How Languages Construct Time Lera Boroditsky Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, USA

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

How Languages Construct Time Lera Boroditsky Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, USA CHAPTER 20 How Languages Construct Time Lera Boroditsky Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, USA Summary (OW DO PEOPLE CONSTRUCT THEIR MENTAL REPRESENTATIONS OF TIME ) FOCUS ON WORK EXAMIN ING THE ROLE THAT SPATIAL METAPHORS AND BASIC SPATIAL REPRESENTATIONS PLAY IN CONSTRUCTING REP RESENTATIONS OF TIME ACROSS LANGUAGES 4HE RESULTS REVEAL THAT THE METAPHORS WE USE TO TALK ABOUT TIME HAVE BOTH IMMEDIATE AND LONG TERM CONSEQUENCES FOR HOW WE CONCEPTUALIZE AND REASON ABOUT THIS FUNDAMENTAL DOMAIN OF EXPERIENCE (OW PEOPLE CONCEPTUALIZE TIME APPEARS TO DEPEND ON HOW THE LANGUAGES THEY SPEAK TEND TO TALK ABOUT TIME THE CURRENT LINGUISTIC CONTEXT WHAT LANGUAGE IS BEING SPOKEN AND ALSO ON THE PARTICULAR METAPHORS BEING USED TO TALK ABOUT TIME IN THE MOMENT &URTHER PEOPLE WHO CONCEPTUALIZE SPACE DIFFERENTLY ALSO CON CEPTUALIZE TIME DIFFERENTLY SUGGESTING THAT PEOPLE CO OPT REPRESENTATIONS OF THE PHYSICAL WORLD EG SPACE IN ORDER TO MENTALLY REPRESENT MORE ABSTRACT OR INTANGIBLE ENTITIES EG TIME 4AKEN ALL TOGETHER THESE lNDINGS SHOW THAT CONCEPTIONS OF EVEN SUCH FUNDAMENTAL DOMAINS AS TIME DIFFER DRAMATICALLY ACROSS CULTURES AND GROUPS 4HE RESULTS REVEAL SOME OF THE MECHA NISMS THROUGH WHICH LANGUAGES AND CULTURES HELP CONSTRUCT OUR BASIC NOTIONS OF TIME /NE OF THE GREAT MYSTERIES OF THE MIND IS HOW WE ARE ABLE TO THINK ABOUT THINGS WE CAN NEVER SEE OR TOUCH (OW DO WE COME TO REPRESENT AND REASON ABOUT ABSTRACT DOMAINS LIKE TIME JUSTICE OR IDEAS !LL OF OUR EXPERIENCE OF THE WORLD IS PHYSICAL ACCOMPLISHED THROUGH SENSORY PERCEPTION AND MOTOR ACTION !ND YET OUR INTERNAL MENTAL LIVES GO FAR BEYOND THOSE THINGS OBSERVABLE THROUGH PHYSICAL EXPERIENCE WE INVENT SOPHISTICATED NOTIONS OF NUMBER AND TIME WE THEORIZE ABOUT ATOMS AND INVISIBLE FORCES AND WE WORRY ABOUT LOVE JUSTICE IDEAS GOALS AND PRINCIPLES 4HE ABILITY TO COGNITIVELY TRANSCEND THE PHYSICAL IS ONE OF THE VERY HALLMARKS OF HUMAN INTELLIGENCE 3O HOW IS IT POSSIBLE THAT PHYSICAL ORGANISMS WHO COLLECT PHOTONS THROUGH THEIR EYES RESPOND TO PHYSICAL PRESSURE IN THEIR EARS AND BEND THEIR KNEES AND mEX THEIR TOES IN JUST THE RIGHT AMOUNT TO DEFY GRAVITY ARE ABLE TO INVENT AND Space, Time and Number in the Brain. DOI: 10.1016/B978-0-12-385948-8.00020-7 333 © 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 334 20. HOW LANGUAGES CONSTRUCT TIME REASON ABOUT THE UNPERCEIVABLE AND ABSTRACT 4HE MYSTERY OF ABSTRACT THOUGHT HAS VEXED SCHOLARS FROM 0LATO TO $ARWIN /NE PROPOSED SOLUTION TO THIS MYSTERY IS THAT REPRESENTATIONS OF THE ABSTRACT MIGHT BE CONSTRUCTED THROUGH ANALOGICAL EXTENSIONS FROM MORE EXPERIENCE BASED DOMAINS EG ; = 4HAT