another

176 girl talked of having lost more than 20 pounds, only to regain it all. One girl said she siit her wrists, and highlighted weight-related teasing as important in her distress. Although not assessed in this study, al1 of the girls in the previous research reported teasing was prevalent (Marchessault, 1993a).

Liehter than Average Girls

Weight issues of lighter than average girls are seldom discussed, aside from eating disorders (which are extensively discussed because they fascinate, homfj and mystify).

This section is based on the 16 girls with the lowest BMI (four from each location) and their mothers. The Aboriginal girls had BMIs below 19. The non-Aboriginal girls had

BMIs below 18. Ten of their mothers had BMIs under 25 (with two under 20). Two were between 25 and 27, and two were over 27.

The majonty of these girls were either satisfied with their weight (3) or wanted to gain weight (IO), from 2.5 to 35 pounds. Three girls (al1 Aboriginal) said they wanted to lose weight, and two of them said they were dieting. Nine of the mothers wanted to be smaller, and five said they were dieting. There were no families in this group where both rnother and daughter were dieting, but there were nine with no dieters.

A slight majonty of the mothers and daughters agreed that they did (8) or did not

(1) discuss weight issues. In al1 but one of the remaining families, the mothers said they discussed weight, and their daughters said they did not. In the one fdlywhere this was reversed, the mother said she did not âiscuss weight with her eighth grader, but she did with a younger daughter who had a different body build. The concerns expressed by this group were somewhat different than those discussed so far. Only one mother (from the First Nations community) gave her daughter advice in a weight control context. It was brief, and the daughter said she didn't discuss weight with her mother, although she was concemed about her weight. The majonty of mothers (11) gave their daughters advice that fccussed on nutrition and eating enough.

Three mothers (and one girl from a different family) were concerned about overly zealous weight control. Three of these were Aboriginal families. Other mothers indicated they watched for, but did not see, risky behaviours in their daughters.

Of the nine girls who said they discussed weight with their mothers, there was some similarity in their responses in four interviews. Five girls said their mothers wanted them to eat more, and one said her mother's advice was to eat a greater variety of foods.

About half of the mothers spoke positively of their daughters' body shape or weight, although sometimes it was simply a denial that she was skinny. Two of their daughters repeated this commentary.

Compared to the previous quiiitile, there was slightly more acknowledgement of mothers' compliments, and more matching of non-dieting status within pairs. There was less agreement in mother-daughter reports of dieting, the occurrence of discussions on weight and the content of such discussions. These mothers and daughters seerned to talk about other issues as much as the former group. The lower congruence regarding their weight discussions may have resulted because the mothers' issues were different than their daughters'. Most of the girls were not concerned with weight control. The girls' aligh tl y easier affirmation of compliments accords with this speculation. Just for Good Health

The majonty of mothers (11) gave advice focussed on eating regular meaIs, eating when hungry, eating enough to satisfy hunger, and eating more fniits and vegetables and fewer sweets. Several mothers also talked about the importance of activity for building healthy bodies and healthy attitudes. Here is a sampling of their comments:

FMI: [We don't] really taZk about weight. I just want to know ifshe 's eating properly, to make sure that she S eating. Because I always tell her, men ifshe's not really hungry, "Just have a fruit or vegetable in the afiemoon, and then something to drink I said, "because you'll be tired, " 1said "qyou don't eat

p roperly. "

RM3: She has breakfiast. ... She hus that to set her off in school. She gets money to buy her lunch. Ifshe doesn't buy sornething thut's nutritious, it doesnlt mer. Supper time, she'll have a nurniious meal. Then she has a snack before going to bed. So to me thut's all Z worry about is making sure she eats regular meals.

W6: Z'd like her to be more active definitely. Shek thin, and thin doesn't mean healthy. So I've told her that too. ... but ifyou can have a positive feeling about yourself: not jus? because you're thin, but because you're healthy, because you exercise and you 're involved.

Only two of the daughters of these 11 mothers said they discussed weight or eating with their mothers. SD16's summary of her mother's advice was "Oh, I don't know. And my mom sometimes goes like, 'You have to eut more or something. '" In addition to the above comments, her mother had talked about the importance of eating nutritiously, not

skipping rneals, and not relying on potato chips and coke for lunch. The other girl responded, "Shejust says to not be so pic@ about what you eut. " Her mother had talked

about the food guide, her own weight problems and had advised her daughter that she

shouldn't woqabout being skinny.

Possibly the messages refiect advice given at other times. However, weight did not seem to be very salient for these girls, and it seems likely that the girls did not attend to their mothers' comments. It's also possible that because the mothers' and daughters' body shapes and concems were more varied, the messages were also more varied and therefore harder for girls to grasp. The mothers may not have perceived weight to be a problem for their daughters, and so may not have discussed it with them to any great extent. Weight had become an issue in some families, however, and the responses of mothers and daughters in these farnilies were more consistent.

Womed Mothers

Most of the girls who weighed less than the median seemed to be following their growth trajectory as indicated by discussions of long-term efforts to gain weight, recent gains in height and the lack of dieting and weight control taik in the majority of their interviews. Several mothers indicated they were watching for signs of an eating disorder because their daughters were thin, but most concluded that there was no necessity to intervene. A suburban mother said:

My daughter's thin, you know, because she's growing nght now. She's only 13, and she's grown su ta11 in the last linle while that she is just seeming to be al1 legs and am.But no, I would discuss it witk her if1felt she was being really ridiculous about eating and watching her weight, or thinking she was heavy or something.

One of the Rural mothers said her dentist had told her that her daughter had "more corrosion behind her teeth rhan she should've, and that would only be caused by stomach acids. " This mother said she had talked to her daughter, watched her carefully for signs of bulimia, and three years later she continued to think it was not an issue. She said her daughter was always thin, but had recently "started tofill out, " which was naturally reassuring. She, like other mothers, was concemed about her daughter's eating habits.

Three of the four Urban Aborigind girls in this category had recently lost enough weight to disturb their families. One girl's story was reported in Chapter 5. The second mother, as with the Rural mother above, had watched closely and decided her daughter was doing okay. "1 was womed when she had los? weight so drastically, but then Z did watch. She does eut, but she just eats diflerently now. She eats a lot of more nutrition things, not too many fatty things. No butter whatsoever on anything. Very little bread. "

The third mother tells how she became alarmed at her daughter's weight loss:

I think what had happened before was 2 was just letting her, kind of ta& it for granted that she was eating her meals, and then al1 of a sudden I notice she 's getting really thin. And it happened in a matter of rnaybe two or three month, eh? [Fathec Yeah it didn 'r tuk long at all.] She was losing, losing really fast, you know, yeah. Quite unheaIrhy looking.

The mother said this occurred as her daughter was preparing to write exams and "she was getting al1 stressed out" in her effort to maintain her record of straight As. Beginning to date was another issue that came up, discussed as a pressure that directed attention to appearance. She said her daughter told her she was feeling stressed and expenencing dizzy spells. The mother reported that her daughter is "starting to eut more now. I have to

be fim with her, though. "

Both girls were aware of their mothers' concems. One was concerned herself and trying to gain weight. The other seemed dismissive, reporting that she wanted to lose

eight pounds, and adding "just Ming," a phrase she used repeatedly through her interview. Descriptions of Daughters

More mothers (at least half) indicated they told their daughter that she was fine, or expressed appreciation of their daughters' size or appearance, but it was often expressed as a denial of her "skinniness." Two of their daughters indicated this in their response.

FD3 said, "Z jus? hep walking around the house saying, 'Oh Z'm skinny and I want to gain a little bit of weight. "' 1 asked how her parents responded, and she replied, "My mom tells me, they tell me that Z'm no? skinny, but I think Z am." RMl1 said she told her daughter that she was thin, but she would start filling out soon. RD1 1 said her parents

told her "Theyjus? tell me jus? be happy with the way you look " This was as close as the girls got to repeating the complimentary remarks of their mothers.

Again, mothers aimost always offered compliments reactively, in response to their daughters' concerns. SM15 said, "She's mentioned to me, sorne people makefun of her because they think she'i too skinny, but Z told her that she's no? too skinny. SheS gonna fil1 out. " UM3 talked about reassuring her daughter that she was not big, saying, "Last year Z was talking about 'I'm gaining too much.' And then we noticed that she was starting to say the same things. Z mean luter, you know. Z didn't notice at the time but she was doing rhat, and Z'd say 'No. You 're perfect. You're fine. You 're healthy. "

Most mothers indicated they thought their daughters were doing fine. It seemed that this was so obvious that it need not be mentioned. Considerably more girls with a slighter build rated themselves as "very happy" with their looks than any of the other groups, as indicated in Table 7.4. Table 7.4 Girls' Reported Sausfaction with their Looks by Body Mass Index @MI)

- -- Very happy, Somewhat Neutra1 Somewhat or BMI n % happy % % very unhappy % Highest 15 13.3 46.7 26.7 13.3

Lowest 16 50.0 37.5 6.3 6.3 Other 28 25.0 46.4 14.3 14.3

Average Weirrht Girls

Al1 of the BMIs of the 16 girls in the median weight quintile also feii within

pediatric guidelines for healthy weights (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention,

2000; Cole et al., 2000). The BMIs of the Abonginal girls in this category were 1.6 to 2

units larger than the non-Aboriginal girls. This may have affected perceptions of what a

normative weight or body shape was, in different ways. The First Nations community

participants might judge girls' body weight against the heavier weight noms of their

community. The girls in the snowball sample, on the other hand. would be more likely to

view the lower weights of their non-Aboriginal classmates as normative. (See Chapter 9

for a further discussion each group's perception of attractive, healthy and personally

desireable weights.) About half (9) of the girls' mothers had BMs under 25. One was

under 20. Two were over 30.

Seven girls wanted to be smaller, seven were satisfied and one wanted to gain

weight. A majority of the women (13) indicated they wanted to be smaller, with two

women indicating they were satisfied and one wanting to gain weight. Six participants said they were dieting, including one mother-daughter pair, one family where only the mother said she dieted, and three families where only the daughter reported dieting. In the majority of families (1 l),both said they never or rarely dieted.

Al1 but one mother said she discussed weight or eating issues with her daughter.

Ten daughters also said they discussed weight or eating issues. There was one family where both said they did not discuss weight.

The majority of the mothers discussed nutrition and activity either within the context of weight control or for health with little discussion of weight. Sometimes the discussion focussed almost exclusively on health, with an explanation that weight was not a problem, implying that advice would change if weight changed. Five mothers advised caution. Again, two of the Aboriginal girls reported they had recently lost a fair bit of weight, and it had caused some concern. An additional girl scored above the cutoff for risk on the EAT-26, and several other mothers discussed their own or another daughter's risky behaviours, making risk a strong theme in this category also. A majority of mothea

(1 1) made complimentary remarks to and about their daughters. Four girls said their mothers made such comments, with two pairs matching.

Comments were similar to those discussed already and will not be elaborated.

This chapter will conclude with a look at how mothers discussed their own weight, and their perceptions of their influence on their daughters. Themes seemed similar regardless of girls' weight, and will be discussed for the entire sample. Talkin~about Mothers' Weiaht

Most of the discussion around mothers' weight concemed king heavier than desired. This came up in 61.5% of the 78 families. There were relatively few comments about mothers who were wanting to gain weight (2 mothers; 1 daughter), or unsolicited compliments (2 mothers; 3 âaughters).

Mothers were presented as raising the issue in almost 40% of the families. About one-quarter of the girls (18) stated their mother raised her weight as a problem in a way that required a response. Most of these girls carne from the Suburban location (IO), with the remainder split between the Aboriginal girIs in the two settings.

Girls commonly reassured their mothers they were not fat. For example, FDlO said, "She always says 'Oh, I've gottu rry to lose sume weight. And then Z always tell her

'No, you just lookfine just the way you are. '"Other than using sometimes rather than always, her mother's response is very close, "Sumerimes Z would Say, 'Do you think I shozdd like lose some weighr and ?ha??*She says, 'Mamm you look good the way you are. ' " SDS's response is typical, "Sometimes she says that she thinks she S kind of big and stufi A couple of weeks ago she wen? ouf for dinner, and she was wean-ng this dress, and she said, 'Oh my gosh, I used tom this, and now I can barely fit it. '"And 1jusr keep on

~ellingher that she's no? thar fat at all. " When 1 asked if her mother accepted her advice,

SD5 laughingly pointed out that she continues to remark that she's fat and added, "1 don 't think she really listens to me."

Girls encouraged their mothers in their weight loss efforts and gave advice oriented at weight loss or at dressing to disguise problems. A Suburban mother reported the following interaction with her daughter:

1'11 say things like, "I want to lose weight. I'm too fat. ." I've usked her this before. Just her and I chamhg eh. I'm not obsessed with this, it's just, I've said to her before. "lfyou hud to describe me, what would you say? Ifyou had ?O point me out in a crowd to someboày, would you cal1 me fa?"And she's always said. "No."

Her daughter said her mother would ask her "Do you think I'm fat and st@" And she would tell her "Uh no. and ifyou want to lose weighr then, to start exercising and eating the right foods and stufi" Compliments, coupled with advice to help with weight loss, although sending contradictory messages, seemed intended as support and encouragement. The mothers' responses indicated they perceived this as support.

Sometimes, a girl said her mother's question made her uncornfortable. At other times, advice seemed to be volunteered, as in the following comments:

FM2: They tealways telling me to quit eating and thar IJmgonna be sorry if1 keep eating.

SM2: When I hi pigging out on chocolates, I think she sometimes says something about that. ... Like she 'll just Say, "You are al1 that!"

FD4: Not to stuy home, because when she's home she eats lots.

RD19: Z jus? tell her to exercise more.

These and other similar brief comrnents came up frequently, giving the impression that girls regularly assessed their mothers' weight as in need of remedial action.

As already discussed, sometimes mothers shared their own expenences, hoping perhaps to indicate they understood their daughters' situation, or that their experience would spare the daughter unwanted weight gain, or unreasonable concem about weight.

Girls did not seem to understand the motivation behind this sharing. Perce~tionsof Influence

1asked the mothers, "Do you think the way you feel about your weight affects the way your daughter feels about her weight?" The responses to this question provide a giimpse into motherk perspectives of the influences on their daughters' feelings, attitudes and behaviours towards weight. Overall, approximatel y one-third of the mothers (30.8%) concluded their feelings did not influence their daughters, and almost half (48.7%) that they did.

Most of the mothers who felt they did not influence their daughters' feelings about weight explained that they never talk about weight or suggested the daughter thought for herself or was dealing with different issues. One mother said, "1 don? think so. We're too different. We're cenhtries apart, you know. I'm old [Zaughter]. Thnt's the tnrth. She's in her own little world- " Another said, "M Say, 'Oh, Z've gotta lose some weight to get in these pants, ' or something. But Z wouldn't say it enough that my daughter would ever, ever SV--. 'Cause she's so thin I think that her concem right now is not weighr. " One mother fel t that although she tried to influence her daughter, her efforts did not have any effect: "Not really. She 's very outspoken, and to her, I'm powerless. Like she gaes out and has a treat like àt's nothing wrong with that. " This mother added that she hadn't given up, suggesting that she continued to believe she might yet have some influence.

A majonty of the mothers responded affirmatively. SM19 stated one of the stronges t positions, "1 think that everything you do as a mom, as a role model, aficts what your kids think. And whatever you do, I think the example isfar stronger than what you actuully Say. " This position supports role modelling, whether the orientation is for weight-control or for body acceptance. Mothers cited effects of both-

RM2, who hoped that her daughter would eventually take her weight more

senously, saw her efforts having a short-terni positive effect: "That's probably why she's

trying to do sir-ups. 'Cause she sees me doing aerobics al1 the time. su she might feel a

linle guilty so she tries. " SM8 responded that her exarnple and instruction helped her

daughters set a healthy course, "1 think so. Z think that they know that Z exercise and Z

always tell ?hemyou know, 'Make sure you eut. Make sure you eu? properly. '"

Negaiive effects of weight control were implied by the mother who adrnired her feminist daughter's ideas about dieting when she said "1think when I'm dieting it does. "

SM5 seemed to vacillate between positive and negative effects of her efforts at weight control. She womed about her older daughter mimickïng her behaviour and taking it too fa., saying, "Oh yeah. Z see my 17year-old. and she workr out. She takes my tapes, and she worh out. And she eats salad for dinner. and she's very much like Z am [laughs], which is very scary." However, her next remark suggested her behaviour set a positive example, "Zt makes me want to kind of watch. I don't want to get more overweight and not care how 1 look because I think what Z've been doing has nibbed off on her. She saw

me working out and eating al1 this healthy food and she saw thtI was shaping up. "

Reflecting on nondieting attitudes, FM5 stated a common theme when she said, "1 am overweight and then if1 obsessed about it, and she [say] me duing that, she would probably go the same way. obsess about her weight. Z think it would affect her lots."

UM22S referred to herself *'asa victim to a lot of the fads on exercise. or dieting. " and reflected on a past incident: UM22S: Recently we came across a linle card that was reully interesting about her telling me that if1 went on a diet, everything would be okqy. Like somewhere she got thar message too. [G: Oh.] I don 'r know whether it was from me or my family or a TV told her, you know "Ultra Slim Fast" told her it was al1 gonna be okay-

In addition to the impact of her behaviour on her daughter, her answer realisticall y suggests a multitude of influences. Another Suburban woman spoke of this:

SM18: Z think my daughter gets an earjùl fiom everywhere else. Not just me. 2 mean, she'll get it from herfiends- She'll get it from TV. She gets it fiom everywhere. Al1 women get it. And if2 was excessively over-wom'ed about my weight, then I guess she would pick up on it. But I think she's kind of picked up on my attitude 05 "whatever weight you are is wht you are."

Mothers aiso spoke of specific interventions to counteract influences. Some rnothers spoke of trying to educate their husbands to refrain from making potentially hurtful comments to their daughters, asking granny not to criticize, or talking with the daughter about other people's motives. One rnother said she'd broken up with a long-term live-in boyfkiend to protect her daughter from his influence, explaining:

I broke up because of his unitude towards women. He liked going to see the strippers, and he'd rnake comments about the TV shows, "Baywatch." that kind of stufl And Z told him "I don't want you around my daughter. " ... Because she watches how he responds to women. And ifthut's what she sees men see of women, then that puts more pressure on her.

Some mothers who countered the weight control paradigm looked for, and often found, a similar self-accepting attitude in their daughter. For example, SM6 said, V'snot sornething in Our house that either of us, rn;r husband or 1, talk about really or give much thought tu. And su Z think as a result it S no? something she would concern herself with too much." FM16 said, "Maybe because I dont have an issue about it, she doesn't either. " And RM1 said, "2 don't always make it [weight] the main issue. And hopefilly, she's gonna see tht as well. Like that's not your whole Iife. It's jus your weight. Muk sure that you 're maintaining a healthy li$estyle. ... And Z think she's gotten that."

Several mothers reflected on their experience with their mothers to understand their impact on their daughters, For example, RM13 commented, "I think back to when I was a teenager and some of the things fhat my mom was negative about, Zfind I have to correct myself because those are habits Ipicked up, I think, fi-om my mom. And, I don't want a pattern. I don't want her ta pick up on the habit of being negazive. "

I wondered if girls felt their mothers' feelings about weight affected their own, and added this question to the last interviews. One of 15 responses was affirmative:

"Sometimes my rnom says she's so fat and 2 think 'That's wha? I think about myself sometimes. '" Eleven girls said "no," without hesitation or elaboration, and three didn't know. These girls did not seem to share their mothers' opinion. Harking back to the consensus statements, 85.9% of women, but only 51.3% of girls, agreed that if a rnother was very concerned about her own weight then her daughter would be too. More participants agreed that mothers in general passed on specific concerns about weight than believed such an influence had occurred in their own families.

Sumarv and Discussion

Girls most often mentioned their mothers as an appropriate person to talk to about weight, and about two-thirds indicated they had such discussions with their mothers.

Even more mothers said they taiked about weight with their daughters, and mother- daughter pairs often agreed on this. There was also much agreement on what was talked about, especially as girls' BMI increased. Possibly heavier girls had a pater interest in weight issues. They may have discussed weight at greater length or more often with their rnothers, and may have received more consistent messages. Despite the high levels of agreement, mothers' weight-related advice or dieting example did not predict their daughters' stance towards dieting. More than half of the mothers thought their view of their own bodies had influenced their daughters' views on weight, but fewer daughters said this.

The greatest di sagreement between mothers and daughters was the reporting of compliments. At lest half of the mothers indicated they made complimentary remarks on their daughters' appearance, but only 12 girls repeated their mothers' compliments.

Nichter (2000, p. 156) also noted the "paucity of positive messages from parents." At a later point, she wonders if the message was tmly absent or did parents' compliments fa11 on deaf ears. This research suggests that mothers did attempt to compliment their daughters, but for a variety of reasons, girls didn't register their mothers' approval. First, girls expect mothers to approve of them no matter what, leading some to discount their compliments. Several girls said this, or implied it by saying if you wanted an honest answer you asked your fnends, not yow mother. Approximateiy 90% of both mothers and daughters agreed to the consensus statement expressing a similar sentiment. Even with fiiends, girls commoniy discounted compliments, knowing how often they, themselves, dished out insincere flattery. Most of the mothers' compliments were in response to daughters' direct questions or critical self-appraisals. Pro-active compliments might be more credible to girls. Mothers who wish to deemphasize appearance can compliment their daughters' style and self-expression, and capabilities that depend on the body.

There were two main approaches to weight issues evident in mothers' advice to their daughters. One focussed on the importance of weight control, including weight loss if necessary. The other de-emphasized weight and focussed more on behaviours affecting health. The mothers of the heaviest girls most commonly advised weight control, but some promoted self acceptance. Mothers of the lightest girls largely focussed on health, with or without messages about weight. Mothers also expressed concern or were watchful when girls weighed less than their peers, or when they had lost a lot of weight. These concems were discussed at every BMI level.

The promotion of weight control or avoidmce of talk about weight might seem to be diarnetrically opposed, but there was much that was comrnon to both. Mothers favouring either strategy were concemed with their daughters' physical and psychological health, their overall success and happiness. Both groups recomrnended the same behaviours--healthy eating and regular physical activity. Both groups of mothers recomrnended moderation, expressing uneasiness about some girls' over-concem about weight and resultant unhealthy, sometimes extreme, efforts to lose.

The main difference was the focus or pointed avoidance of focus on monitoring or changing weight. Those favouring the weight control approach believed that restricting food and increasing activity was desirable and would lead to weight loss or weight control. Other mothers seemed to worry that a focus on weight would be more likely to cause eating problems or damage self-esteem than to resolve weight problems. A Parallel Discussion in the Heaith Literature

Approaches to weight are controversial within the health cornmunity as well

(Marchessault, Spigelman, Ornichinski, & Hanison, 1999). The recently published

Manual of Clinical Dietetics, a joint effort of the Amencan Dietetic Association and

Dietitians of Canada (2000a; 2000b; SOOûc), reviews position papers on weight

management in adults and children. The chapter on obesity lists the arguments for several

positions, concluding that different approaches may be appropnate in different situations.

Arguments for vigorously promoting the tradi ti onal treatment goals for obese

clients include the high costs of obesity to society and the enormous personal, economic

and social burdens associated with obesity. Studies indicate that successful weight loss is

possible. although chal~enging.~~Almost dl obese people who achieve weight loss expenence short-term reductions in health risks or medical problems. Although no study

has demonstrated lower mortality with weight loss, benefits are thought to outweigh the

risks. A focus on preventing further weight gain is suggested when weight loss goals,

including a consideration of maintenance, are not seen as achievable. The review cites the

Canadian Task Force on Preventive Health Care's recommendation for weight loss for

adults with BMIs over 27 with CO-morbidconditions. This recommendation was based on

decreased nsks and lower requirements for dnig therapy. The Task Force did not

'qhe studies cited were all based on volunteer sarnples (Bal1 GDC, 1999; Colvin & Olson, 1983; Fletcher, 1W4;Foreyt & Goodnck, 199 1; Schachter, 1982). Volunteer sarnples provide useful data about successful weight loss, but they do not provide generalizeable success rates. Paul Emsberger has suggested the appropriate denominator for these samples is the national population trying to lose weight (personal communication, ApriI29,2000), which creates even more dismal statistics than the oft-cited 95% failure rate for weight loss programs. recomrnend weight loss for obese people without CO-morbiditiesbecause of the poor long-terrn effectiveness of treatment. This is contrasted with recommendations for aggressive treatment starting at BMIs of 25, or less if recent weight gains of 10 or more pounds have occurred. This recornmendation is sirnilar to that of the National Institutes of

Health and the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (1998) to intervene at BMIs of

25 or waist circumferences of 88 cm in women and 102 cm in men, if patients are willing.

In support of the promotion of healthy lifestyles independent of changes in body weight, the Manual argues that weight loss proponents have not addressed "the fact that there is currently no effective, long-term, safe treatment for obesity" (American Dietetic

Association & Dietitians of Canada, 2000b, p. 370). This lack of efficacy, coupled with the pressure to be thin, is said to have created new problems that impair the psychological and social well-king of people who are heavy. Risks of dieting are listed as weight cycling (repeated bouts of losing and regaining body weight) and associated adverse psychological and physical consequences, including higher risk for binge-eating disorder.

Epiderniological data link repeated fluctuations of body weight with increased mortality.

Several recent studies suggest intentional weight loss has no effect on either cardiovascular or total mortality. Emerging evidence suggests that physical activity is more relevant to health and longevity than weight.

The manual then lists and explains a variety of approaches to weight problems, including general dietary recommendations, size acceptance, calorie-reduced and very-

Iow-calorie diets, prepackaged meal programs, fad diets, physical activitylexercise, behaviour modification, phannacotherapy, swgery and al temative weight loss products. Weight issues in children and youth are addressed separately, although not as thoroughly (American Dietetic Association & Dietitians of Canada, 2000a; 2000~).The recornmendations are based on one expert review cornmittee (Barlow & Dietz, 1998).

The main recommendation-to establish healthy eating habits and regular physical activity to maintain baseline weight while growth occurs-is consistent with other reviews

(Canadian Task Force on the Penodic Health Examination, 1994). The manual suggests that children under age seven with BMIs above the 95" percentile, or older children with

BMIs above the 85" percentile (of the National Center for Health Statistics CDC growth charts, one presurnes) who are experiencing complications related to obesity may benefit from weight loss. Clinicians are advised to encourage and empathize rather than criticize, to avoid rapid weight loss, to begin intervention early if the farnily is ready, to involve the family and al1 caregivers, to make small, gradua1 and permanent changes, and to teach families about the medical complications of obesity and how to monitor eating and activity.

Other reviews offer a different emphasis. The Canadian Task Force on the

Periodic Health Examination (1994) concludes that there are few known physical health risks to obese children. It cautions that screening for obesity could result in the child and family becoming anxious, citing reports of parental anxiety about obesity leading to malnutrition in their children. As with the adult interventions, the Task Force's evaiuation revealed that most weight reduction programs have limited long-tem effectiveness. The

Task Force did not recommend for or against screening for obesity, or for or against exercise programs or intensive farnily-based pmgrams for most obese children. It recommended against very-low-kilojoule diets for pre-adolescents. It noted that the

Amencan Academy of Pediatrics and the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force have made simi lar recomrnendations.

The Canadian Task Force on Preventive Health Care, mentioned above, recommended prevention of obesity should be a high pnonty for healthcare providers because of the limited long-term effectiveness of weight-reduction methods (Douketis et al., 1999). This was similar to an earlier position of the Canadian Dietetic Association

(1 988). "Vitaiity," Canada's national weight management philosophy, is based on three principles intended to shift the foçus from weight to health. "Vitality" advises people to

"Enjoy eating well, being active and feeling good about yourself' (Health and Welfare

Canada 1991b; Health Canada, 2000). This approach is recommended for people of al1 ages and for those who need to lose or gain weight for health reasons. The approach is based on expert reviews of the Iiterature (Health and Welfare Canada, 1988b; 1991a).

Qualities that predict or improve success in stmctured weight-loss programs have not been determined (American Dietetic Association, 1997; Dalton, 1997; Miller, Koceja,

& Hamilton, 1997; Miller & Lindeman, 1997). Recent studies suggest that attempting to lose weight is associated with weight gain. The Finnish Twin Cohort Study of 3,536 men and 4,193 women found a weak association between dieting and subsequent major weight gain after 6 to 15 years of follow-up even when initial weight was controlled (Korkeila,

Rissanen, Kapno, Sorensen, & Koskenvuo, 1999). Stice (1998) found that teen-aged girls' moderate efforts to lose weight were associated with weight gain. ûnly girls who made extreme lifestyle changes lost weight, but they were at risk for bulimia. Obviously, parents are not alone in finding weight issues challenging. Some mothers pointed out girls are sumounded by messages about weight. Weight may become an issue regardless of a mother's concem over her own or her daughter's weight. It may be that if mothers want to help their daughters avoid becoming obsessed with weight, they need to confront weight preoccupation head on. A small group of mothers seemed to be attempting to do this and their efforts are explored in more detail in the next chapter. Participants' Advice to Other People

"One is not bom, but rather becomes, a woman" (Beauvoir, 195U1989, p. 267).

This chapter extends the discussion beyond personal weight management strategies. Asking how girls (or women) in general should deal with their weight was intended to elicit participants' opinions, particularly when they did not see their own weight as a problem. Participants readily gave advice making it possible to compare mothea' and daughters' perspectives even though weight was not a personal issue.

Most of the participants' advice seemed aimed at facilitating self-acceptance.

Some participants saw the route to self-acceptance as conditional upon altenng the body; others saw it as more dependent upon altering attitudes towards the body. 1 have labelled these positions weight control and body acceptance orientations. In the weight control orientation, participants gave suggestions intended to assist in weight control G; weight loss. Participants with a body acceptance orientation advised others to strive to accept their body or themselves as they were, or not to worry about weight. Some participants gave both orientations. Others said they did not give advice, accounting for most of the unclassified comrnents. Table 8.1 lists selected responses from pairs of mothers and daughters to demonstrate each possible permutation. Table 8.2 shows the distribution of responses for women, girls and mother-daughter pairs across locations.

Almost half of the participants' recommendations supported weight control exclusively (45.5%)' and almost a quarter recornmended body acceptance exclusively Table 8.1 Advice About Weight to Others from Selected Mother-Daughter Pairs

Responses of both are oriented to weight loss: FMIO: 1 know that it always has to do with exercise. FDIO: Exercise.

Responses of botb are oriented to body acceptance: UMI6S: I don 'r see it as an issue myseg I think society puts zoo much emphasis on it, and al1 that emphasis, time and energy could be put to better things. UDI6S: They should just be happy the way they are.

Responses of both are oriented to both body acceptance and weight loss: SMI6: Whateverfits their schedule. You don 't have to lose the forty pounds, but muybe ten would be fine for their own feelings if that S how they want to be. I think some people are very satisfied with their weight and that S wonde&l. And it is sort of a coming to grips with it- So I think thar whatever you feel cornfortable with, ... then to me, as long as you 're healthy and you're gem-ng out to do something, like walkïng or whatever you're doing. I think that part's important, I don 'r know that the weight is as important as being generally healthy. SDI6: At my age they shouldn't worry about it. If you 're really ovenveight maybe get into sports and stu8 and get active. And gyou 're undenveight and you cm't help it, then don 't do anything about it. Jusr don 'r worry about it that much. lfyou like eut tons then don? eut ut nighr. ... Just eut your meals and snacks in between. Jusr don 't worry about it too much I think.

Mother's response is oriented to body acceptance; daughter's to weight loss: RM3: Maybe itS because the way Z believe, the good lord put you on this earth, your body is gonna come to the size it's supposed to come and thrS it. RD3: [laughs] Go to their doctor's before they do any diet plans-

Mother's response is oriented to weight loss; daughter's to body acceptance: UM22S: I guess education. A cornbitiation of physical activities and I think there S still a lot to look at with weighr issues on how the combination of foods affect weight, dijJerent foodr in difieren? cornbinations. High carbohydrates and meut and potato. ... I still think there S some key- UD22S: Not tu change yourselfto fit evetyone else S expectations.

Mother is oriented to body acceptance and weight loss; daughter to weight 1oss: Sb27: Z can only talk for myselfbut until you have the willpower there S not anything you can do about it. Hopefilly sometime, though, there will be a generation, they 71 like the way they are and it wont matter to them. SD7: Ipersonally feel, just deal with it. [taughs] There are things out there thar can hurtyou, ifyou rake them ifyou 're trying to diet, and there 's things out there that can help.

Mother oriented to body acceptance; daughter to body acceptance and weight los: UM3: It a be nice if they 'd be encouraged to work ut their self-esteem . UD3: That she 's still growing and she shouldn 't have to lose weight or she should sfart eating healthier if she 's really pudgy. [Laughs] Table 8.2 Participants' Advice About Weight to Others by Generation and Location

Weight Body Both Neither Total Generation and Location loss % acceptance % 96 % % Women, N = 78 44.9 28.2 14.1 12.8 Suburban 30.0 25.0 35.0 10.0 20.0 Rural community 30.0 50.0 10.0 10.0 20.0 Urban Aboriginal 50.0 25 .O 10.0 15.0 20.0 First Nations cornrnunity 72.2 11.1 0.0 16-7 18.0 Girls, N = 78 Suburban Rural community 50.0 15.0 25.0 10.0 20.0 Urban Aboriginal 20.0 45.0 25.0 10.0 20.0 First Nations comrnunity 66.7 5.6 0.0 27.8 18.0 Mother-daughter pairs, n 18 6 6 2 32

(23.7%). Both orientations were given by 16.7% of participants. Support for a weight control position in the First Nations cornrnunity participants was high. Only three respondents from this location gave advice that supported a body acceptance orientation.

