Neurolinguistics Sarah F

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Neurolinguistics Sarah F Neurolinguistics Sarah F. Phillips Language & Mind 13 November 2019 Lecture Roadmap • Foundations of Neurolinguistics • Lesion Analysis: Broca’s and Wernicke’s Area • The Classic Model • The Brain • Techniques used in Neurolinguistics • Hemodynamic techniques (PET, fMRI, fNIRS) • Electrophysiological techniques (EEG, MEG) • Integrating Theory, Methods, and Techniques • ELAN, N400, and P600 effects in EEG • Decomposing the N400 using MEG 1 Defining Neurolinguistics Neurolinguistics The scientific study of the neural bases of language Neurolinguistics Neurolinguistics Having to do with The scientific study the nervous system of language and its (the brain) structure 2 Foundations of Neurolinguistics: Broca’s Area • Pierre Paul Broca (1824-1880) • French physician and anatomist • Worked with patients who suffered from aphasias • In 1861, he first reported a link between a fluency aphasia and damage to a portion of the inferior frontal gyrus • This type of fluency aphasia is now known as “Broca’s Aphasia” • This region is often referred to as “Broca’s Area” Person with Broca’s Aphasia 4 Foundations of Neurolinguistics: Wernicke’s Area • Carl Wernicke (1848-1905) • German neurologist/neuropsychiatrist • Interested in linking behavioral abnormalities and brain lesions • In 1873, he studied a stroke patient who had difficulty with comprehension • This type of aphasia is now known as “Wernicke’s Aphasia” • His work studied to link this type of behavioral abnormality to a region now known as “Wernicke’s Area” 5 Person with Wernicke’s Aphasia 6 What have we learned so far? • Broca and Wernicke were interested in people with aphasia • Aphasia: any language disorder that results from brain damage caused by disease or trauma. • Broca’s Aphasia is characterized by slow, non-fluent speech • Wernicke’s Aphasia is characterized by fluent speech that is difficult to interpret/comprehend • Broca and Wernicke found that each of these types of aphasia were associated with damage to distinct regions of the brain 7 What have we learned so far? 8 Foundations of Neurolinguistics: Lesion Analysis as a method • These earlier ideas about the neural bases of language were largely built from lesion analysis • Broca and Wernicke studied the brains from people with aphasia port-mortem • The logic behind lesion analysis is that damage to a certain region must be what causes a particular problem in language production • Problem: There is no one-to-one correspondence between a lesion site and a symptom observed in language production • However, this early evidence argued for a modular organization of language in the brain 9 Foundations of Neurolinguistics: The Classic Model From Tremblay & Dick 2016, p. 62 (Fig.1) 10 Foundations of Neurolinguistics: Problems with the Classic Model • There is little consensus as to the location and size of Broca’s Area and Wernicke’s Area 11 Where is Broca’s Area? From Tremblay & Dick 2016, p. 65 (Fig.3) 12 Where is Wernicke’s Area? From Tremblay & Dick 2016, p. 64 (Fig.2) 13 Foundations of Neurolinguistics: Problems with the Classic Model • There is little consensus as to the location and size of Broca’s Area and Wernicke’s Area • Model is not spatially precise enough to make specific hypotheses about brain regions and their role in language behavior • Broca’s Area is involved with syntax? • Wernicke’s Area is involved with semantics? • What about phonology and morphology (or, the lexicon)? • Model ignores other cortical structures, subcortical structures, and white-matter pathways 14 Foundations of Neurolinguistics: The Brain • Cerebrum is the largest part of our brain, consisting of cortex and certain subcortical structures • Cerebellum is a part of the hind brain best known to be involved in motor control • Brainstem connects the cerebrum and cerebellum to the rest of the nervous system Image from https://mayfieldclinic.com/pe-anatbrain.htm 15 Foundations of Neurolinguistics: The Brain • Cerebrum consists of two hemispheres, the left and the right • The cortex is the outermost layer of the cerebrum • The cortex can be sectioned into four lobes in each hemisphere: frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital • These areas of cortex are connected by white matter tracts • The two hemispheres are connected by the corpus callosum Image from https://mayfieldclinic.com/pe-anatbrain.htm 16 Techniques used in Neurolinguistics • Current technology allows us to study language in healthy brains in vivo, i.e. while people are still alive! • There are two main categories for the technology that is used: • Hemodynamic techniques • Electrophysiological techniques • Using these techniques, we present typical, healthy participants with language-based tasks and measure brain responses 17 Techniques used in Neurolinguistics: