UC San Diego Electronic Theses and Dissertations

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

UC San Diego Electronic Theses and Dissertations UC San Diego UC San Diego Electronic Theses and Dissertations Title Space and Time: Relationships among Language, Body, and Mind Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/2mk948b0 Author Hendricks, Rose Katherine Publication Date 2017 Peer reviewed|Thesis/dissertation eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, SAN DIEGO Space and Time: Relationships among Language, Body, and Mind A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree Doctor of Philosophy in Cognitive Science by Rose Katherine Hendricks Committee in charge: Professor Lera Boroditsky, Chair Professor Benjamin K. Bergen, Co-Chair Professor David Barner Professor Seana Coulson Professor Federico Rossano 2017 Copyright Rose Katherine Hendricks, 2017 All rights reserved. The dissertation of Rose Katherine Hendricks is approved, and it is acceptable in quality and form for publication on microfilm and electronically: Co-Chair Chair University of California, San Diego 2017 iii TABLE OF CONTENTS Signature Page . iii Table of Contents . iv List of Figures . ix List of Tables . x Acknowledgements . xi Vita . xii Abstract of the Dissertation . xiii Chapter 1 Introduction . 1 1.1 Metaphor & Thought . 1 1.2 Language & Thought . 3 1.2.1 The Causal Problem . 6 1.2.2 The Nature of Effects of Language on Thought . 9 1.3 Spatio-Temporal Metaphors: Bridging Case Study . 11 1.3.1 Does Language Play a Causal Role? . 15 1.3.2 Mechanisms: Testing Verbal & Spatial Working Mem- ory with Dual Tasks . 17 1.4 Overview of Studies . 19 Chapter 2 New Space-Time Metaphors Foster New Non-Linguistic Represen- tations . 23 2.1 Abstract . 23 2.2 Experiment 1 Introduction . 24 2.2.1 Spatio-Temporal Metaphors: A Brief Overview . 24 2.2.2 Experiment 1 Overview . 26 2.3 Experiment 1 Methods . 27 2.3.1 Participants . 27 2.3.2 Procedure . 28 2.3.3 Participant & Trial Inclusion Criteria . 32 2.3.4 Analyses . 33 2.4 Experiment 1 Results . 34 2.4.1 Main (Hypothesis-Relevant) Results . 34 2.4.2 Ancillary Results . 35 2.5 Experiment 1 Discussion . 38 2.5.1 Open Questions . 40 2.6 Experiment 2 Introduction . 41 iv 2.7 Experiment 2 Methods . 42 2.7.1 Participants . 42 2.7.2 Materials & Procedure . 43 2.7.3 Participants & Trial Inclusion Criteria . 43 2.7.4 Analyses . 44 2.8 Experiment 2 Results . 44 2.8.1 Comparison to Experiment 1 . 45 2.9 Experiment 2 Discussion . 46 Chapter 3 Chinese speakers’ space-time associations . 47 3.1 Abstract . 47 3.2 Introduction . 48 3.3 Experiment 3 Methods . 52 3.3.1 Participants & Trial Inclusion . 52 3.3.2 Materials & Procedure . 53 3.3.3 Analyses . 54 3.4 Experiment 3 Results . 55 3.4.1 Time Judgment Reaction Times . 55 3.4.2 Time Judgment Accuracies . 59 3.4.3 Interference Accuracies . 59 3.4.4 Comparison to Experiment 1, Metaphor Training . 61 3.5 Experiment 3 Discussion . 62 3.6 Experiment 4 Methods . 63 3.6.1 Participants & Trial Inclusion . 63 3.6.2 Materials & Procedure . 63 3.7 Experiment 4 Results . 64 3.7.1 Time Judgment Reaction Times . 