As the comrnents in Table 8.1 illustrate, mothers and daughters sometimes responded similarly, and sometimes they responded differently. Overall, 32 of the 78 families gave matching responses when categonzed by their major orientation (41.0%).

The proportion of mother-daughter pairs who gave similar answers was as follows: a 50.7% of the 45.5% of participants who gave weight control advice were paired. 32.4% of the 23.7% who gave body acceptance advice were paired. 46.2% of the 16.7% who gave both were paired. a 18.2% of the 14.1% who gave neither category were paired. Becausc there was poor agreement between the advice given by a mother and her daughter," and the advice given in the weight control paradigm was similar to that already discussed, 1 tumed to the interviews promoting body acceptance. This focus is of interest for its potential for interventions with teen-aged girls, as 1have discussed elsewhere (Marchessault, 2000).

The Struegle for Bodv Acce~tance

In 1993, the leader of Weight Watchers in Manitoba wrote a colurnn for the

Winnipeg Sun in which she urged readers to 'Tell yourself that you are OK, just the way you are." She then went on to promise success at weight loss would surely follow. This message ru accept yourself: and then Zose weight, rnight seem contradictory, but it is likely that if asked, many people would suggest both are worthy goals. Eighty people

(50.6% of participants) agreed to both of the following consensus statements:

"If people felt good about themselves, they would accept their body as it is, whatever their size and shape" (91% agreed). "If people felt good about themselves, they would take the time and energy needed to lose weight" (53% agreed).

There was evidence of a struggle for body acceptance throughout the 163 interviews. The difference between the body acceptance orientation and weight control

"Kappa statistics were used to test if the proportion of pairing was higher than would be expected, given the proportion of overall responses categorized in the same category. Kappa was .O5 for advice oriented to weight control, .12 for advice oriented to body acceptance, .36 for those who gave both and .O5 for the no-response category. Results were similarly non-signi ficant for a cornparison of Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal families. The kappa was -11 and .11 for weight loss; -17and .19 for body acceptance, and .23 and .3 1 for both orientations, respectively. This dedout the possibility of opposing trends cancelling each other. orientations was one of frequency and consistency, not complete separation.

Logically, those who both gave body acceptance advice and attempted to practice it themselves, could be expected to have the most consistent perspective. Participants' descriptions of their personal weight history were used to screen out those who were currently trying to lose weight in order to identify interviews with a strong body

acceptance orientation. Twenty-one such participants were identified.35

Mother-Daughter Pairs

Participants with a strong body acceptance orientation were distributed as follows:

4 women, 2 girls from the Suburban site (încluding 2 mother-daughter pairs). 6 women, 1 girl from the Rural site (including 1 pair). 3 women, 4 girls from Urban Aboriginal families (including 3 pairs). 1 woman, O girls from the First Nations cornmunity (no pairs).

The kappa statistic for this comparison was .51 indicating fairly strong agreement beyond that expected by chance for this comparison. Mothers with a consistent promotion of body acceptance were more likely to have daughters who expressed a similar philosophy.

This post-hoc analysis finding of association contradicts most of the other findings, and is offered in the exploratory tradition as of sufficient interest to ment further re~earch.~~

Two-thirds of the girIs who gave both orientations had mothers who gave at least

'This included pariicipants who did not give advice but expressed a striving towards the body acceptance position for themselves.

'6To be consistent, 1 also compared participants who both gave weight control advice and were currently trying to lose weight as per their openended explanations of how they managed their weight. The kappa statistic, although approaching significance, indicated poor agreement at .30. some body acceptance advice in their answers. There were no cases where mothers gave mixed advice and their daughters gave body acceptance advice. There were, however, 15 girls who raised some degree of body acceptance whose mothers did not. This suggests the likely direction of influence is from mother to daughter and that mothers' discussion of body acceptance leaves an impression on girls, but also that girls have other opportunities to lem about body acceptance.

Nichter (2000, p. 39) reported similar responses from the girls she intewiewed but interpreted them as indicative of the adolescent conflict between their struggle for autonomy and their need to give and receive social approval. This may be the case for some girls, especially those who waffled back and forth. However, the agreement of participants to similar opposing statements on the more neutrally phrased consensus questions, the number of wornen who responded similarly, and the increasingly common presence of such comments in descriptions of weight loss programs suggests that more than adolescent angst is operating. The mixed responses may reflect some confusion in the girls as well as the women. Although the discussion was seldom pure body acceptance, there was sufficient emphasis on this orientation to explore how this emerging paradigm (Berg & Marchessault, 2000) was interpreted by participants.

The unmatched mothers and daunhters. It is not possible to determine the reasons why mothers and daughters gave similar or different advice. However, severai observations seem pertinent. There were eight mothers who gave strong body acceptance advice but whose daughters did not. The daughters could be characterized as follows:

Four expressed linle concem about weight. Two were trying to lose weight (one "just watching, " one "eating less, doing more "). One girl started crying while describing her father's comrnents about her eating habits. The mother described this as well- One had an EAT-26 score above the cutoff for nsk of an eating disorder.

This Iast family Iived in the First Nations cornmunity. The mother articulated clear body acceptance advice (not quoted due to confidentiality concems). Her daughter reported that her mother was not trying to lose weight, and gave her advice to just eat healthy, be active and not worry about what other people thought. Both mother and daughter described how classrnates, grandparents, the father, and various other relatives di warned her that if she ate too much, she was going to get as fat as another young girl in her school. The mother was very concerned about her daughter's extreme efforts to lose weight and was trying hard to protect her from these potentially hannful concerns. Her message seemed to be overwhelrned by the innumerable others the daughter was hearing.

The one unmarched girl who promoted body acceptance had a mother who emphasized body acceptance particularly when she talked about her communication with her daughter. The mother was not included in this group because she was attempting to lose weight.

Size Issues

While it might seem to be easier to promote body acceptance if you are smaller, this was not always the case. BMIs for both girls and women ranged from less than 20 to over 30. Interestingly, there was oniy one participant (a woman) with a BMI in the 25 to

27 range, while there were seven with BMIs over 27. Since there were a similar number of participants in both BMI ranges, this suggests a BMI of 25 to 27 might be particularly difficult to accept, possibly because at the lower range of heavier weights, weight loss continues to be viewed as a viable option.

Nor does a strong promotion of body acceptance indicate that the participants were necessarily satisfied with their own bodies. It was quite obvious that most were not.

A strong body acceptance orientation indicated only that these participants were struggling to accept their bodies rather than trying to change them. A comparison of their current and desired weights indicated that 14 of the 21 wished to lose weight (from 5 to

82 pounds), two wished to gain (10 to 20 pounds), and four said they were satisfied.

Some larger women taiked about an active and on-going stmggle to achieve body acceptance, which they viewed as critical to a reasonably happy life. This sometimes came after a long and painfully unproductive struggie to lose weight. Some of the wornen who viewed themsetves as too thin reasoned that because they could not gain weight, weight loss must be just as difficult. Several mothers had corne to focus on body acceptance, not for themselves, but because their daughters were struggling with weight and self-esteem issues.

Self-Acce~tanceand Health Issues

Self-acceptance at any size and health were major themes discussed by participants with a body acceptance orientation. A third theme, control, will be contrasted with the perspectives of participants with a weight control orientation.

Self-acceptance and health were ofken raised together, as can be seen in some of the following quotes. Advice from mother-daughter pairs is cited first, with the mother's

advice, followed by her daughter's. (See also UM16S and her daughter whose quotes

appear in Table 8.1.)

SM9: 1just feel that people have to accept themselvesfor who they are, and that itk really unfair to try and box people into something that some people, say five percent of the population or whatever it is, achieve naturally. I just don k think that the rest of the world should be trying tofrr themselves into that mold. 1 think we should be far more accepting. ... Like I said about other cuitures, there S nothing inherently great about any one particular look Ifyou could value al1 the different types, and jus? be pleased that there is al1 this diversity, and be able to celebrate yourseij and not worry so much about that. Just try and do what you need tu do tu be healthy and$?.

SD9: They should just not be so obsessed with how theirfiends' ... Everyone's gsnna have dzfferent shapes, and that's wha~makes them interesting. Zfwe al1 were exactly the same bodies a~zdstufi it would be not that interesting. They should take that as kind of a good thing. They look diflerent than theirfn'end.

RMS: I think self-esteem. Because I've been heavy al1 of my lqe, I'm the person Z am and Z know how Zfeel about myself It probably took a lot of years to get tu that, but I'm comfortable with myselfand 1 thinkpeople, maybe who are thinner to star? with, ifthey tend to gain weight, it's a big change for thern.

RD5: I think they should accept themselvesfor who they are. Unless you have like a problem, then you should get help. [G: Whut kind of a pi-oblem?] Like if you're overweight or underweight to the point that it's causing a health problem.

UMISS: I think if it 's gonna affect their health, then I think they should take care of it. But I think ifthey're healthy at the weight they are, Z think that'sfine.

UDI5S: I think they should just eut properly and just forget about it. ... It doesn't matter--what they look like.

Participants suggested the importance of self-esteem, that one should not worry about what other people think, or drew on the relative unimportance of looks, often in cornparison to qualities of personal character. Ignoring or discounting the importance of the body isn't quite the same as accepting it, but worrying about what other people think about your body plausibly weakens body acceptance. Their advice lays a foundation for

body acceptance.

The struggle to accept your body's deviation from the popular ideal, the

internalization of that ideal, or even a personaily constmcted variation of it, came up,

particularly when people discussed their personal weight history. Severai participants

cornmented on the diffkulty of sustained weight loss, and concluded that it was probably

best not attempted. More women than girls related such experiences. We have already

discussed one girl's conclusion that her crash diet was "a stupid thing to do" since she

had re-gained al1 of the lost weight "and more. " Here, the women relate some of their

thinking behind their own conclusions that weight loss efforts were not appropriate:

RM4: Some people just have very metabolic diflerences and, metubolism, genetics al1 those kinds of things, that are kind of givens forpeople. Z guess that can be modified somewhat by diet or eating style and exercise pattern. [G: Somewhat?] Yeah there's not much justice in it. [Laughs.] Zr seems like some people can get away with sedentary lijiestyle and eating whatever they please, and others con 't get to have any treats mer without gaining but they're just kind of pre- dispositioned to body size. ... I don't think everybody could decide that they were going to weigh a hundred and twenty pounds and do it. Some people jus? aren't meant to be. ... And I also don't think it's worth the bother. [Luughs.]

RM5: We al1 have sort of basics where we 're meant to be and rhere's extremes on either side of W.... You can probably control anything. ... But at whar cost? Where's the cost involved? Health-wise. Everything. Z think ifyou try to go beyond a certain point, you 'rejust doing more harm than good.

SM4: Z tried and tried and tnèd for years, and al1 Z ever did most of the time was get very frustrated. And the more frustrated 2 got, the worse it was for me. SoJ 1 got to the stage where Z leamed to deal with it. And I became a lot happier with mysew

A qualification implying the necessity of weight loss if health seems to be affected by weight was a common stipulation. RM3, cited in Table 8.1, articulated a clear position on the immutability of the body and the futility of trying to change it: "You don'? control thar as far as Z'm concemed. You can't control that. ... Once you reach the point the good

L.urd expected you to be ut, that's asfar as you're gonna go and that's al1 there is to it. So to me ifs unchangeable. " Recognizing that some people do succeed in losing weight, however, she continued, explaining why this did not alter her view: "1 can see losing a few pounds or adding a few pounds. 1 can't see a real drastic change, and ifthat happens

... is it gonna stay off? Like ifit's a real rnega change."

Despite this strong position that body weight is essentidly unchangeable, she prefaced her answer with "Unless it's a health issue Z can't see why anybody would want

to change. " This draws out a common contradiction: If you're motivated by health concems, changing the unchangeable body becomes an acceptable goal.

RM4 also started off by considenng health as a possible exception: "1 guess I don't think if [what you weigh] really does matter, I mean unless there's health concerns. " However, she continued:

RM4: But I think ifthere is, then we should be concerned about Our health not about the weight. Lots of people are very heavy and don 'Z have health problems as a consequence. So it's unfortunate that women put their lives on hold sometimes because 05 well, weight preoccupation.

People who were advising body acceptance were not advising giving up health goals. On the contrary, some suggested that losing the focus on weight loss helped them to achieve a healthier lifestyle. Some expected or hoped that weight wouId adjust naturally as a result of their efforts, but, as with the advice to their daughters, most stressed that heaith, not weight loss was their goal. Participants were clear that they cared about their health. Several raised health issues as reasons to alter their lifestyle:

UMI6S: Z'm wom-ed [aboud my weighz simply because my family S prone to diabetes. So Z know Z have to watch and not over-indulge and I'm in the process of learning about the diabetic diet. My mom was diugnosed a year ago. so I've been learning with her kind of: That would be my concem. Not so much as the weight. Zt would be more of the health. ... I've sturted making some of the changes slowly.

Another Urban Aboriginal woman also spoke of changing her diet and walking her dog on a daily basis because of a family history of health problems.

UM22S: &art diseuse runs in both sides of my family. That 3 how rny father died. And my mother had a heart atrack before she wasfifir. So Z looked ar that and said "Well 1 think it's a matter of tirne not of a matter of if1 will. " Z'm very high n'sk.

S he, too, suggested, "I don't necessarily think skinny or thin, but just physically strong. whatever that looks like. ... Z don 't think big muscles but just strong physically. ... I don't think it necessarily means you're skinny. I think big people are also healthy. "

One participant commented on how healthy she was despite king larger: "1jus? had my physical, January 19.and my doctor zold me, I'm asflt as a horse. Z might be a big horse, but Z'm as fit as a horse. And I'rn probably in better shape than most of the women she sees who are slim. " She then discussed her personal experience with weight being used as a rneasure of fitness in her job:

We used to have to mn a mile and a halj ... In the 20 years Z was in the military, I've never failed my physical fintess test. And Z smu girls that I played bal1 with, in the military. They were ta11 and slim, and ha1fwu-y through the test. they'd be, "Z can't do this any more." And they would give up. And I'm looking ut them. "Z'm twice your size and Z can do mine. Whar's wrong with yuu?" ... But about the las? jïve years that Z was in. they went more to the BMI. ... They wentfiom one extreme to the other extreme, to now everybody's supposed to look like Arnold Schwarzenegger. ... Slim, physical fit, strong.

This participant was critical of the military's substitution of the BMI for the fitness test. She, who could run a mile and a half, would not have passed the weight test, but her thin friends would pass even though they could not complete the ninning test. The confounding of weight, fatness and fitness is also apparent in SD9's reference to a movie she had seen:

SD9: And ir was a rnother, who was really fat, and she hadn 't walked or went anywhere. She sa? in the same spot for seven years or something. And then she, died because she jus? moved upstairs. She diedfiom tht. [G: Thar was too much exertion?] Yeah. So, I think that ifyou 're like really, really fat it S bad for you.

Although a sedentary lifestyle is likely to increase body weight, it seems plausible, even likely, that a fit and equally fat lady would not have died ftom moving upstairs. SD9 ignored my implication that it was inactivity that was the problem, and continued to emphasize that the woman in the movie died from being fat. Later, she commented that

"Some reullyfit people are heavier, " a comment which recent research supports. It has been found that men and women with poor fitness levels, but no other nsk factors have greater overall mortality than those with moderate-to-good fitness levels and multiple nsk factors, including king overweight (Blair et al., 1996). This confounding of body weight with fitness and eating habits, cornrnon also in the health Iiterature, may explain the frequency with which health issues were raised dong with body acceptance.

Control Issues bv Orientation

Participants' attitudes towards control ais0 seemed pivota1 to the difference between a weight control or a body acceptance orientation (see Table 8.3). Although asked as an open-ended question, participants promoting body acceptance were most likely to respond "sometimes" or "tu a certain extent "when asked if people could control their weight. Even when they said that it could or could not be controlled, they qualïfied their responses in this way. A slight majority of participants with a weight control orientation thought that most people should be able to control their weight most of the time, and generally seemed much more certain of their responses.

Table 8.3 Perspectives on Control from Participants with Different Orientations

Orientation n Mostly yes % Mostly no % Sometimes/to an extent % Body acceptance 21 23.8 28.6 52.4 Weight control 34 52.9 8.8 44.1

Responses of ~artici~antswith a bodv acceptance orientation. Some participants

explained that if you got your other problems under control, or if you ate well and got

exercise, your weight would naturally maintain its appropriate level, although there was

some scepticism about this. SM9, the Suburban mother who talked about the value of

di versi ty, ex p Iained, "lf you 're sort of reasonable about health and frrness. then you '11 be fine. You won 't be either tem'bly ovenveight or tem-bly underweight. ItllL just--happen.

[Laughs.] Wouldn't thnt be nice?" The long pause, her laughter and her ironic comment

"Wouldn't rhnt be nice?" may suggest she's aware that if you eat well and are active,

your weight will not necessady be fine. SM19 said "Zfyou eat healthy and you practise a

healthy lifestyle, then you'll be at your ideal weight. But I don't think thar to stay thin

should be the focus for doing th." Although she accepted a direct link between lifestyle

21 1 and weight, she put the emphasis on the behaviour, not the weight.

Both these women ended up concluding that weight could sometimes be controlled or could be controlled to a certain extent. As already stated, these were the most cornrnon responses from the participants with a body acceptance orientation:

RMS: Probably everybody has a basic weight that you were meant to weigh. Mine was probably 40 pounds ago. And, that would be easy [to] maintain, so. ... I think tltat for me, Z could control that to a degree. But Z could never be thin. Z don 1 think thar is in my genetic make-up.

SD9: I guess sometimes you could. Say someone just sat around and never exercise[d] and ate al1 this food. And that would be controlling weight by not doing anything about it. But also, some people can 't get thinner. ... Bigger people think that they could be like thinner people, but they can't because that's just nut the way they're built.

SMI9: Whether or not they eut a hgof potato chips [laughs] a night would be under control. What genes you're born with are not under control. Z really think you're boni with a certain physicalfi-ame and you can do two things with it. You can either accept it and work with it, or always go against it. [G: Hmm. That sounds like you've done a fair bit of thinking about this issue.] Only because I would say that my girls were born with a very dzTerent Lframe J, and Z never wanted to make them feel like they were d~rerent.

Some participants' attitudes to the control issue have been included in the quotes already cited, particularly those who were most certain that weight was not something that individuals could control. Recall the participants' discussion of the difficulty of sustaining weight loss, often based on their personal weight histones. Participants referred to metabolism or genetics, or made comments such as, "They're just kind of pre- dispusitioned to body size, " and "You don't control that as far as Z'm concemed. ... So to me it 's unchangeable. '* Others explained that people did not have as rnuch control over their weight as they thought, or emphasized circumstances under which individuals did not have control over their weight:

RM16: Ifyou te obviously an overweight person because you te inactive and eating poorly, I think then you cmdo something about it. You have sorne people that they ire been larger as children and they tegrown into larger adults. 2 meun itk a life-long problem. It isn 't as easy to get a handle on. And maybe you never will.

UD9: Ifyou had a big family, then you have a good possibiliq of going big yourself: [G: And you wouldn t be able to do anything about that?] Z don L think so- May[be], you might. I don 't think so though.

UMISS: Because believe me, Z wouldn 't like to be as heavy [laughs] as Z am now, if1can control it. I just think people get comfortable in their lifestyles.

RM7: No, not really. I'm not saying to sit at the buffet and just pork out. I guess they 'd have control there. That 'd be the only place [laughs].

Examples of the mostly yes responses are represented with comments such as:

RM6: Z hope they could. [G: MmmHmm.1 Well, yeah qthey want, Z suppose they could. Maybe there 's a few cases where they can 't control iî, Z suppose if they have a--a--1dont know [laughs] Z'm not a doctor.

SD21: ZtS probably not always true but, probably, 7 out of 10 times it is.

FM: Yeah, they have conrrol over it. [G: Always, would you say?] Hmm. Yeah, because well they 're sticking anything in their mouth, tu you know feed you, 2 think they always have control over their weight. [Not identtped to preserve confidentialityl

The degree to which respondents qualified their answers, even when they were generally responding negatively or affirmatively charactenzed the overall responses to this question with a sense of uncertainty or hesitation to make a strong pronouncement. Res~onsesof artici~antswith a weinht control orientation. Thirty-one of 34 participants from this onentation responded that weight was something people could control, at least somewhat. The three girls who disagreed did not elaborate. The responses differed largely in the degree of certainty that weight could be controlled. First, comments from those who said weight could sometirnes be controlled:

SDI 7: [Laughs.] Like Z said before, if you fe lazy you can control it, and if it k in your genes, you cmfight it, but you can 'r control it.

SD20: Maybe ifthey work out, they 71 lose weight and then right after that, they like won 't go eat junk food. Maybe ifthey fe hungry they 71 eut something healthy instead of like having to gain al1 that weight back that they jus? los?from working out.

UM4: Most of the people 1know they 're skinny, not too big. ... They only gained a few pounds here and there but they still lose. They bse it afrer awhile by walking around. 1 guess by exercising, controlling it, and jus? watch what you 're eating.

UM14S: I think some people can control their weight and others can % G: What makes the difference between those who can and those who can 't? UMIQS: Biology and genetics. Like Z know a lot of ovenueight women who are ovenveight, Z think directly related because they were sexually abused as youth and so there 's a different issue in that. ... Whereas other people Z know ... they tte just been really chubby babies, and just never lost it. Like they 'rejust overweight and to try to lose that weight. as adults would probably be very self-defeating.

The majority of responses in this category indicated certainty that weight was something that could be controlled:

FM2: Oh yes. [G: Yeah. Always?] Z think so. Yeah, ifyou really, like really put your rnind to it. Z believe it can be controlled.

RM21: 1personally have a very dimcult tirne. But 2 guess, [sigw it would seem you should be able tu control it. IrS very diflcult but [laughs] logically you should be able to control it* right?

SB:If they really tried to. G: Are there any circurnstances under which they wouldn'r be able to? SD5: HmNo. I can'i think of anything. [loughs.]

SD8: They jus? have to eat right, and exercise- [Laughs.]

W2: Yeah. I think ifthey really wan~edto they could control [it].

Much of the advice for weight loss and these comments about control imply that heavy people currently have unhealthy eating and activity habits, and that if they improved their lifestyle they would lose weight and their health wouId improve. These assumptions are also evident in scientific studies and advice given by health professionals

(National Institutes of Health & National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, 1998; World

Health Organization. 1998), although it has been dificult to establish that heavier people cumently eat more or expend less energy than lighter people (McCabe, Mills, & Polivy,

1999; Miller & Lindeman, 1997; Teufel & Dufour, 1990). Problems in establishing the long-tenn impact of intentional weight loss on health and mortality have aiready been discussed.

One participant, recognizing the difficulty in correcting weight imbalance, recommended catching it before it became a problem. She is one of the few in this category to emphasize that weight loss is difficult and requires constant vigilance. FM4 said "lt'ssomething thut you have to work with rïght away. Don't let it get too cam-ed away. And if's sornething you can't let down. You have work on it at al1 times. Well, you can control it ifyou're willing to work on it at al1 times. "

Another participant also stated the difficulty and feasibility of weight loss in the same breath. She was optimistic, but ended on a cautionary note:

RMl7: Look ut rhe picture andfind out whut is causing it. Be curefil if you do Lose weight. Just take it one day ut a time. Itk a hard job. but there are good benefisjkorn it. You do see the weight come OB;you know, little by liîtle. And Little by Little the clothes get smaller on you and then when you look in the mirror -- yes you can start feeling bener about yourself:Zt will lifr your self-es~eemup. But there's also scary benefirsfrom it too. You can get cam-ed mvoy with it and you could end up gettïng sickfiom it

Comments about the need for willpower come closest to dealing with the lack of

success that most people have when they attempt weight loss. As SM7 (cited at the

beginning of thjs chapter) put it, "1 can only ralk for myselfbut until you hm>e the

wilLpower there's not anything you can do about if." Several girls said, "Just deal with

it. " RM19 says "Z would think you should just, ifyou really feel very strongly about it. just rake it into your own hands and do something about it. "

These familiar comments do not sound extrene or unusual. They do, however, distract from the difficulties inherent in weight loss and place the focus on the individual's ability to persevere. Comments about willpower admit or imply the difficulty of weight loss and then move quickly to focus on success in terms of self-control.

Even those pronoting body acceptance did not discuss the immense difficulty of the weight loss venture. Few people focussed on what is actually involved in weight loss.

One participant recomrnended "jus?. kind of conte to peace with yourself; " and then in considenng why this was so difficult, she expressed resentment over umeal expectations being set by older models:

UMIOS: The only thing that bothers me about comrnercials is like Z said, Gddie Hawn is fifry years old, and shets like, "Z've got a body of like a eighteen year old. " That really bugs me. 'Cause it's not natural. You have to work dam hard to have a body like that nt$". And I just don't feel like I'm going to have the energy or the time when Itmfifry 20 do tht. Summarv and Discussion

When asked how girls or women should deal with their weight, alrnost half of the

participants (45.5%) recommended lifestyle changes onented at weight control or weight

loss. About half of these cautioned against extreme measures to lose weight. Participants

recommended watching what you eat, cutting down on food, and increasing exercise.

Consistent with their advice, a slight majonty of the participants with a weight loss

orientation responded that most people should be able to controi their weight and most of

the remaining participants said that it could sometimes be controlled. The First Nations

comrnunity participants were most likely to recomrnend weight control actions-

About one-quater of the participants recommended attempting to accept your

body as is, with attention to food and exercise for the purpose of maintaining health rather

than losing weight. A change in health status could alter this position for some

participants, although others suggested that it was appropnate to focus on the behaviours

rather than the weight even when weight was assumed to be negatively aff'ting health.

Some participants, drawing on their own experience, were sceptical about the possibility

of lasting weight loss. About one-third said that weight loss was not under the

individual's control. Others thought that if you got lifestyle correct, weight would follow,

and therefore the focus should be on lifestyle, not weight. Most thought you could control

weight "to an extent, " but some added that the arduous effort to achieve weight loss

might not be worth it. Although frequently discussed as an achieveable goal if one is sufficiently disciplined, if examined in the context of the enormity of the daily effort required, this ordinary-seeming goal looks quite extraordinary?'

A further 16.7% supported both these positions, often saying they supported whatever the individual wanted to do. Occasionally, participants suggested that a person should accept their body and attempt to change it. About half of the participants (50.6%) agreed to a statement linking self acceptance with body acceptance and another linking self acceptance with weight loss. These responses suggest that self acceptance may not suggest body acceptance to people. The slogan of Canada's national weight management prograrn is "Enjoy eating well, king active and feeling good about yourself. That's

Vitality" (Health Canada, 2000). The text accompanying the Vitality promotion makes it clear that Tee1 good about yourself' is intended to communicate the acceptance of your body. The Vitality information links body image with the other components of a healthy lifestyle, moving from body acceptance to taking care of yourself and accepting the weight that results. Vitdity stresses this approach even for people who need to lose or gain weight for health reasons.

There was much agreement in the advice given by mother-daughter pairs, but cornparisons were not significant with one exception, a pst-hoc analysis of a small group of participants with the most consistent body acceptance orientation. This may suggest little difference in a daughter's orientation to weight in families where the mother is

"~heoretically, an energy deficit of 500 calories per day results in one pound of weight loss per week. A weight loss of one to two pounds per week is frequently recommended. Because recidivism is almost universal, some recomrnend lower weight loss goals (e.g. 114 pound per week). Assuming the formula works. a modest weight loss of 25 pounds would take 12.5 to 100 weeks, or up to 700 days of being slightly under-nourished in tems of energy balance, with continued lifestyle monitoring to maintain the loss. neutral about or supportive of weight control. A mother's active promotion of body acceptance may play a role in her daughter's consideration of the same advice. Given al1 the other factors promoting the importance of weight to girls, whether or not a mother is weight preoccupied may be less influentiai than active carnpaigning to prevent weight preoccupation. This is offered as an area for Merexploration. While this was the one area where a cornparison of the mothers' and daughters' weight perceptions were significantly associated, the number of such families in this study was small and the andysis was post hoc. It also raises the possibility that high background similarity is masking a srnall degree of mother-daughter similarity in other respects.

The next chapter makes statistical cornparisons based on the three administered questionnaires. There is sufficient power in these tests to respond to some of these concerns. This final results chapter looks at the associations between a mother and her daughter's scores. It also tests for differences between the generations, the Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal participants and the urban and rural participants. Analysis of Three Questionnaires

"The Nature of This Flower 1s to Bloom." Alice Walker.

This chapter describes the participants' responses to three questionnaires frequently used to study weight issues: (a) a modified version of the Restraint Scale, (ô) the Eating Attitudes Test @AT-26)' and (c) a questionnaire based on the Body Shape

Drawings (BSDQ). As reviewed in Chapter 3, these scdes measure (a) dieting tendencies,

(b) risk for an eating disorder, and (c) perceptions of and satisfaction with body shape.

The emphasis of the analyses in this chapter is to test for differences in specific weight- related attitudes between the groups and for association in the responses of mothers and daughters.

The hypotheses king tested are as follows:

1. There will be differences in mean (or median) responses on the Restraint Scale, the

EAT-26 and the BSDQ in comparing:

Generations (women and girls). Self-Identified Ethnicity (Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal). Regions (urban and rural). Locations (Surburban, Rural, Urban Aboriginal and First Nations community).

2. There will be an association between mothers' and daughters' test scores.

3. Cornparing girls with the 10 highest and 10 lowest Restra.int scores:

Mothers of the high-sconng girls will score higher on the Restraint Scale and the EAT-26 than mothers of the low-scoring girIs. There will be an association between the girls and their mothers' Restraint and EAT scores in these 20 families. Samvle- Size and Power Considerations

The qualitative portion of this investigation determined a sample size goal of 20 mothers and 20 daughters at each of the four locations. This in turn set the sample size for regionai and ethnic cornparisons at 40 (within each generation), and for generational cornparisons at 80. The minimum detectable difference, assuming two-tailed testing, a

Type 1 error probability of .05,80% power, and using the standard deviation of the collected data (Hassard, 1991), is presented for these sample sizes for paired and unpaired comparisons in Appendices H-1 and H-2. Power calculations are based on assumptions for parametric testing and will be lower than indicated for the non-parametric tests.

Comparisons between locations would have the power to detect a minimum mean difference of 2.9,4.7 and 6.1 for the Modified Restraint Scale, Restraint Scale, and EAT-

26 respectively. There would be sufficient power to detect differences in scores of approximately 0.5 body shape for most of the BSD questions, but only one body shape for the question conceming perceived body shape. Differences of approximately half this size could be detected with the largest samples. Slightly smaller differences could be detected in paired samples of comparable size.

The critical value was set at -01 as a reasonable compromise between type 1 and type II errors. Although multiple questions were asked, Bonfernoni cntical values were not used? Bonferroni values guard against the danger of abandoning an old treatment for

-

'qhe Bonferroni value is .O04 for analyses of the four locations for two generations). This was calculated as follows: .05/c; where c = number of cornparisons The number of comparisons = k x (k- 1)/2; k= number of groups (Hassard, 199 1). A cntical value of .O1 with 12 comparisons provides a .89 probability that results are not due to chance (.99 raised to the power of 12; personal communication, P. Martens, June 25, one that may not be better, which is not a concern in this research. Missing differences was considered equally as problematic as erroneously concluding there were significant differences when none exist. An enor in either direction would lead to misunderstanding people's concern about weight, Results at the p s- -05 are reported as non-significant trends to highlight patterns in the data that merit fürther research.

Data Analvsis

Data were analysed using the Number Cmncher Statistical System 1997 (NCSS

Hintze, 1997). Scatterplots and descriptive statistics were processed for each scale.

Nonparametric tests were used when the data were not normally distributed (Hasard,

199 1). Correlations between two measures of weight (BMI and perceived body shape), the two versions of the Restraint Scale and the EAT-26 are given in Appendix 1-1 for girls and 1-2 for women. These results are based on al1 respondents unless otherwise indicated. This includes three women in addition to the 80 mother-daughter pairs. There were no significant differences in test scores of the random and snowball Urban

Aboriginal samples, and so they were combined for these analyses. Five families where the daughter's ancestry was different from the mother's were omitted from al1 comparisons involving ethnicity in a paired sample. This includes the univariate and multi-variate analyses of the Modified Restraint Scale, Restraint Scale and the EAT-26.

High scores on the EAT-26 were not considered outliers because they recur in

2000). Given the exploratory nature of this research, this was deemed acceptable. A critical value of .O5 provides a .54 probability that results are not due to chance. This is unacceptable for individual comparisons, but may be useful in highlighting patterns. most studies (Garner & Garfinkel, 1979; Leichner et al., 1986; Rosen et al., 1988b; Wood

et al., 1992). As such, they represent valid proportion of the sample and removing them

would create a bias. Nine Restraint questionnaires with one missing response were

included in the analysis of the full Restraint Scaie. These scores were generally above the

average score, so omitting them would have created more bias than including them. Eight

questionnaires (4.9%) with more than one missing response were omitted from the

analysis of the full Restraint Scale. These were distributed throughout the ~arnple?~There

were no missing responses on the Modified Restraint Scale or the EAT-26. There were

from 1 to 11 missing responses per question on the BSDQ? A maximum of 6% of

respondents gave either the midpoint or a range of shapes in their msponse to a particular

question. The rnidpoint was used in this analysis.