Hemodynamic • When a brain region becomes active, blood carrying oxygen rushes to that region • This Blood Oxygen Level-Dependent (BOLD) response is what’s measured by hemodynamic techniques • Positron-Emission Tomography (PET) 18 Positron Emission Tomography (PET) 19 Techniques used in Neurolinguistics: Hemodynamic • When a brain region becomes active, blood carrying oxygen rushes to that region • This Blood Oxygen Level-Dependent (BOLD) response is what’s measured by hemodynamic techniques • Positron-Emission Tomography (PET) • Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) 20 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) Image from https://www.eurekalert.org/multimedia/pub/188089.php 21 Techniques used in Neurolinguistics: Hemodynamic • When a brain region becomes active, blood carrying oxygen rushes to that region • This Blood Oxygen Level-Dependent (BOLD) response is what’s measured by hemodynamic techniques • Positron-Emission Tomography (PET) • Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) • Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS) 22 Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS) Image from https://www.cephalon.eu/products/navigation/brainsight-nirs/ 23 Techniques used in Neurolinguistics: Hemodynamic • When a brain region becomes active, blood carrying oxygen rushes to that region • This Blood Oxygen Level-Dependent (BOLD) response is what’s measured by hemodynamic techniques • Positron-Emission Tomography (PET) • Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) • Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS) • Pro: High Spatial Resolution • Con: Poor Temporal Resolution 24 Techniques used in Neurolinguistics: Electrophysiological • When a neuron is sending information, an electrical signal passes through the axon • Electroencephalography (EEG) measures the flow of electric current produced by neurons Image from https://www.scienceabc.com/humans/electricity-generated-neurons-brain.html 25 Electroencephalography (EEG) Image from https://www.nhs.uk/conditions/electroencephalogram/ 26 Techniques used in Neurolinguistics: Electrophysiological • When a neuron is sending information, an electrical signal passes through the axon • Electroencephalography (EEG) measures the flow of electric current produced by neurons • Magnetoencephalography (MEG) measures the magnetic fields created by the electric current Image from https://www.scienceabc.com/humans/electricity-generated-neurons-brain.html 27 Magnetoencephalography (MEG) Image from NYU Abu Dhabi 28 Techniques used in Neurolinguistics: Electrophysiological • When a neuron is sending information, an electrical signal passes through the axon • Electroencephalography (EEG) measures the flow of electric current produced by neurons • Magnetoencephalography (MEG) measures the magnetic fields created by the electric current • Pro: High Temporal Resolution • Con: Low Spatial Resolution Image from https://www.scienceabc.com/humans/electricity-generated-neurons-brain.html 29 Integrating Theory, Methods, and Techniques • EEG recordings are chunked into epochs for analysis • EEG-measured brain activation that results from a stimulus are referred to as event related potentials (ERPs) • ERPs are labeled by activation polarity and latency • Activation polarity refers to whether the signal goes in a positive (P) or negative (N) direction • Latency refers to how long (typically in milliseconds) it takes for the peak to occur after the onset of the stimulus 30 Friederici’s (1995) Model of Language Processing • Evidence from ERPs suggests (at least) three stages of language processing: • ELAN à Word Categorization • LAN/N400 à Semantic Integration • P600 à Syntactic reanalysis 31 Early Left Anterior Negativity (ELAN) 32 Left Anterior Negativity (LAN)/ N400 From Kutas & Hillyard (1980) 33 P600 From Osterhout et al. 1994, p. 790 (Fig. 2) 34 Integrating Theory, Methods, and Techniques • MEG recordings are chunked into epochs for analysis • MEG-measured brain activation that results from a stimulus are referred to as event related fields (ERFs) • Spatial resolution is better than EEG (but not as good as any hemodynamic technique) 35 Decomposing the N400 with MEG From Pylkkänen & Marantz 2003, p. 188 (Fig. 1) 36 In Summary… • Based on foundational work by Broca and Wernicke, we have conceptualized a modular organization of the brain • Current neurolinguistic research uses hemodynamic and electrophysiological techniques • Hemodynamic techniques (e.g. fMRI) give better spatial resolution • Electrophysiological techniques (e.g. EEG, MEG) give better temporal resolution • Classical findings in the electrophysiological literature (ELAN, N400, P600) give us the basic neural correlates of language processing 37 Bonus! What about bilinguals? From Bemis & Pylkkänen 2011 38 Bonus! What about bilinguals? 39 Bonus! What about bilinguals? 40.
