Opposing Dominance Within a Color Gene Block Is Associated with Maladaptive Hybrid Signal Discordance

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Opposing Dominance Within a Color Gene Block Is Associated with Maladaptive Hybrid Signal Discordance bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.02.18.431741; this version posted February 18, 2021. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. de Zwaan et al., 17 Feb 2021 – preprint copy - BioRxiv Opposing dominance within a color gene block is associated with maladaptive hybrid signal discordance Devin R. de Zwaan1, Jacqueline Mackenzie2, Else Mikklesen2, and Silu Wang2,3* 1 Department of Forest and Conservation Sciences, 2424 Main Mall, University of British Columbia, BC, V6T1Z4, Canada 2 Department of Zoology, 6270 University Blvd, University of British Columbia, BC, Vancouver, BC, V6T1Z4, Canada 3 Current address: Department of Integrative Biology, University of California, Berkeley * Correspondence: [email protected] Abstract Color divergence is increasingly recognized as important for speciation in songbirds through its influence on social dynamics. However, the behavioral mechanisms underlying the eco-evolutionary feedback that acts across species boundaries is poorly understood. The hybrid zone between Setophaga occidentalis (SOCC) and S. townsendi (STOW) in the Cascade mountain ranges provides a natural observatory to test the interplay between genetics and social behaviour in maintaining species boundaries. Recently, we found that selection within a gene block underpinning color variation (ASIP-RALY) has maintained a stable and narrow hybrid zone. Here we investigated the social signaling roles of cheek darkness and flank streaking, two color traits linked to ASIP-RALY that reflect opposing dominance of SOCC and STOW alleles. We found that both traits act as honest badges of status, as they predicted male breeding quality. The opposing dominance effects of ASIP-RALY resulted in signal discordance for heterozygotes, which in turn was associated with inferior hybrid territorial performance, a fitness proxy quantified by vocal and physical responses of resident males to a decoy intruder. Taken together, this study highlights a potential behavioral mechanism underlying selection acting on a simple genetic architecture that has maintained a stable species boundary over decades despite significant gene flow. Keywords: speciation, ASIP, RALY, color genes, badge of status, hybrid fitness, honest signal, territoriality, Setophaga. Introduction secondary contact, divergent systems may lead to signal interference and Bird plumage is often characterized by distinct patches of compromised territorial performance among hybrid individuals with pigments (e.g., carotenoids, melanins) where the patch size or color discordant badges (Birch 1957; Schultz and Switzer 2001; Gröning and intensity can relay information about an individual’s genetic or Hochkirch 2008). Incompatible badges of status within hybrids may thus phenotypic quality, including ‘good genes’, nutritional state, incur reduced hybrid fitness. Badges of status and their genetic basis are immunocompetence, parental investment, or territorial dominance often investigated within species or populations, but rarely across species (McGraw and Ardia 2003; Hill 2006; Searcy and Nowicki 2010; boundaries. Here we assessed multiple badges of status within allopatric Svensson and Wong 2011). In the context of a social environment, and hybrid zone populations of two closely related wood-warbler species, ‘badges of status’ may signal social dominance as a mechanism to limit Setophaga townsendi and Setophaga occidentalis, to understand the costly combats among territorial individuals (Rohwer 1975; Studd and potential role of plumage traits and social dominance in speciation. Robertson 1985). Social dominance can be costly due to detrimental Divergent badges of status may be involved in speciation levels of testosterone along with frequent energy- and time-demanding between S. townsendi (hereafter abbreviated as ‘STOW’) and S. territorial challenges (Goymann and Wingfield 2004). Therefore, badges occidentalis (‘SOCC’) due to frequent heterospecific male competitions of status are thought to emerge and be maintained in populations (Pearson and Rohwer 2000) within the narrow hybrid zone along the characterized by individuals that vary in their ability to overcome