Restraint Scale

The distribution of responses to both versions of the Restraint Scale met the

assumptions of nomality and equality of variance when tested with a one-way analysis of

variance (ANOVA). Participants' scores were included in a one-way ANOVA to examine

the univariate influence of generation, ethnicity, region and location, for each scde

separately. A split-unit or repeated measures ANOVA included region and ethnicity in the

'qhe distribution of missing scores was: three women, one in each group except the First Nations cornmunity; five girls, one in each group with two in the First Nations communi ty. 'OMissing responses were distributed as follows: perceived - 1 girl; desired - 2 girls women attractive - 4 girls, 4 women; girls attractive - 5 girls, 6 women; guys prefer - 1 girl, 2 women; women healthiesr - 3 girls; girls healthiest - 4 women. mode1 using family to nest generation. Non-signifîcant terms were dropped in sequence.

The research design was unbalanced with 19,21, 17 and 18 pairs of mothers and

daughters for the Modified Restraint Scale, and 18, 18, 17 and 13 pairs for the Restraint

cale? NCSS generates approximate F-tests for unbalanced ANOVA designs.

Eatin~Attitudes Test

The distribution of results for the EAT-26 did not meet the assumptions of

normdity or equality of variance when tested with a one-way ANOVA. There were high

scores in four groups. Consequently, the EAT-26 was analysed with the non-pararnetric test, the Kmskal-Wallis. There is no non-parametric equivalent of the spli t-unit ANOVA.

The Wilcoxon Signed Rank Sum test with the continuity correction factor was used to

test for differences in EAT-26 scores in mother-daughter pairs.

Conelations. The association between a mother and her daughter7sscores for the

Restraint Scales and the EAT-26 were calculated, again using Pearson's comlation coefficient and Spearman's rank correlation coefficient for normally and non-normaliy distri buted scores, respective1y.

Comparin~Families of Hi ~h-and Low-Restrained Girls. Two subgroups of farnilies were formed. The first consisted of the 10 girls with the highest Restraint scores

"NCSS is unable to handle missing values in the split-unit ANOVA (Hintze, 1997). A filter was used to screen out rnissing scores. Both rnother's and daughter's scores were excluded if either one was missing (relevant only for the full Restraint Scale). and their mothers. The second group consisted of the 10 lowest-scoring girls and their mothers. Two-tailed t-tests were used to test the separation of the two groups of girls.

One-tailed T-tests were used to test if mothers of the high-scoring girls scored higher on the Restraint Scale and the EAT-26 than mothers of the low-scoring girls. Pearson's product-moment correlation coefficient was used to assess the association between the 20 pairs of girls and their mothers for scores on both scales. This portion of the analysis is a partial replication of Hill, Weaver and Blundell (1990) and therefore the full Restraint

Scale was used. The analysis was repeated with the Modified Restraint Scale and non- pararnetnc tests, where appropriate.

Bodv Sha~eDrawines Ouestionnaire

The non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis test was used to assess differences in responses to individual questions by location. The Mann-Whitney U test (corrected for ties and with the correction factor for continuity) was used to assess differences by region or ethnicity. Groups were combined only if there were no significant differences between

them (using p s .O5 as the critenon).

Logistic regression analysis was used to test if generation, ethnicity or region

(with up to three-way interactions) could predict the likelihood of yedno responses to the

following questions: (a) 1s desired body shape smaller than perceived body shape? (b)

Are participants making the same selehion for desired and healthiesr body shape? and (c)

Are participants making the same selection for the shape they find most attracrive and the one they think guys prefer? Participants' responses for their own generation were used for questions referring to most czrzracz:'ve and healthiest. If no significant differences were indicated, the responses from al1 participants were combined and a chi-square test was used to assess responses within a question set. The agreement between a mother's and daughter's level of body satisfaction (a cornparison of responses to the first question listed above) was assessed using methods devised for establishing inter-rater reliability. A kappa value above -4 indicates pater agreement than would be expected by chance

(Fleiss, 1981).

Results

Restraint Scale

Girls' and women's mean Modified Restraint scores were 4.83 (SD of 3.18) and

5.68 (SD of 3.23), respectively for the 75 mother-daughter pairs. (Fwther descriptive statistics are given in Appendix J.)

There were no significant differences between the generations (Z = 1.58, df = 148, p = .12). In separate univariate analyses for each generation, there were non-signi ficant trends by ethnicity (H= 3.74, df = 1, p = -05, using Kniskal-Wallis due to suspected non- normality) and location (H= 8.95, df = 3, p = .03, differences were specified as between the Rural and the First Nations cornmunities) for the girls, and by region for the women (t

= 2.22, df = 73, p = .03). (See Appendix K for details.)

In the split-unit ANûVA, generation (the nested term) was not significant (F =

2.82, df = 1,74, p = .10 N.S.). There was a non-significant trend for the interaction between ethnicity and region (F = 4.36, df = 1,71, p = .04). Figure 9.1 illustrates that the First Nations community participants (noted as nuai Abonginai) had higher mean

Modified Restraint scores than the theother locations. (See Appendices L-1 and L-2for the ANOVA table and the results of the Tukey-Kramer MultipleComparison test.)

The results of a similar analysis of the full Restraint Scale are given in Appendix

M 1-4. Mean Restraint scores for girls and women were 9.25 (SD of 5.17) and 11.75 (SD of 5-20},respectively. In univariate analysis, there was a non-signifcant trend by

14,001 REGION O Rural A Urban

c 10.00- -E - Fie- C3 Y a 6.00: O Q iE .L 2 = 2-00:

-2.wi 1 Nm-AboriginaiAboflginal Ethnicity

Figure 9.1. Mean scores for Modified Restraint Scale. Higher scons indicate pater dietary restraint. The maximum score is 19. Results are hma Split Unit Analysis of Variance with generation as the nested term and ethnicity and region as factors (F = 4.36, df = 1,71,p = .04; N = 75 mother-daughter pairs.) generation (t = 2.47, df = 142, p = .02; women scored higher). The Aboriginal girls scored significantly higher (t = 2.97, df = 66, p = -004). In multi-variate analyses, there were differences by ethnicity (F = 8.94, df = 1,64, p = .01), and by generations (F = 6.49, df =

1,64, p = .01). These results are confounded by actual body weight (higher Restraint scores for heavier participants). Different slopes for the weight measures precluded controlling the influence of weight in the Restraint Scde comparisons.

Eating Attitudes Test

The median EAT-26 scores for the girls and women were 4 and 3, respectively?

Figure 9.2 plots the rnedian scores for girls and women by location (with further descriptive details in Appendix N). A Kruskd-Wallis test on ranks was used to test for univariate differences. There were no significant differences between EAT-26 scores by generation (H= 1.58, df = 1, p = .21).

Univariate analyses for each generation by ethnicity, region and location found significant difference only for locations for the women (H= 11.21, df = 3, p = .01). Rwal women were specified as different from rhe women at the three other locations. The greatest difference was with the First Nations comrnunity. (See Appendix O for details.)

A Wilcoxon Signed-Rank Test of medians found no significant differences between paired mother-daughter scores (z = 0.91, p = -37N.S. for the 57 families without

'?For comparative purposes, the mean EAT-26 score was 6.75 (SD of 8.39) for the girls and 4.66 (SD of 4.88) for the women. Generation 0 0 Girîa A Wornen

Location

Figure 9.2. Median scores of Eating Attitudes Test-26 by generation and location. Higher scores indicate greater concem about weight. The maximum score is 78. (N= 75 mother-daughter pairs).

the Fint Nations community; z = 0.65, p = -51 for the 58 pain without Rurai families; z =

0.60,p = .55 for non-Abonginal families; z = 0.76, p = .46 for Aboriginal families.)

Results were similarly non-significant at each location.

Nine of 163 participants (5.5%)scored above the cut-off for risk of an eating disorder, distributed as follows:

21%of First Nations community girls (4 of 19), 14% of Urban Abonginal girls (3 of 21), 5% of Suburban girls (1 of 20), and 5% of First Nations community women ( 1 of 19). Mother-Daughter Com~arisons

Correlations. There were no significant correlations between a mother and her daughter's scores on the Restraint Scales or the EAT-26 for dl pairs of participants, or when examined by ethnicity, region or at any location (see Appendix P). For example,

Pearson's correlations for the Modified Restraint and Restraint scales for the 80 families were .IO (p = .36, N.S.) and -03 @ = -78, N.S.), respectively. Spearman's correlation was

.O4 (p = -73, N.S.) for the EAT-26. These correlations indicate the variability in Restraint or EAT-26 scores shared by mothers and daughters was very low, less than 1%.

Cornparine Families of Hieh- and Low-Restrained Girls. Analysis of the Restraint scores confirmed the separation of the 10 highest scoring girls from the 10 lowest scoring girls (t = 21.21, df = 18, p < .00001). The mean scores for the high and low-restrained girls were 18.2 (I 0.65 SE) and 2.2 (+ 0.39 SE), respectively. The Restraint scores of the mothers of these two groups were not significantly different (one-tailed t = 0.00, df = 18, p = .50, N.S.). The mean score of the rnothers of both groups of girls was 11.4 (_c 1.3 1 SE for mothers of high-scoring girls; I1.77 SE for mothers of the low-sconng girls). The correlation between the Restraint scores of the rnothers and daughters was low and non- significant (Pearson's r = -.01, p = .98).

Separation was also confirmed for the EAT-26 scores of the two groups of girls (t

= 4.14, df = 18, p = .0006).The highly restrained girls had a mean EAT-26 score of 20.4

(A 4.0 1 SE). The low-restrained girls had a rnean EAT-26 score of 3.5 (k 0.73 SE). Again there was no significant difference between the mothers (one-tailed t = -0.54, df = 18, p =

.30 N.S.). The mean EAT-26score of the mothers of the high-scoring girls was 3.5 (k 1.48 SE). The mean score of the mothers of the low-scoring girls was 4.7 (k 1.64 SE).

Pearson's conelation coefficient for EAT-26 scores of mother-daughter pairs was -.14 (p

= .57, N.S.). The results were similar when the anaiysis was repeated using the Modified

Res train t Scale and non-parametric tests (results not shown).

Bodv Sham Drawinm Ouestionnaire

Girls' and women's responses to the BSDQ are given in Table 9.1. More than

80% of participants chose D or E as theK response for ai1 questions except their own

Table 9.1 Responses of Girls and Women to the Body Shape Drawings Questionnaire

Question Participant responses (%)

Girls: n = 80 Perceived Des ired Women amactive Girls attractive Guys prcfer Wornen heaithiest Girls hcalthiest

Women: n = 83 Perceived Desired Womcn attractive Girls attractive Guys prcfcr Women healthicst Girls healthiest O 114835 5 00 0

23 1 perceived body shape. Overall, 69% of the girls (N =79) selected D or E, with 2 1% choosing heavier and 9% choosing lighter shapes. Sixty percent of the women (N= 83) selected drawings E or F to represent their current body shape, with a further 26% choosing heavier, and 148 choosing Lighter shapes.

Figure 9.3 displays the median response to each of the seven questions for girls and women. Girls' median response was E forperceived body shape, D for their desired shape, the most nitractive and the healthiest shape for girls and the shape guys prefer.

They selected E as the most attractive and healthiest shape for women.

Figure 9.3. Median responses to Body Shape Drawings Questionnaire by generation. (Only the relevant portion of the figure scale is shown, including 4 of the 9 figures.) Girls' responses are graphed above the line; women's below. Answers pertaining to the other generation are bracketed. Letters in superscnpt =fer to the mode, given only when it differs from the median. Differences between generations were assessed with the Mann Whitney U test and significance is indicated as follows: """ p < .000001; "" p < .0001; 0. p < .01; * p 1.05.) Women's median response for theirperceived body shape was between E and F.

They selected E as their desired shape, the most attractive and healthiest shape for women

and D as attractive and healthiestfor girls and the shape guys prefer.

Generations were compared with a Mann-Whitney U test unless otherwise noted.

Women chose significantly heavier selections forperceived (z = 5.50, p c .00001) and

desired body shape (z = 3.89, p = ,0001). Girls chose a slightly heavier shape as healthiest for women (z = 2-90'p = -004). There were non-significant trends for three other

questions: Girls selected a larger body shape as attractivefor women (z = 2.17, p < -03

N.S.) and smaller shapes as attractivefor girls and the one guys prefer (Kolmogorow-

Srnirnov test, Dmn = 0.22, p = -04 N.S.;z = 2.08, p = .OS, N.S., respectively). There were

no significant differences between girls and women in their choice of healthiest shape for

girl^.'^ (The details of these tests are given in Appendix Q.)

Looking at the differences by ethnicity, the Aboriginai girls selected a heavier

perceived body shape (E) than the non-Abonginal girls @; Mann-Whitney U test, 2 =

3.35, df = 72, p = -0008).There were no other significant differences between Aboriginal

and non-Aboriginal girls (see Appendix R) .M

43~heKniskal Wallis Multiple-Comparison 2-Value test indicated medians for the women were different at p .O5 on al1 questions except healthiest for girls. Normally, this precludes combining groups for further tests. However, the medians for mothers and daughters varied in a consistent direction. For example, women at each location perceived their shape as heavier than girls. Since it is the relationship between women and girls that is being compared, the locations were combined and tested.

Wisis a widely discussed point, therefore the distribution of choices was examined in more detail. About 5% more Aboriginal than non-Aboriginal girls selected heavier shapes as healthiest. About 78more Aboriginal girls selected lighter shapes as healthiest. Fewer Aboriginal girls (5.7%) selected heavier shapes as attractive and As with the girls, the Aboriginal women at both locations selected a heavier median response (F) than the non-Aboriginal women (E) to represent theirperceived shape (Mann Whitney U test, z = 2.81, df = 76, p = -005).There were non-significant trends for Aboriginal women to select slightiy heavier shapes than non-Aboriginal women for desired body shape, most attractive for girls, and healthiest for girls (Mann

Whitney U test, z = 1.98, df = 76, p = .OS; Kolmogorow-Smirnov test, Dmn = 0.30, df =

71, p = .05, z = 2.35, df = 74, p = .02, respectively). There was no significant difference by ethnicity for the shape participants thought men would prefer. Responses for the shapes that were most attractive and healthiestfor women could not be tested because differences between Suburban and Rural women precluded combining the groups me differences between Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal women are shown in Figure 9.4.

Details of the tests are given in Appendix S.)"

There were no significant differences between the urban and rural regions for the girls for six of seven questions. Differences between perceived body shape were not tested because of differences between Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal girls.

Regional differences for the women could not be tested for six of the seven

BSDQs because differences between the Suburban and Urban Aboriginal women precluded combining groups. There were no significant differences for the question that attractive to guys, and more (-5%) selected lighter shapes as attractive tu guys. This does not provide any evidence that Aboriginal girls prefemd heavier shapes than non- Aboriginal girls.

45~pproximately5 to 7% more Abonginal women selected heavier shapes as healthiest, most attractive and attractive to guys than the non-Aboriginal women. More Aboriginal women (-7%) also selected lighter shapes as attractive to guys. Figure 9.4. Median responses to Body Shape Drawings Questionnaire by self-identified ethnicity for women. (Only the relevant portion of the figure scale is shown, including 4 of the 9 figures.) Non-Aboriginal women's responses are graphed above the line; Aboriginal women's bdow. DiHerences were assessed with the Mann Whitney U test and significance is indicated as follows: '0 p 1.01; ' p 5 .OS; indicates response was not tested (due to differences between Suburban and Rural locations at p s .OS). could be tested-guys ' preference.

Median responses to the BSDQs are graphed by location in Figure 9.5. Only questions with a significant difference (or trend as indicated by p .05) between locations or ethnicity are shown. (Appendices T and U give test results for girls and women. respectively .)

For the girls, perceived body shape di ffered significantl y by location (H= 12.38, df = 3, p = .006). The Kruskal Wallis Multiple Comparison Z Value test specified the

Urban Aboriginal giris differed fiom the Suburban and Rural girls @ -01for Urban Location Ethnicity (p val-) ls: 0.006 0.0008

Suburt#n, Ur- Aboriginal Rural First Nations Hlomen's Most Attraftive for Wanyg: 0.04NS X s&utmuE" UttanAborigbalw -Ruml First Nations

Women's Most 0.OSNS 0.05NS Subudm Urban AôoriginaI m7 FiNations

Figure 9.5. Median responses to Body Shape Drawings Questionnaire by location and self-identified ethnicity. Letter on the line graph indicates participants' median response to question at the left. Only relevant figures and questions with differences at p .O5 are shown. Lines join locations that are different. "indicates p .01. X indicates not tested because groups to be combined differed (at p .OS). Superscript gives the mode when it differs from the median. The Mann Whitney U test and the Kniskal Wallis test assessed for differences by ethnicity and location, respectively. Abonginal Rural cornparison), The First Nations community girls also differed fiom

Rural girls.

For the women, there were non-significant trends for differences between the

locations for perceived body shape, most attractive and healthiest for women and most

attractive forgirls. (H=8.19,p=.04N.S.;H=8.51,p= .04;H=7.84,p= .OS; H=

7.75, p = -05, respectively with each test having 3 degrees of freedom).

The Suburban women selected a siightly smaller shape as most attractive for

women compared to the one chosen by the Urban Aboriginal women (p < .01, Kruskal

Wallis Multiple Comparkon Z Value test). They also selected a smaller median shape as

most attractive than the Rural women (a non-significant trend). The Suburban women's

median c hoice for the healthiest shape for women was smaller than all three other

locations, and their selection for their desired shape was smaller than the Urban

Aboriginal women's. The Urban Abonginai women selected a heavier size than the

Suburban and Rural women as their perceived size and also as the most attractive and

healthiest shape for girls.

The differences, as shown in Figure 9.5, seem to be pattemed. Girls differed only

on perceived body shape. Women differed to some extent between locations on al1

questions except for the shape they thought men preferred. Suburban and Urban

Aboriginal women varied on six of seven questions. Urban Aboriginal and First Nations

cornmunity women did not demonstrate significantiy different responses (or a trend) for

any of the questions. There were no significant differences between the First Nations community and the Rural community women either. ([irban Aboriginal women varied from Rural women on four questions; Suburban women varied from Rural women on two and from the First Nations community women on one question.)

The differences between women in different locations have implications for the differences between women and girls at the individual locations (See Appendix V). All groups of women selected heavierperceived shapes than the girls at the same location.

This was highly significant for the non-Aboriginal participants and was a non-significant trend for the Aboriginal participants. The Suburban and Rural women' s desired body shapes were not significantly different than their daughters, whereas the Aboriginal women chose heavier desired shapes than their daughters (p = -007for Urban Aboriginal and -02,N.S. for First Nations community participants). The Suburban women selected lighter shapes than their daughters as most attractive and healrhiest for women @ = -002 and -004, respective1y). The Urban Aboriginal women selected heavier shapes as most anractive @ = .02)and healthiest @ = .03, N.S.) for girls than their daughters selected.

These patterns parallel the differences between locations for women.

Next, logistic regession (with generation, region, ethnicity and up to three way interactions) was used to predict probable responses to the following questions:

1. Is desired body shape smaller than perceived body shape (or not)? 2. Are selections for desired and healthiest body shape the same (or not)? 3. Are selections for most artractive body shape and the shape guys would prefer the same (or not)?

Results of the logistic regression modelling were: Only the first relationship, conceming body dissatisfaction, was significant, with

pneration and ethnicity remaining in the equation (see Figure 9.6). Overali, 70% of

respondents' indicated their desired shape was different from their current perceived

shape. Sixty percent of respondents desired a smaller shape, and 1070 wanted to be

larger. Seventy-two percent of women wanted to be smaller compared to 46% of girls

(Odds Ratio = 3.2, confidence limits = 1.6,6.4). Seventy-three percent of Aboriginal

participants wanted to be smaller compared to 51% of non-Aboriginal participants (Odds

Ratio = 2.7, Confidence Iimits = 1.3,5.5). Dissatisfaction, as measured by the discrepanc y between percei~~edand desired body shape, ranged from a low of 25% for

Rural girls to a high of 82% for Urban Aboriginal women.

Figure 9.6. Proportion of participants selecting a larger perceived than desired body shape by ethnicity and generation. The logistic regression analysis indicated no significant differences by generation, region or ethnicity for the other comparisons, so responses were combined. a 39% chose a desired body shape different from the one selected as heulrhiesr (27% wanted to be smaller than what they perceived as healthiest); a 43% seiected different shapes for guys' preference and their own choice for mm arrractive shape (33% indicated men's preference was smder han their own).

Each of these responses was significantly different than would be expected had the participants given simila.responses to both questions. (Chi square 2 = 309.9 and 446.3, respectively with df = 2, and p < .ûûûûûû for both). See Appendix W for further details, including an analysis that indicated the words desired and attractive have different meanings for most participants)-

Last, the measure of body satisfaction mentioned above (perceived - desired) was compared to test if mothers and daughters responded more similarly to each other than would be expected by chance. The results were: a Kappa = .30 for the Suburban mothers and daughters (n = 20). Kappa = .18 for the Rural mothers and daughters (n = 20). rn Kappa = .29 for the Urban Aboriginal mothers and daughters (n = 19). a Kappa = -.19 for the Suburban mothers and daughters (n = 19). a Kappa = -18 for al1 mothers and daughters (and -29without the First Nations community families; n = 78 and 59, respectively).

The results for First Nations mothers and daughters were contrary to the direction of the other locations, and therefore kappa values were calculated for the overall sample with and without their responses. Results were non-significant at each location and for both tests of the overall sample.

Table 9.2 summarizes the major findings reported in this chapter. Table 9,2 Summary of Findings for Body Shape Drawings Questionnaire, Modified Restraint Scale and the Eating Attitudes Test (EAT-26) by Location, Region, Ethnicity, and Generation

Body Shape Drawing Questions Woinen Girls Guys Women Girls Shape Modi ficd Perceived Desired Attractive Attractive Prefer Healthiest Healthiest ~issatisfaction~ ~estraint~ EAT-26' Girls

Women Location T -- T T -- T -- X FNcT T Reg ion X X X X -- X X ..- Interaction --

Ethnicity S* T X T -- X T *ilr Interaction --

Generations ** JI i T T T rlr* -.. ** -- -- Mother-Daughter Association -- ..- --

** p p' -01; T Non-significant trend (p < ,05); -- p > ,05, X Not testeâ because of differences between groups to be combined (at p ,05) were significantly different. Dissatisfaciion with body shape was measured by subtraciing desired body shape from perceived body shape; b~ultivariatcanalyses; Univariate analyses. Summarv and Discussion

Generation

The results of BSDQ indicated body dissatisfaction was more prevalent arnongst women than girls. There were no significant differences between the generations on either the Modified Restraint Scale or the EAT-26, although eight of nine high EAT-26 scores were in girls. This indicates more extreme levels of concem about weight in some girls.

There were differences between girls' and women's median responses on six out of seven of the body shape drawing questions. As anticipated, girls selected smaller median shapes than women for their perceived and desired shapes (significantly different) and as preferred by guys (non-significant trend). Girls also selected a larger shape as healrhiest for wornen than the one selected by women (significantly different). Each generation selected a slightly heavier median as most attractive (trend) for the other.

There were no significant differences on the shape seen as healthiesrfor girls. Both generations represented their perceived size as larger than the shapes selected for desired, most artractive, healthiest and the one guys prefer.

Overall, 70% of participants selected different shapes for their perceived and desired body shapes, with 60% of the total sarnple wanting to be smaller. Women were more likely than girls to want to be smaller (72% vs 46 %), probably a reflection of women's larger size.

Taken together, these results suggest that women are more dissatisfied with their body shape than girls, however more girls are concemed to an extreme degree as shown by the distribution of EAT-26 scores, and the unhealthy behaviours used to lose weight discussed in the interviews. Other studies have repoxted simïlar or pater weight dissatisfaction or dieting in women than in girls, but have not compared measures of risk for the different ages (Lafkmce et al., 2000; Rozin & Fallon, 1988; Thelen & Cormier,

1995).

More than a quarter of participants selected desired body shapes that were smaller than those they selected as healihiest, suggesting, as others have found, that attractiveness rather than health is often the primary goal (Cockerham, Kunz, & Lueschen, 1988;

Garner, 1997). This did not Vary significantly by generation, ethnicity or region.

That participants chose an attractive body shape smdler than their own perceived body shape is not surpxising. However one-third of participants' selection for the shape guys prefer was smaller than their own selection for attractive. This did not vary significantly by generation, ethnicity or region. Rozin and Failon (1988) found little difference between coliege women's own choice of attractive body shape and what they said men would find attractive. They found women exaggerated men's preference for thinness in wornen compared to men's actual choices. They speculated that women may be misinformed about men's preference, or may be infemng the elite male preference from television and magazines. This perception may have more impact on girls than women, given their interest in dating. The cornparison of male and female views merits further investigation. It offers potential for ameliorating concem in some girls.

Looking at differences across location, girls differed significantly only on perceived body shape. There was a trend to differences between locations for women on al1 questions except for the shape they thought men preferred. That girls at al1 locations chose sirnilar figures for desired most artractive and healrhiest, despite differences in their mothers' selections, suggests other influences on girls than simply their mothers' choices.

Correlations between mothers ' and daughters' scores were consistent1y Iow and non-significant for each scale across the entire sample (Pearson's r = -10,p = .36 for the

Modified Restraint Scde; Pearson's r = -03, p = -78for the Restraint Scale; and

Spearrnan's r = -04,p = -73for the EAT-26). Correlations by self-identified ethnicity, region or individual location were also low and non-significant, ruling out the possibility that opposing trends in the subgroups were cancelling out important effects. There was no compelling evidence that mother-daughter pairs gave sirnilar reports of body satisfaction

(or dissatisfaction) at any location or overdl (kappa for the overall sample = .18). Nor did pairing mothers and daughters increase the explanatory power in the split unit ANOVA of the Modified Restraint scores, as would be anticipated if there were similarities in restraint within families.

To rnaximize the ability to detect effects, an analysis modelled after that of Hill,

Weaver and Blundell(1990) was carried out. The mothers of the 10 girls who scored highest on the Restraint Scale were compared with mothers of the 10 girls who scored the lowest. There were no significant differences in Resuaint or EAT-26 scores between the two groups of mothers. In fact, the mean Restraint scores were identical, and the median

EAT-26 scores ran contrary to expectations. The mothers of the hi@-sconng girls scored Iower than the mothers of the low-scoring girls (N.S.). There were no significant associations between mothers' and daughters' scores in the analyses of these 20 families.

The associations were negative, aithough again non-significant.

Hill, Weaver and Blundell's (1990) results were in the expected direction for both scales. The findings were significant for the Restraint Scales (r = -68).They concluded there was "a strong family link between mothers and their 10-year-old daughters in their motivation to diet" (p. 347). No such conclusion arises from this study despite an initially larger sarnple, similar analyses and a good separation of the two groups.

The cross-cultural nature of this sample may have influenced results. It is possible that parental influence is more important when culture is stable and children can rely on their parents for culturally appropriate information. This could be tested with other cultural groups experiencing transition, for example immigrant families. It is also plausible that 10-year-old girls are more similar to their mothers than 13 and 14 year-old girls. In a subsequent study with girls closer to age 12, findings were similar to those of this study (Hi11 & Franklin, 1998).

These results do not provide any evidence to suggest that girls leam their concern about weight from modelling their mothers' weight concems, at least not at this age.

Ethnicitv. RePion and Location

A higher proportion of Aboriginal than non-Aboriginal participants indicated they were dissatisfied on each of these measures. The First Nations community participants scored higher on the Modified Restraint Scale, (a non-significant trend in the split unit ANOVA). This is consistent with the higher prevalence of and support for dieting reported earlier.

There were significant differences in the EAT-26 scores by location. The Rural women scored lower, indicative of less concem, than the women at the other three locations, and this achieved significance for the cornparison with women in the First

Nations community. More importantly, the prevalence of scores above the cutoff for nsk of an eating disorder was concentrated in Aboriginal girls. Seven of 40 (17.5%) had high scores. One of 40 (2.5%) non-Aboriginal girls scored this high. One of 19 First Nations community woman also had an at-risk score (5.3%).

The prevalence of at-nsk scores in the Aboriginal girls is comparable or higher than the rates for same-aged girls reported by others. Leichner and colleagues (1986) reported a rate of 23.7% in a Manitoba sample, Wood and colleagues (1992) reported a rate of 9.2%. As noted, the higher non-response rate in the Suburban and Rural samples likely leads to an under-estimation of prevalence. Girls were not promised confidentiality in the event of a high score, so this may have prevented some girls from disclosing eating problems, Ieading to further under-estimation. For these reasons, the finding of higher prevalence of at-nsk scores in the Aboriginal girls than the non-Aboriginal girls is not valid. However, the results indicate that Aboriginal girls living in and within dnving distance of an urban centre are vulnerable to weight pressures.

High scores are an indication for referral, not confirmation of an eating disorder.

Al1 of the Aboriginal girls with high scores discussed severely restricting their food intake or vorniting to control their weight. The EAT-26 sometimes raised issues not discussed in the interviews, but clarified in a foilow-up interview. High scores were discussed fmt with the participant, and then in the case of girls, with their mothers or a counsellor. The

EAT-26 had face validity as a screening tool for senous eating problems with the

Aboriginal samples used in this research.

Comparing individual's choices for perceived and desired body shape, the

Abonginai participants were more likely than the non-Aboriginal participants to want to be smaller (73% vs 51%). The discrepancy was greater in the women (83% and 62%) than in the girls (66% and 33%).

Aboriginal girls selected a larger perceived body shape than the non-Aboriginal girls. This was the only difference in the median responses to the BSDQ for the girls (and it was highly significant). The non-Abonginal girls' median perceived body shape was consistent with their other selections, but the Abonginal girls' selections for desired, mosr attractive, attractive to guys, and healthiest were al1 smaller than their perceived. This discrepancy would explain the higher levels of body dissatisfaction in the Aboriginal girls. The results did not provide any evidence that Aboriginal girls preferred larger shaped bodies as attractive or healthy.

Aboriginal women also perceived themselves as heavier (significant) and selected heavier s hapes than non-Aboriginal women for their desired body shape (trend). Women have been reported to adjust their personally desired shape in accordance to what they realistically feel is possible for them to achieve (Allan, 1988). This is suggested here in that participants made different selec tions for their desired body shape and the shape they viewed as rnmt attractive. The finding of a heavier desired body is probably linked more to Aboriginal women's heavier weight noms than to heavier ideals for attractiveness (see

Appendix W). The Urban Aboriginal women's selection of heavier shapes for most attractive and healthiesr for girls, however, does suggest some preference for, or at least acceptance of, heavier shapes. This was weak (non-significant trends) and requires fwther research for confirmation. There were no significant differences between Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal women's selection of the shape that men prefer. Differences between

Suburban and Rural women precluded testing for differences between Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal women in their choices for most atrractive and healthiest shapes for women. The analysis by locations adds some pertinent information.

There were differences between the Suburban and Urban Aboriginal women on six of seven questions (at the p < -05 Ievel). By contrast, there were no such differences between the First Nations comrnunity women, and either the Rural or the Urban

Abonginal women. That is, most of the differences were between the Aboriginal and non-

Aboriginal women in the city, with no differences between Aboriginal and non-

Aboriginal women in the rural setting or between the urban and rural Aboriginal women.

(Urban Aboriginal women varied from Rural women on three questions; Suburban wornen varied from Rural women on two and from the First Nations community women on one question. These differences precluded testing for regional differences on al1 but one question for the women. Similar differences precluded testing one of the questions for the girls. All perfomed tests were non-significant for both girls and women.)

These results suggest that only in the city did Abonginal women prefer larger sizes as heavier and more attractive. However, it may be more appropriate to Say that the Suburban women preferred smaller body shapes than the other women (Rural as well as

Aboriginal women) than to focus on differences between the Aboriginal and non-

Aboriginal participants. Suburban women seemed to have namower personal ideals for slenderness as inferred from their smaller median selections for desired, most attractive and healthiest shapes for women. They also had a smaller median perceived body shape than the women at the other three sites- Additionally, it was the Urban Aboriginal women who seemed most accepting of heavier shapes as most atîractive and healthiest for girls, rather than First Nations community women. Consequently, it is not clea.from the findings of this study that Abonginal women preferred larger sizes as attractive and healthier. Sample size is small for tests by location, and the cornparisons are complex for an ordinal scale and parametric statistics, so further research is needed to clarify these results.

There are widespread anecdotal references to First Nations peoples' preferences for heaviness. Several studies report results that support these impressions, but these are studies of older men and women, often diagnosed with a chronic disease, and often living in more isolated communities (Garro & Lang, 1993; Hickey & Carter, 1993). Results quite plausibly could differ in such different samples.

Gittelsohn and his colleagues (1996) studied a sarnple that included younger First

Nations people living in an isolated Northem Ontano community. They also reported the

Ojibway-Cree people surveyed preferred relatively larger body shapes compared to those reported in the literature for Anglo populations. They recognized this cornparison was problematic because the usual population surveyed is school-aged and college-aged women. They used the same body shape drawings (although as an interval scale). The results can quite easily be compared with the findings of this project.

The Ontario girls selected a mean perceived current body shape slightly bigger than E, and mean desired and healthiest shapes that were slightly smaller than D.

AIthough a broad range of ages (10 to 19) was incIuded, this is consistent with the choices of the Aboriginal girls reported here. The 30-39 year-old-women chose a mean perceived current body shape of F, sirnilar to the Manitoba Aboriginal women. The

Ontario Aboriginal women's mean selections for desired and healthy shapes were slightly smaller than D. These choices were sirnilar to or smaller than the median choices at al1 locations in this study. When compared to same-aged samples, the Ontario Aboriginal girls and middle-aged women did NOT select larger desired and healthy body shapes than non-Abonginai girls and women.