Recommended publications
  • Gesellschaft Und Psychiatrie in Österreich 1945 Bis Ca
    1 VIRUS 2 3 VIRUS Beiträge zur Sozialgeschichte der Medizin 14 Schwerpunkt: Gesellschaft und Psychiatrie in Österreich 1945 bis ca. 1970 Herausgegeben von Eberhard Gabriel, Elisabeth Dietrich-Daum, Elisabeth Lobenwein und Carlos Watzka für den Verein für Sozialgeschichte der Medizin Leipziger Universitätsverlag 2016 4 Virus – Beiträge zur Sozialgeschichte der Medizin Die vom Verein für Sozialgeschichte der Medizin herausgegebene Zeitschrift versteht sich als Forum für wissenschaftliche Publikationen mit empirischem Gehalt auf dem Gebiet der Sozial- und Kulturgeschichte der Medizin, der Geschichte von Gesundheit und Krankheit sowie an- gren­­zender Gebiete, vornehmlich solcher mit räumlichem Bezug zur Republik Österreich, ihren Nachbarregionen sowie den Ländern der ehemaligen Habsburgermonarchie. Zudem informiert sie über die Vereinstätigkeit. Die Zeitschrift wurde 1999 begründet und erscheint jährlich. Der Virus ist eine peer-reviewte Zeitschrift und steht Wissenschaftlerinnen und Wissenschaftlern aus allen Disziplinen offen. Einreichungen für Beiträge im engeren Sinn müssen bis 31. Okto- ber, solche für alle anderen Rubriken (Projektvorstellungen, Veranstaltungs- und Aus stel lungs- be richte, Rezensionen) bis 31. Dezember eines Jahres als elektronische Dateien in der Redak- tion einlangen, um für die Begutachtung und gegebenenfalls Publikation im darauf ­­fol genden Jahr berücksichtigt werden zu können. Nähere Informationen zur Abfassung von Bei trägen sowie aktuelle Informationen über die Vereinsaktivitäten finden Sie auf der Homepage des Ver eins (www.sozialgeschichte-medizin.org). Gerne können Sie Ihre Anfragen per Mail an uns richten: [email protected] Bibliografische Information der Deutschen Nationalbibliothek Die Deutsche Nationalbibliothek verzeichnet diese Publikation in der Deutschen Nationalbi - bli o grafie; detaillierte bibliografische Daten sind im Internet über http://dnb.d-nb.de abrufbar. Das Werk einschließlich aller seiner Teile ist urheberrechtlich geschützt.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Language and the Brain Colin Phillips, University of Maryland
    Language and the Brain Colin Phillips, University of Maryland & Kuniyoshi L. Sakai, University of Tokyo Many species have evolved sophisticated communication systems, but human language stands out as special in at least two respects, both of which contribute to the vast expressive power of human language. First, humans are able to memorize many thousands of words, each of which encodes a piece of meaning using an arbitrary sound or gesture. By some estimates, during the preschool and primary school years a child learns an average of 5-10 new words per day, on the way to attaining a vocabulary of 20,000-50,000 words by adulthood. Second, humans are able to combine words to form sentences and discourses, making it possible to communicate infinitely many different messages, and providing the basis of human linguistic creativity. Furthermore, speakers are able to generate and understand novel messages quickly and effortlessly, on the scale of hundreds of milliseconds. Linguists and cognitive neuroscientists are interested in understanding what special properties of the human brain make such feats possible. Efforts to answer this question go back at least 150 years. A great deal of attention has been given to the issue of which regions of the human brain are most important for language, first using findings from brain-damaged patients, and in recent years adding a wealth of new information from modern non-invasive brain recording techniques such as positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). However, it is important to bear in mind that knowing where language is supported in the human brain is just one step on the path to finding what are the special properties of those brain regions that make language possible.