these Cascade mountain range (Rohwer and Wood 1998; Wang et al. 2019, costs (i.e., body condition, aggression), as well as repeated territorial 2020b). Recent evidence revealed that the key genomic differentiation contests for mates and limited resources (1,2). To be considered an between allopatric parental populations resides within a color gene block, ‘honest’ signal, plumage badges should be variable among counterparts ASIP-RALY, which pleiotropically underpins variation in multiple color of the same sex, correlate with quality or combat ability of the carrier traits among hybrids sampled in the hybrid zone; particularly the extent (Rohwer 1975, 1982; Rohwer and Rohwer 1978), and be a component of of black pigmentation (melanin) and yellow (carotenoid) coloration in the aggressive interactions among counterparts, either by dissuading combat crown, cheek, breast, and flank, all of which are diagnostic for the two among individuals of different status or triggering combats between species (Wang et al. 2020b). Spatiotemporal analysis of ASIP-RALY individuals with similar badges (Rohwer 1975). Support for honest allele frequency revealed selection acting on this gene block (Wang et al. badges of status is often context dependent, but there is ample 2020b), although the mechanism of selection remained unclear. observational and experimental evidence to suggest that this mechanism One potential mechanism underlying selection against of territorial signaling is widespread (e.g., (Studd and Robertson 1985; individuals heterozygous for ASIP-RALY genotypes could be reduced Møller 1987; Safran et al. 2008; Hund et al. 2020). hybrid fitness through incompatible badges of status, or the breakdown Badges of status may be divergent among closely-related of signal reliability (Vortman et al. 2011, 2013). Species-specific species (Rohwer 1982) and thus a potentially important component of carotenoid and melanic color features likely serve as badges of status in speciation. When badges established in isolated populations come into this system, as they vary in intensity within species (Jackson et al. 1992; 1 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.02.18.431741; this version posted February 18, 2021. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. de Zwaan et al., 17 Feb 2021 – preprint copy - BioRxiv Wang et al. 2020b) and are involved in male-male competition (Pearson Badges of status 2000; Pearson et al. 2000). Two of these ASIP-RALY-associated traits, To test whether flank streaking and cheek coloration may act flank streaking and cheek darkening, demonstrated opposing dominance: as badges of status or signal individual quality, we conducted linear the STOW allele is dominant in flank streaking, whereas the SOCC allele regression of breeding condition PC1 against each of the plumage is dominant in cheek darkening (Wang et al. 2020b). Based on this variables accounting for population (parental STOW, SOCC, or hybrid pleiotropic opposing dominance effect of ASIP-RALY gene block, we zone): predicted discordant cheek and flank ancestry in heterozygous individuals. If cheek darkening and flank streaking act as honest signals lm (condition = plumage ´ population) [1] within species and are divergent between species, individuals with discordant badges of status may disproportionately suffer the costs of A significant slope would indicate that these plumage traits are associated territorial disputes, resulting in reduced social dominance and fitness. with individual condition within populations and therefore have the We investigated the mechanism underlying selection on the potential to act as badges of status. A significant interaction term would ASIP-RALY gene block that may maintain or reinforce the STOW- indicate divergence in signal honesty among populations. SOCC species boundary by addressing three questions: (1) Do cheek and flank plumage patches act as badges of status within Color genotype species and diverge between species? (2) Does the pleiotropic opposing dominance effect of ASIP-RALY lead Genotype data was acquired from our previous study (Wang et to discordance of badges of status in the hybrid zone? al. 2020b), in which the genotypes of color gene block ASIP-RALY was (3) Are hybrid individuals with discordant badges of status characterized denoted as OO, OT, TT for homozygous SOCC, heterozygous, and by reduced territorial performance? homozygous STOW, respectively. Dominance coefficients for each plumage trait were determined by the following equation: Materials and Methods ℎ = (�̅"" − �!"̅ )/(�̅"" − �!!̅ ) [2] Study site ̅ ̅ ̅ We carried out this study in the hybrid zone within the where �!!, �!", and �"" are mean phenotype individuals with OO, OT, Cascades mountain range of Washington as described in our previous and TT ASIP-RALY genotype. The STOW allele is dominant if h = 0, study (Wang et al. 2019). Detailed sampling information is summarized whereas the SOCC allele is dominant if h = 1. The bootstrap distribution