There have been sirnilar findings of greater dissatisfaction wi th body weight in

Native youth and women in several recent studies in the United States (for a summary, see Marchessault, 1999). These studies find higher levels of self-reported potentially dangerous methods of weight control in Aboriginal people than in non-Aboriginal people, regardless of age. This is the fmt report of a higher level of concern about weight for

Canadian Aboriginal people, other than my earlier study (Marchessault, 1993a).

Aboriginal participants living in or close to the city are already concemed about weight and weight control. These results, particularly in combination with the responses given in the open-ended interviews and the results of other studies, suggest vulnerability to weight preoccupation exists in Aboriginal girls and women. Teaching Ophelia to Swim

'This above al1 - to thine own self be me; And it must follow, as the night the day, Thou canst not then be false to any man" (Shakespeare, 1604/1963,1.3.78).

Two of the women interviewed for this project mentioned they had read Mary

Pipher's (1995) popular book, Reviving Ophelia: Swing the Selves of Adolescent Girls.

Pipher, a clinical psychologist who works with troubled girls, takes Hamlet's Ophelia as the touchstone for what happens to girls as they reach adolescence. Pipher sees a universal theme in Ophelia's transition from a happy and carefree girl to a young woman who loses herself in her attempt to please others. Ophelia falls so deeply in love with

Hamlet that she lives only for his approval. She sacrifices her "self' to please Hamlet, a sacrifice that leaves her with no resourçes when he rejects her. She goes mad and drowns, ever so elegantly, dragged down by the weight of her slcirts in a flower-strewn Stream with no evidence of any struggle to Save herself.

"To thine own self be mie," Ophelia's father advised her brother. However, he urged his daughter to please Hamlet, a direction that leads to Ophelia's demise. Pipher interprets this scenario based on the work of psychologist Carol Gilligan and her colleagues who argue that our society continues to encourage this sacrifice of self in girls.

Their research with adolescents suggests that somewhere between the ages of 10 and 13, girls begin to lose their authentic selves in their efforts to fit into cultural ideals of femininity that endanger their psychological health (Brown & Gilligan, 1992; Gilligan, Rogers, & Tolman, 1991). In Our society, the importance of appearance and thinness to fernale identity and women's success is seen as central, as portrayed in Eric Stice's sociocultural model of influences on bulimia (1994). An adapted version of this model was presented in Chapter 2 to demonstrate the myriad factors that influence weight perceptions in addition to the mother-daughter relationship. Although my purpose was not to test Stice's model, the results of this study have implications for its applicability. In this final chapter, I sumrnarize the findings with respect to the main research questions, suggest their policy implications and end by suggesting promising areas for further research. 1 will start by reviewing the three methods used and how they influenced the results.

Limitations and Strengths of the Study

The purpose of this project was to discover the extent to which Aboriginal and non-Abonginai girls and their mothers shared understandings about weight. Families were selected frorn an urban centre and from rural communities within driving distance of this centre. Sample selection allowed cornparisons to be made within mother-daughter pairs as well as between the two age groups, and the Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal participants at urban and rural locations.

Three data collection methods were used to explore agreement and variation within and between these groups. The first two, the dual interview design modelled on the work of Garro (1996), provided complementary sets of data. The open-ended portion of the interview allowed participants to raise salient issues in their own words. The language they used and the emphasis they gave certain ideas provided clues to cultural meanings

(Fine, 1994). These meanings provided the framework for understanding participants' perspectives and informed the analysis of the more systematically elicited data. The fixed- structure values statements, analysed with cultural consensus theory, facilitated comparability between the various groups by obtaining responses to a standard set of statements. These statements were based on an earlier set of interviews with similar-aged inner city participants and were written to preserve their perspectives. The three questionnaires, on the other hand, were drawn from published psychology studies. Their useage allowed an assessrnent of the extent of restraint, risk for eating disorder and body dissati sfac tion relative to established noms. These questionnaires enhanced comparability to other research and facilitated an understanding of participants' attitudes to weight from the perspective of health professionals.

Combining these three interview techniques worked well: When al1 three methods resulted in similar results, confidence in their validity was increased. When conflicting results were obtained, it was necessary to push the analysis futher and deeper to understand apparent contradictions. Exarnining the topic from different angles ultimately provided a richer understanding of the meaning and role of weight to participants.

The major disadvantage of usine three data collection rnethods was the additional tirne required for administration and andysis. Participants were generous with their time and although it was a long interview, everyone completed the questionnaires. This added approximately 5 to 10 minutes to the interview. This additional time provided an opportunity to observe the environment, to reflect on the interview and to consider if additional information was needed-

Al1 three methods used were based upon a single interview and relied on self- report. People typically are inaccurate when they talk about their behaiiour (Bernard,

1994)' and discrepancies may be greater when observations about other people are requested. Although interviewing mother-daughter pairs may have provided a check on conscious emors, the distinction between perspectives and behaviour remains important.

The main focus in this study was on participants' perspectives and understandings about weight. The findings would have been e~chedby collecting other kinds of data.

However, methods were chosen based on a consideration of their appropriateness and feasibility for a single-investigator study that focuses on people's perspectives. Partnering, greater involvement of Aboriginal researchers or participatory action research is recornmended for future projects (Cohen, 1995; Postl, Moffatt, & Sarsfield, 1987;

Travers, 1997).

The interviews captured the participants' perspectives at a single point in time.

People's ideas about weight change over time and may be context dependent. It would have been ideal to have multiple interviewers (including collaborators of Aboriginal ancestry) and to interview people more than once in a variety of settings. Given the scope of the project, and the available time and money, this was not possible. The cross sectional design of the study precludes conclusions about causality.

The large number of qualitative interviews in this study (163) is both a limitation and a strength. Interviewing multiple participants is another way of triangulating data

(Huberman & Miles, 1994). The amount of data collected necessitated focussing on particular questions and constrained the depth to which themes could be pursued.

However, it also increased the ability to detect patterns across diverse samples, a major goal of the research. More detailed analysis of particular sites remains possible. Although finding consensus in al1 participants except the First Nations community women is an interesting finding, the effort at comparability limited the ability to tailor the consensus

statements to this one location. To explore their perspective further with this rnethod

wouid require re-interviewing with statements derived from their interviews.

The credibility of the findings relies on people's honesty, interest and ability to communicate their thoughts about a complex topic. The samples were primady random,

and some participants, particularly the girls, were shy. Most, however, warmed to the

interview early on, and the questionnaires provided an alternative way for them to communicate their thoughts. A volunteer sample may have been more forthcoming but

would have limited understanding of the range of interest in weight issues. Additionally,

the random nature of the sample helps to establish that participants were not selected to

prove the point king investigated (Patton, 1990). The emergence of quite different points

of view is evidence that the questions allowed people to express their own thoughts.

As others have noted, objectivity in social research is a myth (Bernard, 1994;

Denzin, 1994; Olesen, 1994). Although the objective was to understand the participants'

perspectives on weight issues, the report more accurately reflects my interpretation of

their perspectives. This cannot be avoided because analysis requires the selection and

interpretation of evidence. 1 have attempted to represent the range of views present in the

interviews and to present my analysis as distinct from the participants'. It is possible, however, that 1 have missed significant patterns or misinterpreted the data (Marshall &

Rossman, 1989). These considerations, which pertain to al1 studies, are particularly important when cultural differences are involved (Cannon, Higginbotham, & Leung,

1991; Parker et al., 1995; Riessman, 1987). To guard against this, interested participants are sometimes invited to read sections of draft reports. The relationships between participants limited the use of this check. The irequent citation of participants' own words was used as an alternative to enable readers to assess the sufficiency of evidence and the validity of my interpretation and analysis. As mentioned above, the purpose of triangulating data collection is to establish validity. Using data from questionnaires alongside participants' responses to open-ended questions aids interpretation of the approprïateness and completeness of each interview format.

In qualitative research, it is the investigator's responsibility to provide sufficient detail to enable the reader to judge the credibility of the findings and to determine the extent to which they apply in other situations. In the final analysis, this will ascertain if the data and interpretations are valid and useful.

Surnmarv of Research Findines

The Abori ~nal/Non-Aboriginal- Com~arison

The first research question concemed the extent to which the well-established weight concems of middle-class females in our culture were shared by Aboriginal girls and women. The findings of this study demonstrate that weight is an important, but perhaps different issue to some of the Aboriginal paiticipants, especially in the First

256 Nations community. It was not as clearly centrai to fernale identity, although there were some indications that it may be becoming more central.

Al1 three rnethods of analysis provided evidence that the First Nations community participants were most concemed about weight. In their responses to the semi-stnictured interview questions, First Nations cornmuni ty participants supported weight control and weight loss beyond that of the participants at the other locations. For exarnple, the participants in the First Nations community overwhelmingly gave advice oriented to weight control or weight loss, whereas participants at the other locations had a more mixed orientation with some advice promoting acceptance of the body at its current size.

Analysis of the 24 weight-related values statements indicated consensus in al1 participants except the First Nations community women." There was more support for weight control, although there was also evidence of competing values in the way they responded to other questions. The trend to higher scores on the Modified Restraint Scale for the First Nations community participants also suggested more dieting intent. Although non-significant, this would be worth following up, particularly since sarnple size at the community level was sma11."'

Concurrent with these expressions of concem, there was also a decidedly different tone to the interviews. The First Nations community women were most Iikely to respond

46~oral1 participants except the First Nations community women, the consensus factor was -64 (SD of .16) and the ratio of the fmt to second eigenvalue was 6.93. For the First Nations cornrnunity women, results were .SB (SD of .19) and 2.46, respectively.

47~hiswas tested with a Split Unit Analysis of Variance with generation as the nested term and ethnicity and region as factors (F = 4.36, df = 1,71, p = .W). that weight was no? more important to women than to men, with the non-Aboriginal women almost unanimous that it was (and the Urban Aboriginal wornen's responses split both ways.) The First Nations cornmunity women's explanations for why people were concemed about weight focussed on health in very personal terms. Those who saw weight as more important to women than men tended to speak of physiologicd differences between the sexes that made it easier for women to gain and harder to lose. The non-

Aboriginal women spoke of cultural influences that made weight part of a woman's iden tity . Several non-Aboriginal women made statements about weight , such as "for

women, it's who they are. " First Nations community wornen did not talk about weight this way, nor did they talk about feeling judged in terms of appearance or weight. The manner in which weight was discussed relative to other issues fiuther supported an impression that weight was not central to identity. The lack of patterning in the First

Nations community women's responses revealed by the cultural consensus analysis suggested either greater variability or that the framework used to structure the questions was inappropriate to their perspective. These results suggest that although the concerns look similar, the very central feature of Stice's model does not apply to the First Nations comrnunity women. The research assumptions about women's weight preoccupation embodied in this model were supported by the observations of the non-Aboriginal participants, and by some of the Urban Aboriginal participants. There were subtle differences in the First Nations community that became apparent when the group responses were compared. This analysis questions the centrality of weight to ferninine identity in the First Nations community. Alternative models were not explored with cultural consensus anaiysis because of the way the consensus statements were derived.

The analysis suggested consensus for First Nations community girls, both arnong themselves and with the other girls. Despite these findings, there were differences between the First Nations community girls and the girls at the other locations @ = .002), and no significant differences between the girls and the women within this community @

= .IO)? The First Nations community girls seemed to be responding both similarly to and differently from both the other girls and their mothers. In the open-ended interviews, the

First Nations community girls agreed that weight was more important to girls than to boys and gave similar reasons as the other girls. There were no significant differences between

Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal girls in their body shape selections for desired, healthiest, mos? anractive, and attractive tu guys. The Aboriginal girls, however, selected heavier perceived body shapes than the non-Aboriginal girls. The results provided no evidence that Aboriginal girls viewed larger body shapes as more attractive or healthier than the non- Aboriginal girls.

The Aboriginal women selected larger perceived and desired body shapes than the non-Aboiginal women. Their choices for most attractive and healthiest body shapes could not be tested due to differences between Suburban and Rural women. Tests for differences between the locations specified the differences for these questions were between the Suburban wornen and the women at the other thtee locations. There was no

"mese differences were indicated by QAP. The First Nations cornmunity girls had a consensus factor of .56 (SD of .23) with a first to second eigenvalue ratio of 3.46. The consensus factor for al1 girls combined was .65 (SR of.18) with an eigenvalw ratio of 7.29. significant difference between Aborigind and non-Abriginal women's selection of the shape that would be attractive to men. Taken together, these resülts suggest the

Aboriginal women's larger desired selections were practical choices stemming from their larger perceived size. Some researchers have asked about desired body shape or weight, assuming these represent ideals for attractiveness. The findings reported here indicate that this cm Iead to inappropriate conclusions.

There was, however, a non-significant trend for the Aboriginal women to choose larger shapes as most artractive and healthiest for girls. If confinned, this implies greater differences in what mothers see as appropnate weights for younger people. This did not seem to influence the girls' choices, and consequently may lead to Aboriginal mothers being taken by surprise if their daughters become weight preoccupied.

There were a number of cornparisons where differences between Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal participants were significant (with the greatest number of difierences at the p < -05 Ievel occuring between the Urban Aboriginal and the Suburban women). The dissatisfaction with weight was significantly greater in Aboriginal participants at both the urban and rural locations." More Abonginal girls (66%) than non-Aboriginal girls (33%) wanted a smdIer body shape. More Aboriginal women (83%) than non-Aboriginal women (62%) wanted a smaller body shape. As indicated, weight status was also significantly different in the expected direction whether based on self-reported height and

4Qifferences by generation and ethnicity (but not region) were indicated in a logistic regression analysis examining satisfaction with body shape: 2 = 18.02. df = 2, p = .0001. weight or perceived body shape.'

There were more Aboriginal girls with high scores on the EAT-26, indicating risk for an eating disorder at both locations. The rates for Aboriginal girls (17.5%) are consistent with rates reported for girls in the general population (Leichner et al., 1986;

Wood et al., 1992). The rates for non-Aboriginal girls (2.58) were low and rnay be under-estimated due to the weight-related refusals in the non-Abonginal families.

Although some of the refusals may have related to mothers' concems about their own weight, it is possible that serious eating problems had occurred and ken dealt with at a younger age in the non-Aboriginal girls. It is possible that rates for both samples are under-estimated because girls were not prornised confidentiality, and some were hesitant to bnng up their weight control efforts. Nevertheless, 26% of the First Nations community girls (and 16% of the women) either scored high or indicated on the EAT-26 that they had vomited for the purpose of weight control (with concerns confirmed in follow-up interviews). Another 25% of the First Nations community girls indicated they had considered vomiting to control their weight.

The interviews were consistent with this finding of serious concerns. Some of the

Urban Aboriginal and First Nations community women reported a similar history of serious concern about weight when they were girls, indicating that risky attempts to control weight have existed for some Abonginal girls for at least a generation.

qheresults of ANOVA comparing BMI were F = 7.49, df = 1, 137,p = .007. Cornparisons of perceived body shape were significant (z = 3.35, df = 72,p = .CUI8 for girls; z = 2.8 1, df = 76, p = .O05 for women, Mann Whitney U test). Differences between Abonginal and non-Aboriginal participants precluded cornbining the samples to test for differences between the urban and rural regions on

many questions. There were no strong differences or indications of pattemed results

between the cornparisons that were made except the contrast in EAT-26 scores was

strongest between the Rural women and the First Nations community women? There

were no other significant differences (or trends) between the First Nations community

participants and either the Rural or the Urban Aboriginal participants. The differences

between Aboriginal and non-Abonginai participants were greater than those between the

urban and rural participants, suggesting the differences in the First Nations community

were not due to "small town" effects.

The Mot her/Dau ghter Com~arison

Children are socialized first withïn their families, and it seems logical that

mothers (among other family members) would be key in the transmission of cultural

values to their children. The evidence is far from clear, and the second major research

focus was on the mothers' advice to and modelling of weight-related behaviours to their

13- and 14- year-old daughters.

A majority of girls in this study and approximateiy two-thirds of the mothers said

they discussed weight with each other. There was much agreement on content of their

''Univariate analysis indicated significant differences in EAT-26 scores between the Rural and First Nations community women (H = 11.21, df = 3, p = .01, Ktuskal-Wallis test). discussions, especially as the weight of girls increased. An exception to this was the area of compliments. At least four times as many mothers reported complimenting their daughters as girls reported receiving compliments. A partial explanation for this discrepancy may be seen in the tendency of mothers to give their positive cornments reac tively in response to their daughters' self-criticism or a direct question concerning appearance.

I also looked at other aspects of mother-daughter simiIarity, including open-ended reports of the advice they said they gave to others, their body dissatisfaction, dieting habits, and responses on structured questionnaires. Despite much agreement, the results do not make a strong case for mothers' influence over and above the broader cultural positions on weight, at least at this age in terms of modelling specific views.

As with general surveys of older and younger women, mothers were not significantly different than daughters when the generations' responses to the Restraint

Scale or the EAT-26 were comparedSZAs already discussed, the finding of consensus for participants on the 24 statements supports a conclusion of shared cultural values regarding weight at the three locations where it made sense to aggregate results. At the fourth, the First Nations community, there were no significant differences between the generations."

"In the split unit ANûVA, of the Modified Restraint Scale, generation, the nested term, was not significant (F = 2.82, df = 1,74, p = .IO, N.S.). Nor were there any significant differences between the generations on the EAT-26 (Kniskal-Wallis test on ranks H = 1.58, df = l,p= .21).

53Q~Pindicated no significant differences in the consensus responses between the generations for the First Nations community famiiies @ = .IO). Despite an overall lack of difference behueen generations, there was little association evident between individual mother-daughter pairs. This was indicated by non- significant associations for the 80 families for the Modified Restraint scde (Pearson's r =

-10,p = .36, N-S-), the Restraint scale (Pearson's r = -03,p = .78, N.S.) and the EAT-26

(Spearman's r = .CM, p .73,N.S.). There was no evidence that a daughter's level of body dissatisfaction was associated with her mother's (kappa = -18). A comparison of matches and correlations between mother-daughter p&, in the consensus analysis dso indicated agreement was no greater than in unrelated participants. Nor were there any significant differences between the mothers of the girls with the highest and lowest Restraint scores.S4

Part of the explanation for this finding of no significant difference between generations, but no association within mother-daughter pairs, was that more of the women were heavier and expressed moderate concerns about their weight. Significantly more women than girls selected heavier perceived and desired body shapes Significantly more women were dissatisfied with their bodies than girls (72% and 46% respectively, based on a comparison of their body shape selections). There was a much broader range of concern in the girls, from disinterest to extreme concern about weight issues. More girls were extremely dissatisfied as measured by the EAT-26 and according to the revelations in their interviews. A mother's and daughter's level of concem about weight

"Mean Reseaint scores for mothers of both groups of girls were the same (1 1.4). Contrary to expected, mean scores on the EAT-26 were slightly lower for the mothers of the high-scoring girls (3.5 I1.5 SE) than the mothers of the low-scoring girls (4.7 I1.6 SE), although the differences were non-significant (t = -0.54, df = 18, p = .30, N.S.). rareIy matched. At this age, the dieting mothers did not have dieting daughters, at least not in greater numbers than the non-dieting mothers.

There was no real evidence to support the "like-mother, like-daughtei' assumptions that are still cornmon currency as the rationale for how girls learn to fear fat and diet. This finding seems plausible because girls have multiple opportunities to be exposed to culturally shared ideas about weight. The influence of fathers, brothers and sisters, grandmothers, friends and other class mates (especidl y boys), coaches, teachers and health professionals, media (especially television), and others came up in participants' interviews. These sources usually transmit consistent messages about the importance of king thin, so girls are likely to leam their lessons whether it is from their rnothers or others. That is, girls are Iikely to absorb these messages even if their mothers are not concerned about weight.

There was one exception to this pattern of non-significant mother-daughter

associations, although it was based on a post hoc analysis of 21 participants and should tr; viewed more as a hypothesis than a finding. Mothers who gave advice that focussed on body acceptance had daughters who were more Iikely to follow suit (kappa = SI). This suggests that mothers who have a point of view that departs from the standard messages may have daughters who express simila.counter-culture views. There may, of course, be other factors in these families that facilitate this manner of coping with weignt issues, but it was not possible to draw any conclusions about this based on two short interviews per family. Nevertheless, this finding, that mothers who speak of the importance of self- acceptance (specificaily including weight and body shape), are more Likely to have daughters who repeat these messages, if verified by further research, offers a new way to

Iook at weight interventions.

Whether mothers focussed on promoting body acceptance or weight control, there was much common ground in their advice to their daughters. Mothers favouring either strategy were concemed about their daughters' overail physical and psychological health and happiness. Both groups recommended the same behaviours (healthy eating and king active). Both groups of mothers recommended moderation and expressed uneasiness about girls' excessive concerns with weight and resulting extreme efforts to lose weight.

Al1 of these concems came up in both orientations across the range of girls' body weight.

The main difference between the mothers was the belief that the recommended behaviours would (or would not) lead to weight control or weight loss, and the resulting focus (or pointed avoidance of focus) on weight.

In addition to the well-documented higher incidence of heavier weight norms in

Aboriginal people, this research found evidence of weight preoccupation, including reports of detrimental weight control behaviours in Aboriginal girls living in or close to the city. Although heaith professionals have not caused this weight preoccupation, it is important that they be aware of its existence when talking to Abonginal people about weight, both to ensure that appropriate help is offered as needed, and to prevent inadvertently increasing weight preoccupation and disordered eating.

An integrated approach to weight control and weight preoccupation should be

266 considered in programs that promote healthy weights. One way to do this is to shift health promotion messages away from weight to focus on desirable behaviours (Berg, 2001;

Health Canada, 2000; Ornichinski & Hanison, 2000; Rice, 1993; Satter, 1987). Messages about weight should enhance selfesteem and encourage youth to feel good about themselves and to express respect for other people. This is particularly important with children who are emotionally vulnerable. This came up in this study and others (French,

Story, Downes, Resnick, & Blum, 1995; Marc hessault, 1993a).

There is a need to develop and test programs that intepte prevention of obesity and weight preoccupation. Some fomal programs using this approach have been tested with adults (Ciliska, 1990; Ornichinski & Harrison, 1995), but such programs have not been developed or tested with teens or with Aboriginal people. An integrated approach to weight seems consistent with traditional values and could be frmed within lifestyle programs that focus on Aboriginal heri tage and pride and pnnciples such as those embedded in the medicine wheel (Bartlett, 1995). Such programs could build on the findings of ongoing research on diabetes and obesity prevention programs in Aboriginal cornmunities (Caballero, 1999; Davis et al., 1999; Macaulay et al., 1996). They could also draw on aspects of existing programs to prevent weight preoccupation (Friedman, 1994;

Piran, 1999), taking care to adapt messages to meet the needs of the intended audience.

Although the lessons embedded in these programs may be useful, a gendered analysis may not be the most appropriate approach. It is best to start with lived experience of the intended audience and this means involving them in the development of educational approaches. Using an integrated approach to weight issues would also meet the needs of some of the mother-daughter pairs who were caught in the contradictions inspired by weight.

For example, mothers who wished to communicate love and acceptance, while addressing their daughters' health needs could do so by side-stepping the issue of weight control. If these mothers focus their attention on their daughters' nutritional and activity needs, the conflict between self-esteem and physical health dissipates.

In addition to knowing what to advise their daughters, mothers need to cornmunicate effectively with their daughters. Some mothers intuitively communicated that they understood and were empathetic to their daughters' concerns even if they desired different goah for her. This is important because (among other things) these mothers were then in a position to help their daughters temper a focus on weight, if needed. Weight problems caught some families by surprise and attending to issues earlier wouId also be helpful. This does not mean restncting food intake, as there is some indication that this is associated with the problems it is intended to resolve (Birch &

Fisher, 2000; Stice, 1998). Rather it suggests that nutrition issues need to be approached in the context of broader relationships. Parents need to cornmunicate to their daughters that their love is not conditional upon weight status, This may seem obvious, but some girIs seemed inclined to interpret weight messages this way.

Rather than focussing on weight, the emphasis can be put on eating nutritiously for the sake of health, being active because it is fun and fee1s good, increasing self- acceptance and appreciation for the body one has, and respecting diversity in body size and shape. Girls need information about these areas. They also need information about and positive reinforcement for the increase in body fat that cornes with puberty.

Specific and global affirmations of positive aspects of girls' appearance, character

and many talents, given pro-actively as well as reactively might help girls hear and

internalize compliments and build body pnde. Compliments do not have to accord with

culturally vaIued traits, but can confirm alternative points of view. Focussing on what the

body cm do, rather than what it looks like can change the framework for evaluating the

body. Parents can help adolescents cope with teasing and other forms of discrimination,

express respect for other people and critique the media methods and images. Mothers

(and others) can actively teach a critical consciousness about society's weight

preoccupation. The amount of time parents spend with their children, including eating

meals together, has been shown to positively effect both weight and attitudes to weight

(Swarr & Richards, 1996; Graber et al., 1999).

Other than the association between mothers and daughters for messages promoting body acceptance, this research did not find evidence that 13- and 14-year-old girls model their attitudes to weight mainly on their mothers'. These negative results suggest that the context needs to be broadened beyond the mother-âaughter relationship.

Farnily, peers, the media and health systems were noted as chers of influence in the adaptation of Stice's (1994) model. As many have noted, and as seems particularly relevant here, interventions need to be broadly based and inter-sectoral to effect social change (Graber et al., 1999; Levine & Piran, 1999; Ritenbaugh et al., 1999). Recommendations for Further Research

There is a need to monitor weight status and the prevalence of potentially harmful weight loss concerns and practices of Aboriginal people, and particularly youth, in a

Iarger number of more diverse communities. This will require both the broader survey approach and the more in-depth examination of participant perspectives.

There is a need to survey Abonginal youth to determine more broadly what their weight concems and practices are. There is particularly a need to examine how weight is perceived by people living in more northerly and more isolated cornrnunities where history and living conditions Vary greatly from those examined here. The First Nations communi ty that participated in this study had year-round roads and was qui te sirnilar to the rural community in its access to grocery stores, exercise and extra-cunicular activities, educational and employment opportunities. There are also differences in housing and language that may affect exposure to mainstream weight messages.

Consequently, these concems need to be explored in a greater variety of First Nations communi ties.

There is a need to develop and evaluate culturally appropnate obesity prevention programs that also address weight preoccupation and disordered eating. In addition to focussing on the needs of individuals, ways to address the larger context in which people live their Iives need exploration. This is especially appropriate to obesity prevention.

There is opportunity to use participatory action research in a First Nation community to focus on community infrastructures that affect peoples' lives. For example, supplying clean drinking water and increasing its social acceptability as a replacement for soft drinks may do more for weight noms than educational programs (personai communication, Healther Dean, Apri130,2001).

The preliminary findings of this study suggested a role for mothers in promoting resistance to societal attitudes to weight in their daughters. This constmct of resistance to weight preoccupation ments Merattention, particularly with respect to parental and schooi interventions. Resistance to weight preoccupation seems to be cited more often in

Canadian research (Chapman, 1999; Wakewich, 2000) or in research with Black women

(Nichter, 2ûûû; Parker et al., 1995) than in research focussing on the general public in the

U.S. or Bntain (Grogan, 1999; Nichter, 2000). This may reflect differences between interests of individual researchers or diflerences between samples. There is reason to suspect a paradigm shift, and this rnight be worth exploring in relation to Canada's

Vi tali ty program.

Studies with younger girls suggest a positive association in vanous mother- daughter attributes and should be repeated with controls for body weight. There would be benefit to comparing the perceptions about weight of older teens and young women with their mothers to examine this relationship once girls' attitudes have matured. Testing mother-daughter similarities in immigrant families with greater contrasts between generations could also clariQ the discussion. Studying male perceptions of weight (their own and their role in making weight important to women), older men and older women also have potential to deepen Our understanding of cultural values pertaining to weight.

Stice (1998) has reported that girls who diet end up heavier, except for those who diet to extremes. Nichter (2000) has reported that girls who diet seriously have inadequate nutrient intakes. There is a need for longitudinal studies to document what girls do when they Say they diet, how this affects the nutritionai adequacy of their diet and what happens ta their weight. This data would help refine messages to girls about their weight.

In the meantirne, let us teach Ophelia to swim, both literally and metaphoncally.

We cm continue to encourage active living and healthy lifestyles while teaching girls to swim against the current, or more aptly, cross currents, for the North Amerricm culture presents contradictory messages about weight. Shakespeare said "To thine own self be me." The words of the Aboriginal elder were "You've got to follow your heart." Some of the mothers said they told their daughters, "You're just pe~ectthe way you are. " Girls need to know that striving for the thin ideal is fa.from ideal. REFERENCES

Allan, J. D. (1988). Knowing what to weigh: Women's selfae activities related to weight. Advances in Nursing Science, 11,4760-

Allan, J. D. (1989). Women who successfully manage their weight. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 11,657-675.

Allon, N. (1973). The stigma of overweight in everyday life. In G. A. Bray (Ed.), Obesiry in perspective (Vol. 2[Part 21, pp. 83- 102). Washington, D.C.: Fogarty International Center Senes on Preventive Medicine; U.S. Govemment Printing Office.

Allon, N. (1979). Self-perceptions of the stigma of overweight in relationship to weight- losing patterns. The American Journal of Clinicul Nutrition, 32, 470-480.

American Dietetic Association. (1997). Position of the American Dietetic Association: Weight management. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 97,7 1-74.

American Dietetic Association & Dietitians of Canada. (2000a). Adolescents, Manual of clinical dietetics (6th ed., pp. 105-108). Chicago, IL: Amencan Dietetic Association.

Amencan Dietetic Association & Dietitians of Canada. (20ûûb). Obesity, Munual of clinical dietetics (6th ed., pp. 365-386). Chicago, IL: Amencan Dietetic Association.

American Dietetic Association & Dietitians of Canada. (2000~).Schwl-age children, Manunl of clinical dietetics (6th ed., pp. 99-104). Chicago, IL: Amexican Dietetic Association.

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

Andersen, A., Cohn, L., & Holbrook, T. (2000). Making weight: Healing men 3 conflicts with food, weight di shape (Vol. preview copy). Carlsbad, CA: Gune Books.

Anderson, K. (200). A recognition of being: Reconstructing Native womanhood. , Ontario: Second Story Press.

Aronson, H., Fredrnan, M., & Gabriel, M. (1990). Personality correlates of eating attitudes in a nonclinical sarnple. Intematioml Joumal of Euting Disorders, 9, 103-107. Artz, S. (1998). Sex, power, & the violent school girl. Toronto, Ontario: Trifolium Books.

Ashley, F. W., & Kannel, W. B. (1974). Relation of weight change to changes in atherogenic traits: The Framingham Study. J. Chron. Dk,27, 103-1 14.

Attie, I., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1989). Development of eating problems in adolescent girls: A longitudinal study. Developmentul Psychology, 25,70-79.

Ball, G. D. C., Fimrite, A., Villetard, K., Kayman, S., & McCargar, L. J. (1999). Weight relapsers, maintainers and controls: Metabolic and behavioural ciifferences. Canadian Journal of Applied Physiology, 24,548-558.

Barlow, S. E., & Dietz, W. H. (1998). Obesity evaluation and treatment: Expert Cornmittee recommendations. The Matemal and Child Health Bureau, Health Resources and Services Administration and the Department of Health and Human Services. Pediatrics, IO2(3), E29.

Bartlett, J. (1995, November). Aboriginal Women 's Health: "The Medicine Wheelfor Comprehensive Development. " Paper presented at the Canadian Advisory Council on the Status of Women 1994 Policy Symposium on Selected Women's Heaith Issues, Sept. 25-28, 1994.

Batchelder, W. H., & Romney, K. A. (1988). Test theory without an answer key. Psychomerrika, 53,71-92.

Beauvoir De, S. (1989). The second sex (H. M. Parshley, Tram.). New York: Vintage Books. (Original work published 1952)

Berg, F. M. (1997). Three major U.S. studies describe trends. Healthy Weight Journal, 11,67-74.

Berg, F. M. (200).Children und teens ufiuid to eat: Helping youth in today S weighr- obsessed world. Hettinger, ND: Healthy Weight Network.

Berg, F. M. (2001). Women afi-aid to eat: Breaking fiee in today 's weight-obsessed world. Hettinger, ND: Healthy Weight Network.

Berg, F. M., & Marchessault, G. (2000). Na-ming the revolution. Healthy Weight Journal, 14, 10-1 1,14.

Berg, M. (1998). Choose sensitive, accurate ternis. Healthy Weight Journal, 12, 12-13. Berland, N. W., Thompson, J. K., & Linton, P. H. (1986). Correlations between the EAT- 26 and the EAT40,the Eating Disorders Inventory, and the Restrained Eating Inventory. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5,569-574.

Bernard, H. R. (1994/ 1988). Research rnethods in cultural anthropology (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Bernard, L., Lavallee, C., Gray-Donald, K., & Delisle, H. (1995). Overweight in Cree schoolchiidren and adolescents associated with diet, low physical activity, and high television viewing. Journal ofthe Amencan Dietetic Association, 95, 800- 802.

Bemdt, T. J., & Hestenes, S. L. (1996). The developmental course of social support: Family and peers. In L. Smolak & M. P. Levine (Eds.), nie developmental psychoparhology of eating disorders (pp. 77-106). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Beumont, P., Al-Alami, M., & Touyz, S. (1988). Relevance of a standard measurement of undemutrition to the diagnosis of anorexia nervosa: Use of Quetelet's Body Mass Index @MI). International Journal of Eating Disorders, 7,399-405.