    [Show full text]
  • Paul Ferdinand Schilders Bedeutendste Neurowissenschaftliche Und Neurologische Arbeiten
    Paul Ferdinand Schilders bedeutendste neurowissenschaftliche und neurologische Arbeiten Dissertation zur Erlangung des akademischen Grades Dr. med. an der Medizinischen Fakultät der Universität Leipzig eingereicht von: Martin Jahn geboren am 11.08.1982 in Tübingen angefertigt in der: Forschungsstelle für die Geschichte der Psychiatrie Klinik und Poliklinik für Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie Universität Leipzig Betreuer: Herr Professor Dr. rer. medic. Holger Steinberg Beschluss über die Verleihung des Doktorgrades vom: 1 Inhaltsverzeichnis 1. Bibliografische Beschreibung…………………………………………………....…3 2. Einführung……………………………………………………………………………...4 2.1 Motivation und historischer Kontext…………………………….……………..…4 2.2 Schilders Biografie und Werk………………………………………….……........7 2.3 Methodik……………………………………………………………………..….....14 2.4 Bedeutung der Arbeit……………………………………………….….………....17 3. Publikationen………………………………………………………………..……..….19 3.1 Jahn M, Steinberg H (2019) Die Erstbeschreibung der Schilder-Krankheit. Paul Ferdinand Schilder und sein Ringen um die Abgrenzung einer neuen Entität. Nervenarzt 90:415-422 3.2 Jahn M, Steinberg H (2020) Die Schmerzasymbolie – um 1930 von Paul F. Schilder entdeckt und heute fast vergessen? Schmerz, Online First Article vom 25.02.2020 unter https://doi.org/10.1007/s00482-020-00447-z 4. Zusammenfassung der Arbeit……………………………………………..……...37 5. Literaturverzeichnis…………………………………………………………………42 6. Spezifizierung des eigenen wissenschaftlichen Beitrags……………………48 7. Erklärung über die eigenständige Abfassung der Arbeit…………………….49 8.
    [Show full text]
  • Neurolinguistics
    Neurolinguistics 1 What is neurolinguistics? Neurolinguistics studies the relation of language and communication to different aspects of brain function, i.e. it tries to explore how the brain understands and produces language and communication. This involves attempting to combine theory from neurology/neurophysiology (how the brain is structured and how it functions) with linguistic theory (how language is structured and how it functions). • "human language or communication (speech, hearing, reading, writing, or non-verbal modalities) related to any aspect of the brain or brain function" (Brain and Language: "Description") • The common problem area of relating aspects of language or communication to brain function in this dynamic formulation, is stated as a common question by Luria in "Basic problems in neurolinguistics": • "what are the real processes of formation of verbal communication and its comprehension, and what are the components of these processes and the conditions under which they take place" • (Luria, 1976, p.3) Interdisciplinary enterprise • linguistics,neuroanatomy, neurology, neurophysiology, philosophy, psychology, psychiatry, speech pathology and computer science, neurobiology, anthropology, chemistry, cognitive science and artificial intelligence. • Thus, the humanities, as well as medical, natural and social sciences, as well as technology are represented. Different views on the relation between brain and language • Localism tries to find locations or centers in the brain for different language functions. Associationism places language functions in the connections between different areas of the brain, making it possible to associate, for example, perceptions of different senses with words and/or “concepts”. • Dynamic localization of function assumes that functional systems of localized sub-functions perform language functions. Such systems are dynamic, so that they can be reorganized during language development or after a brain damage.