IS IN ORDER TO CONSTRUCT MENTAL REPRESENTATIONS OF ABSTRACT OR INTANGIBLE ENTITIES WE CO OPT THE REPRESENTATIONS WE HAVE DEVELOPED FOR MORE TANGIBLE AND CONCRETE DOMAINS )N THIS PAPER ) WILL FOCUS ON THE DOMAIN OF TIME AND THE ROLE THAT REPRESENTATIONS OF SPACE PLAY IN CONSTRUCTING REPRESENTATIONS OF TIME 4IME IS A TOPIC OF CENTRAL INTEREST IN OUR CULTURE 4HE WORD hTIMEv IS THE MOST FREQUENT NOUN IN THE %NGLISH LANGUAGE WITH OTHER TEMPORAL WORDS LIKE hDAYv AND hYEARv ALSO RANK ING IN THE TOP ; = 4IME IS UBIQUITOUS YET EPHEMERAL )T FORMS THE VERY FABRIC OF OUR EXPERIENCE AND YET IT IS UNPERCEIVABLE WE CANNOT SEE TOUCH OR SMELL TIME 3O HOW DO WE MENTALLY REPRESENT AND ORGANIZE THIS FUNDAMENTAL DOMAIN OF EXPERIENCE 4O REPRESENT TIME PEOPLE AROUND THE WORLD RELY ON SPACE 7E SPATIALIZE TIME IN CULTURAL ARTIFACTS LIKE GRAPHS TIME LINES ORTHOGRAPHY CLOCKS SUNDIALS HOURGLASSES AND CALENDARS WE GESTURE TEMPORAL RELATIONS AND RELY HEAVILY ON SPATIAL WORDS EG FORWARD BACK LONG SHORT TO TALK ABOUT THE ORDER AND DURATION OF EVENTS EG ;n= 0EOPLES PRIVATE MENTAL REP RESENTATIONS OF TIME ALSO APPEAR TO BE BASED IN SPACE IRRELEVANT SPATIAL INFORMATION READILY AFFECTS PEOPLES JUDGMENTS OF TEMPORAL ORDER AND DURATION ;n= AND PEOPLE SEEM TO IMPLIC ITLY AND AUTOMATICALLY GENERATE SPATIAL REPRESENTATIONS WHEN THINKING ABOUT TIME ;n= (OWEVER THE PARTICULAR WAYS THAT TIME IS SPATIALIZED DIFFER ACROSS LANGUAGES AND CUL TURES 2ESEARCH DONE AROUND THE WORLD HAS UNCOVERED DRAMATIC VARIABILITY IN REPRESENTA TIONS OF TIME ACROSS CULTURES AND GROUPS 3EVERAL ASPECTS OF LINGUISTIC CULTURAL AND PERSONAL EXPERIENCE APPEAR TO SHAPE PEOPLES TEMPORAL REASONING 1. 4HE PATTERN OF SPATIAL METAPHORS THAT PEOPLE USE TO TALK ABOUT TIME ;n= 2. 4HE SET OF SPATIAL REPRESENTATIONS AND REFERENCE FRAMES THAT ARE AVAILABLE FOR CO OPTING FOR THINKING ABOUT TIME EITHER IN THE LINGUISTIC OR CULTURAL ENVIRONMENT MORE GENERALLY OR IN THE IMMEDIATE CONTEXT MORE SPECIlCALLY ;n= 3. /RGANIZATIONAL PATTERNS IN CULTURAL ARTIFACTS EG WRITING DIRECTION ; n= 4. !SPECTS OF CULTURAL OR INDIVIDUAL DISPOSITION AGE AND EXPERIENCE ;n= )N THE FOLLOWING SECTIONS ) FOCUS ON WORK REVEALING THE ROLE THAT SPATIAL METAPHORS AND BASIC SPATIAL REPRESENTATIONS PLAY IN CONSTRUCTING REPRESENTATIONS OF TIME ACROSS LANGUAGES ,ANGUAGES AROUND THE WORLD RELY ON SPATIAL TERMS TO TALK ABOUT TIME )N SOME CASES IT IS DIFlCULT OR IMPOSSIBLE TO TALK ABOUT TIME WITHOUT INVOKING SPATIAL LANGUAGE (OWEVER LANGUAGES DIFFER IN THE SPATIAL TERMS THAT ARE MOST COMMONLY USED TO TALK ABOUT TIME &OR EXAMPLE DEPENDING ON THE LANGUAGE WE ARE SPEAKING WE MIGHT TALK ABOUT THE FUTURE AS IF IT LIES AHEAD OF US IN %NGLISH BEHIND US IN !YMARA OR BELOW US IN -ANDARIN #HINESE $O SUCH DIFFERENCES IN METAPHORS MATTER FOR HOW PEOPLE MENTALLY ORGANIZE THE DOMAIN OF TIME THE AXES OF TIME /NE PROMINENT EXAMPLE OF HOW SPATIOTEMPORAL METAPHORS SHAPE TEMPORAL