Recommended publications
  • Avian Vision and the Evolution of Egg Color Mimicry in The
    evo_65_7_cover 6/22/11 7:16 PM Page 1 Volume 65, Number 7, July 2011 VOLUME 65, NUMBER 7, JULY 2011 EVOLUTION VOLUME 65, NUMBER 7, PAGES PERSPECTIVE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF ORGANIC EVOLUTION Convergence, Adaptation, and Constraint Jonathan B. Losos 1827–1840 ORIGINAL ARTICLES 1827 Neutral Biodiversity Theory can Explain the Imbalance of Phylogenetic Trees but not the Tempo –2132 of their Diversification T. Jonathan Davies, Andrew P. Allen, Luís Borda-de-A´gua, Jim Regetz, and Carlos J. Melián 1841–1850 The Impact of Gene-Tree/Species-Tree Discordance on Diversification-Rate Estimation Frank T. Burbrink and R. Alexander Pyron 1851–1861 Dealing with Incomplete Taxon Sampling and Diversification of a Large Clade of Mushroom-Forming Fungi Martin Ryberg and Patrick Brandon Matheny 1862–1878 EVOLUTION Diversity and Demography in Beringia: Multilocus Tests of Paleodistribution Models Reveal the Complex History of Arctic Ground Squirrels Kurt E. Galbreath, Joseph A. Cook, Aren A. Eddingsaas, and Eric G. DeChaine 1879–1896 The Genetic Architecture of Adaptation Under Migration-Selection Balance Sam Yeaman and Michael C. Whitlock 1897–1911 The Influence of an Innovative Locomotor Strategy on the Phenotypic Diversification of Triggerfish (Family: Balistidae) Alex Dornburg, Brian Sidlauskas, Francesco Santini, Laurie Sorenson, Thomas J. Near, and Michael E. Alfaro 1912–1926 The Tempo and Mode of Evolution: Body Sizes of Island Mammals Pasquale Raia and Shai Meiri 1927–1934 Mammals Evolve Faster on Smaller Islands Virginie Millien 1935–1944 Evolutionary Advantage of Small Populations on Complex Fitness Landscapes Kavita Jain, Joachim Krug, and Su-Chan Park 1945–1955 Interspecific X-Chromosome and Mitochondrial DNA Introgression in the Iberian Hare: Selection or Allele Surfing? July 2011 José Melo-Ferreira, Paulo C.
    [Show full text]
  • Reinforcement and the Genetics of Hybrid Incompatibilities
    Genetics: Published Articles Ahead of Print, published on March 17, 2006 as 10.1534/genetics.105.048199 Reinforcement and the genetics of hybrid incompatibilities Alan R. Lemmon1 and Mark Kirkpatrick Affiliation: Section of Integrative Biology, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78712 1 Running head: Reinforcement and hybrid incompatibility Keywords: hybridization, speciation, reinforcement, incompatibility, sex linkage, incompatibility sieve 1Correspondence: Alan R. Lemmon Section of Integrative Biology The University of Texas at Austin 1 University Station #C0930 Austin, TX 78712 USA Email: [email protected] Tel: 512-471-3760; Fax: 512-471-3878 2 ABSTRACT Recent empirical studies suggest that genes involved in speciation are often sex-linked. We derive a general analytic model of reinforcement to study the effects of sex linkage on reinforcement under three forms of selection against hybrids: one-locus, two-locus, and ecological incompatibilities. We show that the pattern of sex linkage can have a large effect on the amount of reinforcement due to hybrid incompatibility. Sex linkage of genes involved in postzygotic isolation generally increases the strength of reinforcement, but only if genes involved in prezygotic isolation are also sex-linked. We use exact simulations to test the accuracy of the approximation and find that qualitative predictions made assuming weak selection can hold when selection is strong. 3 Speciation is the evolution of prezygotic or postzygotic isolation. Postzygotic isolation is thought to evolve through the accumulation of genetic incompatibilities during allopatric separation. Prezygotic isolation can evolve through reinforcement, which is the evolution of increased prezygotic isolation as a result of selection against hybrids (Dobzhansky 1940; Blair 1955; Howard 1993).