64 3.7.2 Proficiency & Congruency Effects . 65 3.7.3 Accuracies . 66 3.7.4 Comparison to Experiment 3, Natural Language Con- gruency Effects . 68 3.7.5 Comparison to Experiment 1, Metaphor Training for English Speakers . 68 3.8 Experiment 4 Discussion . 70 3.9 General Discussion . 70 Chapter 4 Consistencies in space-time and space-number associations under spatial interference . 74 4.1 Abstract . 74 4.2 Introduction . 75 4.3 Experiment 5: Space-Time Associations from Visuomotor Experiences . 78 4.4 Experiment 5 Methods . 78 v 4.4.1 Participants & Trial Inclusion . 78 4.4.2 Materials & Procedure . 78 4.4.3 Analyses . 79 4.5 Experiment 5 Results . 79 4.5.1 Judgment Reaction Times . 79 4.5.2 Accuracies . 80 4.5.3 Comparing Behavior Shaped by Learning New Metaphors (Exp 1) to Behavior Shaped by Visuomotor Experi- ence (Exp 5) . 81 4.6 Experiment 5 Discussion . 83 4.7 Experiments 6 & 7: Mental Time- vs. Number Lines . 84 4.8 Experiments 6 & 7 Methods . 87 4.8.1 Participants & Trial Inclusion Criteria . 87 4.8.2 Materials and Procedure . 88 4.8.3 Time Judgments . 89 4.8.4 Analyses . 90 4.9 Experiments 6 & 7 Results . 90 4.9.1 Judgment Reaction Times . 90 4.9.2 Judgment Accuracies . 91 4.9.3 Interference Accuracies . 92 4.10 Experiments 6 & 7 Discussion . 93 4.11 Experiment 8: Mental Timeline under Horizonal vs. Vertical Interference . 96 4.12 Experiment 8 Methods . 98 4.12.1 Participants & Trial Inclusion . 98 4.12.2 Materials & Procedure . 98 4.12.3 Analyses . 99 4.13 Experiment 8 Results . 99 4.13.1 Interference Distractors . 99 4.13.2 Judgment Reaction Times . 100 4.13.3 Judgment Accuracies . 101 4.13.4 Interference Accuracies . 101 4.14 Experiment 8 Discussion . 101 4.15 General Discussion . 103 Chapter 5 Interim Meta-Analyses . 105 5.1 Abstract . 105 5.2 Methods . 106 5.3 Results . 107 5.3.1 Congruency Effects over Time . 107 5.3.2 By Trial Number . 108 5.3.3 Congruency Effects & Individual Differences . 111 5.3.4 Congruency Effects by Stimuli Features . 113 vi 5.4 Discussion . 117 Chapter 6 New Space-Time Associations in the Body . 121 6.1 Abstract . 121 6.2 Introduction . 122 6.2.1 Gestures . 123 6.3 Methods . 126 6.3.1 Participants . 126 6.3.2 Materials & Procedure . 126 6.3.3 Gesture Analyses . 128 6.4 Results . 129 6.4.1 Item Analyses . 129 6.4.2 Gestures on the Sagittal Axis . 131 6.4.3 Gestures on the Lateral Axis . 132 6.5 Discussion . 133 6.5.1 Mental Timelines are Dynamic . 134 6.5.2 Linguistic Metaphors Shapes Gestures . 135 Chapter 7 Do metaphors move from mind to mouth? Evidence from a new system of linguistic metaphors for time . 137 7.1 Abstract . 137 7.2 Introduction . 138 7.2.1 The Coupling of Language and Thought about Time 142 7.2.2 Case Study: The U.S. Military . 144 7.3 Study 10 Methods . 145 7.3.1 Participants . 145 7.3.2 Materials . 146 7.3.3 Procedure . 147 7.3.4 Exclusions and Analyses . 147 7.4 Study 10 Results . 148 7.5 Study 10 Discussion . 151 7.6 Study 11: Lateral Linguistic Metaphors in Different Contexts 152 7.7 Study 11 Methods . 152 7.7.1 Participants . 152 7.7.2 Materials . 153 7.7.3 Procedure . 153 7.7.4 Exclusions & Analyses . ..