THINKING COMES FROM COMPARISONS OF %NGLISH AND -ANDARIN -ANDARIN SPEAKERS ARE MORE LIKELY TO THE AXES OF TIME 335 TALK ABOUT TIME USING VERTICAL METAPHORS THAN ARE %NGLISH SPEAKERS ;n= !RE -ANDARIN SPEAKERS ALSO MORE LIKELY TO THINK ABOUT TIME VERTICALLY THAN ARE %NGLISH SPEAKERS 4O TEST THIS "ORODITSKY ;= COMPARED %NGLISH AND -ANDARIN SPEAKERS REPRESENTATIONS OF TIME )N THE STUDIES %NGLISH AND -ANDARIN SPEAKERS MADE TEMPORAL JUDGMENTS FOLLOW ING HORIZONTAL OR VERTICAL SPATIAL PRIMES 0ARTICIPANTS RESPONSE TIMES TO THE TARGET QUESTIONS ABOUT TIME FOLLOWING EITHER THE HORIZONTAL OR VERTICAL PRIMES WERE THE MEASURE OF INTER EST 4HE RESULTS REVEALED A BEHAVIORAL PATTERN CONSISTENT WITH THE LINGUISTIC OBSERVATION -ANDARIN SPEAKERS APPEARED MORE LIKELY TO THINK ABOUT TIME VERTICALLY THAN DID %NGLISH SPEAKERS "EYOND COMPARING %NGLISH AND -ANDARIN SPEAKERS THE STUDIES ALSO COMPARED THE RESULTS OF -ANDARIN SPEAKERS WHO HAD LEARNED %NGLISH AT DIFFERENT STAGES OF LIFE AND FUR THER COMPARED THE RESULTS OF %NGLISH SPEAKERS TESTED WITH AND WITHOUT TRAINING TO TALK ABOUT TIME VERTICALLY )N EACH CASE MORE EXPERIENCE WITH TALKING ABOUT TIME VERTICALLY LEAD TO MORE VERTICAL REPRESENTATIONS OF TIME 2ESULTS USING THIS PARADIGM HAVE BEEN CHALLENGED SEE ;n= BUT MORE RECENT WORK USING A VARIETY OF METHODS HAS CONlRMED THESE CROSS LINGUISTIC DIFFERENCES ; = &OR EXAMPLE WHEN NATIVE %NGLISH AND NATIVE -ANDARIN SPEAKERS WERE ASKED TO SPATIALLY ARRANGE TEMPORAL SEQUENCES SHOWN IN PICTURES -ANDARIN SPEAKERS ARRANGED THE PIC TURES IN VERTICAL ARRAYS OF THE TIME n DEPENDING ON GROUP WHEREAS %NGLISH SPEAKERS NEVER DID SO ;= )N A $ VARIANT OF THIS TASK "ORODITSKY ;= ASKED %NGLISH AND -ANDARIN SPEAKERS TO ARRANGE TIME BY POINTING IN $ SPACE AROUND THEM -ANDARIN SPEAKERS TESTED IN -ANDARIN ARRANGED TIME ON THE VERTICAL AXIS OF THE TIME WHEREAS %NGLISH SPEAKERS DID SO ONLY OF THE TIME &URTHER THE STUDIES FOUND THAT THE MORE PROlCIENT THE PARTICIPANTS WERE IN -ANDARIN THE MORE LIKELY THEY WERE TO ARRANGE TIME VERTICALLY -ANDARINn%NGLISH -% BILINGUALS WHO WERE TESTED IN -ANDARIN WERE MORE LIKELY TO ARRANGE TIME VERTICALLY WHEN THEY WERE TESTED IN -ANDARIN AS OPPOSED TO IN %NGLISH ,AI AND "ORODITSKY ;= EXAMINED WHETHER METAPHOR USE PLAYS A CAUSAL IN THE MOMENT ROLE IN HOW PEOPLE CONSTRUCT REPRESENTATIONS OF TIME 4HE RESULTS REVEALED THAT -ANDARIN SPEAKERS ARE MORE LIKELY TO CONSTRUCT FRONTnBACK REPRESENTATIONS OF TIME WHEN UNDERSTAND ING FRONTnBACK METAPHORS AND MORE LIKELY TO CONSTRUCT VERTICAL REPRESENTATIONS OF TIME WHEN UNDERSTANDING VERTICAL METAPHORS )T APPEARS THAT -ANDARIN SPEAKERS mEXIBLY REOR GANIZE TIME ALONG THE FRONTnBACK OR UPnDOWN AXIS DEPENDING ON WHETHER THEY ARE PROCESS ING FRONTnBACK OR UPnDOWN METAPHORS FOR TIME &INALLY SEVERAL STUDIES HAVE USED A NON LINGUISTIC IMPLICIT SPACEnTIME ASSOCIATION TASK TO MEASURE HOW %NGLISH AND -ANDARIN SPEAKERS SPATIALIZE TIME ; = 