Birch, L. L., & Fisher, J. 0. (2000). Mothers'child-feeding practices influence daughters' eating and weight. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 71, 1054-1061.

Birmingham, C. L., Muller, J. L., Palepu, A., Spinelli, J. J., & Anis, A. H. (1999). The cos t of obesity in Canada. Canadian Medical Association Joumal, 160,483-48.

Blair, S. N., Kampert, J. B., Kohl, H. W., Barlow, C. E., Macera, A. C., Paffenbarger, R. S., & Gibbons, L. W. (1996). Influences of cardiorespiratoy fimess and other precursors on cardiovascular disease and dl-cause mortality in men and women. Journal of the American Medical AssociationJ 2 76,205-2 10.

Bordo, S. (1993). Unbearable weight: Feminism, western culture, and the body. Berkeley: University of California Press.

Borgatti, S. P. (1994). Cultural domain analysis. Joumal of Quantitative Anthropology, 4, 26 1-278.

Borgatti, S. P. (1996). ANTHROPAC [Version 4.0, Cornputer program and reference manual]. Natick, MA: Analytic Technologies.

Boster, J. S. (1987). Introduction: Why study variation? American Behavioral Scientist, 31, 150-162. Bngman, B. (1994). Four generations of women: Our bodies and lives. In P. Fallon, M. A. Katzman, & S. C. Wooley (Eds.), Femniist perspectives on eating disorders (pp. 115-131). New York: Guilford Press.

Broussard, B. A., Johnson, A., Himes, J. H., Story, M., Fichtner, R., Hauck, F., Bachman- Carter, K., Hayes, J., Frohlich, K., Gray,N., Valway, S., & Gohdes, D. (1991). Prevalence of obesity in Arnerican Indians and Alaska Natives. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 53, lS3SS-1542S.

Brown, C., & Jasper, K. (Eds.). (1 993). Consuming passions: Feminist approaches to weight preoccupation and eating disorders. Toronto, Ontario: Second Story Press.

Brown, C. G. (1987). Feeding into each other: Weight preoccupation and the contradictory expectations of women. Unpublished master's thesis, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg.

Brown, L. M., & Gilligan, C. (1992). Meeting at the crossroads: Womenk psychology and girls' development. New York: Ballantine Book.

Brownell, M., Kozyrskyj, A., Fergusson, P., & Lerfald, J. (2001). Social deterrninants of health, Assessing the health of children in Manitoba: A population-based study (pp- 177-209). Winnipeg: University of Manitoba, Manitoba Centre for Hedth Policy and Evaluation.

Bruch, H. (1978). The golden cage: The enigma of anorexïa nervosa. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.

Brumberg, J. J. (1989). Fasting girls: nte history of anorexiu nervosa. Markharn, Ontario: Penguin Books Canada.

Brumberg, J. J. (1997). The body project: An intimate history of American girls. New York: Random House.

Brundige, J., Fedorick, J., & Finn, G. (1993). Fighting two worlds. In G.Finn (Ed.), Limited edition: Voices of women, voices of feminism (pp. 38 1-399).Halifax, Nova Scotia: Fernwood.

Bruyere, J., & Gamo, L. (2000). Nehinaw (Cree) understandings of diabetes: "He travels in the body". Canadian Nurse, 96(6),25-28.

Byely, L., Archibald, A. B., Graber, J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000). A prospective study of familial and social influences on girls'body image and dieting. International Journal of Eating DLÎotders, 28, 155-164. Caballero, B. (1999). Obesity prevention in Amencan Indian schoolchildren: Pathways - Iniroduction. Amencan Joumal of Clinical Nutrition, 69(4 Suppl. S), 745s-7463.

Canadian Dietetic Association. (1988). Obesity: A case for prevention. Official position of the Canadian Dietetic Association. Joumal of the Canadian Dietetic Association, 49, 1 1-16.

Canadian Task Force on the Periodic Health Examination. (1994). Periodic health examination, 1994 update: 1. Obesity in childhood. Canadian Medical Association Joumal, 150, 871-879. Retrieved Oct. 15,2000 fkom http://www.cma.ca/cmaj/vol%872D1 SO/O87 le.htrn#tab087 1

Cannon, L. W., Higginbotham, E., & Leung, M. L. A. (1991). Race and class bias in qualitative research on women. In M. M. Fonow & J. A. Cook (Eds.), Beyond methodology: Feminist scholarship as lived research (pp. 107-1 1 8). Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

Cash, T. F. (1997). The body image workbook: An 8-step program for learning to like your luoks. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.

Cash, T. F., & Roy, R. E. (1999). Pounds of flesh: Weight, gender, and body images. In J. Sobal & D. Maurer (Eds.), bterpreting Weight: nie social management of fatness and thinness (pp. 209-228). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

Catlin, G. (1 988). Canada 's Health Promotion Survey. Technical Rep0l.r Series: Guidelinesfor community-based health promotion surveys. Ottawa, Ontario: Minister of National Health and Welfare, Canada.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2000, May 30). National Centerfor Health Statistics CDC growth charts: United States. Retrieved Nov. 30,2000 from http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/about/major/nhanes/growthchartdcharts.htm

Chapman, G.E. (1999). From "dieting" to "healthy eating": An exploration of shifting constmctions of eating for weight control. In J. Sobal & D. Maurer (Eds.), bterpreting Weight: The social management of fatness and thinness (pp. 73- 87). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

Chapman, G., & Maclean, H. (1990). Qualitative research in home economics. Canadian Home Economics Joumal, 40, 129-134.

Chapman, G. E., & Maclean. ES. (1993). Junk food and healthy food: Meanings of food in adolescent women7sculture. Journal of Nutrition Education, 25, 108-1 13. Ci liska, D. (1990). Beyond dieting. Psychoeducational interventionsfor chronically obese women: A non-dieting approach. New York: BrunnerfMazel.

Cockerham, W. C., Kunz, G., & Lueschen, G, (1988). On concem with appearance, health beliefs, and eating habits: A reappraisal comparing Americans and West Germans. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 29,265-269.

Cohen, B. (1995). What is parricipatory research? An exploration of its principles, practice, problems, and possibilities. Unpublished manuscript in author's possession.

Cohn, L. D., Adler, N. E., Irwin, C. E., Millstein, S. G., Kegeles, S. M., & Stone, G. (1987). Body-figure preferences in male and female adolescents. Journal of Abnonnal Psychology, 96,276-279.

Cole, T. J., Bellizzi, M. C., Flegai, K. M., & Dietz, W. H. (2000). Establishing a standard definition for child overweight and obesity worldwide: International survey. British Medical Joumal, 320, f 240- 1243.

Colvin, R. H., & Olson, S. B. (1983). A descriptive analysis of men and women who have lost significant weight and are highly successful at maintaining the loss. Addictive Behaviors, 8,287-295.

Costin, C. (1997). Your dieting daughtec Zs she dying for attention? New York, NY: BrunnerIMazel .

Crago, M., Shisslak, C. M., & Estes, L. S. (1996). Eating disturbances among American minonty groups: A review. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 19,239- 248.

Cutting, T. M., Fisher, J. O., Grimm-Thomas, K., & Birch, L. L. (1999). Like mother, like daughter: Familial patterns of overweight are mediated by mothers' dietary disinhi bition. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 69,608-6 13.

Dairy Bureau of Canada. (1993). Myths and malnutrition: A growing problem. A summary of clinical papers and recent studies. Toronto, Ontario: Author.

Dalton, S. (1997). Body weight terminology, definitions, and measurement. In S. Dalton (Ed.), Overweight and weight management: The health professional's guide to understanding and practice (pp. 1-33). Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen. Darling-Wolf, F. (2000). From airbrushing to liposuction: The technological reconstruction of the female body. In B. Miedema, J. M. Stoppard, & V. Anderson (Eds.), Women's bodies/Wornen's lives: Health, well-being and body image (pp. 277-293). Toronto, Ontaxio: Sumach Press.

Davis, C., & Webster, T. (Eds.). (1998). DK Zllustrated Oxford dictionary. Toronto, Ontario: Dorling Kindersley & Oxford University Press.

Davis, S., Gomez, Y., Lambert, L., & Skipper, B. (1993). Primary prevention of obesity in Arnerican Indian children. Annals New York Academy of Sciences, 699, 167- 180.

Davis, S. M., Scott, B. G., Helitzer, D. L., Teufel, N. I., Snyder, P., Gittelsohn, J., Metcalfe, L., Arviso, V., Evans, M., Smyth, M., Bnce, R., & Altaha, J. (1999). Pathways: A culturally appropriate obesity-prevention program for American Indian schoolchildren. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 69(4 Suppl. S), 796s-802s.

Delisle, H., Mavrikakis, S., & Strychar, 1. (1995, May). A school based nutrition education program for primary prevention of diabetes in an Algonquin communiiy of Quebec. Paper presented at the 3rd International Conference on Diabetes and Indigenous Peoples, Winnipeg, Manitoba.

Denzin, N. K. (1994). The art and politics of interpretation. In Handbook of qualitative research (pp. 500-5 15). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Denzin, N. K., & Lincoln, Y. S. (1994). Introduction: Entenng the field of qualitative research. In Handbook of qualitative research (pp. 1-22). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Diamond, N. (1985). Thin is the feminist issue. Feminist Review, 19,45-64.

Douketis, J. D., Feightner, J. W., Attia, J., Feldman, W. F., & Canadian Task Force on Preventive Health Care. (1999). Periodic health examination, 1999 update: 1. Detection, prevention and treatment of obesity. Canadian Medicnl Association Journal, l6O,5 13-524.

Edmunds, H., & Hill, A. J. (1999). Dieting and the family context of eating in young adolescent children. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 25,435-440.

Egan, C. (1999). Inuit women S perceptions of pollution. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg. Evers, S. (1987). Economic and social factors associated with obesity in adult Canadians. Nutrition Research, 7,3-13.

Fallon, A. E., & Rozin, P. (1985). Sex differences in perceptions of desirable body shape. Journal of Abnomal Psychology, 94,102-105.

Fallon, P., Katzman, M. A., & Wmley, S. C. (Eds.). (1994). Feminist perspectives on eating disorders. New York: Guilford Press.

Featherstone, M. (1982). The body in consumer culture. nieory Culhire & Society. 1(2), 18-33,

Ferreira, M. K. L. (1998). Slipping through sky holes: Yurok body imagery in northern Califomia. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 22, 17 1-202.

Fine, M. (1994). Working the hyphens: Reinventing self and other in qualitative research. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative research (pp. 70-82). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Fleiss, J. L. (198 1). The measurement of interrater agreement. In Author (Ed.), Stutistical methods for rates and proportions (2nd cd., pp. 212-236). Toronto, Ontario: John Wiley & Sons.

Fletcher, A. M. (1994). Thin for Life. Shelburne, VT: Chapters.

Foreyt, J. P., & Goodrick, G. K. (1991). Factors cornmon to successîÙ1 therapy for the obese patient. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 23,292-297.

French, S. A., Story, M., Downes, B., Resnick, M. D., & Blum, R. W. (1995). Frequent dieting among adolescents: Psychosocial and hedth behavior correlates. Amencan Journal of Public Health, 85(5), 695-7 10.

Friedman, S. S. (1994). Girls in the 90's: Facditator 's manual. , British Columbia: Sala1 Books.

Friedman, S. S. (1997). When girls feel fat: Helping girls through adolescence. Toronto, Ontario: HarperCoIlins.

Fumharn, A., & Alibhai, N. (1983). Cross-culmrd differences in the perception of femde body shapes. Psychological Medicine, 13,829-837. Galuska, D. A., Serdula, M., Pmuk, E., Siegel, P. S., & Byers, T. (1996). Trends in overweight among US adults from 1987 to 1993: A multistate telephone survey. American Journal of Public Health, 86,1729-1735.

Gm,S. M. (198 1). Socioeconomic aspects of obesity. Contemporary Nutrition, 6, 1-2.

Garner, D. M. (1997, February). The 1997 body image survey results. Psychology Today. pp. 30-44,7584.

Garner, D. M., & GafinkeI, P. E. (1979). The Eating Attitudes Test: An index of the symptoms of anorexia nervosa. Psychological Medicine, 9,273-279.

Garner, D. M., Garfinkel, P. E., Schwartz, D., & Thompson, M. (1980). Cultural expectations of thinness in women. Psychological Reports, 47, 483-491.

Garner, D. M., Olmsted, M. P., Bohr, Y., & Garfinkel, P. E. (1982). The Eating Attitudes Test: Psychomettic features and clinical correlates. Psychological Medicine, 12, 87 1-878.

Garner, D. M., Olmsted, M. P., & Polivy, J. (1983). Development and validation of a multidimensional eating disorder inventory for anorexia nervosa and bulimia. International Journal of Edng Disorders, 2, 15-34.

Garner, D. M., & Wooley, S. C. (1991). Confronting the failure of behavioral and dietary treatments for obesity. Clinical Psychology Review, 11,729-780.

Garro, L. C. (1983). Variation and consistency in a Mexican folk illness belief system. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Irvine.

Garro, L. C. (1986). Intracultural variation in folk medical knowledge: A comparison between curers and noncurers. Arnerican Anthropologist, 88,351-370.

Garro, L. C. (1988). Explaining high blood pressure: Variation in knowledge about ilhess. Amencan Ethnologist, I5,98-119.

Garro, L. C. (1995). Individual or societd responsibility? Explanations of diabetes in an Anishinaabe (Ojibway) community. Social Science and Medicine, 40,37-46.

Garro, L. C. (1996). Intracuhrai variation in causal accounts of diabetes: A comparison of t hree Canadian Anishinaabe (Oji bway) communities. Culrure. Medicine and Psychiatry, 20, 14. Garro, L. C. (2000). Remembering what one knows and the construction of the past: A comparison of cultural consensus theory and cultural schema theory. Ethos, 28, 275-3 19.

Garro, L. C., & Lang, G. C. (1993). Explanations of diabetes: Anishinaabeg and Dakota deliberate upon a new illness. In J. R. Joe & R. S. Young (Eds.), Diabetes as a disease of civilization: The impact of culture change on Indigenous Peoples (pp. 293-328). New York: Mouton de Gruyter-

GiIIigan, C., Rogers, A. G., & Tolman, D. L. (Eds.). (1991). Women, girls & psychotherapy: Refiaming resistance. New York: Haworth Press.

Gingras, J. R. (1998). Body image dissatisfaction: A framework of development and recommendations for dietitians. Canadian Journal of Dietetic Practice and Research, 59, 132-137.

Gittelsohn, J., Hanis, S. B., Thome-Lyman, A. I., Hanley, A. J. G., Bamie, A., & Zinman, B. (1996). Body image concepts differ by age and sex in an Ojibway-Cree community in Canada. Joumal of Nutrition, 126,2990-3000.

Glickman, R. L. (1993). Daughters of feminists. New York: St. Martin's Press.

Goldblatt, P. B., Moore, M. E., & Stunkard, A. J. (1965). Social factors in obesity. Journal of the American Medical Association. 192, 1039- 1044.

Grâber, J. A., Archibald, A. B., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1999). The role of parents in the emergence, maintenance, and prevention of eating problems and disorders. In N. Piran, M. P. Levine, & C. Steiner-Adair (Eds.), Preventing eating disorders: A handbook of interventions and special challenges (pp. 44-62). Levittown, PA: Brunner/Mazel.

Gralen, S. J., Levine, M. P., Smolak, L., & Mumen, S. K. (1990). Dieting and disordered eating dunng early and rniddle adolescence: Do the influences remain the same? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 9, 50 1-5 12.

Greaves, E. (1997, Manih 29). Wnter bridges poverty gap. The Globe and Mail, p. D10.

Green, K. L., Cameron, R., Polivy, J., Cooper, K., Liu, L., Leiter, L., Heatherton, T., & Canadian Heart Health Surveys Research Group. (1997). Weight dissatisfaction and weight loss attempts arnong Canadian adults. Canadian Medical Association Joumal, 157(1 Suppl.), S17-S25. Green, R-(1 980). Native Amencan women: Review essay. Signs: Journal of Women in Culture and Society, 6,248-267.

Grilo, C. M., Wilfiey, D. E., Brownell, K. D., & Rodin, J. (1994). Teasing, body image, and self-esteem in a clinical sarnple of obese women. Addictive Behaviors. 19, 443-450.

Grogan, S. (1999). Body image: Understanding body dissatisfactiun in men. women and children. New York: Routledge.

Guba, E. G., & Lincoln, Y. S. (1994). Competing pmdigms in qualitative research. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative research (pp. 105- 117). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Hall, T. R., Hickey, M. E., & Young, T. B. (1993). Many farms revisited: Evidence of increasing weight and non-insulin dependent diabetes in a Navajo community. In J. R. Joe & R. S. Young (Eds.), Diabetes as a disease of civilization: The impact of culture change on Indigenous Peoples (pp. 129-146). New York: Mouton de Gruyt er.

Hali, J. M., & Stevens, P. E. (1991). Rigor in feminist research. Advances in Nursing Science. 13(3), 16-29.

Hamilton, A. C., & Sinclair, C. M. (1991). RepoH of the Aboriginal Justice Inquiry of Manitoba. Volume I. The justice system and Aboriginal people. Winnipeg, Manitoba: Queen's enter.

Hanley, A. J. G., Hamis, S. B., Gittelsohn, J., Wolever, T. M. S., Saksvig, B., & Zinman, B. (2000). Overweight among children and adolescents in a Native Canadian community: Prevalence and associated factors. American Journul of Clinical Nutririon, 71,693-700.

Harnack, L., Story, M., Rock, B. H., Neumark-Sztainer, D., Jeffery, R., & French, S. (1999). Nutrition beliefs and weight loss practices of Lakota Indian adults. Journal of Nutrition Eîiucation, 3I,lO- 15.

Harrison, G. G., & Ritenbaugh, C. K. (1992). Obesity among North Amencan Indians. In P. Bjomtorp & B. N. Brodoff (Eds.), Obesiîy (pp. 610-618). Philadelphia: J. B. Lippincott.

Harwood, A. (198 1). Introduction, Ethniciîy and medical care (pp. 1-36). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Hassard, T. (199 1). Understanding Biostatistics- St Louis, MO: Mosby.

Hau .dek, C., Roity, M., & Henker, B. (1999). The role of ethnicity and parental bonding in the eating and weight concems of Asian-Amencan and Caucasian college women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 25,425433-

Havens, B. (197 1 - 1996). Aging in Manitoba Study Interview. Winnipeg: University of Manitoba.

Health and Welfare Canada. (1975). Nutrition Canada: The Indian Su- report. Ottawa, Ontario: Author.

Health and Welfare Canada. (1988a). Promoting healthy weight~A discussion paper. Ottawa, Ontario: Author.

Heal th and Welfare Canada. (1988b). Canadian guidelines for healthy weights: Repon of an expert group convened by Health Promotion Directorate, Health Services and Promotion Branch. Ottawa, Ontario: Author.

Health and Welfare Canada. (1991a). Repon of the Task Force on the Treatment of Obesiiy. Ottawa, Ontario: Author.

Health and Welfare Canada. (1991b). Vitality (Cat No. H39-230/1991E). Ottawa, Ontario: Minister of Supply and Services, Canada.

Health Canada. (2000). The Vitality approach: A guide for leaders (Cat No. H39- 525/20E). Ottawa, Ontario: Author.

Heatherton, T. F., Herman, C. P., Polivy, J., King, G. A., & McGree, S. T. (1988). The (mis)measurement of restraint: An analysis of conceptuai and psychometric issues. Journal of Abnonnal Psychology, 97, 19-28.

Heman, C. P., & Mack, D. (1975). Restrained and unrestrained eating. Journal of Personality, 43,647-660.

Heman, C. P., & Polivy, J. (1975). Anxiety, restraint, and eating behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 84,666-672.

Herman, C. P., & Polivy, J. (1980). Restrained eating. In A. Stunkard (Ed.), Obesity (pp. 208-225). Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders.

Hesse-Biber, S. (1996). Am 2 thin enough yet? The cult of thinness and the commercialization of identity. New York: Oxford University Press. Hickey, M. E., & Carter, J. S. (1993). Cultural barriers to delivenng health cm:The non- Indian provider perspective. In J. R. Joe & R. S. Young (Eds.), Diabetes as a diseuse of civilization: The impact of culture change on Indigenous Peoples (pp. 453-470). New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

Hill, A. J. (In press). Body dissatisfaction and dieting in children- In P. J. Cooper & A. Stein (Eds.), Feeding problems and eating disorders in children and adolescents . Ready: Harwood Academic.

Hill, A. J., & Franklin, J. A. (1998). Mothers, daughters, and dieting: Investigating the transmission of weight control. British Journal of Clinical P~ol~gy,37,3-13.

Hill, A. J., & Pallin, V. (1998). Dieting awareness and low self-worth: Related issues in 8-year-old girls. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 24,405413-

Hill, A. J., Weaver, C., & Blundell, J. E. (1990). Dieting concerns of 10-year-old girls and their mothers. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 29,346-348.

Hintze, J. L. (1997). Number Cruncher Statistical System 1997 [Computer program & reference manual]. Kaysville, UT: Author.

Honeycutt, K. (1999). Fat worldhhin world: "Fat busters," "Equivocators," "Fat boosters," and the social construction of obesity. In J. Sobal & D. Maurer (Eds.), Interpreting Weight: The social management of futness and thinness (pp. 165- 18 1). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

Huberman, A. M., & Miles, M. B. (1994). Data management and analysis methods. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative research (pp. 428- 444). Thousand O&, CA: Sage.

Hubert, L., & Schultz, J. V. (1976). Quadratic assignment as a general data analysis strategy. British Journal of Mathematicd and Statisticd Psychology, 29, 190-241.

Jackson, M. Y. (1993). Height, weight, and body mass index of American Indian schoolchildren, 1990-1991. Journal of the Arne* Dietetic Association. 93, 1136-1 140.

Jarnieson, K. (1981). Sisters under the skin: An exploration of the implications of feminist-rnaterialist perspective research. Cunudian Ethnic Stzdies. 13, 130-143.

Joanisse, L., & Synnott, A. (1999). Fighting back: Reactions and resistance to the stigma of obesity. In J. Sobal & D. Maurer (Eds.), Interpreting Weight: The social management of fatness and thinness (pp. 49-70). New York: Aldine de Gruyter. Joe, J. R. (1993). Perceptions of diabetes by Indian adolescents. In J. R. Joe & R S. Young (Eds.), Diabetes as a diseuse of civilization: The impact of culture change on Zndigenous Peoples (pp. 329-356). New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

Johnson, C. A. (1995). Self-esteem comes in al1 sizes: How to be happy and healthy ut your natural weight (2nd ed.). Toronto, Ontario: Doubleday/Main Street Books.

Katzman, M. A., & Lee, S. (1997). Beyond body image: The integration of feminist and transcultural theories in the understanding of self starvation. International Journal of Euting Disorders, 22,385-394.

Katzmarzyk, P. T., & Malina, R. M. (1998). Obesity and relative subcutaneous fat distribution among Canadians of First Nation and European ancestry. Zntemational Journal of Obesity, 22, 1127-1 131.

Kaufert, P. A., & O'Neil, J. (1993). Analysis of a dialogue on risks in childbirth. In S. Lindenbaum & M. Lock (Eds.), Knowledge, Power & Practice: The unthropology of rnedicine and everyday life (pp. 32-54). Berkeley: University of California.

Keel, P. K., Heathenon, T. F., Hamden, J. L., & Homig, C. D. (1997). Mothers, fathers, and daughters: Dieting and disordered eating. Euting Disorders, 5,216-228.

Kempton, W., Boster, J. A., & Hartley, J. A. (1995). Environmenral values in American culture. Cambridge, MA: MIT.

Korkeila, M., Rissanen, A., Kapno, J., Sorensen, T. 1. A., & Koskenvuo, M. (1999). Weight-loss attempts and nsk of major weight gain: A prospective study in Finnish adults. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 70,965-975.

Koslow, R. E. (1988). Age-related resons [sic] for expressed interest in exercise and weight control. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 18, 349-3 54.

Krefting, L. (1991). Rigor in qualitative research: The assessrnent of tmstworthiness. Arnerican Journal of Occupational Therapy, 45,214-222.

Lachapelle, C. (1982). Beyond barriers: Native women and the women's movernent. In M. Fitzgerald, C. Guberrnan, & M. Wolfe (Eds.), Still ain 't satisfied! Canadian feminism today (pp. 257-264). Toronto, Ontario: The Women's Press.

Lafrance, M., Zivian, M. T., & Myers, A. M. (2000). Women, weight and appearance satisfaction: An ageless pursuit of thinness. In B. Miedema, J. M. Stoppard, & V. Anderson (Eds.), Women's bodied'omen 's lives: Health, well-being and body image (pp. 227-236). Toronto, Ontario: Sumach Press. LaFromboise, T. D., Heyle, A. M., & Ozer, E. J. (1990). Changing and diverse roles of women in American Indian cultures. Sex Roles, 22,455-476.

Lambert-Lagace, L. (1989). The nutrition challenge for women: A guide to wellness without dieting. Toronto, Ontario: Stoddart.

Lang, G. C. (1989). "Making sense" about diabetes: Dakota narratives of illness. Medical Anthropology, 11,305-327.

Leichner, P., Arnett, J., Rallo, J- S., Srikameswaran, S., & Vulcano, B. (1986). An epidemiologic study of maladaptive eating attitudes in a Canadian school age population. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5,969-982.

Levine, M. P., & Piran, N. (1999). Reflections, conclusions and future directions. In N. Piran, M. P. Levine, & C. Steiner-Adair (Eds.), Preventing eating disorders: A handbook of interventions and special challenges (pp. 3 19-329). Levittown, PA: Brunner/Mazel.

Levine, M. P., & Smolak, L. (1998). The mass media and disordered eating: Implications for pnmary prevention. In W. Vandereycken & G. Nwrdenbos (Eds.), The prevention of eating disorders (pp. 23-56). New York: New York University Press.

Lifshitz, F., & Moses, N. (1988). Nutritional dwarfing: Growth, dieting, and fear of obesity. Joumal of the American College of Nutrition, 7,367-376.

Limbert, J., Crawford, S. M., & McCargar, L. J. (1994). Estimates of the prevalence of obesity in Canadian children. Obesizy Research, 2,321-327.

Lock, M., & Kaufert, P. A. (1998). Introduction. In M. Lock & P. Kaufert (Eds.), Pragmatic women and body politics (pp. 1-27). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

Macaulay, A., Cross, E. J.9 Saad-Haddad, C., McComber, A. M., Kirby, R., & Paradis, G. (1996). The Kahnawake Schools Diabetes Prevention Project: Intervention, Theory, reality, hope: Proceedings of the 3rd Intemational Conference on Diabetes and Zndigenous Peoples (pp. 83-86). Winnipeg, Manitoba.

Macdonald, S., Reeder, B., Chen, Y., Despres, J., & the Canadian Heart Health Surveys Research Group. (1997). Obesity in Canada: a descriptive analysis. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 157(1 suppl.), S3-S9. Mackenzie, M. (1985). The pursuit of slenderness and addiction to self-control: An anthropological interpretation of eating disorders. Nutrition Updne, 2, 174-194.

Marchessault, G. (1991, Feb. 20). Diet: A four-letter word The Winnipeg Sun, p. 21.

Marchessault, G. (1998). Urban Aboriginal mothers' and daughters' expressed concems about weight: An interview study. In J. Oakes & R. Riewe (Eds.), Issues in the No~h(Vol. III, pp. 3- 12). Winnipeg, Manitoba: Canadian Circumpolar Institute: . (Ftepnnted from Theory, Reality, Hope: Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Diabetes and Zndigenous Peoples, 1995, 106-11 l]

Marc hessault ,G. (1999). Weigh t perceptions and practices in Native youth. Healthy Weight Journal, 13,71-73,79.

Marchessault, G. (2000). One rnother and daughter approach to resisting weight preoccupation. In B. Miederna, J. M. Stoppard, & V. Anderson (Eds.), Women S bodiesM.'ornen's lives: Health, well-being and bodj image (pp. 203-226). Toronto, Ontario: Sumach Press.

Marchessault, G., Spigelman, M., Ornichinski, L., & Harrison, K. (1999). What's in a narne? Debate invited on "Obesity Canada" vs "Vitality" approach. Practice: A practice update publication of Dietitians of Canada, 6, 3.

Marchessault, G. D. M. (1993a). How mothers and daughrers talk about weight. Unpublished master's thesis, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg.

Marchessault, G. D. M. (1993b). Weight preoccupation in North Amencan culture. Journal of the Canadian Dietetic Association, 54, 138- 142.

Mars hall, C., & Rossman, G. B. (1 989). Designing qualitative research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Martens, P. J. (1999). Evaluating the efectiveness of a breasfeeding promotion conzrnunity strategy in Sagkeeng First Nation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg.

Martin, E. (1987). The woman in the bdy: A cultural analysis of reproduction. Boston, MA: Beacon Press.

McCabe, R. E., Mills, J. S., & Polivy, 3. (1999). Exploding the mm: Dieters eat less than nondieters. Healthy Weight Journal, 13, 11- 13. McCargar, L. J., & Yeung, H. (1991). The effects of weight cycluig on metabolism and health. Journal of the Cunadian Dietetic Association, 52, 101-106.

McCourt, J., & Waller, G. (1995). Developmental role of perceived parental control in the eating psychopathology of Asian and Caucasian schoolgirls. Zntenzatio~l Journal of Eating Disorders, 17,277-282.

Mchtyre, L., & Shah, C. P. (1986). Prevalence of hypertension, obesity and smoking in three hdian communities in northwestern On tario. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 134,345-349.

Mechanic, D. (1989)- Medical sociology: Some tensions among theory, method, and substance. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 30, 147-160.

Miller, W. C., Koceja, D. M., & Hamilton, E. J. (1997). X meta analysis of the past 25 years of weight loss research using diet, exercise or diet plus exercise intervention. International Journal of Obesiîy, 21,941-947.

Miller, W. C., & Lindeman, A. K. (1997). The role of diet and exercise in weight management. In S. Dalton (Ed.), Ovenveight and weight management: The health professionalk guide to understanding and practice (pp. pp. 405). Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen.

Millman, M. (1980). Such a pretïy face: Being fat in America. New York: W.W. Norton.

Mohs, M. E., Leonard, T. K., & Watson, R. R. (1988). Interrelationships arnong alcohol abuse, obesity, and type II diabetes mellitus: Focus on Native Americans. World Rev Nutr Diet, 56,93-172.

Mokdad, A. H., Serdula, M. K., Dietz, W. H., Bowman, B. A., Marks, J. S., & Koplan, J. P. (1999). The Spread of the Obesity Epidemic in the United States, 1991-1998. Journal of the Arnehn Medical Association, 282, 1519- 1522.

Mokdad, A. H., Serdula, M. K.. Dietz, W. H., Bowman, B. A., Marks, J. S., & Koplan, J. P. (2000). The continuing epidernic of obesity in the United States [Research letter]. Journal of the American Medical Association, 284, 1650-1651. Retrieved Nov. 30,200 from http://jama.ama-assn.org/issues/vl28h 16 13/ffuWjlt1004- 1654.html

Monture, P. (1993). 1know rny name: A First Nations woman speaks. In G. Finn (Ed.), Limited edition: Voices of women, voices of feminism (pp. 328-344). Halifax, Nova Scotia: Fernwood. Monture-Angus, P. (1995). Organizing against oppression: Aboriginal women, law and feminism, Thunder in my soul: A Mohawk woman speaks (pp. 169-188). Halifax, Nova Scotia: Femwood.

Moore, M. E., Stunkard, A., & Srole, L. (1962). Obesity, social class, and mental illness. Journal of the AmehMedical Association, 281,962-966-

Moore, P. E., Kmse, H. D., Tisdall, R. F., & Comgan, R. S. (1946). Medical swvey of nutrition among the northern Manitoba Indian. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 54,223-233. -

Morse, J. (1994). Designing funded qualitative research. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative research (pp. 220-235). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Mueller, W. H., Joos, S. K., & Schull, W. J. (1985). Alternative measurements of obesity: Accuracy of body silhouettes and reported weights and heights in a Mexican American sample. Zntentational Journal of Obesity, 9, 193-200.

National Institutes of Health, & National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. (1998). Clinical guidelines on the identification, evaluation, and treatment of overweight and obesity in adults--The evidence report. Obesity Research, 6(Suppl. 2).

Neumark-Sztainer, D., Jeffery, R. W., & French, S. A. (1997). Self reported dieting: How should we ask? What does it mean? Associations between dieting and reported energy intake. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 22,437-449.

Neumark-Sztainer, D., Story, M., Resnick, M. D., & Blum, R. W. (1998). Lessons learned about adolescent nutrition from the Minnesota Adolescent Health Survey. Joumal of the American Dietetic Association, 98, 144%1456.

Nichter, M. (2000). Fat talk: What girls and theirparents say about dieting. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Nichter, M., Ritenbaugh, C., Nichter, M., Vuckovic, N., & Aickin, M. (1995). Dieting and "watching" behaviors among adolescent fernales: Report of a multimethod study. J Adolesc Health, 17, 153-162.

Nichter, M., & Vuckovic, N. (1994). Fat talk: Body image among adolescent girls. In N. Sault (Ed.), Many Mimrs: Body image and social relations (pp. 109-131). New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University. Ogden, J., & Chanana, A. (1998). Explaining the effect of ethnic group on weight concern: Finding a role for family values. Zntemational Journal of Obesizy, 22, 64 1-647.