    [Show full text]
  • Sex Differences in the Recombination Landscape*
    vol. 195, no. 2 the american naturalist february 2020 Symposium Sex Differences in the Recombination Landscape* Jason M. Sardell† and Mark Kirkpatrick Department of Integrative Biology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas 78712 Submitted May 12, 2018; Accepted April 26, 2019; Electronically published December 9, 2019 Online enhancement: appendix. abstract: Sex differences in overall recombination rates are well Nearly all attention to this question has focused on over- known, but little theoretical or empirical attention has been given all map lengths. Achiasmy, in which recombination is lost to how and why sexes differ in their recombination landscapes: the in one sex, has evolved about 30 times, and the loss always patterns of recombination along chromosomes. In the first scientific occurs in the heterogametic sex (Haldane 1922; Huxley review of this phenomenon, we find that recombination is biased to- 1928; Burt et al. 1991). A much more common but less un- ward telomeres in males and more uniformly distributed in females in derstood situation is heterochiasmy, in which both sexes re- most vertebrates and many other eukaryotes. Notable exceptions to combine but at different rates. In these cases, sex differences this pattern exist, however. Fine-scale recombination patterns also fre- quently differ between males and females. The molecular mechanisms in map lengths are not correlated with which sex is hetero- responsible for sex differences remain unclear, but chromatin land- gametic (Burt et al. 1991; Lenormand 2003; Brandvain and scapes play a role. Why these sex differences evolve also is unclear. Hy- Coop 2012), although overall recombination tends to be potheses suggest that they may result from sexually antagonistic selec- higher in females across animals and outcrossing plants (re- tion acting on coding genes and their regulatory elements, meiotic viewed in Lenormand and Dutheil 2005; Brandvain and drive in females, selection during the haploid phase of the life cycle, Coop 2012).
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of Sexual Imprinting Through Reinforcement
    Assortative mating by an obliquely transmitted local cultural trait promotes divergence Justin Yeh University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill Do learned birdsong dialects lead to speciation? • Molecular similarity might correlate with dialect similarity (Baker 1975; Baker & Thompson 1982; Zink & Barrowclough 1984) • Genetic cline overlap with cultural cline, but at lower steepness, and the birdsong does not predict genotype (Kenyon et al. 2016) • Bird lineage with song learning has more species than that without (Lachlan & Servedio 2004) Justin Yeh 2 correlation ⇏ causation no correlation ⇏ no causation Justin Yeh 3 Oblique learning: from unrelated individual of the previous generation • Juvenile songbirds learn from adult neighbors • Obliquely transmitted traits are immune to sexual selection (Yeh & Servedio 2015) • If any juvenile can learn from any adult, there should not be correlation between gene and culture Justin Yeh 4 Spatial structure creates gene- culture association • Learning is spatially restricted • Cultural traits can be locally adapted – High-freq vocal signals in cities (Luther & Derryberry 2012) – Tonal languages in humid environment (Everett et al. 2014) Justin Yeh 5 Hypothesis Local genes & Local song & local local oblique reproduction learning Justin Yeh 6 Hypothesis Local genes & Local song & local local oblique reproduction learning Gene-culture association Justin Yeh 7 Hypothesis Local genes & Local song & local local oblique reproduction learning Divergent selection Gene-culture & sexual selection association
    [Show full text]
  • Rebecca Jo Safran
    Safran – CV, Page 1 September 2016 C U R R I C U L U M V I T A E Rebecca Jo Safran Associate Professor Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology University of Colorado Boulder, Colorado 80309 E-MAIL [email protected] TELEPHONE 303.734.1495 MOBILE: 303.249.8089 Lab website: [email protected] EDUCATION Ph.D. Cornell University Ecology and Evolutionary Biology January Minor Degree in Biometry 2005 (MA equivalent) M.S. Humboldt State University Ecology May 1997 B.S. University of Michigan School of Natural Resources and December the Environment 1991 RESEARCH INTERESTS Integrative evolutionary ecology: phenotype evolution, gene X environment influences on phenotype, evolution by sexual and natural selection, role of adaptation in the evolution of reproductive isolation, behavioral reproductive isolation, patterns of genomic divergence in speciation, genetic and hormonal control of behavior and morphology, relationships between scales of analysis: individuals to populations APPOINTMENTS Associate Professor, Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology. University of Colorado, Boulder, Colorado. August 2014 – present. Assistant Professor, Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology. University of Colorado, Boulder, Colorado. January 2008 – August 2014. Postdoctoral Fellow, Council on Science and Technology and Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey. September 2005 – December 2007 AWARDS & HONORS 2016 Elected Fellow, American Ornithological Society 2016 Invited Participant, Presidential Debate, Conference of the American Society of Naturalists, Asilomar, CA 2015 Elected co-chair (2017)/chair (2019): Gordon Conference on Speciation 2015 Faculty Sustainability Award, University of Colorado 2015 Invited Speaker: Gordon Conference on Speciation, Ventura California 2015 Storer Lecture: College of Biological Sciences, University of California, Davis Safran – CV, Page 2 September 2016 2014 Invited Speaker: National Evolutionary Synthesis Center, Evolutionary Synthesis, Past and Future.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolution of Male Life Histories and Age-Dependent Sexual Signals Under Female Choice Joel J