    [Show full text]
  • The Vertical Occipital Fasciculus: a Century of PNAS PLUS Controversy Resolved by in Vivo Measurements
    The vertical occipital fasciculus: A century of PNAS PLUS controversy resolved by in vivo measurements Jason D. Yeatmana,b,c,1,2, Kevin S. Weinera,b,1,2, Franco Pestillia,b, Ariel Rokema,b, Aviv Mezera,b, and Brian A. Wandella,b,2 aDepartment of Psychology and bCenter for Cognitive and Neurobiological Imaging, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305; and cInstitute for Learning and Brain Sciences, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195 Contributed by Brian A. Wandell, October 14, 2014 (sent for review May 2, 2014; reviewed by Charles Gross, Jon H. Kaas, and Karl Zilles) The vertical occipital fasciculus (VOF) is the only major fiber bundle This article is divided into five sections. First, we review the connecting dorsolateral and ventrolateral visual cortex. Only history of the VOF from its discovery in the late 1800s (1) a handful of studies have examined the anatomy of the VOF or through the early 1900s. Second, we link the historical images of its role in cognition in the living human brain. Here, we trace the the VOF to the first identification of the VOF in vivo (15). contentious history of the VOF, beginning with its original discovery Third, we describe an open-source algorithm that uses dMRI in monkey by Wernicke (1881) and in human by Obersteiner (1888), data and fiber tractography to automate labeling of the VOF in to its disappearance from the literature, and recent reemergence human (github.com/jyeatman/AFQ/tree/master/vof). Fourth, we a century later. We introduce an algorithm to identify the VOF in quantify the location and cortical projections of the VOF with vivo using diffusion-weighted imaging and tractography, and show respect to macroanatomical landmarks in ventral (VOT) and that the VOF can be found in every hemisphere (n = 74).
    [Show full text]
  • Historical Introduction to Models of Language and Aphasia
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE Review article provided by Bern Open Repository and Information System (BORIS) Language and brain: historical introduction to models of language and aphasia Heinz Krestel Department of Paediatric Neurology, Inselspital, Bern University Hospital, and University of Bern, Switzerland Funding / potential competing interests: No financial support and no other potential conflict of interest relevant to this article were reported. Summary that the tissue softening spread evenly in all directions. From Broca’s original description, it can be deduced that the A paradigm shift occurred when Paul Broca (1824–1880) assigned the site of third-frontal convolution as the origin of the language defi- language to cerebral convolutions of the left hemisphere against the then cit was his extrapolation because a) the third-frontal con- prevailing dogma by Franz Joseph Gall (1758–1828) that language and other volution presented the largest tissue loss, and b) the missing cognitive functions reside in brain regions/organs that develop and form the posterior half of this gyrus was in the centre of the entire skull accordingly. Further models (localisationistic or associationistic) by e.g., t issue loss (p. 353). He also noticed the cavity’s extension to Carl Wernicke (1848–1905), Ludwig Lichtheim (1845–1928) focused on the corpus striatum and its communication with the lateral l anguage processing as being locally specialised. In 1906, a denial of the role ventricle, i.e., to subcortical regions including the oval of the third-frontal convolution in language by Pierre Marie (1853–1940) c entre and the basal ganglia.
    [Show full text]
  • Carl Wernicke and the Neurobiological Paradigm in Psychiatry
    ARTICLE ACTAVol. 5, No. 4, 2007, 246-260 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICA CARL WERNICKE AND THE NEUROBIOLOGICAL PARADIGM IN PSYCHIATRY Frank Pillmann Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Martin Luther University Halle-Wittenberg, Halle, Germany Key words: history of psychiatry, biological psychiatry, lesion studies, aphasia, connectionism, hebephrenia, psychosis SUMMARY Carl Wernicke (1848-1905) was among the most outstanding and influ - ential neuro-psychiatrists of the 19th century. He made numerous contri - butions to both clinical neurology and psychiatry. His network view of brain function, in some ways premature at the time, foreshadowed today’s con - nectionist concepts. To understand the nature and importance of his findings, the scientific background during the second half of the 19th Century shall be briefly reviewed. INTRODUCTION Over the course of the 19th century, the large psychiatric hospitals ceased to be the driving force in psychiatry, in favor of the newly founded university hospitals. Repeatedly, psychiatry had been said to lack scientific foundation and to be dependent on other medical disciplines (e.g. Kahlbaum 1863). As a consequence, there was a strong tendency to fill university chairs of psy - chia try with scientists of neuroanatomical, neuropathological or neurophy - siological fame. Good examples of this include the brain pathologist Theodor Meynert (1870) in Vienna or Paul Flechsig, also with anatomical-pathological tendencies, who in 1887 became professor of psychiatry in Leipzig. By the middle of the 19th century, enormous empirical and conceptual pro - gress had taken place in the understanding of brain functioning. Cortical lo - calization, which had fallen into disfavor after Gall’s pseudo-science of phrenology had been discredited, found renewed interest when in Paris the surgeon and anthropologist Paul Broca localized “aphemia.” In 1861 he reported a disturbance of speech resulting from a lesion of the second and 246 Pillmann, Carl Wernicke and neurobiological psychiatry third frontal convolution of the left hemisphere.