Ogden, J., & Elder, C. (1998). The role of family status and ethnic group on body image and eating behavior. Zntemational Journal of Eating Disorders, 23,309-3 15.

Ogden, J., & Steward, J. (2000). The role of the mother-daughter relationship in ex pl ai ning weight concern. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 28,78-83.

Olesen, V. (1994). Feminisms and models of qualitative research. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative research (pp. 158-174). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Omichinski, L., & Harrison, K. (2000). Weight management for teens: A non-diet approach to health andfimess for adolescents (2nd ed.). San Marcos, CA: Nutrition Dimensions.

Omichinski, L., & Hamson, K. R. (1995). Reduction in dieting attitudes and practices after participation in a non-diet lifestyle program. Journal of the Canadian Dietetic Association, 56,8 1-85.

Orbach, S. (1981). Fat is a feminist issue: A self-help guide for compulsive eaters. New York: Berkley Books.

Orenstein, P. (1994). SchoolGirls: Young women,self-esteem, and the confidence gap. Toronto, Ontario: Doubleday.

Osennontion and Skonogan1eh:ra. (1989). Our world: According to Osennontion and Skonogan1eh:ra. Canadian Woman Studies, 1O(2&3), 7-19.

Parker, S ., Nichter, M-, Nichter, M., Vuckovic, N., Sims, C., & Ritenbaugh, C. (1995). Body image and weight concems among African Amencan and White adolescent fernales: Differences that make a difference. Human Organization, 54, 103-1 14.

Patton, M. Q. (1990). Qualitative evaluation and research methods (2nd ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Paxton, S. J., Schutz, H. K., Wertheim, E. H., & Muir, S. L. (1999). Frïendship clique and peer influences on body image concems, dietary restra.int, extreme weight-loss behaviors, and binge eating in adolescent girls. Journal of Abnomal Psychology, 108,255-266. Perez, G. (1995, May). Nathe Arnerïcan Diabetes Project: Healthy Lijèstyle Curriculum- Paper presented at the 3rd International Conference on Diabetes and Indigenous Peoples, Winnipeg, Manitoba.

Pike, K. M., & Rodin, J. (199 1). Mothers, daughters, and disordered eating. Journal of Abnoml Psychology, 100,198-204.

Pipher, M. (1995). Reviving Ophelia: Saving the selves of adolescent girls. New York: Ballantine Books.

Piran, N. (1999). The reduction of preoccupation with body weight and shape in schools: A feminist approach. In N. Piran, M. P. Levine, & C. Steiner-Adair (Eàs.), Preventing eating disorders: A handbook of interventions and speciul challenges (pp. 148-159). Levittown, PA: Brunner/Mazel.

Pittaway, K. (1998, September 23). Take back the fat. The Globe and Mail, pp. A32.

Pliner, P., Chaiken, S., & Flen, G. L. (1990). Gender differences in concem with body weight and physical appearance over the life span. Perso~lizyand Social Psychology Bulletin, 15,263-273.

Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (1986). Why diet? Modem Medicine of Cana&, 41,573-576.

Postl, B. (1995). The health of Manitoba S Children. Winnipeg, Manitoba: Manitoba Health.

Postl, B., Moffatt, M., & Sarsfield, P. (1987). Epidemiology and interventions for Native Canadi ans [Editorial] . Canadian Journal of Public Health, 78, 2 19-220.

Potvin, L., Desrosiers, S., Tnfonopoulos, M., Leduc, N., Rivard, M., Macaulay, A. C., & Paradis, G. (1999). Anthropomebic characteristics of Mohawk children aged 6 to 11 years: A population perspective. Journal of the Arnerican Dietetic Association, 99,955-961.

Qualitative Solutions and Research Pty. (1997). QSR NUD*IST: Software for Qualitative Data Analysis wersion 4, Cornputer program and reference manual]. Thousand Oaks, CA: Scolari, Sage Publications Software.

Rabinor, J. R. (1994). Mothers, daughters, and eating disorders: Honoring the mother- daughter relationship. In P. Fallon, M. A. Katzman, & S. C. Wooley (Eds.), Feminist perspectives on eating disorders (pp. 272-286). New York: Guilford Press. Reeder, B. A., Chen, Y., Macdonald S. M., Angel, A., Sweet, L., & Canadian Hem Health Surveys Research Group. (1997). Regional and rural-urban differences in obesity in Canada. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 157, S 10-16.

Rice, C. (Rd.). (1993). Promoting healthy body image: A report and resource List for program planners. Based on a workshop held in October 1993for the Best Start demonstration sites. Toronto: Ontario Prevention Clearinghouse.

Riessman, C. K. (1987). When gender is not enough: Women interviewing women. Gender & Society, 1, 172-207.

Ritenbaugh, C., Kumanyika, S. K., Antipatis, V. J., Jeffery, R. W., & Morabia, A. (1999). Caught in the causal web: A new perspective on social factors affecting obesity. Healrhy Weight Jouml, 13,88-89.

Robertson, H.-J. (1991). A cappella: A report on the realities, concerns. expectations and barneersexperienced by adolescent women in Canoda. Ottawa, Ontario: Canadian Teachers' Federation.

Robison, J. (1998). Needed: New terminology for the new paradip. Healthy Weight Journal, 12,58-59.

Rodin, J., Silberstein, L., & Striegel-Moore, R. (1985). Women and weight: A nonnative discontent. In 1. B. Sonderegger (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 32. Psychology and Gender, pp. 267-307). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

Rolland-Cachera, M. F., Sempe, M., Guilloud-Bataille, M., Patois, E., Pequignot- Guggenbuhl, F., & Fautrad, V. (1982). Adiposity indices in children. Americun Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 36, 178-184.

Romney, A. K., Batchelder, W. H., & Weller, S. C. (1987). Recent applications of cul turai consensus theory. American Behavioral Scientist. 31, 163- 177.

Romney, A. K., Weller, S. C., & Batchelder, W. H. (1986). Culture as consensus: A theory of culture and informant accuracy. American Anthropologist. 88,3 13-338.

Rosen, L. W., Shafer, C. L., Dummer, G. M., Cross, L. K., Deuman, G. W., & Malmberg, S. R. (1988a). Prevalence of pathogenic weightcontrol behaviors arnong Native American women and girls. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 7, 807- 811. Rosen, J. C., Silberg, N. T., & Gross, J. (1988b). Eating Attitudes Test and Eating Disorders Inventory: Noms for adolescent girls and boys. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 305-308.

Rothblum, E. D. (1990). Women and weight: Fad and fiction. The Journal of Psychology, 124,524.

Royal Commission on Aboriginal Peoples. (1996). Report of the Royal Commission on Aboriginal Peoples. Ottawa, Ontario: Minister of Supply and Services, Canada.

Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. (1988). Body image, attitudes to weight, and misperceptions of figure preferences of the opposite sex: A cornparison of men and women in two generations. Journal of Abnonnal Psychology, 97,342-345.

Ruderman, A. J. (1986). Dietary restraint: A theoretical and empirical review. Psycholog ical Bulletin, 99,247-262.

Ruther, N. M., & Richman, C. L. (1993). The relationship between mothers' eating restrain t and their children's attitudes and behaviors. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 31,217-220.

Satter, E. (1987). How tu get your kid to eut ... but not too much. Pa10 Alto, CA: Bull.

Schachter, S. (1 982). Recidivism and self-cure of smoking and obesity. American Psychologist, 37,436-444.

Schneider, J. A. (1986). Rewriting the SES: Demographic patterns and divorcing families. Social Science and Medicine, 23,211-222.

Schwartz, D. J., Phares, V., Tantleff-Dunn, S., & Thompson, J. K. (1999). Body image, psychoiogical functioning, and parental feedback regarding physical appearance. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 25,339-343.

Shakespeare, W. (1963). Hamlet. In C. Hoy (Ed.), Hamlet: An aurhoritative tes, inrellectual backgrounds; extractsji-om the sources, essays in criticism (pp. 3-98). New York: W. W. Norton. (Original published 1604)

Silberstein, L. R.., Striegel-Moore, R. H., Timko, C., & Rodin, J. (1988). Behaviord and psychological implications of body dissatisfaction: Do men and women differ? Sex Roles, 19,S19-232.

Silverman, D. (1993). Interview data, Znterpreting qualitative data: Metho& for analyzing talk ta,and interaction (pp. 90-1 14). London: Sage. Silverstein, B., & Perlick, D. (1995). The cost of cornpetence: Why inequality causes depression, eating disorders, and illness in women. New York: Oxford University Press.

Sinclaire, M. (1997). Barneersto food procuremenc nte experience of urban Aboriginal women in Winnipeg. Unpublished master's thesis, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg.

Srnead, V. S., & Richert, A. J. (1990). Eating Attitude Test factors in an unselected undergraduate population. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 9, 211-215.

Smith, J. E., & Krejci, J. (199 1). Minonties join the majority: Eating disturbances among Hispanic and Native American youth. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 10, 179-186.

Smolak, L.,& Levine, M. P. (1996). Adolescent transitions and the development of eating problems. In L. Smolak & M. P. Levine (Eds.), The developmental psychopathology of eating disorders (pp. 207-233). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Smolak, L., Levine, M. P., & Schermer, F. (1999). Parental input and weight concems among elementary school children. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 25, 263-27 1.

Snow, J. T., & Harris, M. B. (1989). Disordered eating in South-western Pueblo Indians and Hispanics. Journal of Adolescence, 12,329-336.

Sobal, 1. (199 1). Obesity and socioeconomic status: A framework for examining relationships between physical and social variables. Medicol Anthropology, 13, 23 1-247-

Sobal, J., & Stunkard, A. J. (1989). Socioeconomic status and obesity: A review of the literature. P.sychological Bulletin, 105,260-275.

Spielmann, R. (1993). "You're so fat!" Cultural differences in collaborative research. In A. Mawhiney (Ed.), Rebinh: Political, economic, and social development in First Nations (pp. 3442). Toronto, Ontario: Dundum Press.

Spitzack, C. (1988). Body talk: The politics of weight loss and female identity. In B. Bates & A. Taylor (WS.), Women communicating: Szudies of woman's talk. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Spitzack, C. (1990). Confessing excess: Women and the polirirs of body reduction. Albany, New York: State University of New York Press.

Stacey-Moore, G. (1993). In our own voice: Aboriginal women demand justice. Henions: Women 's News & Ferninist Views, 6(4), 2 1-23.

Stati stics Canada. (1993). Zncome a#er tax, distributions by size in Canada: 1991 (Catalogue number 13-210). Ottawa, Ontario: Minister of Supply and Services, Canada.

St ati stics Canada. (1994). Profle of census divisions and subdivisions in Manitoba, Pan B (Catalogue number 95-359). Ottawa, Ontario: Minister of Industry, Science and Technology, Canada.

Stati stics Canada. (1995a). National Population Health Survery Overview 1994-95 (Cat . No. 82-567). Ottawa, Ontario: Health Statistics Division.

Statistics Canada. (1995b). Profile of Canada k Aboriginal Population: Aboriginal data (Catalogue number 94-329). Ottawa, Ontario: Minister of Industry, Science and Technology, Canada.

Steiger, H., Stotland, S., Trottier, J., & Ghadirian, A. M. (1996). Familial eating concems and psychopathological traits: Causal implications of transgenerational effects. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 19, 147-157.

Stevens, J., Story, M., Becenti, A., French, S. A., Gittelsohn, J., Juhaeri, S. B. G., Levin, S., & Murray, D. M. (1999). Weight-related attitudes and behaviors in fourth grade Amencan Indian children. Obesiry Research, 7,1442.

Stewart, A. L. (1982). The reliability and validity of self-reported weight and height. J. Chron. Dis, 35,295-309.

Stice, E. (1994). Review of the evidence for a sociocultural mode1 of bulimia nervosa and an exploration of the mechanisms of action. Clinical Psychology Raiew, 14,633- 661.

Stice, E. (1998). Prospective relation of dieting behaviors to weight change in a community sample of adolescents. Behavior-nierapy, 29,277-297.

Story, M., Evans, M., Fabsitz, R. R., Clay, T. E., Holy, R. B., & Broussard, B. (1999). The epidemic of obesity in Amencan Indian communities and the need for childhood obesi ty-preven tion programs. Amencan Joumal of Clinical Nutrition. 69(4 Suppl. S), 747s-7543. Story, M., French, S. A., Resnick, M. D., & Blum, R. W. (1995). Ethnic/racial and socioeconomic differences in dieting behaviors and body image perceptions in adolescents. International Joumal of Eating Disorders, 18, 173-179.

Story, M., Hauck, F. R., Broussard, B. A., White, L. L., Resnick, M. D., & Blum, R. W. (1994). Weight perceptions and weight control practices in Amencan Indian and Alaska Native adolescents: A national survey. Archives Pediatnc and Adolescent Medicine, 148,567-671.

Story, M., Strauss, K. F., Zephier, E., & Broussard, B. A. (1998). Nutritional concems in American Indian and Alaska Native children: Transitions and future directions. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 98, 170-176.

Story, M., Tompkins, R. A., Bass, M. A., & Wakefield, L. M. (1986). Anthropomeüic measurements and dietary intakes of Cherokee Indian teenagers in North Carolina. Joumal of the American Dietetic Association, 86, 1555-1560.

StriegeI-Moore, R. H., & Keamey-Cooke, A. (1994). ExpIoring parents' attitudes and behaviors abut their children's ph ysical appearance. International Joumal of Eating Disorders, 15,377-385.

Striegel-Moore, R. H., Silberstein, L. R., & Rodin, J. (1986). Toward an understanding of risk factors for bulimia. American Psychologist, 41,246-263.

Stunkard, A., Sorensen, T., & Schulsinger, F. (1980). Use of the Danish Adoption Register for the study of obesity and thinness. In S. Kety (Ed.), The genetics of neurological and psychiatnk disorders (pp. 1 15-120). New York: Raven.

Stunkard, A. J., & Albaum, J. M. (1981). The accuracy of self-reported weights. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 34, 1593-1599.

Stunkard, A. J., & Messick, S. (1985). The Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. Journal of Psychosornatic Research, 29,71-83.

Sugannan, J. R., White, L. L., & Gilbert, T. J. (1990). Evidence for a secular change in obesity, height, and weight among Navajo Indian school children. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 52,960-966.

Swarr, A. E., & Richards, M., H. (1996). Longitudinal effects of adolescent girls'pubertal development, perceptions of pubeital timing, and parental relations on eating problems. Developmentd Psychology, 32,636-646. Terry, R. D., & Bass, M. A. (1984). Obesity among Eastern Cherokee Indian women: Prevalence, self-perceptions and experiences. Ecology of Food and Nurrïtion, 14, 117-127.

Teufel, N. I., & Dufour, D. L. (1990). Patterns of food use and nutrient intake of obese and non-obese Hualapai hdian women of Arizona- Journal of the Arnen'can Dietetic Association, 90, 122%123 5.

Thelen, M. H., & Cormier, J. F. (1995). Desire to be thinner and weight control among children and their parents. Special Series: Body dissatisfaction, binge eating, and dieting as interlocking issues in eating disorders research. Behmior Therapy, 26, 85-99.

Thompson, B. W. (1992). "A way outa no way": Eating problems among African- American, Latina, and White women. Gender & Society, 6,546-56 1.

Thompson, B. W. (1996). A hunger so wide and so deep: A multiracial view of women's eating problems. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

Thompson, J. K., & Heinberg, L. J. (1999). The media's influence on body image disturbance and eating disorders: We've reviled hem, now can we rehabilitate them? Journal of Social Issues, 55,339-353.

Tiggeman, M., & Wilson-Barrett, E. (1998). Children's figure ratings: Relationship to seif-esteem and negative stereotyping. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 23, 83-88.

Tookenay, V. F. (1996). Irnproving the health status of aboriginal people in Canada: New directions, new responsibilities. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 155, 1581-1583.

Travers, K. D. (1 997). Reducing inequities through participatory research and community empowerrnent. Health Education and Behavior, 24,344-356.

Tremblay, M. S., & Willms, J. D. (2000). Secular trends in the Body Mass index of Canadian children. Canadian Medical Association Joumal, 163,1429- 1433. Retrieved Oct. 16, 2000 from http://www.cma.ca/cmaj/vol-1163lissue-141 llissue- 141l.htm

Tsosie, R. (1988). Changing women: The cross-currents of Amencan Indian ferninine identity . American Indian Culture and Research Journal, 12, 1-37. Tuckcl; R. (2000, Nov. 30 - Dec. 2). Young women's experiences with dieting and weight watching. Poster presented at the Health Policy Research Nexus: A Conference for NHRDP Personnel Award Holders, Kingston, Ontario.

Van Strien, T.,Fnjters, J. E., Bergers, G. P., & Defares, P. B. (1985)- Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire for assessrnent of restrained, emotional, and external eating behavior. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5,295-3 15.

Vivian, R. P., McMillan, C., Moore, P. E., Robertson, E. C., Sebrell, W. H., Tisdall, F. F., & McIntosh, W. G. (1948). The nutrition and health of the James Bay Indian. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 59,505-5 18.

Wakewich, P. (2000). Contours of everyday life: Women's reflections on embodiment and health over time. Ln B-Miedema, J. M. Stoppard, & V. Anderson (Eds.), Women S budiesmumen 's lives: Health, well-being and body image (pp. 237- 253). Toronto, Ontario: Sumach Press.

Waldram, J. B., Hemng, D. A., & Young, T. K. (1995). New epidernics in the twentieth cen tury, Aboriginal health in Canada: Historical, cultural, and epidemiological perspectives (pp. 65-96). Toronto, Ontario: University of Toronto Press.

Walters, V., Lenton, R., & Mckeary, M. (1995). Wornen'k health in the context of women's lives: A repofl submitted to the Healrh Promotion Directorate, Health Canada. Ottawa, Ontario: Health Canada.

Waterhouse, D. (1997). Like mother, like daughtec How wornen are influenced by their mothers' relationship with food and how to break the pattern. New York: Hyperion.

Weller, S. C., Pachter, L. M., Trotter II, R. T.. & Baer, R. D. (1993). Empacho in four Latino groups: A study of intra- and intercultural variation in beliefs. Medical Anthropology, 15,109-136.

Weller, S. C., & Romney, A. K. (1988). Systernaiic data collection. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Welty, T. K. (1991). Health implications of obesity in Amencan Indians and Alaska Natives. Amencan Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 53(6 Suppl.), 16 16s-1620s.

West, K. M. (1974). Diabetes in American Indians and other Native populations of the New World. Diabetes, 23,841-855 White, K. L. (n-d.). Reflections on the pst and challengesfor the fiture. The U.S . National Cornmittee on Vital & Health Statistics. Retrieved May 9,2001 from h ttp://ncvhs.hhs.gov/ncvhs5Owhite.htm

Will, J. C., Denny, C., Serdula, M., & Muneta, B. (1999). Trends in body weight among American Indians: Findings from a telephone survey, 1985 through 1996. American Joumal of Public Health, 89,395-398.

Wilson, ReTGraham, C., Booth, K. G.3 Sr Gohdes, D. (1993). Community approaches to diabetes prevention. In J. R. Joe & R. S. Young (Eds.), Diabetes as a disease of civilization: rite impact of culture change on Indigenous Peoples (pp. 495-503). New York: Mouton de Gmyter.

Winnipeg School Division No. 1. (1996). School demographics - 1994/95. Winnipeg, Manitoba: Author.

Wiseman, C. V., Gray, J. J., Mosirnann, J. E., & Ahrens, A. H. (1992). Cultural expectations of thinness in women: An update. International Joumal of Eating Disorders, 11, 85-89.

Wolf, A. M., & Colditz, G. A. (1998). Current estimates of the economic cost of obesity in the United States. Obesity Research, 6,97-106.

Wood, A., Waller, G., Miller, J., & Slade, P. (1992). The development of Eating Attitude Test scores in adolescence. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 11,279- 282.

World Health Organization. (1998). Obesity: Preventing and rnanaging the global epidemic. Report of a WHO consultation on obesity. Geneva: Author.

Wright, J. (1994). A Stdy of the female perception of healthy body weights: A cornparison between health professionals and a randornized group outside the hospital. Unpublished manuscript in author's possession.

Young, T. K. (1994). Emergence of chronic diseases. In The health of Native Americans. Toward biocultural epidemiology (pp. 94-175). New York: Oxford University Press.

Young, T. K., Dean, H. J., Flett, B., & Wood-Steiman, P. (2000). Childhood obesity in a population at high risk for type 2 Diabetes. Joumal of Pediatrics, 136,365369.

Young, T. K., & Sevenhuysen, G. (1989). Obesity in northern Canadian Indians: Patterns, determinants, and consequences. Arne- humai of Clhical Nutrition, 49,786- 793. APPENDICES Descriptions of Questionnaires

Weight/Eatino Related Scales

Bodv Mass Index @MI) kilogram divided by metres squared.

Versions of the Eatina Attitudes Test (EAT) EAT-40 (Garner & GarFrnkei, 1979) -This 40-item, 6-point Likert scale (with responses fkom never to always) indicates the presence of disturbed eating patterns or eating pathology. Scores of over 30 indicate risk of an eating disorder (used by Marchessault, 1994; 28 questions selected by Hi11 et al., 1990). EAT-26 (Garner, Olmstead, Bohr, & Garfinkel, 1982) -This 26-item version of the EAT measures the extent of body weight concem and dieting behaviows. Scores of 20 or more indicative of eating disorder risk (used by Thelen & Connier, 1995; Steiger et al, 1996). -There are three subscales: the Diet subscale assesses pathological avoidance of fattening foods and preoccupation with thinness, with higher scores indicative of more problematic eating and pathology. The Bulimia and food ~reoccu~ation subscale has items related to thoughts about food and bulimic behaviours such as binging and vomiting. The Oral control subscale measures self-control of eating and perceived social pressure to gain weight. (Byely et al., 2000 used 7 items of the Diet subscale. Anie & Brooks-Gunn, 1989 used 3 items from each factor.) ch(EAT) (Mdoney, McGuire & Daniels, 1988 adapted EAT-26) -The EAT-26 was simplified with third grade level language. It was also a 6-point scale (used by Thelen & Cormier, 1995).

Eating Disorder Inventory (Garner, Olmsted, & Polivy, 1983) -This 64-item self-report, multiscale measure assesses psychological and behavioural traits common to anorexia nervosa and bulirnia There are three subscales measuring attitudes and behaviours related to disordered eating: Drive for thinness. Bulimia. and Body dissatisfaction. Five subscaies measuing psychological constructs thought to be related to psychopathology of eating disorders are: Ineffectiveness, Perfectionism, Intemersonal distrust, Lack of interoceative awareness and Maturitv fears (used by Pike & Rodin, 1991).

Scales to Measure Restrained Eating Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (Van Strien, Frijters, Bergers & Defares, 1986) -The Restrained eatine section consists of a 33-item questionnaire that assesses dietary restraint, emotional eating, and extemal eating behaviour. Each scale has a range of means from 1 to 5, with higher scores indicating higher levels of restraint (used by Hill & Franklin, 1998; Ogden & Steward, 2000; Steiger et al, 1996; Ogden & Elder, 1998; Ogden & Chanana, 1998). Three Factor Eating Questionnaire (Stunkarà & Messick, 1985) -Three subscales assessing cognitive (dietary) resuaint, emotionally based disinhibited eating and perceived hunger. The Dietq restraint subscale measures cognitive control of fdintake and assesses the restriction of certain foods, energy intake or macronutrients (e.g., "1 count calories as a conscious means of controlling my weight. ") The Dietarv disinhibition subscale measures overeating and the extent to which eating is triggered by emotional, social and environmental influences ("Sometimes things jus? taste so good that 2 keep on eating even when I am no longer hungry." (used by Cutting et al. 1999; Stice et al. 1999; Birch & Fisher, 2000). Restra.int Scale merman & Polivy, 1975, 1980) -This questionnaire measures chronic dieting and attitudes toward weight and eating, (1980 version used by Keel et al, 1997; (& slightly simplified) by Hill et al., 1990). Modified for children (Davis, Best & Hawking, 1981 adapted Herman & PoIivyls scale) -30 questions were used to measure dieting concem and eating behaviours, 15 to give restraint score (with higher scores indicative of high eating restraint), and t 5 for descriptive purposes (e.g. How much do you weigh? How much would you like tu weigh? (used by Ruther & Richman, 1993).

Eating. Patterns Ouestionnaire (Bennett, 1982) -A retrospective account of the previous week's eating behaviow, with a fwus on reasons for eating, situation variables and eating behaviours. It was developed to measure eating patterns dunng an obesity treatment program (used by Hill, et al., 1990).

Child-Feedin~Ouestionnaire (Birch & Fisher, 2000) -This self-report questionnaire measures perceptions of child's or parents' overweight, concerns about overweight and parents1child-feeding attitudes and practices. It consists of a 6-item Perceived child weieht subscale, which quenes perceptions of daughters' weight during different childhood stages with responses from 1 (markedly undenveight) to 5 murkedly ovenveight. The 3-item Concems about child overweieht subscale asks about future problerns or necessity of dieting with responses from 1 (unconcemed) to 5 (concerned). The 8-item Restriction subscale measures mothers' attempts to restrict access to types and/or amounts of foods. Responses are scored from 1 (disagreee) to 5 (agree);e.g. Z have to be sure ihaz my child does no? eut tao ninny high-farfoods. Monitoring subscale has 3 items measuring the mothers' monitoring of their daughters' consumption of energy-dense foods. Responses range from 1 (never) to 5 (always); e.g. How much do you keep truck of the snack food (potato chips, Dodos, cheese puffs) that your child eats? Restricted-Access Ouestionnaire (Birch & Fisher, 2000) -This scale measures mothers'restriction of their daughters' access to 10 snack foods. Sen questions (e.g. limitations on the time of day the food is ailowed; extent of upset if the daughter gets the food without asking) are asked about each item.

Self-Esteern/Bodv Image Ouestionnaires

Silhouettes - (Stunkard, Sorenson & Schulsinger, 1980, 1983) -9 Iine drawings of male or female figure are presented in order, from extremely thin to obese. Participants are usually asked what they currently look like, and what they would like to look like, with the differential used to calculate body dissatisfaction or the desire to be thinner (used by Thelen & Cormier, 1995; 7 figures used by Hill & Franklin, 1998). -Set of 12 fernate body silhouettes randomly presented from emaciated to very obese (used by Ogden & Elder, 1998; Ogden & Chanana, 1998).

Self-Image Ouestionnaire for Young Adolescents (Petersen, Schulenberg, Abramowitz, Offer & Jaracho, 1984) -Body image subscale is a 6-point Likert scale, with higher scores indicative of more positive body image (used by Attie & Brooks-Gunn, 1989; Byely et al, 2000 used a shortencd 9-item version). -FamiIv Relations subscale is 6-point Lïkert-type scale from 1 describes me not at dl, to 6, describes me very well, with higher scores indicative of warmer and less conflicted iamïly relations (used by Attie & Brooks-Gunn, 1989; Byely et al, 2000). -Puberta1 status, Personality subscales (used by Attie & Brooks-Gunn, 1989).

Bodv Di ssati sfaction Scale (Garner, Olmsted & Polivy, 1983) -This 9-item scale measures attitudes toward body parts; concems with eating, weight and body image (used by Steiger et al, 1996).

Bodv Shape Ouestionnaire (Cooper, Taylor, Cooper & Fairbum, 1987) -Higher scores on this 10-item scaie indicate greater dissatisfaction (used by Ogden & Steward, 2000; shorter version used by Ogden & Elder, 1998; Ogden & Chanana, 1998).

Piers-Hanis Children's Self-Conce~tScale (Piers, 1969) -This 80-item yes-no questionnaire measures self-esteem with a focus on physicai self-esteem. Higher scores indicate higher self esteem (used by Ruther & Richman, 1993). General Self-esteem -This was a list of 10 positive and 10 negative adjectives taken from self-concept scales (used by Ruther & Richman, 1993).

Locus of Control (S tanford Preschool Internai-Extemal Scale (Mischel, Zeiss & Zeiss, 1974) -This scale presents 14 scenaIios with two responses. Higher and lower scores indicate more intemal or extemal locus of control (used by Ruther & Richman, 1993).

Bodv Esteem Scale (Mendelson & White, 1982, 1993) -This is a 24-i tem questionnaire of yes-no responses (1982 version used by Hill & Franklin, 1998). Smolak et al, 1999, used six questions fiom the 1993 version focussed directly on weight and shape concerns e.g. My weight makes me unhappy. Scores ranged from 6 to 12 with higher scores indicative of more positive body esteem.

State Self-esteem Scale (Heatherton & Polivy, 1991) -This is a 20-item questionnaire with three subscales: Performance self-esteem (7 items), Social self-esteem (7 items) and &-puance self-esteem (6 items). The combined subscdes give a total self-esteem score (used by Hill & Franklin, 1998).

Family Environment Scale (Moos & Moos, 1987) -This is a 90-item questionnaire with 10 subscales, measunng socio- environmental aspects of family life, including Relationshi~dimension (Cohesion, Ex~ressiveness,and Conflict subscales); Personai growth (Tndeoendence, Achievement orientation, In tellectual-cul tural orientation, Active-recreational orientation and Mord-relieious ern~hasissubscales). System maintenance dimension (Or~anizationand Control subscales) The "Real Form" measures people's current perceptions of conjugal or nuclear family life (used by Hi11 & Franklin, 1998; Attie & Brooks-Gunn, 1989).

Other rneasures: Acculturation Rating Scale, Rating Scale of Child's Actual Behavior, Texas Social Behavior Inventory, Self-Perception Profile for Children (used by Hall et al., 1991). Dramatic-erratic Traits: Affective Instability Stimulus Seeking, Rejection, Narcissism and Anxiousness. Obsessive-Compulsive traits: Restricted Expression, Anxiousness (used by Steiger et al, 1996). Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (used by Attie & Brooks-Gunn, 1989). Farnily Adaptability and cohesion Evaiuation Scale III (used by Pike & Rodin, 1991). Studies of Mother (M.) and Daughter (D) Associations in Weight-Related Attitudes and Self-Reported Behavioursa

1 Aged 5 yean and under Abramovitz, 197 pairs M + D. Weight -Mothers' and daughters' weight conctnis wcre et al., 1998 Mean age 5.4 yrs concenis scaie; self significantly comlated (r=I6; p c02). [abstract] esteem masure- -Mothers using unhealthy weight control M: weight loss bchaviours (6296) had daughtefs who werr almosc behaviours. as likely to understand the concept of a diet. D: "What is a diet?"

Cutting. et 75 pairs + fathers M + Fr Body mass -No relationship bctween moihcrs' dierary al., 1999 (35 girls; 40 boys) index (BMI), dietary rcstraint and daughters' eating & wcight Mean age = 5.0 restraint. and dietary OUtCOMS. y- (3 to 6) disinhibition (Eating -Thmeof 24 parent-child variables wm -MostIy white Disorder Inventory), significant: Hcavier mothers had heavier -Recruitcd hma C: weight-for-height; daughters; rnaternd disinhibition was associatcd university &y kilojoules of paiatable with both daughters' overweight & nte access cm. snacks eaten in the intake. absence of hunger. -Mothersl disinhibition predicted daughters' overweight indcpcndcntly of daughtcn' frct acccss intakes. (No significant associations for fathcr-daughtcr or eiihcr parent-son )

Stice. et al., 216 pairs (100 M: BMI; Three Factor -Infant BMI, sucking duration, matemal 1999 girls, 1 16 boys) Eatuig Questionnaire; characteristics (body dissatisfaction, -hngitudinal Eating Disorder intemalization of the thhideal, dieting, bulimic design. O to 5 yrs Inventory; report of symptoms) and maternai and paternai body mass -Most Caucasian - chiid's inhibited prospective1y predicted the emergcnce of Median college eating. secretive disturbed eating. education eating, overeating and -Disturkd eating increased annually. vomiting. Infant: BMI; sucking duration.

Birch. & 156 pairs M: %MI.Restraint -Matemai BMI associated with daughters' Fisher, Mean age 5.4 Subscale of Three relative weight. 2000 yean (4.6 - 6.4) Factor Eathg -As daughters' relative weight increased, -Recniited by Questionnaire; Child- mothers' perceptions and concem about adverrisements Feeding daughters' weight incrcased. -White, non- Questionnaire; -Highcr matemal restraint was associateci with Hispanic Restrictcd Access mamdrestriction of daughrcrs' food intake. -Well-educated Questionnairr; -Matemal restriction prcdictcd less self regdation Daughter's energy of energy intakc, less adjustment for energy IeveI hm24-hour recalls. of prc-load, and higher frcc access snack intake in D: weight-for-height, absence of hunger in daughters. energy compensation .Daughtersi short-ierm encrgy inrake was following pre-load associated with 24-hour energy intake. drialr; kilojoules of snacks cattn. Hill, et al., 20 pairs (10 high M + D: Restraint -Mothers of high-scoring girls scored higher on 1990 & 10 low- scoring Scale; Eating Restraint scale, but not on the EAT. girls selected hm Attitudes Test; Eating -Mothcrs7 and daughters' Resmïnt scores were 52 respondents to Patterns significantly associami (r= -68). EAT scores Remaint Scate.) Questionnaire. werc not (r = .35)- Mean age 10-1 y., -(Higldy rcstrained respondents more Liely to (9 - 11). Inner- attributc eating to negative mdstatcs.) city, w,U.K. -Did not control for body weight. Hali, et al.. 37 pairs (19 M + D: BMI;Figm -The daughters' global self-worth was related to 1991 mothers hm drawings of girls @s mothers' size. daughten' age and size, but not to weight loss group; perceived and ideal mothers*self-concept. Only daughters' age was 18 from church). size, attractiveness. related to physical appearance esteem. Aged 7- 12 years M: Acculturation -Daughters perceived themsclves as largcr than (Mean 8.9 & 9.2) Rating Scale; Rating mothers perceived them. daughters selected -Mexican- Scale of Child's thinner figures for their ideai than mothm Americans Actual Behavior; selectcd; daughters selected thïnner ideals than -Response = 848 Texas Social Behavior perceived, but mothers' (group) ranlangs of theu Inventory. daughm' ideal and actual si= wetc congnrmt. D: Self-Perception -(Then were no difierences between the groups Profile for Children. of amthers in perceivcd and ideai size of daughters or attractiveness ratings of obcse and thin figures.)