    Evolution of male life histories and age-dependent sexual signals under female choice Joel J. Adamson Ecology, Evolution and Organismic Biology, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, United States ABSTRACT Sexual selection theory models evolution of sexual signals and preferences using simple life histories. However, life-history models predict that males benefit from increasing sexual investment approaching old age, producing age-dependent sex- ual traits. Age-dependent traits require time and energy to grow, and will not fully mature before individuals enter mating competition. Early evolutionary stages pose several problems for these traits. Age-dependent traits suVer from strong viabil- ity selection and gain little benefit from mate choice when rare. Few males will grow large traits, and they will rarely encounter choosy females. The evolutionary origins of age-dependent traits therefore remain unclear. I used numerical simu- lations to analyze evolution of preferences, condition (viability) and traits in an age-structured population. Traits in the model depended on age and condition (“good genes”) in a population with no genetic drift. I asked (1) if age-dependent indicator traits and their preferences can originate depending on the strength of selection and the size of the trait; (2) which mode of development (age-dependent versus age-independent) eventually predominates when both modes occur in the population; and (3) if age-independent traits can invade a population with age- dependent traits. Age-dependent traits evolve under weaker selection and at smaller sizes than age-independent traits. This result held in isolation and when the types co-occur. Evolution of age-independent traits depends only on trait size, whereas Submitted 29 August 2013 Accepted 26 November 2013 evolution of age-dependent traits depends on both strength of selection and growth Published 17 December 2013 rate.
    [Show full text]
  • II Joint Congress on Evolutionary Biology Montpellier 2018, France August 18-22, 2018
    II Joint Congress on Evolutionary Biology Montpellier 2018, France August 18-22, 2018 Congress book TABLE OF CONTENTS Welcome ................................................................................................................... 04 In memoriam ............................................................................................................ 05 Information ............................................................................................................... 06 Conference information .................................................................................. 06 Congress area map ........................................................................................ 09 City map .......................................................................................................... 10 Corum maps .................................................................................................... 12 Plenary talks ..................................................................................................... 14 Other special events sponsored by societies ................................................ 15 Workshops ........................................................................................................ 20 Other meetings ................................................................................................ 23 Social events .................................................................................................... 24 Outreach .........................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Janette Boughman
    Seminar janette boughman Fellow, scas. Professor of Integrative Biology, Michigan State University, East Lansing How is Biodiversity Created and Maintained in Our Changing World? Tuesday, 15 May, 11:15 a.m. In the Thunberg Lecture Hall scas, Linneanum, Thunbergsvägen 2, Uppsala www.swedishcollegium.se about janette boughman Janette Boughman is currently affiliated with the Department of Integrative Biology; the Ecology, Evolutionary Biology, and Behavior program; and the Beacon Center for the Study of Evolution in Action at the Michigan State University. She earlier held the posi- tion of Visiting Scientist at the Swiss Federal Institute of Aquatic Science and Technology and was Assistant Professor of Zoology at the University of Wisconsin–Madison and a Post- doctoral Fellow and Assistant Lecturer at the University of British Columbia. She has been awarded a Fulbright Fellowship in Arctic Research in Iceland, is the Vice Presi- dent of the Society for the Study of Evolution, and has been an nsf International Re- search Fellow, a nato Postdoctoral Fellow, an nih Postdoctoral Fellow, and a Smith- sonian Fellow. She earned a Ph.D. in Biology at the University of Maryland and an ma in Linguistics from the University of New Hampshire. Boughman’s research uses an integrative approach to investigate the evolu- tion of behavior and the process of speciation. She has authored sixty scientific research papers in leading international scientific journals. She has been continuously funded by major grants from the us National Science Foundation for twenty-three years and currently leads an nsf Dimensions of Biodiversity collaborative research program on rapid evolution in response to changing environments.