4HE STUDIES CONSISTENTLY lND DIFFERENCES BETWEEN THE TWO LANGUAGE GROUPS ALONG THE VERTICAL AXIS -ANDARIN SPEAKERS SHOW AN IMPLICIT VERTICAL PATTERN OF SPACEnTIME ASSOCIATION CONSIST ENT WITH VERTICAL SPACEnTIME METAPHORS IN -ANDARIN WITH EARLIER EVENTS ABOVE AND LATER EVENTS BELOW %NGLISH SPEAKERS DO NOT SHOW EVIDENCE OF THIS VERTICAL SPACEnTIME ASSOCIA TION 4HE lNDINGS ARE CONSISTENT WITH OTHER WORK SHOWING THAT EXPERIENCE WITH SPEAKING -ANDARIN ;= AND PROCESSING VERTICAL TIME METAPHORS IN PARTICULAR ; = HELPS CRE ATE AND MAINTAIN -ANDARIN SPEAKERS REPRESENTATIONS OF TIME ON THE VERTICAL AXIS 4HESE RESULTS REVEAL THAT PEOPLE AUTOMATICALLY INSTANTIATE SPATIAL REPRESENTATIONS OF TIME THAT ARE CONSISTENT WITH THE SET OF SPATIOTEMPORAL METAPHORS IN THEIR LINGUISTIC ENVIRONMENT EVEN IN NONLINGUISTIC TASKS 336 20. HOW LANGUAGES CONSTRUCT TIME MOTION IN TIME ,INGUISTIC ANALYSES HAVE SUGGESTED THAT -ANDARIN RELIES MORE HEAVILY ON TIME MOVING AS OPPOSED TO EGO MOVING METAPHORS THAN DOES %NGLISH 4HAT IS IN -ANDARIN METAPHORS THAT SUPPOSE AN OBSERVER MOVING ALONG A STATIONARY TIMELINE ARE LESS LIKELY THAN THOSE THAT SUPPOSE A STATIONARY OBSERVER AND A MOVING TIME LINE $O
Recommended publications
  • Language in Mind : Advances in the Study of Language and Thought
    Language in Mind This page intentionally left blank Language in Mind Advances in the Study of Language and Thought edited by Dedre Gentner and Susan Goldin-Meadow A Bradford Book The MIT Press Cambridge, Massachusetts London, England ( 2003 Massachusetts Institute of Technology All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced in any form by any electronic or mechanical means (including photocopying, recording, or informa- tion storage and retrieval) without permission in writing from the publisher. This book was set in Sabon on 3B2 by Asco Typesetters, Hong Kong. Printed and bound in the United States of America. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Language in mind ; advances in the study of language and thought / edited by Dedre Gentner and S. Goldin-Meadow. p. cm. ‘‘A Bradford book.’’ Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 0-262-07243-2 (hc. : alk. paper)—ISBN 0-262-57163-3 (pbk. : alk. paper) 1. Psycholinguistics. 2. Cognition. I. Gentner, Dedre. II. Goldin, Susan. P37 .L357 2003 4010.9—dc21 2002029578 Contents Contributors vii Acknowledgments ix I Introduction 1 Whither Whorf 3 Dedre Gentner and Susan Goldin-Meadow II Position Statements 15 1 Languages and Representations 17 Eve V. Clark 2 Language and Mind: Let’s Get the Issues Straight! 25 Stephen C. Levinson 3 The Key Is Social Cognition 47 Michael Tomasello III Language as Lens: Does the Language We Acquire Influence How We See the World? 59 4 Sex, Syntax, and Semantics 61 Lera Boroditsky, Lauren A. Schmidt, and Webb Phillips 5 Speaking versus Thinking about Objects and Actions 81 Barbara C.