    [Show full text]
  • 576 Book Reviews Material, Which Is Presented in a Humorous, Appealing Style That I Would Welcome from More Historians
    576 Book Reviews material, which is presented in a humorous, appealing style that I would welcome from more historians. A thoroughly enjoyable and rewarding read. Matthew Smith University of Strathclyde, UK doi:10.1017/mdh.2016.69 Katja Guenther, Localization and its Discontents: A Genealogy of Psychoanalysis and the Neuro Disciplines (Chicago, IL; London: University of Chicago Press, 2015), pp. 296, $35.00, hardback, ISBN: 978-0-226-28820-8. At just under 300 pages, this brilliant book punches far above its weight. Katja Guenther, associate professor of history of science at Princeton, has worked as a physician in hospitals in Germany, France and the UK. She also has a research degree in neuroscience from Oxford University and a PhD in history of science from Harvard. This combination of clinical experience, scientific expertise, native German language skills and historical training makes her an ideal guide to the intricacies of nineteenth- and twentieth-century neuropsychiatry. Guenther begins with a fissure in the contemporary sciences of the brain. On the one hand we have the avalanche of data issuing from fMRI studies, telling us which discrete physical areas of the brain are used in different cognitive tasks. On the other hand we have the ‘connectome’, a multi-site initiative to map the brain’s ‘network architecture’, revealing how cognition emerges from dynamic signaling processes. Guenther points out that these two impulses sit uneasily with one another: ‘When scientists trace the passage of a nervous signal across a brain circuit, it is very difficult to privilege any one part of the path and identify it as the “cerebral location” of a particular function’ (3).
    [Show full text]
  • Neurolinguistics and Language Function
    Journal of US-China Public Administration, January 2018, Vol. 15, No. 1, 34-42 doi: 10.17265/1548-6591/2018.01.004 D DAVID PUBLISHING Neurolinguistics and Language Function Rohaidah Kamaruddin University Putra Malaysia, Serdang, Selangor, Malaysia Zuraini Seruji University Malaysia Sarawak, Sarawak, Malaysia Atikah binti Mohd Noor, Nur Marjan Mat Yassin, Siti Izarina binti Yajib University Putra Malaysia, Serdang, Selangor, Malaysia Neurolinguistics is known as a study of relationship between brain and language. Human brain consists of three main parts such as forebrain (crucial part), midbrain (the smallest part), and hindbrain (the lowest part). Basically, human language centre is located in frontal lobe of human brain where the left and the right hemispheres are located. Language use sometimes is more dominant in the left hemisphere although the right hemisphere functions for language. Specifically, the areas that are involved in language are frontal lobe (Broca) and temporal lobe (Wernicke). The lesion on brain will scientifically cause language disorder which is known as aphasia. The types of aphasia are Broca’s aphasia, Wernicke’s aphasia, pure word deafness aphasia, conduction aphasia, anomic aphasia, apraxia aphasia, global aphasia, and aphasia reading and writing (dyslexia). The physical condition of a healthy brain is very important to prepare the activities of thinking and acting. Thus, the brain must be protected appropriately. Keywords: neurolinguistics, brain, language, aphasia Neurolinguistics is a field of study that comprises the linguistics and medical science that study about the relationship between language and human brain. How does the language process occur in human brain? Harry Whitaker (1971) is a well-known journal founder who explained neurolinguistics as technical term in the field for the use in academic field.