    [Show full text]
  • The Interaction Between Learning and Speciation
    THE INTERACTION BETWEEN LEARNING AND SPECIATION D. Justin Yeh A dissertation submitted to the faculty at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of PhD in the Biology Department in the School of Arts and Sciences. Chapel Hill 2018 Approved by: Maria R. Servedio Marcus W. Feldman Joel G. Kingsolver David W. Pfennig Karin S. Pfennig © 2018 D. Justin Yeh ALL RIGHTS RESERVED ii ABSTRACT D. Justin Yeh: The Interaction between Learning and Speciation (Under the direction of Maria R. Servedio) Assortative mating displays, preferences, or both can be affected by learning across a wide range of animal taxa, but the specifics of how this learning affects speciation with gene flow are not well understood. I use population genetic models with trait learning to investigate how the identity of the tutor affects the divergence of a self-referent phenotype-matching trait. I find that oblique learning (learning from unrelated individual of the previous generation) and maternal learning mask sexual selection and therefore do not allow the maintenance of divergence. In contrast, by enhancing positive frequency-dependent sexual selection, paternal learning can maintain more divergence than genetic inheritance, but leads to the loss of polymorphism more easily. Furthermore, paternal learning inhibits the invasion of a novel self- referent phenotype-matching trait, especially in a large population. Reinforcement is the process through which assortative mating evolves by natural selection to reduce costly hybridization. Sexual imprinting could facilitate reinforcement by decreasing hybridization, or it could impede the process if heterotypic pairs imprint on each other.
    [Show full text]
  • David W. Pfennig
    David W. Pfennig April 8, 2021 Dept. of Biology, CB#3280, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC 27599 email: [email protected] web: https://www.davidpfenniglab.com Synopsis of Research I’m broadly interested in evolution, ecology, behavior, and development. My current research seeks to understand how individual organisms assess and respond to changes in their environment and how such developmental ‘plasticity’ impacts ecology, evolution, and health. Education 2018 Leadership Development Program, Center for Creative Leadership, Greensboro, NC 1999-2002 Summer Institute in Statistical Genetics, North Carolina State University 1991-1993 NSF Postdoctoral Fellow, Cornell University (with P. W. Sherman) 1990-1991 Center for Insect Science Postdoctoral Fellow, University of Arizona 1989-1990 Maytag Postdoctoral Fellow, Arizona State University (with J. P. Collins) 1989 University of Texas at Austin, Ph.D. in Zoology (with J. J. Bull & M. Kirkpatrick) 1982 University of Texas at San Antonio, M.S. in Biology (with G. J. Gamboa) 1979 University of Texas at Austin, B.S. in Zoology 1979 University of Texas at Austin, B.S. in Geology Professional Experience 2012-2015 Caroline & Thomas Royster Distinguished Professor for Graduate Education, Dept. of Biology, University of North Carolina (UNC) 2012-2015 Director, Royster Society of Fellows, The Graduate School, UNC 2007-2012 Zachary Taylor Smith Distinguished Professor for Excellence in Undergraduate Education, Dept. of Biology, UNC 2007 Visiting Scholar, National Evolutionary Synthesis Center, Duke University 2004-present Professor, Dept. of Biology, UNC 2003-2012 Co-director of Graduate Studies, Dept. of Biology, UNC 1999-2004 Associate Professor, Dept. of Biology, UNC 1996-1999 Assistant Professor, Dept.