    [Show full text]
  • Metaphoric Structuring: Understanding Time Through Spatial Metaphors
    COGNITION Cognition 75 (2000) 1±28 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Metaphoric structuring: understanding time through spatial metaphors Lera Boroditsky* Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Jordan Hall, Bldg. 420, Stanford, CA 94305-2130, USA Received 17 December 1998; received in revised form 22 September 1999; accepted 23 October 1999 Abstract The present paper evaluates the claim that abstract conceptual domains are structured through metaphorical mappings from domains grounded directly in experience. In particular, the paper asks whether the abstract domain of time gets its relational structure from the more concrete domain of space. Relational similarities between space and time are outlined along with several explanations of how these similarities may have arisen. Three experiments designed to distinguish between these explanations are described. The results indicate that (1) the domains of space and time do share conceptual structure, (2) spatial relational infor- mation is just as useful for thinking about time as temporal information, and (3) with frequent use, mappings between space and time come to be stored in the domain of time and so thinking about time does not necessarily require access to spatial schemas. These ®ndings provide some of the ®rst empirical evidence for Metaphoric Structuring. It appears that abstract domains such as time are indeed shaped by metaphorical mappings from more concrete and experiential domains such as space. q 2000 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Metaphoric structuring; Understanding time; Spatial metaphors 1. Introduction How do we come to represent and reason about abstract domains like time, love, justice, or ideas? There are at least two interesting puzzles here.
    [Show full text]
  • Languages and Worldview
    LANGUAGES AND WORLDVIEW Manon Allard-Kropp University of Missouri–St. Louis University of Missouri–St. Louis Languages and Worldview Manon Allard-Kropp This text is disseminated via the Open Education Resource (OER) LibreTexts Project (https://LibreTexts.org) and like the hundreds of other texts available within this powerful platform, it freely available for reading, printing and "consuming." Most, but not all, pages in the library have licenses that may allow individuals to make changes, save, and print this book. Carefully consult the applicable license(s) before pursuing such effects. Instructors can adopt existing LibreTexts texts or Remix them to quickly build course-specific resources to meet the needs of their students. Unlike traditional textbooks, LibreTexts’ web based origins allow powerful integration of advanced features and new technologies to support learning. The LibreTexts mission is to unite students, faculty and scholars in a cooperative effort to develop an easy-to-use online platform for the construction, customization, and dissemination of OER content to reduce the burdens of unreasonable textbook costs to our students and society. The LibreTexts project is a multi-institutional collaborative venture to develop the next generation of open-access texts to improve postsecondary education at all levels of higher learning by developing an Open Access Resource environment. The project currently consists of 13 independently operating and interconnected libraries that are constantly being optimized by students, faculty, and outside experts to supplant conventional paper-based books. These free textbook alternatives are organized within a central environment that is both vertically (from advance to basic level) and horizontally (across different fields) integrated.
    [Show full text]
  • How Does Our Language Shape the Way We Think?
    To arrive at the edge of the world's knowledge, seek out the most complex and sophisticated minds, put them in a room together, and have them ask each other the questions they are asking themselves. HOW DOES OUR LANGUAGE SHAPE THE WAY WE THINK? Lera Boroditsky [6.11.09] For a long time, the idea that language might shape thought was considered at best untestable and more often simply wrong. Research in my labs at Stanford University and at MIT has helped reopen this question. We have collected data around the world: from China, Greece, Chile, Indonesia, Russia, and Aboriginal Australia. What we have learned is that people who speak different languages do indeed think differently and that even flukes of grammar can profoundly affect how we see the world. Language is a uniquely human gift, central to our experience of being human. Appreciating its role in constructing our mental lives brings us one step closer to understanding the very nature of humanity. LERA BORODITSKY is an assistant professor of psychology, neuroscience, and symbolic systems at Stanford University, who looks at how the languages we speak shape the way we think. Humans communicate with one another using a dazzling array of languages, each differing from the next in innumerable ways. Do the languages we speak shape the way we see the world, the way we think, and the way we live our lives? Do people who speak different languages think differently simply because they speak different languages? Does learning new languages change the way you think? Do polyglots think differently when speaking different languages? These questions touch on nearly all of the major controversies in the study of mind.