Ruther & 44 pairs (26 girls; M + D-Restraint scale -Mothers' restraint was associatcd with Ric hman, 18 boys) C: Understand dietiag; daughters' restraint (r = -35); but not sons' (r = 1993 4&grade desire to change .09). -High SES weight; self-esteem; -No wntrol for body weight. locus of control. Thelen 6C 70 pairs + fathers M + D: BMI; desire to -No significant association between DTBT and Cormier. (35 girls; 35 boys) be ttiinner(DTBT) weight conml measures of parents and childrcn. 1995 4&grade; age 9- using silhouettes; self- -Daughtersl body weight. DTBT. and die~gwere 10.5 years reported diet comlated with both parents' encouragement to -Most Caucasian frequency; EAT-26 or control weight. When BMI was controlltd. only -Response rate: cMEAT); dieting and fathers' encouragement significant- 358 families M + F: Concem with -25% of mothers and 24% of fathers said they contactcd child's weight- ncver encouraged weight loss in their daughters; C: Perception of 43% & 46% of girls reported heu mothcr and parents' concern. fathcr never encouraged weight loss. Girls' dieting comlatcd more with parental reports. - - Smolak. et 148 families (13 1 M + F: Questions -Child more concemed about weight loss when d., 1999 mothers, 89 about frequency of both parents commented on weight than just one. fathers; 72 dieting; importance of -Mohs' (but not fathers') comments about couples). weight; weight daughters' weight were significantly conelated 4& & 5&grade complaints; bclief in with weight loss attempts, Body Esitem scores, -White, working to efficacy of calorie and concern about gaining weight. middle class restriction; frequency -Mothers' concern about theu own weight and the -Rurd of comments to chitd importance of weight to the father werc related to -Response ratc - about child's weight. daughtcn' conceni about gaining weight. 552 families C: Body Estecm -Commnts hmparents, espccially mothers, contacted. Scale; wcight secmed more influcntial than parental modelling. concems; frtquency of Parental modclling was associatcd with childrcn's weight loss anempts. beiiefs and bchaviaurs, especiaily girls. Weight was not controlled. ) Middle School-Aged

40 pairs (used M + D-BMI; Dietary -There were no significant differc~lccsbccn Restraint Scale to Restraint Scale @utch &ers in Resrraint scores, having dietai; age of select 20 girls with Eating Behanour iktdiet, maximum weight lost by dicting, or highest scores; 20 QucstionnauC); body current BMI. with scom shape preferencts -Both groups of mothers saw themselves as bctween Seand using silhouettes; slightly ovcnueight. sclccttd thuincr pcfd 25"' percentile of Body esteem; State than current shapes and did not ciiffer on self- 106 respondents). Self-estecm Scaie; esteem. Mean age 11-75 Dicting history and -Both groups viewed theu daughtcrs' cumnt Y=ars self-perception; weight as "just right" and slightly niore attractive -Most Caucasian Famüy Environment than other same-age girls- Mothcn of hi& -Lower micicile Scale. Resfraïnt girls rated thcm les atîmXîve. class -Mothers of high Resnaint girls had slightly -Urban, U.K. grCater past weight range and wmmm likely to -Response 35/38. snack; 20% had fasteci vs 5% of other mothcrs. -high Restraint girls were hcavier, xored lower on total self-esteem; 10% sh'pped brr?kfast; 15% had fastcd for a whole &y; (vs O of low-lestraint girls) and were twice as lïkely to have a brother- -Notes that differcnccs in family functioning (lower cohcsion, moral-rcligious emphasis & organization) occur in families with wcight prroccupied girls w-thout dngdisorclers- -Dïd not conwl for body weight.

77pairsatTime 1 -Body image (M & -Mothers' dicting and body image did not prrdict 55 pairs at Time 2 D), Fanily Relations girls' dietïng or body image eithcr concurrcntly or -Mean age = 12.2 @ only) subscales of one year later. y, grade 67at the Self-image -Girls* perceptions of family relations and Ti1 Questionnaire for mothers' perceptions of daughtcrs' wcight at -White. urban, Young Adolescents; Time 1 predicted diethg at Timt 2. middle-to upper- Diet subscale of -Girls' body image and dietïng at Ti1 middle class, well Eating Attitudes Test; predicted more body dissatisfaction at Tim 2- educated families. Ds' & Ms' assessrnent -Controllcd for BMI and age. -Longitudinal of girls' relative design. weight; dieting history and encouragement @ only); BMI. - - 20 pairs M + D: BMI; EAT- There was no evidence of significant difierence Agcd 13-14 years, 40; frcquency of between mofhers' and daughtcn' EAT scores 8" IPde dieting; weight (Mean and standard deviacion for rnothcrs = -Pnmarily lower satisfaction; 16.05 * 10.3; for daughters = 16.55 * 14.3). income Qualitative interview. -The= was no association betwecn their scores -Ethnidy diverse (r = 2,N.S.). -Urban. Canada There was extensive vatïability within each -Rcs~o~s~80%. generation and within pairs in test scores and in open-ended respo~lscsto interviews. -Did not conml for body weight. 1 High School Aged - 193 pairs M + D: EAT-26; -Mothen' body image and &grec of compulsive Mean age = 13.9 Body lmage Scale eating did not contribute çignincantly to & 16.1 yrs atTime hmSelf-image predïctionof compulsive eating ammg daughters. 1 and2,PtolW Questionnaire for Mothers' EAT-26 scores approached significance grade (initially) Young Adolescents @ < -1). -White (SIQYA); Center for -Body image at time 1 was the strongcst prcdictor -Middle-class Epiidemiological of eating problems at tirne 2. -Response = 85% Studies Depression -Stresscd physique in early adolescena; early at Time 1; 23% Scaie; Family adolescent mitudes and family functioning in dropped out at Envûonmcnt Scale. oldcr adolescence- Ti2. D: Height & weight; -Tm1: Physical manirity explaincd 10% of pubertal scatus; variance in EAT scores; negative body image personality and family 18%; psychopathology 8%- relationship scales -Ti= 2; body image explained 15%of variance; hmSIQYA; Youth depression and ineffcctivencss also contributcd to Behaviour Profile; cating problems. Pubertal status was no longer Eating Disorciers significant. Inven tory (Mothers' perceptions (not daughters') of family -Longitudinaldesign. relationships were associateci with catiag problems.) 5 1 pairs + fathers AU: Restraint Scale; -nicm was no significant association bctwcen Mean age 14.8 questions about Resuaint scores, or fi-equency and stringcncy of years (12-18) disordered eating dieting of daughter and either parent. -Cauwian bchaviours; weight -Parental commcnts about daughten' weight, -Middleclass perception and mothcrs' describing hem as ovtnvcight and the -S uburban satisfaction; diet fathcrs' own weight dissatisfaction were -Response = 26% frequency and associatcd with outcoms: mother variables with stringency; BMI. daughters' Restraint score, fathcr variables with àaughters' weight dissatisfiction. -Daughtcrs' self-rcported BMI was not associatcd with their own (or parental) rating of her weight. however daughtcrs' self-perception of their weight status was relatcd to parental ratings of hcr weight, suggesting common family standards. -Daughters' self-reported BMI was not associatcd with th& weight satisfaftion or Restraint score. ------77 pairs (used -M: BMI; Eating -Mothers of daughters with disordcred cating had Eating Disorders Disorders hventory; dieted at earlier age (21.3 vs. 27.4); scored higher hventory to Questions about on the Eating Disordcrs Inventory (1 7.8 vs. 12.1); divide 350 girls weight & ideal weight, were more aitical of their daughtcrs than of into disordered dieting history; ratings thernsclves; wanted their daughters to be thinner eating girls (> 79' of attractiveness; (12.1 Ib vs. -0.1 lb., controlled for daughters' percentile) and Family Adaptability weight); ratcd their daughters as less attraEtive controls (10 - 35'" and Cohesion than daughters ratcd themselvcs (rated themselves percentile). Evaluation Scale IH as comparable to othcr womcn their own age). -Mean age 46yrs -'Ihere werc no significant differences betwccn -Grade 9 to 12 groups in BMI; prcvalencc of dieting (90% vs. -White 79%); largest amount of wcight ever lost by -Middle-class dieting, and desircd amount of weight loss. -Uh,suburban -GHs with disordercd eating were hcavier than -Responsc = 57% those without (BMI= 22.9 vs 20.7). (-Mothers' dissatisfaction with family cohcsion was rclatcd to daughters' disordercd eating.) Ogden & 30 pairs -Restraincd cating -niere was no significant association betwcen Steward, Mean age 17.1 section of Dutch mothers' and daughten' rtsirained eating (r = - 2000 years (16-19) Eating Behaviour -01) and body dissatisfa-on (r = -21), despite -Majority white & Questionnaire; Body significant contlations for BMI (r = 58). upper class, U.K. Shape Questionnaire; (-Daughten' rcstraincd cating and body -Response = 52%. M-DRelationship dissatisfaction was associatcd with aspects of the questions; BMI. mothcr-daughter relationship (autonomy and projection.) 1 University stud;nts 50 pairs (25 Asian; -Silhouettes; Body -nie= was no matching between mothers and 25 White} ShapeQuestionnaire; daughters on body image or eating behaviour- Mean age 20 yean Calorie conccm; -Mothm reportcd pater body dissatisfrtion. (18-26) Restraïned eating large? currcnt and "ideai" body shapcs. -Daughtcrs in section of Dutch -White daughtcrs were the most dissarisficd with medical school, Eating Behaviour their bodies; White mothers were most satisfied U.K. Questionnaire; with their bodies. acculturation; BMI. -- - 40 pairs + fatha -Body Shape -Daughters' restraincd eating score was rclatcd to & a sibling (20 Questionnaire; mothers' higher valuation of physicai -ce Asian; 20 White) silhouettes; and siblings' lower valuation of conipctitivcness Mean age 20.5 y Restraincd eating and non-traditional roles for womn. -Daughters in 3* section of Dutch -Daughters' body dissatisfaction was relatai to year of medical Eating Behaviour higher ratings of materialism. fathers' prcfer~~la school. U.K- Questionnaire; for thinner fernale bodies, siblings' lower rating questions on values; of non-traditional role for wom, lower BMI. concordance within the family concerning tht value of compctitivcncss and higher eduutionai Ievel of the 'hcad of the family.'

-24 "Resmctor 11 sel f-report scales to -Mean BMI of mstrictors, bingcrs, psychiatrïc eating disordcrcd" crcatc thme factors: 1) controls and Normal-controls was 17.7; 212; (+ 21 M. 15 F, and Eating conccrns and 22.3; 25.8. respectively. Weight was not 17 sisters) symproms (ECS controllai for, but BMI r 29 was exclusion -64 "Binge eating (includcd scores for criterion. disordered" (+ 51 Restraint. Ernotional -niere werc significant associations betwcen M. 35 F. 31 S) Eating. €AT-26 and mothers and both daughters' eating concmis (r = -42 "Psychiatnc Body Dissatisfaction -48 and .39 for normal conbols and ihcù sisters; r controls" (+ 33 M, des)2) Dramatic- = 22 and -17 for total sample and sisten). 22 F 19 Sistcrs) erratic Traits @ET: -Correlations wcrc not significant for fathers (r = -59 "Normal Affective instability, -03 and .22 for normal controls and their sisters; r controls" (+ 53 M. Stimulus Secking, = -.O4 and -07 for total samplc and sisters). 32 F, 35 sisters) Rejec tion, Narcissism, -found relationships between parents' DET and -Mean Age = 27.8; and ANuousness) and Daughten' ECS and ncgative association bctween 25.6; 29.1; 24.6. 3) Obsessivc- parents' OCT and daughtcrs' ECS. respectively, compulsive traits -found traits characteristic of eating disorder -Volunteers. (OCT Rcstriçted suffercrs were familiai, but did not distinguish Expression and relatives. hvestigators suggest thaf traits am Amiousness). BMI. familial but insufficicnt on thcir own to cause cating disorders-

"Questionnaires are described in Appendix A-1 APPENDX B

Interview Protocol and Questions

RECORD OF INTERVIEW Code: Mother- Daughter-

1. Date of Interview: MontM)ay/Year

Start End Length hours minutes

2. Physical Setting:

General Directions to researcher:

Name on consent foms only. Code al1 forms immediately. 1. Information about the Study - leave with Participant 2. Consent Form -- leave copy with Participant 3. Background Information - mothers only 4. Oral Interview -- tape-record 5. Consensus Questions 6. Body Shape Drawings 7. Eating Attitudes Test 8. Restraint Scale 9. Consent form for Daughters -- mothers only; leave copy 10. Field notes BACKGROUND INFORMATION QUESTIONNAIRE TO BE GIVEN ORALLY TO MOTHERS ONLY

1. Where were you bom? 2. How long have you lived in this neighbourhood (community)? 1 SINCE BIRTH; 2 SINCE CHILDHOOD; 3 SINCE ADOLESCENCE;4 SINCE ADULTHOOD 3. If not a long-time resident, when did you move here? Where did you live before you moved here? 4. What is your present marital status? (Circle number.) 1NEVER MARRIED; 2 COMMON LAW; 3 MAR-; 4 DIVORCED; 5 SEPARATED; 6 WIDOWED 5. How many children do you have? 6. What is the age of each child? 7. How old are you? 8. Who lives in your home? 9. How much fonnal education do you have? (Circle number,) 1 NO FORMAL EDUCATION; 2 LESS THAN GRADE SEVEN; 3 GRADE 7-9; 4 GRADE 10-12; 5 COMPLETED HIGH SCHOOL;6 SOME COL.LEGE OR UNIVERSmY; 7 COMPLETED COLLEGE OR UNDERGRADUATE DEGREE; 8 SOME GRADUATE WORK; 9 COMPLETED GRADUATE WORK Level: 10. How much forma1 education does your spouse/partner have? (Use above codes) 11. Do you speak any language other than English? 1 YES; 2 NO 12. If yes, what other language(s) do you speak? 13. How would you describe your ethnic background (nationality; ancestry)? 14.1s your ethnic background important to you in your everyday Life? 1 YES ;2 NO 15. What is your spouse/partner's primary ethnici ty or national ancestry? 16. How would you describe your daughter's ethnicity? 17. Where were your parents bom? (What is their ethnicity?) Mother; Father 18. What was your father's occupation when you were growing up (annind age 16)? 19. Father's education? (Use above codes) 20. What was your mother's occupation when you were growing up (around age 16)? 21. Mother's education? (use codes above.) 22. What religion were you raised in? 23. What is your present religion? 24. Are you presently? (Circle number.)l WORKING FWLL TIME; 2 WORKING PART TIME; 3 UNEMPLOYED; 4 RETLRED; 5 FULL-TIME H0MEMAKE.R; 6 STUDENT 25. What jobs have you had since you finished school or left your parents'house? (Tnclude homemaking and retuming to schwl.) Currently? Before that? Before that? 26.1s your spouselpartner presently working? (Include former spouse.) (Use codes above.) 27. What is your spouse/partner's occupation? 28. Has he had other jobs in the past? 29. What is your annual household income? 1 LESS THAN $5,000; 2 $5,000 - $9,999; 3 $10,000 - $14,999; 4 $15,000 - $19,999; 5 $20,000 - $24,999 6 $25,000 - $29,999; 7 $30,000 - $34,999; 8 $35,000 - $39,999; 9 $40,000 - $44,999; 10 $50,000 - $54,999; 11 $55,000 - $59,999; 12 $60,000 - $64,999; 13 $65,000 - $69,999; 14 $70,000 - $74,999; 15 $75,000 OR MORE 30. What idare the source(s) of income for your household? 3 1. Do you rent or own your home? 1 RENT; 2 OWN

33. How well do you think your income (inciuding that of your spouse, if appropriate) satisfies your needs? 1 VERY WEU; 2 ADEQUATELY; 3 WïïH SOME DIFFICULTY; 4 NOT VERY WELL; 5 INADEQUATE

INTERVIEW GUIDE FOR MOTHERS

1 am going to ask a few questions just to help me get to know you before we start the interview . 1. How would you describe yourself to someone you are just meeting? 2. Imagine you could instantly change anything you wanted about yourself, What would you change? Anything else? Which is most important to you? 3. What are a couple of things about yourself that make you feel gwd? Probes: What kinds of things do other people Say about you? Think about one of your fnends. Why do you like her? 4. Do you think wornen have different concems in their Iives than men? Probe: What sorts of things do women worry about? 5. What is your idea of the ideal woman? 6. What is your idea of the ideal man? 7. Do you think girls have different concerns than women? 8. Do you think weight is a problem for women? If so, in what way? Why do you think that? Probe: What would that (being overweight/underweight) mean to you? 9. Do you think weight is more of an issue for women than for men? 10. If yes, why do you think women are more concemed? Probes: What do you think influences the way women feel about their weight? 1s there anything you see as affecting the way you feel about your own weight? 1s there anything that's different for men? 11. Do you think weight is more of an issue for women or for girls? 12. What does the word "diet" mean to you? 13. Are happy with the way you look? Would you Say you are: 1) very happy; 2) some- what happy; 3) neither happy nor unhappy; 4) somewhat unhappy; 5) very unhappy? 14. It would be helpful to know your weight history. Thinking now of your whole life, have you done anything to try to change your weight? If yes, what did you try and how did it work? What was your age when you tried this? Were there any other times you tned to change your weight? (Repeat as necessary.) 15. Why do you think some people gain more weight than they would like? Probe: What do you think causes overweight? 1s there anything here that's different for men and women? 16. Why do you think some people have trouble losing weight? 17. How do you think king overweight might affect a woman's life? 18, How are overweight people treated? Probe: Are overweight women and men treated di fferentl y? 19. Are you ever afraid of getting fat? Why or Why not? 20. Sometimes women who arent heavy will Say things like "I'm fat." Do you see women doing this? Why do you think they Say this? 21. Why do you think some people are underweight? Robe: What do you think causes people to be underweight? 1s there anything here that's different for men and women? 22. How would king underweight affect a woman's Iife? 23. How are undenveight people treated? Probe: Are underweight women and men treated differently? 24. Are you ever afraid of getting too thin? Whylwhy not? 25. Do you ever taik to your daughter about weight? What do you talk about? 26. Do you ever give her advice? If yes, what is the advice? 27. Does she give you advice? If yes, what is the advice? 28. Does having a teenage daughter affect the way you feel about weight? 29. Do ycu think the way you feel about your weight affects the way she feels about her weight? 30. How do you think women should deal with their weight? Probe: Do you think that feminism has afTected the way women deal with their weight? 3 1. Who do you think people should tallc to if they're having problems with their weight? 32. In general, compared to other persons your age, would you Say your health is: 1) Excellent? 2) Very good? 3) Good? 4) Fair? 5) Poor? Why do you rate your health that way? How do you know your heaith is ... ? 33. Do you think weight has anything to do with health? Why or why not? 34. Do you think king fat is a disease? Why or why not? 35. Do you think king too thin is a disease? Why/why not? 36.1s weight something that people can control? Under what circurnstances do they have control? No control? 37. Do you ever talk about weight with your friends? Probe: What do you talk about?

LAST QUESTION OF INTERVIEW:

What is your overall impression of the things we have been talking about today? 1s there anything that you would like to add? INTERVIEW GUIDE FOR DAUGHTERS

1 am going to ask a few questions just to help me get to know you before we start the interview. A. Where were you boni? B. Do you speak any language other than English? C. If yes, what other language(s) do you speak? D. How would you describe your ethnicity? E. 1s your ethnic background important to you in your everyday life? 1. Tell me a little about yourself. How would you describe yourself to someone you are just meeting? Matkinds of things do you like doing? Family; School; Fnends; Hobbies; Goals in life. 2. Imagine you could instantly change anything you wanted about yourself. What would you change? Anything else? Which is most important to you? 3. What are a couple of things about yourself that make you feel good? Probes: What kinds of things do other people Say about you? Think about one of your fnends. Why do you like her? 4. Do you think girls have different concems in their lives than boys? Probe: What sorts of things do girls womy about? 6. What is your idea of the ideal boy? 7. Do you think girls have different concems than women? 5B. What is your idea of the ideal girl? 5A. What is your idea of the ideal woman? 8. Do you think weight is a problem for girls? Probe: If so, in what way? Why do you think that? What would that (king overweighthnderweight) mean to you? 9. Do you think weight is more of an issue for girls than for boys? 10. If yes, why do you think girls are more concemed? Probes: What do you think influences the way girls feel about their weight? 1s there anything you see as affecting the way you feel about your own weight? 1s there anything that's different for boys? 11. Do you think weight is more of an issue for girls than for women? 12. What does the word "diet" mean to you? 13. Are happy with the way you look? Would you Say you are: 1) very happy, 2) some- what happy, 3) neither happy nor unhappy 4) somewhat unhappy or 5) very unhappy? 14. It would be helpful to know your weight history. Thinking now of your whole life, have you done anything to try to change your weight? If yes, what did you try and how did it work? How old were you when you tried this? Were there any other times you tried to change your weight? (Repeat as necessary.) 15. Why do you think some people gain more weight than they would like? 16. Why do you think some people have trouble losing weight? 17. How do you think king overweight might affect a girl's life? 18. How are overweight people treated? Probe: Are overweight girls and boys treated differentl y? 19. Are you ever afraid of getting fat? Why or Why not? 20. Sometimes girls who arent heavy will Say things like "I'm fat." Do you see girls doing this? Why do you think they Say this? 2 1. Wh y do you think some people are underweight? 22. How would being underweight affect a girl's life? 23. How are undenveight people treated? Probe: Are undenveight girls and boys treated dif ferent 1y? 24. Are you ever afraid of getting too thin? Why/why not? 25. Do you ever talk to your parents about weight? If yes, who? What do you talk about? 26. Do they give you advice? If yes, what is the advice? 27. Do you ever give them advice? If yes, what is the advice? 30. How do you think girls should deal with their weight? Probe: Do you think that feminism has affectcd the way girls deal with their weight? 3 1. Who do you think girls should talk to if they9rehaving problems with their weight? 32. In general, compared to other persons your age, would you Say your health is: 1) Excellent? 2) Very good? 3) Gd?4) Fair? 5) Poor? Why do you rate your health that way? How do you know your health is ... ? 33. Do you think weight has anything to do with health? Why or why not? 34. Do you think king fat is a disease? Why or why not? 35. Do you think king too thin is a disease? Why/why not? 36.1s weight something that people can control? Under what circumstances do they have control? No control? 37. Do you ever talk about weight with your fnends? Probe: What do you talk about?

LAST QUESTION OF INTERVIEW:

What is your overall impression of the things we have been talking about today? 1s there anything that you would like to add? CONSENSUS QUESTIONS

Al. I have been asking the girls and women I've been interviewing why they think weight is important. Hem are some reasons they have given me. Piease pick out all the reasons that you agree with.

1. GUYS WANT THiN GIRLFRIENDS

2. SO YOU CAN WEAR NICE CLOTHING

3. SO YOU DON'T GET TEASED

4. TO GET A JOB MORE EASILY

5. BECAUSE OF HOW YOUR FAMILY TREATS YOU

6.FOR YOUR HEALTH

7. TO MAKE FRIENDS MORE EASILY

9. BECAUSE OF WEiAT OTHER PEOPLE THINK

10. SO YOU CAN FEEL GOOD ABOUT YOURSELF

Il. TO HAVE MORE ENERGY TO DO THINGS

12. BECAUSE OF THE MEDIA. ALL THE MODEr ON TV AND IN MAGAZINES ARE THIN

13. BECAUSE OF RELIGION

A2. These are the statements that you agreed were reasons why weight is important to girls and women. Please choose the three most important rossons that girls are concerned about their weight Which is the most, second most and third rnost important? BI. Aere are some reasons people have given for why some people gain more weight than they would like. Please read each statement and pick out the ones you agree with.

1. People gain weight because they have poor willpower. If they put their mind to it they'd be able to control their weight.

2. People gain weight because they are unhappy or bored or womed. Sometimes people eat to make themselves feel better.

3. People gain weight because they eat a lot.

4. People gain weight because they don't do much exercise.

5. People gain weight because they eat fattening foods (such as sweets, chips, coke, fast foods, greasy foods, junk food, beer and so on).

6. People gain weight because they don? know how to balance their food. For example, they might eat too much meat and not enough vegetables.

7. Women gain weight easier after having a baby. It changes your body.

8. Women gain weight because having a baby changes your life. You're home with kids preparing food and there's no time to exercise.

9. Women gain weight because their bodies are designed to make fat. Men's bodies are designed to make muscle.

10. People gain weight because it's natural to gain weight as you reach middle-age.

11. Girls gain weight because it's natural to gain some fat as you reach your teens.

12. People gain weight because they tend to eat the way their parents ate. So if your parents are fat or thin, it is likely that you will be tw.

13. People gain weight because they inherit a tendency to be fat or thin. People take after their parents.

14. People gain weight because of king poor. 15. People gain weight because of dieting.

16. People gain weight because of toxins, chernicals and hormones. It confuses the body and affects its size and shape.

B2. These are the statements that you agreed were reasons why people gain more weight than they would like. Ptease choose the three regsons that you think are most important. Which is the most, second most and third most important? Cl. Please say if you agree or disagree to each of the following statements. They are al1 based on statements that people have made in previous interviews: 1. Teasing a fiiend is a good way to get her to realize she needs to lose weight 2. Weight is not so important. People shouldn't spend so much time thinking about it. 3. People should refuse to buy a product that uses extremely thin models in its advertising. 4. If people felt good about themselves, they would take the time and energy needed to lose weight. 5. It is wrong to judge another person on the basis of their weight. 6. Girls pay more attention to what their fkiends Say than what their mothers Say about how they look. 7. Some people are meant to be big and it would not be healthy to try to change their weight. 8. Models are thinner than they should be, but they look great so it's okay. 9. People should not weigh themselves. 10. If you had the money, it would be worth spending it on plastic surgery or liposuction to improve your appearance. 11. If a cute guy tells a girl she's too fat, and if she likes him she should try to lose weight. 12. Dieting is unhealthy. 13. If people felt good about themselves, they would accept their body as it is, whatever their size and shape. 14. A girl or woman should worry about her weight because more people will iike her, she will have an easier time attracting a boyfriend. and it will help her to get a job. AU other things king equal, she will do better in life if she is slim. 15. Women ought to be more concemed about being fit than about their weight. 16. Mothers should tell theû daughters not to worry about their weight no matter what they weigh. 17. In general, people are not born fat; they let themselves get fat. 18. A heavy woman should not Wear a bathing suit or do things that show too much body. 19. Women should always watch what they eat because it's easier to stay thin than to lose weight. 20. Mothers should help their daughters lose weight if they need to lose. 2 1. If a woman is very concemed about her own weight, then it's lîkely that her daughter will be too. 22. A fat woman can be beautiful. 23. The media should show women of different shapes and sizes. 24. There should be a specid tax on junk food and the money should be spent to build bike paths and swimming pools and to teach people about healthy living. RESTRAINT SCALE Mother - Daughter - Please circle your answer to each question.

1. How often are you dieting?

Never Rarely Sometimes Usually Always

2. What is the maximum amount of weight (in pounds) you have ever lost within one month?

3. What is your maximum weight gain within a week?

0-1 1.1-2 2.1-3 3.1-5 5.1+

4. In a typical week, how much does your weight fluctuate?

0-1 1.1-2 2.1-3 3.1-5 5.1+

5. Would a weight fluctuation of 5 pounds affect the way you live your life?

Not at al1 Slightly Moderately Very Much

6. Do you eat sensibly in front of others and splurge alone?

Never Rarely Often Always

7. Do you have feelings of guilt after overeating?

Never Rarely Often Always

8. How conscious are you of what you're eating?

Not at al1 Slightly Moderately Extremely

9. How many pounds over your desired weight were you at your maximum weight?

0-1 1-5 6-10 11-20 21+

10. How often do you have regular menstrual periods? - Never Karely Sometimes Usually Always

Repnnted, with permission, from Herman, C. P., & Polivy, J. (1980). Restrained eating. In A. Stunkard (Ed.), Obesity (pp. 208-225). Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders. Pltase place an OC) undcr the column which appkbst to crth ofth mmbed JUtCmentl.

1- 1 am terrificd about being ov-@IL 2.1 avoid ean'ng when 1un bmm- 3. I find m2;sdf preoccupied with fiood 4.1 have sont on eating biïu wbe1 iddut 1may nat k abIe to stop. 5.1aitmy~'urtodpicccs. 6.1 am amof the calorie content of fWd tht 1ut 7.1 panïcularfy avoid f& with r bigh CUbO-c content (brcad, potatoes, rice, ...). 8. I feel thas 0th- would prefa if1ate more. 9- 1vomit der I have eatca, 10.1 fnt crcrernely piity af'ter utin& 11.1 am preoccupied with a desire to be thinnu.

12.1 think about burning up calories wkir 1QI&. 13. Other people think chat f am too thin. 14.1 am preoccupied with the thought of hr- fat on my body. 15. I take longer than othcn to ut my me&- 16.1 avoid foock with suw in them. 17.1 tat diet foods. 18.1 feel htfood connofs my life. 19.1 display seif conuo\ around W. 20.1 fiel that others pressut me to at, 2 1. I give too much time and thought to food. 22.1 fetl uncornfortable afta &ng swets. 23.1 engage in dieting behaviour. 24.1 like my stornach to be cmpty. 25.1 enjoy trying new rich foodt. 26.1 have the impulse to vomit rAer d.

Reprinted, with permission fiom: Garner, D. M.. Olmsted, M. P., Bohr. Y.,& Garfinkcl, P. E. ( 1982). The Eating Attitudes Test: Psychomaic featurcs and clinicai comlates. Psychological Medicine, 12, 87 1-878. Look at the above figures to answer the following questions. Choosc a Ietter for your answcr.

1. Which drawing looks most Iik your body shape at this moment? Figure #

2. Which drawing shows the body shape you would most like to look like? Fi,ourt #

3. Which body shape do you think is most attractive? For women? For girls?

4. Which body shape do you think guys would find most attractive? Figure #

5. Which body shape do you think would be healthicst? For women? For girls?

6. What is your pmntwtight? Height?

7. What would you like COweigh?

8. List al1 the words you can think of to describe person B:

9. List all the words you can think of to dcscribe pmon J

10. List al! the worâs you CM thinlr of to âescribe pason F:

Reprintcd witb permission hm: SM*ud. A.. Sorcnsen. T.. & Schulsbgcr. F-(1980). Use of th Danish Adoption Rcgister for tbc study of obcsity adthinness. la S. Kety (Edo).î%e genetics of neurological rmdpsychiattic disorrlcrs @p. 115-120). New Yak: Ravexi APPENDIX C-1

Approval from University of Manitoba Faculty Commiriee on the Use of Human Subjects in Research

NAME: Ms . Gai1 Warchessault OUEI REFERENCE: E96:ltS DATE: July 2, 1996

Cultural Understandings About Weight: A Cornparison of Hothers and Daughters f rom Aboriginal, a on-Aboriginal, Lover and ~igherIncome Families.

HAS BEEN APPROVED BY THE COMMITTEE AT THEIR MEETING OF: June 24th, 1996.

COMMXTTEE PROVISOS OR LWITUIONS:

You may be asked at intervals for a status report. Any significant changes of the protocol should be reported to the Chairinan for the coumittee's consideration, in advance of implementation of such changes.

**THIS XS FOR TES BTHICS OI 8- US8 ONLY. FOR TB1 LOQZSTICS OF P-OMINO TB1 STWDY, APPRO- SffOULD BB BOUGET FROH TEE R8LEVAMT INSTITUTION, IF ReQUIReD* Sincer y yours

4 C PA Gordon Rw Grahame, HwDwr Chairman, Faculty Committee on the Use of Human Subjects in Reserach. TELEPHONE INQUIRIES : 789-3255 - Lorraine Lester Approval from The Winnipeg School Division No. 1Research, Planning and Tec hnology, Researc h Advisory Cornmittee

THE WlNNlPEQ SCHOOL DIWSION NO. 1 RES~PumPcri AND TEOQYOCOOY aeOIibnrDlwA~ibuW-k(iEnou~~

Gail Marchcssault 604 Strathcona Strttt Winnipeg, h4B R3G 3E7

Dear Gail: Re: Rcsearch Rcquest - Cultural Ulrdcntanding About Weight: A Cornparison of Mothen and Daughtcn fmm Aboriginal, Noa-Abor3ginai, bwerand Higber Incomt Families

This letter is to idorni you buthe appropriate officiais of the Winnpcg SchlDivision No. 1 hm reviewcd the above-mentioncd in principlc, providing the approvai fiom the principais O *IilirOIWriYOQIiYPWkPlease contact is voluntaxy and any participant may withdraw at any the-duringyour data coliection procedures.