    [Show full text]
  • Medical Neuroscience | Tutorial Notes
    Associational Cortex of the Temporal Lobe Medical Neuroscience | Tutorial Notes Associational Cortex of the Temporal Lobe MAP TO NEUROSCIENCE CORE CONCEPTS1 NCC5. Intelligence arises as the brain reasons, plans, and solves problems. NCC7. The human brain endows us with a natural curiosity to understand how the world works. LEARNING OBJECTIVES After study of the assigned learning materials, the student will: 1. Discuss the major functions that are localized to the associational cortex of the temporal lobe. 2. Differentiate categories of human memory and discuss the relevant neuroanatomical systems. 3. Differentiate Broca’s and Wernicke’s aphasia. 4. Discuss dementia and relate its associated cognitive signs to associational cortex. TUTORIAL OUTLINE I. Temporal associational cortex A. visual recognition 1. harbors higher order visual pathways in the ventral (“what”) processing stream, in addition to associational cortex that integrates information from other sensory systems to form representations of objects (see Figure 12.15 & 12.162) 2. damage can lead to agnosias (“not knowing”) a. in general, agnosias refer to deficits in the recognition and/or naming of specific objects, despite an awareness of their presence b. e.g., prosopagnosia (“prosopo” Greek, = face or person): inability to recognize faces i. in the inferior temporal gyrus and the lateral occipitotemporal gyrus (fusiform gyrus), there are neurons that respond selectively to (primate) faces ii. some cells are selective for only certain views of faces 1 Visit BrainFacts.org for Neuroscience Core Concepts (©2012 Society for Neuroscience) that offer fundamental principles about the brain and nervous system, the most complex living structure known in the universe. 2 Figure references to Purves et al., Neuroscience, 6th Ed., Oxford University Press, 2018.
    [Show full text]
  • LANGUAGE and the BRAIN What We Have Learned from 30 Years of Brain Imaging
    ARTICLES LANGUAGE AND THE BRAIN What we have learned from 30 years of brain imaging By Pascale Tremblay,1 Université Laval & Anthony Steven Dick, Florida International University With the advancement of cognitive neuroscience and the development of modern brain imaging methods in the 1990s, the feld of language neurobiology—the study of the relationship between the brain and language functions—has grown immensely and rapidly over the past thirty years, and fnds itself at a crossroad with respect to its theoretical underpinning. The main message of our recent article “Broca and Wernicke are Dead, or Moving Past the Classic Model of Language Neurobiology” (Tremblay & Dick, 2016) is that the most historically important model in the feld, the Classic “Wernicke-Geschwind” Model, and associated terminology, is no longer useful to guide research and clinical intervention in the feld. A Brief History of the Classic Model The Classic Model was the frst major model of language neurobiology. It has dominated the feld of language neurobiology for over 150 years now, guiding both research and clinical interventions and infuencing many generations of scientists. The Model was frst introduced in Europe in 1861 and in 1874 as a response to an argument about how the brain works that was prevalent in the nineteenth century. At that time, several scientists were opposed to the notion that specifc areas of the brain had distinct functions, arguing instead that the brain-as-a-whole worked as one functional unit. This meant that a lesion to one part of the brain could be accommodated by the rest of the brain.
    [Show full text]
  • Carl Wernicke (1848–1905)
    J Neurol (2003) 250:1390–1391 DOI 10.1007/s00415-003-0250-x PIONEERS IN NEUROLOGY Frank Pillmann Carl Wernicke (1848–1905) Carl Wernicke was born into mod- [2]. The success of his book created est circumstances. He studied med- an opportunity for Wernicke to join icine in Breslau, now Wroclaw the department of psychiatry and (Poland). In 1871, Wernicke entered nervous diseases of the Berlin Char- the municipal “Allerheiligen” Hos- ité Hospital under Karl Westphal. In pital of Breslau where he served in 1878, Wernicke’s academic career the psychiatric department as assis- was severely disturbed as he entered tant to Heinrich Neumann, extraor- into conflict with the hospital ad- dinary professor of Psychiatry at ministration. He lost Westphal’s Breslau University. He was excited support and had to retreat to private by the fascinating findings of the neurological practice in the follow- up-and-coming neurosciences; for ing years. example, the linking of disordered During this time,Wernicke’s pro- language to the third anterior gyrus ductivity did not cease. He pub- by Paul Broca in 1865 and the dis- lished a variety of neurological pa- covery of the excitability of the cor- pers, including a description of the tex by Hitzig and Fritsch in 1870 [4]. hemianopic pupillary response and Wernicke managed to obtain a leave wrote the well-received “Textbook of absence to visit Theodor Meynert of brain diseases” [8]. It was this in Vienna,who had just begun to es- textbook that contained the de- Carl Wernicke (1848–1905) tablish his fame as the principal au- scription of “Pseudoencephalitic thority in neuropsychiatry.
    [Show full text]