    [Show full text]
  • Do English and Mandarin Speakers Think Differently About Time?
    Do English and Mandarin Speakers Think Differently About Time? Lera Boroditsky ([email protected]) Department of Psychology, 450 Serra Mall, Bldg. 420 Stanford, CA 94305 USA Abstract bounded events, unidirectional change, etc.) appear to be universal across cultures and languages. Do the languages we speak shape the ways we think? Boroditsky, (2001) demonstrated that speakers of English and However, there are many aspects of our concept of time Mandarin think differently about time. This work has recently that are not observable in the world. For example, does time been brought into question (January & Kako, 2007; Chen, move horizontally or vertically? Does it move forward or 2007). Here I present new evidence that again demonstrates a back, left or right, up or down? Does it move past us, or do difference between English and Mandarin speakers’ we move through it? All of these aspects are left unspecified construals of time. Both languages use horizontal and vertical in our experience with the world. They are however, spatial language to talk about time. For example, in English we might say that the best is ahead of us, or we may move a specified in our language —most often through spatial meeting up. In English, vertical metaphors are relatively metaphors. infrequent and horizontal metaphors predominate. In Across languages, people use spatial metaphors to talk Mandarin, both horizontal and vertical metaphors are about time. Whether they are looking forward to a brighter frequent. Importantly, vertical metaphors are much more tomorrow, proposing theories ahead of their time, or falling frequent in Mandarin than they are in English.
    [Show full text]
  • Language, Categories, Cognition
    LANGUAGE, CATEGORIES, AND COGNITION HONORS SEMINAR 01:090:293:01 Instructor PROF. KRISTEN SYRETT (k-syrett (at) ruccs.rutgers.edu) Day/Time 9:50-11:10 pm Tuesdays, Fridays Location Brett Hall Seminar Room (CAC) (RUID card required for access) Office Hours Tuesdays 11:30-12:15 pm (sign up through Sakai) and by appointment CORE CURRICULUM GOALS This course satisfies the SAS Core Goal: WCd (Discipline-Based Writing and Communication) Upon completion of this course, you will be able to (t) communicate effectively in modes appropriate to a discipline or area of inquiry [WCd] (u) evaluate and critically assess sources and use the conventions of attribution and citation correctly (v) analyze and synthesize information and ideas from multiple sources to generate new insights COURSE LEARNING GOALS The goals of the course are to • Engage you in a central debate at the heart of research in linguistics, cognitive psychology, and infant and child development • Equip you with arguing points on both side of the debate based on empirical research in these fields • Hone your skills in critiquing research, taking apart and delivering an argument, presenting research, and writing a review of research COURSE OVERVIEW How does the world we live in shape the language we acquire? What role does language play in shaping the way we think about the world? Does it lead us to inescapably conceptualize the world in a particular way, or does it serve as a guide, interacting with universal tendencies? How do linguistic, cognitive, and social processes constrain and affect each other throughout development and across language and cultures? In the early part of the 20th century, Edward Sapir argued that humans are “at the mercy” of their “language habits.” His student Benjamin Lee Whorf picked up on this idea and claimed that the concepts we acquire depend on our linguistic knowledge and practices.
    [Show full text]
  • How Language Shapes Thought Author(S): Lera Boroditsky Source: Scientific American, Vol
    How Language Shapes Thought Author(s): Lera Boroditsky Source: Scientific American, Vol. 304, No. 2 (February 2011), pp. 62-65 Published by: Scientific American, a division of Nature America, Inc. Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/26002395 Accessed: 02-10-2018 21:47 UTC REFERENCES Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article: https://www.jstor.org/stable/26002395?seq=1&cid=pdf-reference#references_tab_contents You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references. JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at https://about.jstor.org/terms Scientific American, a division of Nature America, Inc. is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Scientific American This content downloaded from 142.104.240.194 on Tue, 02 Oct 2018 21:47:44 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms This content downloaded from© 2011 142.104.240.194 Scientific American on Tue, 02 Oct 2018 21:47:44 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms Lera Boroditsky is an assistant professor of cognitive psychology at Stanford University and editor in chief of Frontiers in Cultural Psychology. Her lab conducts research around the world, focusing on mental representation and the efects of language on cognition.