    [Show full text]
  • Animal Behaviour 104: 175-185
    Animal Behaviour 104 (2015) 175e185 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Animal Behaviour journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/anbehav Exaggerated sexual swellings and male mate choice in primates: testing the reliable indicator hypothesis in the Amboseli baboons * Courtney L. Fitzpatrick a, , Jeanne Altmann b, c, Susan C. Alberts a, c a Department of Biology, Duke University, Durham, NC, U.S.A. b Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, U.S.A. c National Museums of Kenya, Institute of Primate Research, Nairobi, Kenya article info The paradigm of competitive males vying to influence female mate choice has been repeatedly upheld, Article history: but, increasingly, studies also report competitive females and choosy males. One female trait that is Received 18 July 2014 commonly proposed to influence male mate choice is the exaggerated sexual swelling displayed by fe- Initial acceptance 3 November 2014 males of many Old World primate species. The reliable indicator hypothesis posits that females use the Final acceptance 23 February 2015 exaggerated swellings to compete for access to mates, and that the swellings advertise variation in fe- Published online male fitness. We tested the two main predictions of this hypothesis in a wild population of baboons MS. number: A14-00599R2 (Papio cynocephalus). First, we examined the effect of swelling size on the probability of mate guarding (‘consortship’) by the highest-ranking male and the behaviour of those males that trailed consortships Keywords: (‘follower males’). Second, we asked whether a female's swelling size predicted several fitness measures. baboon We found that high-ranking males did not prefer females with larger swellings (when controlling for male mate choice cycle number and conception) and that females with larger swellings did not have higher reproductive Papio cynocephalus reliable indicator hypothesis success.
    [Show full text]
  • EVOLUTION of DIVERSITY in WARNING COLOR and MIMICRY: Polymorphisms, Shifting Balance, and Speciation
    P1: FNE/fgp P2: FhN/fgo QC: FhN/uks T1: FhN September 17, 1999 17:19 Annual Reviews AR093-08 Annu.Rev.Ecol.Syst.1999 .30:201–33 Copyright c 1999 by Annual Reviews. All rights reserved EVOLUTION OF DIVERSITY IN WARNING COLOR AND MIMICRY: Polymorphisms, Shifting Balance, and Speciation James Mallet1 and Mathieu Joron2 1GaltonLaboratory,4StephensonWay,LondonNW12HE,England; e-mail:http://abacus.gene.ucl.ac.uk/jim/;and2Ge´ netiqu´ eet Environnement, CC065 ISEM, Universite´ de Montpellier 2, Place Bataillon, F-34095 Montpellier,cedex5,France Key Words aposematism, Batesian mimicry, M¨ullerianmimicry, defensive coloration, predator behavior ■ Abstract Mimicry and warning color are highly paradoxical adaptations. Color patterns in both M¨ullerianand Batesian mimicry are often determined by relatively few pattern-regulating loci with major effects. Many of these loci are “supergenes,” consisting of multiple, tightly linked epistatic elements. On the one hand, strong pu- rifying selection on these genes must explain accurate resemblance (a reduction of morphological diversity between species), as well as monomorphic color patterns within species. On the other hand, mimicry has diversified at every taxonomic level; warning color has evolved from cryptic patterns, and there are mimetic polymor- phisms within species, multiple color patterns in different geographic races of the same species, mimetic differences between sister species, and multiple mimicry rings within local communities. These contrasting patterns can be explained, in part, by the shape of a “number-dependent” selection function first modeled by Fritz M¨uller in 1879: Purifying selection against any warning-colored morph is very strong when that morph is rare, but becomes weak in a broad basin of intermediate frequencies, allowing opportunities for polymorphisms and genetic drift.
    [Show full text]