As a result of the Division's participation in your study a copy of the hstarch dtsshould be submitted to this office at its completion.

Regards,

/9 Douglas R Edmond Chair, Research Advisory Cornmittee APPENDJX D-1 Code: Mother- CONSENT TO PARTICIPATE IN STUDY Daughter-

1 understand that 1am king asked to take part in a study about weight, called "Mothers' and Daughters' Understandings of Weight" The interviewer will ask me questions about what 1 think about weight. This interview will last between one and two hours. 1have been given an oral and wntten explanation of the study. 1have been given the researcher's name and university address and have been given a chance to ask questions. 1 understand that 1can ask more questions at any time.

1understaad that 1 cmchoose to take part, or not to take part in this study and that 1can stop the interview at any time. My decision to take part or not to take part will not affect the way 1 or my family is treated at school. 1have ken told this study may not benefit me in any way. However, rny participation will add to the knowledge about weight concems of teen-aged girls and their mothers.

1 understand that my identity will not be reveaied and answers to al1 questions will be kept entirely confidential. 1 also understand that a summary of the results of the study will be sent to the school, and a copy of the final report will be sent to the school division. 1 understand that if my scores indicate that 1might have an eating disorder, this will be discussed with me, and if 1am under 18,I will be refemd to either the school counsellor or a counsellor outside of the school, if 1prefer. 1understand that this information will not be disclosed to anyone else without my consent.

My signature on this page indicates that I understand and agree to take part in the study.

Name: (print) Signature of participant: Date: Signature of witness: Date:

If you would like a sumrnary of the results, please pnnt your address below:

Address :

Phone: *** I have explained to the nature and purpose of this research project as descnbed on the information sheet which has been given to the participant. I have asked her if she has any questions about the study and have answered these questions to the best of my ability.

Signature of Investigator: Date: APPENDIX D-2 Code: Mothe,r- MOTHER'S CONSENT FOR DAUGHTER'S PARTICIPATION

1 understand that 1 am king asked to give permission for my daughter to take part in a study about weight, called "Mothers' and Daughters' Understandings of Weight." The intewiewer will ask her questions about what she thinks about weight. This interview will last between one and two hours. 1 have been given a written explanation of the study and the researcher's name, university address and phone number so that 1can ask questions. 1 understand that I can ask more questions at any time.

1understand that my daughter cmchoose to take part, or not to take part in this snidy and that she can stop the interview at any tirne. Her decision to take pmor not to take pari will not affect the way she is tnxted at school. 1have been told this study may not benefit us in any way. However, her participation will add to the knowledge about weight concerns of teen-aged girls and their mothers.

1understand that her identity will not be revealed and her answers to all questions will be kept entirely confidential. 1 also undentand that a surnmary of the results of the study will be sent to the school, and a copy of the final report will be sent to the school division. 1 understand that if my daughters' scores indicate that she might have an eating disorder, this will be discussed with her and she will be referred to either the school counsellor or a counsellor outside of the school, if she prefers. This information will not be disclosed to anyone else without her consent.

My signature on this page indicates that 1understand and agree that my daughter can take part in the study if she chooses to participate.

Daughter's name:

Parent's

Signature of parent: Date:

Signature of witness: Date:

1have explained to the nature and pwpose of this research project as descnbed on the information sheet which has been given to her. 1 have asked her if she has any questions about the study and have answered these questions to the best of my ability.

Signature of Investigator: Date: APPENDIX E INFORMATION ABOUT THE STUDY: MOTHERS' AND DAUGHTERS' UNDERSTANDINGS OF WEIGHT

Proiect Title: Mothers' and Daughters' Understandings of Weight.

Investiaators: Gai1 Marchessault will be doing the research. She is a graduate student in the Department of Community Health Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, University of Manitoba, 750 Bannatyne Ave., Winnipeg, Manitoba, R3E 0W3. Ph: (204) 7744637.

Dr. Linda Garro, Department of Community Health Sciences, is supervising the research. Ph: (204) 789-3365.

Purpose of Studv: The purpose of this study is to lemhow mothers and daughters talk about weight. 1 will taik to mothers and daughters separately because 1am interested in comparing how mothers and daughters talk about weight issues. 1will be taking to a number of grade eight girls hmthree schools and their mothers. The people king asked to take part in this study are selected by chance. Except for the fact that 1 am doing the study through your school, 1 have not chosen people for any special reason. For example, 1 have not selected people because of what they weigh.

The Interview: The interview usually takes between one and two hours. 1will meet with mothers at an agreed upon time and place. 1 will meet with daughters at school during the school day. 1 will interview the mothers first. The questions for both interviews will be about the sarne, except 1 ask the mothers more questions.

Let me tell you about the questions 1 will only ask the mothers. 1will ask the mothers for background information about themselves and their family. This information will help me understand the influences on your thinking about weight. Questions for the mothers will include: how long the family has lived in the neighbourhood, the mother's age and the daughter's age, marital statu, number of chi ldren, religion, occupation, level of education, and income.

After this, the interviews for the mothers and the daughters are just about the same. 1will ask you some questions about what you think about weight. THERE ARE NO RIGHT OR WRONG ANSWJ3tS. 1 am interested in what you think. 1will then ask you about your own weight history: height, weight, what you'd like to weigh, if you've ever tried to change your weight and what happened. Then 1will ask you some true-false questions. The last part consists of two short questionnaires about attitudes towards eating and your weight.

1would like your permission to tape record the interview. 1cannot write as fast as people talk and the interview will go more smoothly and take less of your time if it is done this way. Mso, the interview results will be more accurate if your actual words are tape recorded instead of having me just wnte down a summary. The tapes will be erased at the end of the study. INFORMATION ABOUT THE STUDY: MOTHERS' AND DAUGHTERS' UNDERSTANDINGS OF WEIGHT Confidentialitv: Records of the inte~iewwith you will be coded only with a number and not your name, so that any records of your interview could only be identified by myself. No other person will be given any of the interview data or the records. The consent forms will be the only record with your name on it. Any reports written about this project will not mention your name or provide any description of you that would identiS you. I will treat the tape-recording of the interview in the same confidentid way. A secretary rnay type up the interview, but she will not know who you are. If1 think you might have an eating disorder, 1will help you to see a counsellor. As an eating disorder can be very dangerous, it is important to seek help. I will talk this over with you before talking to anyone else.

Partici~ation:Joining the research is completely up to you. 1hope you will volunteer, but you are under no obligation to join the study. You can decide not to join the study or to drop out at any point in time, even while we are talking. If you decide not to talk with me, this will not affect your treatment at school. 1am not working for the school division and this project is not king done for the school. The school and the school division will oniy receive a report about the whole project. 1will not report back any information about your interview. Mothers will be asked to give consent for themselves at the beginning of their interview and for their daughters at the end of their own interview. Daughters will also be asked to give their consent. Daughters will not be interviewed unless the mother has consented for her daughter to take part.

Risk and Discornfort: In ail research projects camied out by the university, the person doing the project must point out any risks or discornforts for the study. 1do not think this study will cause any problems for you other than 1) taking up your time to answer questions, and 2) although we dont think this will happen, asking questions that may bnng up personal problems. You may refuse to answer any questions that you do not wish to answer. Just let me know if you would like to skip any questions. Benefits: Al1 university projects must also point out if there are any benefits for the study. 1 do not expect the study to have any direct benefits for you. You will not receive any payment for taking part in this study. When completed, this research should help health professionais and school personnel understand more about the thinking and concerns of teen-aged girls and their mothers about weight issues. This could be used to improve health teaching. For More Information: If after the interview you have any Merquestions about the study, please feel free to contact me, Gai1 Marchessault. My telephone number is 774- 4637.1 will also be pleased to provide you with a summary of the study findings if you are interested. APPENDIX F

Summary of Findings for Participants Perspectives on Weight

Thank you to the 163 mothers and daughters who shared their views on weight with me- Each interview was Overall Agreement helpfbl and 1 feel privileged to have had a chance to meet witô you. 1 wodd like to share some of the There was much agreement in people's findings with you. answers to the interYiew questions. In this study, 1examine4 whaî There were also some Merences. For people said about weight in two mai. example, 86% of womai, but ody ways: 1) 1 lwked at the similarity 50% of girls, thought thaî ifa motha between what girls and their mothers was very concerneci about weight, it said; and 2) 1 compared the views of was more likely that her daughter Abonguial and non-Aboriginal would be too. participants. Families were recniited A majority of grils (65%) said fiom four schools, as foliows: that mothers shouid tell theu daughters not to worsy about weight Non-Aboriginal Wes were fiom no matter what îhey weighed. Mothers 1. A Suburh Winnipeg school (65%) said the opposite. But over 80% 2. A RudManitoba schl. of both agreed that mothas should Abonginal t'amilies were fiom: help their daughters lose weight, if 3. An UrhAboriginal school in needed- Winnipeg @lus vohlnteers). Mothers' advice about weight 4. A rurai Manitoba FiNafiuns seemed aimed at increasing seK community school, acceptance either through weight control or through learning to accept Overail, 71.3%ofthe families body size as a given. Both positions asked to participate agreed. Some focussed on eahgwell and king Suburban and Rural mothers declined active. About half of the gkIs repesrted because of pnor problems around their mothersa advice accurately, weight and eating concems. Concem except only 12 girls repeated their about weight is iikely under-estimatecl mothd compliments. at these locations- What was the concem about weight?

This chart shows bow many girfs and women at each location were disddied with their body shape. (Thq seiected a desirad body jhape tk was larger than the one they thought thy looked like.) Dissatidàdon ranged hm25% m the Rural girls to 82% in the Urban Aborigd women. Despite theu high levds of Y) dissatisfâction, few participants tcported they were cumdy dieting-ody 24% ofgiris abd 28% of women There wert meny more wha reporteci "just watcbiag" wbat tbey a!e or trying to be more active, somehfÔr hghtlos, aaburkn Rd Vlbrn Abor FWC sometimes for heaith, somctimcs for both Womrn

More on Dieting

The Restrault Scale is mother way to measure tcodency to diet, The figure indicates that FiNations comrmmity girls and women were the most tilrdy to dia. This came up in the interviews too. hotber scale, the Edng Attitudes Test, indicated risk for eathg disorders in nine participants (four girls and one woman in the FiNations communifv, threc AboriP;nni and one non-Aboriginal giri in the &y)- Some women (both Abonginai and non-Aborigid) told stories of similar concern whthey wece y--

The next page shows participants' responses to some of the questions about body shapes. Median (or middle) responses are given for each location for girls and for womm To read this graph, fmd your location and look up to see the lmer indicated on the lint. This is the ~IISW~~tbat participants at your location gave to tbat question. Oniy rtsponses that wert statistically diffaait between at Ieast two locations are S~WII,Li join locations that gave diffaent rtspollsc3- Gris gave dSerent answers to only one question-their perceived shape. Girls' responses to the other questions are @en in brackets &er the question Both girls and womea thought men pdimd body shape D-Differences between Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal participants are iadicated in the left ma.@ with a checkmarlc (f)for possible diaerences and stars for more catain djfFerences (99 times out of 100). The X means diifferences bctween Aboriginal and non- Aboriginat women couldn't be testd because thecc w«t dimences betwecn the Subirrban and Rural women ** Which drawing looks mmükc your body sbapc at this nionara?

4 Wch dnwing sbaws tbe body shapc you wcmid w>st UKCto look Like? (Girls answcred D)

X Whicb body & yai think is most attractin for womenl (Girls answcricd E)

X Which body sbapt do ym tbhk ù mmaîîmctk for girls? (Gianswrrrd D)

4 Which body shap & ym thùk wouid k heahhiest for wom~~l? (Girls answcred E) who also gave this advice. This was a Like Mother, Like Daughter? smdgroup of Edmiles, but it siiggests that mother-daughter similanty may be most evident when it departs hmthe Perhaps the daughters are on to nom. Girls are not necessdy something. There was iMe evidence emulaîing their mothers' weight that girls foilow their mothers' concems but they may leam to example with respect to weight counter the cultural obsession with concems. There were no signi6.cant weight fiom them. associations between motherdaughter pairs in any of the measures of Aboriginal Girls At-Risk concern about weight (whkh included the values statements, the Restra.int Scale, the Eating Attitudes Test, The findings indicated that Aboriginai dissatisfaction with body shape, and participants7 ôoth girk and women, broad categories of advice both wodd were more concemed about their give to others)- Results sometimes weight than non-Abonginai tended to go in the opposite direction. participants. Abonginal girls and There weren't many dieters in women living in or close to Winnipeg the group, but mothers who dieted did exbi'bited high levels of concern about not have daughters who dieted in any their weight. This suggests both greater numbers than the mothers who weight and weight preoccupation need didn't diet. to be considered in promoting health- There was one exception to this general pattern. Non-dieting mothers who advised people to accept their bodies as they were and focus on healthy living, rather than weight, were more likely to have daughters Introductory Letter to Parent

THE UNIVERSITY OF M&WltOBA

Dear Parent,

My name is Gail Marchessault. 1 un a graâuatc snidcnt at the University of Manitoba. As put of my program, 1 am doing a mdy to leam how gtade eight girls and tbeir motbtn taik about wtigbt issues. 1 am writing Att parents of grade eight guls rt yout school to explain this snidy. I am aise faking ta families from severai otber schools. 1 un tryhg to get an idea of the sorts of opinions kld by girls ad women ftom diercnt backgrounds.

The families askcd to take part in this study arc not sclected because of wôat tbey weigh. t am interestcd in fdigout what people think. even if tbey are not concerned about tkir weight. The study's findings will be more convinchg if more people tlke part.

The interview usually takes about an bout with mothen, and about 45 minutes with dauj$ters. 1 tak to the mother fmt, and then ask permission to approach the daughter. 1 ask girls the same kinda of questions as mothen. Depending on what is most convenient, these interviews could k done eitkr at your home or at school.

Ai1 information gathered tbrough this study wiil be kcpt confidentiat. The dushould klp health professionais and tcachers educatt about wcight issues.

1 am writing CO ask you if 1 can cal1 you to talk about thw study. 1 am not yet zctcinp for y~ur permission to interview you or your daughter. 1 am just asking if 1 can contact you to explah more abut the study, to answer any questions you might have and to fdout if you are willing to participaie. It is, of course, up to you whcthcr you choose to participate in this study, dthough 1 un hophg you wu.

If you do NOT want the schoal to give me your name, address and phone numkr, plsase ull the principal, 9"'fore January 30. I would appreciate it if you wouid aliow your name to be included even if you later decide you don? want to be intcrviewed. Aiiowing your namc to be included dots mt mean you are agrceing to be interviewed. bc happy 1 would to talk with you if you have- any questions or concerns. (Cal1 me coliect at 204-774- 4637.) I look fornard to talking with you.

Gaiï Marchessault, R.D., Pm,Pb-D. (Cd.) Detectable Effect for Group Cornparisons of Different Sample Sizes

Questionnaire Standard deviation Detectable effect

Sample size, n 163 20 40 80

Restraint Scaie 5.32 (4-17-6.10)' 4-70 (3.69-5.40)' 3 -34 2.35

Modified Restraint Scde 3.23 (1.95-3.92) 2.86 (1 -73-3.50) 2.03 1.43 EAT-26 6.89 (2.51-10.69) 6.10 (2.22-9.46) 3.82 3.O5 Body Shape Drawings

Perceived shape 1.27 (0.80-1 -44) 0.99 (0-71-1-27) 0.80 0.25

Other s hapes: Iow O. 55 (0.32) 0.49 (0.28) 0.35 0.24 Other shapes: high 0.77 (1.18) 0.68 (1.04) 0.48 0.34

Note. Calculations were based on standard deviation of the collected data, assuming a Type 1 emor probability of .OS, 80% power and two-tailed testing. "Bracketed numbers refer to minimum and maximum (standard deviation or detectable effect) for the eight groups (girls and women at each of the four locations).

Detectable Effect for Paired Cornparisons of Different Sample Sizes

Questionnaire Standard deviation of difference Detectable effect

Sarnple size, n pairs Restraint Scale Modified Restraint Scde EAT-26

Note. Calculations were based on standard deviation of the collected data, assurning a Type 1 error probability of .OS, 80% power and two-tailed testing. APPENDIX 1- 1

Correlations Between Measures of Weight and Weight-Related Scales for Girls

------Questionnaire Body Mass Index EAT-26 Restraint Scaie Modifieci Restra.int r P r P r P r P Perceived body (.66) tOOOO 1* (-09) -42 (-51) cûûûûl* (-42) .0001* shape

Body mass index (-21) -09 -49 .00005* -35 .W+

Restraint Scale -83 c00001*

Note. Pearsons' r is used when both scales are normally distributed; Spearman's r (in brackets) when one of the scales is ordinal or non-normally distnbuted. *=p~.0001;+=p~.oo1.

APPENDIX 1-2

Correlations Between Measures of Weight and Weight-Related Scales for Women

Questionnaire Body Mass Index EAT-26 Restraint Scaie Modifieci Restraint

Perceived body (-84) .ûûûûûû* (.O61 39 (-47) .ûûûûl* (-26) -02- shape Body Mass Index (-05) -96 -51 -000002* -25 .OZ-

Restraint Scale -82 .000000*

Note. Pearsons' r is used when both scales are normaily distributed; Spearman' s r (in brackets) when one of the scaies is ordinal or non-normaily distributed. *=p1..0001;-=p~.O5 Summary and Discussion of Correlations Between Measures of Weight and Weight- Related Scales

1. There is high agreement between perceived body shape and the BMI (Spearman's r = -66 for girls; .84 for women; p < .O1for both).

2. The correlations between the Restraint Scale and the measures of weight status are higher than between the Modified Restraint Scale and the weight measures (by approximately .2). This is not surprïsing since the full scale includes questions based on weight status.

3. There is a high correlation between the Restraint and the Modified Restraint scales (Pearson's r = -82). This ciifference in agreement (- -18) estimates the effect of the four questions on weight status that were removed.

4. For the girls, there was a significant correlation between the Modified Restraint Scale and perceived body shape (Spearman's r = .42; p = .01). The relationship was lower and only a significant trend for the women (Spearman's r = .26; p = .02). This estimates an association between dietary restraint and body weight, in the expected direction. (Heavier participants were more likely to be classified by the Restraint Scale as "dieters.")

5. The association between EAT-26 scores and perceived body shape was low and non- significant (Spearman's r = .06; p = .59 for women; r = -09,p = -42 for girls), suggesting that extreme attitudes to food restriction are not predicted by body weight. However, the number of high scores was low, and the lack of significance may reflect inadequate sample size.

6. There were moderate and highly significant @ c .00001) correlations between the EAT-26 and the Modified Restraint Scale (Spearman's r = .49, for women; .43 for girls). This suggests a link between the tendency to diet and nsk for eating disorder. Perhaps this is obvious. Participants who score high on the EAT-26 would always score high on the Restraint Scale. Descriptive Statistics for the Modified Restraint Scale

Standard Standard Min- Group n Mean deviation error Median Mode Max AIl Girls Suburban Urban Aboriginal Rural Fïrst Nations community Women Suburban Urban Aboriginal Rural First Nations community Pairs (difference) 80

Note. The Modified Restraint Scale consists of the Restraint Scde with four questions based on weight status removed. APPENDDC: K

Univariate Analyses of Modified Restraint Scale for 75 Paired Participants

Test Variable Df value p Group n Med Mean SE Generation 148 Girls Location 3 H = 8-95" -03 Suburban *Rural Urban Aboriginal *FitNations Ethnicity 1 H = 3-74" .O5 Non-aboriginal Aboriginal H=.99ga .31 Rural Urban Women Location 3 F = 2.14 .10 Suburban Rura! Urban Aboriginal First Nations Ethnicity 73 t = 0.63 .53 Non-Abonginal Aboriginal Region 73 t = 2.22 .O3 Rural Urban

Note. Med = Median; SE = Standard Error. " Kniskal-Wallis test was used due to suspected non-nonnality. * Identified as significantly different by Tukey-Krarner Multiple Cornparison Test. APPENDIX L-1

Split Uni: Analysis of Variance Table for Modified Restraint Scale for 75 Mother-Daughter Pairs

Source Sum of Mean Prob Term DF Squares Square F-Ratio Level A(Ethnicity) 1 46.81691 46.8 1691 4.68 .O3 * B (Region) 1 44.5059 44.5059 4.45 .04* AB 1 43-62759 43.62759 4.36 .W* c(AB) 71 710.61 1 10.00861 1.10 .34 D(Generation) 1 25.62667 25.62667 2.82 -10 S 74 673.3734 909964 Total (Adjusted) 149 1541.573 Total 150 * Term significant at alpha = -05

APPENDIX L-2

Tukey-Krarner Multiple-Cornparison Test to Locate Differences for Split Unit Analysis of Variance for Modified Restraint Scale for 75 Mother-Daughter Pairs

Standard Different Term Error From Groups Al1 A: Ethnicity Non-Aboriginal 0.37 Aboriginal Aboriginal 0.36 Non-Aboriginal B: REGION Rutal 0.38 Urban Urban 0.35 Rural D: Generation Girls Women AB: Ethnicity; Region (0,O) non-Aboriginal; ruriù (Rural) (0,l) non-Aboriginal; wban (Suburban) (1 ,O) Aboriginal; curai (First Nations) (1.1) Aboriginal; urban

Note. Criticai Vaiue=2.82 for Ethnicity and Region; 3.72 for AB (ethnicity, region interaction) . Descriptive Statistics for the Restraint Scale

Standard Standard Group n Mean deviation error Median Mode Range All 155 10.5 5.32 0.43 10 8 0-23

Girls 75 9.3 5.17 0.60 9 8 0-21 Suburban 19 7.6 4-35 1 -00 8 - 0-15 Urban Aboriginal 20 10.8 5.56 1.24 9.5 - 3-21 Rural 19 7.4 4.17 0.96 7 4 2-16 First Nations comrnunity Women 80 11.8 5.20 0.58 115 15 0-23 Suburban 20 11.6 5.28 1.18 11 7 0-21 Urban Aboriginal 21 11.6 4-57 1 .O0 Il - 2-23 Rural 20 11.0 5.05 1.13 10 10 0-20

First Nations 19 12.9 6-10 1 -40 13 17 2-22 cornmunity

Pairs (difference) 72 2.4 7.34 0.87 2.5 7 -17-20

Note. Nine scores with 1 missing answer were included. APPENDIX M-2

Univariate Analyses of Restra.int Scale for Paired Participants

Variable Df Test value p Group n Mean SE Generation Girls Location -04 Suburban *Rural Urban Aboriginal *First Nations Ethnicity .ûO4** Non-aboriginal Abriginal Region .94 Rurd Urban Women Location .53 Suburban Rural Urban Aboriginal First Nations Ethnicity .20 Non-Aboriginal Aboriginal Region .7 1 Rural Uhan

Note. Analyses were repeated with dl participants (including unmatched participants). Results were essentiaily the sarne, except they were stronger. SE = Standard Error.

+ Identi fied as significantl y different by Tukey-Krarner Multiple Cornparison Test. APPENDK M-3 Split Unit Analysis of Variance Table for Restraint for Mother-Daughter Pairs

Source Sum of Mean Rob Term DF Squares Square F-Ratio Level (Alpha=.05) A (Ethnicity) 1 237.8287 237.8287 8.94 .004* B (REGION) 1 4.1 13446 4. L 13446 0.15 -70 AB 1 12.12654 12.12654 0.46 -50 c(AB) 64 1702.895 1702.895 D (Generation) 1 175.98 11 175.981 1 6.49 .Ol* AD 1 3 1-07029 3 1.07029 1.15 -29 BD 1 0.8321514 0.8321514 0.03 -86 ABD 1 2.48 1087 2.48 1087 0.09 -76 CD(AB) 64 1735.211 27.1 1267 S O O Total (Adjusted) 135 3891.382 To ta1 136 * Terrn signifiant at alpha = -05 APPENDIX M-4 Tukey-Kramer Multiple-Cornparison Test Standard Different Terrn Count Mean Error hmGroups Al1 136 10.70 A: Ethnicity Non-Aboriginal 66 9.38 0.63 AboriginaI Aboriginal 70 12.03 0.62 Non-Aboriginal B: REGION Rural 64 10.88 0.65 Urban 72 10.53 0.6 1 D: Generation Girls 68 9.56 0.63 Womn Women 68 11.84 0.63 Girls AB: EthnicityJZEGION Non-Aborigina1,Rural 32 9.25 0.9 1 Non-Aborigina1,Urban 34 9.50 0.89 AboriginaiJtural 32 12.50 0.9 1 Aborigina1,Urban 38 1 1.55 0.84 AD: Ethnicity,Generation Non-Abongina1,Girls 33 7-76 0.91 Abor women & girls Non-Abonginal,Women 33 11.00 0.9 1 Aboriginal,Girls 35 11.37 0.88 Non-Aboriginal girls Aboriginai,Wornen 35 12.69 0.88 Non-Aboriginal girls Critical Value = 2.83 for Ethnicity and Generation; 3.73 for AD (Ethnicity, generation interaction) APPENDIX N

Descriptive Statistics for the Eating Attitudes Test - 26

Standard At-risk Group n Mean Deviation Median Mode Range score, n

All 163 5-7 6.9 4 1 0 -44 9

Girls 80 6.8 8.4 4 3 0-44 8

Suburban 20 4.7 5.0 4 4 0-22 1

Urban Abonginai 21 7.6 10.7 3 3 0 - 44 3 Rural 20 3-5 2.8 3 3 0- 11 O

Fit Nations community

Women 83 4.7 4.9 3 1 0-28 1

Suburban 21 4.8 4.1 4 4 0- 19 O Urban Aboriginal 22 3.1 2.5 3 - 0- 11 O

Rural 21 3.1 2.9 2 I 0-9 O First Nations communi ty Pairs (difference) 80 -2.1 9.6 O -3 -41 - 27 APPENDIX O

Univariate Analyses of Eating Attitudes Test-26 for 75 Paired Participants

Variable Df ChiSquareH P G~OUP n Median Generation Girls Location 3 7.19 .O7 Suburban Rural Uhan Aboriginal First Nations Ethnicity 1 3.48 .36 Non-Aboriginal Aboriginal Region 1 1.27 .26 Rural Urban Women Location 3 11.21 .O1 Suburban *Rural Urban Aboriginal *First Nations Ethnicity 1 1.O0 .32 Non-Aboriginal Aboriginal Region 1 0.18 .67 Rural Urban

Note. Missing data due to ethnic cornparison. Kruskal-Wallis test was used for univariate di fferences. * Significantly different at the Bonfernoni critical value of the Kruskal-Wallis Multiple- Cornparison 2-Value Test. Additionally there were trends at the regular test level for the following: Suburban women differed fkom Rural women; Uhan Aboriginal and Rural women differed from First Nations community women. Mother-Daughter Correlations for Restraint Scaie, Modifîed Restraint Scale and Eating Attitudes Test (EAT-26) Scores for Al1 Families and by Location, Ethnicity and Region

Restraint Modifiecl Restraint EAT-26

Group n Pearson's r p n Pearson's r p n Speannan's r p

Al1 Location Suburban Urban Aboriginal Rurai First Nations community

Ethnici ty Non-Aboriginal Aboriginal Region Urban Rurai Comparing Median Responses to Body Shape Drawings Questionnaire by Generation

Question Girls' n Women's n Girls Women z score P Perceived 79 83 5.0 5.5 5.05 .00001* Desired 78 83 4.0 5.0 3.89 .0001* Women attractive 76 79 5.0 5-0 2.17 .O3 Girls attractive 75 77 4-0 4.0 ~~~0.22~-05 Guys prefer 79 81 4.0 4.0 2.08 .O5 Women healthiest 77 83 5.0 5 .O 2.90 .W* Girls healthiest 80 79 4.0 4.0 0.41 -68

Note. Differences between generations were tested with the Mann-Whitney U Test (with normal approximation to correct for ties and the correction factor for continuity). "KS = Kolmogorow-Smimov Test used (due to possible unequal variances). * =p -01. APPENDIX R Comparing Girls' Body Shape Drawing Responses by Ethnicity and Region

Ethnicity Region

NonAborig- Abonginai z Rurai Urban z Question inal median median n", na score p median median nr,nu score p

Perceived 4 5 36.38

Desired 4 4 36.37

Women 5 5 35.36 attractive

Girls 4 4 33.37 attractive

Guys 4 4 36.38 prefer Women 5 5 36.36 healthiest Girls 4 4 36.39 healthiest

Noie. Mann-Whitney U Test with nodapproximation to correct for ties and correction factor for continuity. n", na; n: nu = sarnple size for the non-Aboriginal, Aboriginal, rural and uhan samples. rcspectively. * = p s -01. X = Groups to be combined differed (at p s -05; Kruskal-Wallis test) and therefore were not tested.

APPENDIX S Comparing Women's Body Shape Drawing Responses by Ethnicity and Region

Ethnicity Region

Non-Aborig Aboriginal z Rurai Urban z Question inal median rnedian n", na score p media. median n", na score R Perceived Desired

Women attractive

Girls 4.0 4.3 35.38 DMN attractive 0.30 ' Guys prefer Women healthiest Girls heal hiest

Note. Mann-Whitney U Test with nodapproximation to comct for tics and correction factor for continuity. na, na; nr, nu = sample size for the non-Aboriginal. Aboriginal, rurai and urban samples, respcctively- 'Kolrnogorow-Smirnov test was used (due to possible unequa1 variances). * = p s -01; ? = p s -05; X not testcd because groups to be combined diffed (at p s -05; Kniskal-Wallis test). Comparing Girls' Median Responses to Body Shape Drawings Questions by Location

Uhan First Nations Chi Question Suburban Rural Aboriginal community square P Perceived 4 4 5 5 12.38 .006* Desired 4 4 4 4 1.51 -68 Wornen attractive 5 5 5 5 0.29 .% Girls attractive 4 4 4 4 0.50 -92

Guys prefer 4 4 4 4 3 .% -27

Women healthiest 5 5 5 5 1-10 -78 Girls healthiest 4 4 4 4 1-42 -70

Note. The df for the test of each question was 3. * =p s -01; Kruskal-Wallis test

APPENDIX U

Comparing Women's Median Responses Body Shape Drawings Questions by Location

Urban Fht Nations Chi Question Suburban Rural Aboriginal community square p Percei ved 5.0 5.0 6.0 6.0 8-19 -Mt Desired 4.0 5.0 5.0 5.0 6.23 -10 Women attractive 4.5 5.0 5.0 5.0 8.51 -047

Girls attractive 4.0 4.0 5.0 4.0 7.75 .Os?

Guys prefer 4-0 4.0 4.0 4.0 1.56 0.67

Wornen hedthiest 4.5 5.0 5.0 5.0 7.84 .05t

Girls healthiest 4.0 4.0 4.5 4.0 7.29 -06

Note. The df for the test of each question was 3. t = p s -05 (non-significant trend); Kntskal-Wallis test. APPENDlX v

Comparing Girls' ruid Women's Median Responses to Body Shape Drawings Questionnaire at Each Incation

- Question Suburban RuraI Urban Abonanal Fmt Nations ni ni nz z nz z

Perceived 20 2.97 20 Dmn 2 1 22 0.46

Desired 20 1.47 19 2.70 2 1 22

Wornen 19 3-13 20 0.06 attractive 20 22

Girls 18 0.21 20 Dmn attractive 20 22 0.54

Guys 20 0.10 20 Dmn prefer 2 1 21 0.33

Women 20 Dmn 20 0.92 heaithiest 21 0.52 22

Girls 20 0.82 21 2.21 heaithiest 20 22

Nofe-DMN = Kolmogorow-Smirnov test was used (due to possible unequal variances). * = significant at p s .01. NS = non-significant trend at p s .O5 Phcipant Satisfaction Rating of Body Shape by Generation, Ethnicity and Region

Al1 Generation Ethnicity Region Non- Women Girls Aboriginal Aboriginal Urban Rural Sample size

Perceived body shape, % larger than desired same as desired srnaller than desired Desired body shape, 9% Larger than attractive Same as attractive Srnaller than attractive

Desired body shape, % Larger than healthy Same as healthy Smailer than healthy

Attractive body shape, % Larger than guys' prefer Sarne as guys prefer Srnaller âhan guys' prefer 10

Note. Numbers may not add up to 100 due to rounding. APPENDIX W-2

Comparing Desired and Attractive Body Shape Selections

Desired and attractive (and ideal) are ofien used interchangeably (Fallon & Rozin,

1985; Gittelsohn et al., 1996; Silberstein et al., 1988; Stevens et al., 1999; Tiggeman &

Wilson-Barrett, 1998). The logistic analyses discussed in Chapter 9 and reported in

Appendix W-1 was extended to test this semantic question: Are participants' selections for desired and attractive body shape the same (or not)? Results of the logistic regression modelling were: 2 = 1.96, df = 1, p = 0.16. N.S.This allowed responses to be combined to assess if respondents had selected the same body shape for both questions. The results were:

24% of participants chose a desired body shape different from the one they selected as most attractive (with 13% selecting a larger desired shape, and 11% selecting a smailer desired shape).

This response was significantly different than would be expected had the participants given similar responses to both questions (XZ = 78.93, df = 2, p < .000000. See Appendix

V for details.)

These results demonstrate that desired and attractive do not necessarily have the

same meaning for people. (And this should be suspected for ideal as well, although it

was not tested.) This is important because it is possible to interpret Aboriginal

participants larger desired weights as indicative of heavier noms for the ideal of

attractiveness. This could be quite misleading . Precise language is required when

discussing the meaning of participants' responses to specific weight questions.