    [Show full text]
  • Re-Evaluating Evidence for Linguistic Relativity: Reply to Boroditsky (2001) Q
    Cognition 104 (2007) 417–426 www.elsevier.com/locate/COGNIT Brief article Re-evaluating evidence for linguistic relativity: Reply to Boroditsky (2001) q David January a,*, Edward Kako b a University of Pennsylvania, USA b Swarthmore College, USA Received 1 February 2006; revised 30 May 2006; accepted 7 July 2006 Abstract Six unsuccessful attempts at replicating a key finding in the linguistic relativity literature [Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought?: Mandarin and English speakers’ con- ceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 1–22] are reported. In addition to these empirical issues in replicating the original finding, theoretical issues present in the original report are dis- cussed. In sum, we conclude that Boroditsky (2001) provides no support for the Whorfian hypothesis. Ó 2006 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Linguistic relativity; Whorfian hypothesis; Temporal cognition; Language and thought 1. Introduction Recently, the psychological community has seen renewed interest in the linguistic relativity hypothesis, the claim (Whorf, 1939/1956, 1941/1956) that the language one speaks structures how one thinks (for varying perspectives, see, e.g., Li & Gleitman, 2002; Levinson, Kita, Haun, & Rasch, 2002; Lowenstein & Gentner, 2005; Papafr- q This manuscript was accepted under the editorship of Jacques Mehler. * Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 215 898 0360; fax: +1 215 573 9247. E-mail address: [email protected] (D. January). 0010-0277/$ - see front matter Ó 2006 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.07.008 418 D. January, E. Kako / Cognition 104 (2007) 417–426 agou, Massey, & Gleitman, 2002 and the volume edited by Gentner & Goldin-Mead- ow, 2003).
    [Show full text]
  • Arxiv:1910.09729V1 [Cs.CL] 22 Oct 2019
    Grammatical Gender, Neo-Whorfianism, and Word Embeddings: A Data-Driven Approach to Linguistic Relativity Katharina Kann New York University, USA [email protected] Abstract 2012). Ultimately, Whorf went on to hypothesize that the Hopi perceive time differently as a result The relation between language and thought of their language’s grammar, kicking off a more has occupied linguists for at least a century. encompassing debate on the relation of language Neo-Whorfianism, a weak version of the con- troversial Sapir–Whorf hypothesis, holds that and thought and engendering one of the larger con- our thoughts are subtly influenced by the gram- troversies in linguistics to date (Deutscher, 2010). matical structures of our native language. One While influential, the strong version of what area of investigation in this vein focuses on has come to be known as the Sapir–Whorf2 hy- how the grammatical gender of nouns affects pothesis has been disproven. For instance, even the way we perceive the corresponding objects. though languages differ in the basic color terms For instance, does the fact that key is mascu- they employ, e.g., Korean has one word that rep- line in German (der Schlussel¨ ), but feminine in Spanish (la llave) change the speakers’ views resents both green and blue, the development of of those objects? Psycholinguistic evidence color terminology and perception is subject to uni- presented by Boroditsky et al.(2003, x4) sug- versalist constraints due to biology (Berlin and Kay, gested the answer might be yes: When asked 1969). Recent years, however, have witnessed a to produce adjectives that best described a key, resurgence of a milder strain of the hypothesis, al- German and Spanish speakers named more ternatively known as neo-Whorfianism or the weak stereotypically masculine and feminine ones, Sapir–Whorf hypothesis (Boroditsky, 2003).
    [Show full text]
  • Popularizing Linguistic Relativity
    article doi:10.17684/i11A160en DIACRONIA ISSN: 2393-1140 Impavidi progrediamur! www.diacronia.ro Popularizing linguistic relativity Spiros A. Moschonas‹ Faculty of Communication and Media Studies, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, 1 Sofocleous Str., 10559 Athens, Greece Article info Abstract History: This paper distinguishes three phases in the popularization of linguistic relativ- Received December 5, 2019 ity: the phase initiated by Benjamin Lee Whorf himself; a second phase during Accepted May 4, 2020 which linguistic relativity was formulated and tested as a research hypothesis; Published June 10, 2020 and the current phase during which language-relativistic assumptions have pen- etrated the mass media. To diagnose the spread of relativistic assumptions, 560 Key words: articles in both English and Greek print and electronic media were considered. linguistic anthropology The articles were published over the period 2010–2019. They fall, roughly, into psycholinguistics eighteen categories. Some of the articles report explicitly on linguistic relativity ethnolinguistics research, while others presuppose language-relativistic ideas in handling issues history of language as disparate as the effectiveness of managerial discourse, the appropriateness of political correctness, or the possibility of communicating with aliens. The large number of article categories as well as the tacit assumption of linguistic relativity in a growing number of articles are indicators of how popular linguistic relativity has become in folk-linguistic discourse. 1. Introductory remarks “Whorf ’s name has shared the fate of those of many physicians who first propose a differential diagnosis, in that the condition or ‘disease’ he diagnosed now bears his name, Whorfianism”. Thus spoke Silverstein (2000, p.
    [Show full text]
  • Perceptual Foundations of Abstract Thought
    Perceptual Foundations of Abstract Thought by Daniel J. Casasanto G.P.D., Vocal Performance Peabody Conservatory of the Johns Hopkins University, 1996 B.A., English and Vocal Performance Oberlin College and Oberlin Conservatory of Music, 1993 Submitted to the Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy In Cognitive Science at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology September, 2005 © 2005 Massachusetts Institute of Technology All rights reserved Signature of Author: ___________________________________________________________________ Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences 7 September, 2005 Certified by: ___________________________________________________________________ Lera Boroditsky Assistant Professor of Psychology, Stanford University Thesis Supervisor Accepted by: __________________________________________________________________ Earl K. Miller Picower Professor of Neuroscience Chairman, Department Graduate Committee 2 Abstract How do people think about things they can never see or touch? The ability to invent and reason about domains such as time, ideas, or mathematics is uniquely human, and is arguably the hallmark of human sophistication. Yet, how people mentally represent these abstract domains has remained one of the great mysteries of the mind. This dissertation explores a potential solution: perhaps the mind recruits old structures for new uses. Perhaps sensory and motor representations that result from physical interactions with the world (e.g., representations of physical space) are recycled to support our thinking about abstract phenomena. This hypothesis is motivated, in part, by patterns observed in language: in order to talk about abstract things, speakers often recruit metaphors from more concrete or perceptually rich domains. For example, English speakers often talk about time using spatial language (e.g., a long vacation; a short meeting).
    [Show full text]
  • English and Mandarin Time in 3D
    Cognitive Science 35 (2011) 1305–1328 Copyright Ó 2011 Cognitive Science Society, Inc. All rights reserved. ISSN: 0364-0213 print / 1551-6709 online DOI: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01193.x How Linguistic and Cultural Forces Shape Conceptions of Time: English and Mandarin Time in 3D Orly Fuhrman,a Kelly McCormick,a Eva Chen,b Heidi Jiang,c Dingfang Shu,d Shuaimei Mao,d Lera Boroditskya aDepartment of Psychology, Stanford University bGraduate School of Education, Harvard University cDepartment of Cognitive and Linguistic Sciences, Brown University dShanghai International Studies University Received 5 October 2010; received in revised form 18 February 2011; accepted 3 March 2011 Abstract In this paper we examine how English and Mandarin speakers think about time, and we test how the patterns of thinking in the two groups relate to patterns in linguistic and cultural experience. In Mandarin, vertical spatial metaphors are used more frequently to talk about time than they are in English; English relies primarily on horizontal terms. We present results from two tasks comparing English and Mandarin speakers’ temporal reasoning. The tasks measure how people spatialize time in three-dimensional space, including the sagittal (front ⁄ back), transverse (left ⁄ right), and vertical (up ⁄ down) axes. Results of Experiment 1 show that people automatically create spatial representa- tions in the course of temporal reasoning, and these implicit spatializations differ in accordance with patterns in language, even in a non-linguistic task. Both groups showed evidence of a left-to-right representation of time, in accordance with writing direction, but only Mandarin speakers showed a vertical top-to-bottom pattern for time (congruent with vertical spatiotemporal metaphors in Manda- rin).
    [Show full text]