Recommended publications
  • Language in Mind : Advances in the Study of Language and Thought
    Language in Mind This page intentionally left blank Language in Mind Advances in the Study of Language and Thought edited by Dedre Gentner and Susan Goldin-Meadow A Bradford Book The MIT Press Cambridge, Massachusetts London, England ( 2003 Massachusetts Institute of Technology All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced in any form by any electronic or mechanical means (including photocopying, recording, or informa- tion storage and retrieval) without permission in writing from the publisher. This book was set in Sabon on 3B2 by Asco Typesetters, Hong Kong. Printed and bound in the United States of America. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Language in mind ; advances in the study of language and thought / edited by Dedre Gentner and S. Goldin-Meadow. p. cm. ‘‘A Bradford book.’’ Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 0-262-07243-2 (hc. : alk. paper)—ISBN 0-262-57163-3 (pbk. : alk. paper) 1. Psycholinguistics. 2. Cognition. I. Gentner, Dedre. II. Goldin, Susan. P37 .L357 2003 4010.9—dc21 2002029578 Contents Contributors vii Acknowledgments ix I Introduction 1 Whither Whorf 3 Dedre Gentner and Susan Goldin-Meadow II Position Statements 15 1 Languages and Representations 17 Eve V. Clark 2 Language and Mind: Let’s Get the Issues Straight! 25 Stephen C. Levinson 3 The Key Is Social Cognition 47 Michael Tomasello III Language as Lens: Does the Language We Acquire Influence How We See the World? 59 4 Sex, Syntax, and Semantics 61 Lera Boroditsky, Lauren A. Schmidt, and Webb Phillips 5 Speaking versus Thinking about Objects and Actions 81 Barbara C.
    [Show full text]
  • Metaphoric Structuring: Understanding Time Through Spatial Metaphors
    COGNITION Cognition 75 (2000) 1±28 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Metaphoric structuring: understanding time through spatial metaphors Lera Boroditsky* Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Jordan Hall, Bldg. 420, Stanford, CA 94305-2130, USA Received 17 December 1998; received in revised form 22 September 1999; accepted 23 October 1999 Abstract The present paper evaluates the claim that abstract conceptual domains are structured through metaphorical mappings from domains grounded directly in experience. In particular, the paper asks whether the abstract domain of time gets its relational structure from the more concrete domain of space. Relational similarities between space and time are outlined along with several explanations of how these similarities may have arisen. Three experiments designed to distinguish between these explanations are described. The results indicate that (1) the domains of space and time do share conceptual structure, (2) spatial relational infor- mation is just as useful for thinking about time as temporal information, and (3) with frequent use, mappings between space and time come to be stored in the domain of time and so thinking about time does not necessarily require access to spatial schemas. These ®ndings provide some of the ®rst empirical evidence for Metaphoric Structuring. It appears that abstract domains such as time are indeed shaped by metaphorical mappings from more concrete and experiential domains such as space. q 2000 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Metaphoric structuring; Understanding time; Spatial metaphors 1. Introduction How do we come to represent and reason about abstract domains like time, love, justice, or ideas? There are at least two interesting puzzles here.
    [Show full text]
  • How Languages Construct Time Lera Boroditsky Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, USA
    CHAPTER 20 How Languages Construct Time Lera Boroditsky Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, USA Summary (OW DO PEOPLE CONSTRUCT THEIR MENTAL REPRESENTATIONS OF TIME ) FOCUS ON WORK EXAMIN ING THE ROLE THAT SPATIAL METAPHORS AND BASIC SPATIAL REPRESENTATIONS PLAY IN CONSTRUCTING REP RESENTATIONS OF TIME ACROSS LANGUAGES 4HE RESULTS REVEAL THAT THE METAPHORS WE USE TO TALK ABOUT TIME HAVE BOTH IMMEDIATE AND LONG TERM CONSEQUENCES FOR HOW WE CONCEPTUALIZE AND REASON ABOUT THIS FUNDAMENTAL DOMAIN OF EXPERIENCE (OW PEOPLE CONCEPTUALIZE TIME APPEARS TO DEPEND ON HOW THE LANGUAGES THEY SPEAK TEND TO TALK ABOUT TIME THE CURRENT LINGUISTIC CONTEXT WHAT LANGUAGE IS BEING SPOKEN AND ALSO ON THE PARTICULAR METAPHORS BEING USED TO TALK ABOUT TIME IN THE MOMENT &URTHER PEOPLE WHO CONCEPTUALIZE SPACE DIFFERENTLY ALSO CON CEPTUALIZE TIME DIFFERENTLY SUGGESTING THAT PEOPLE CO OPT REPRESENTATIONS OF THE PHYSICAL WORLD EG SPACE IN ORDER TO MENTALLY REPRESENT MORE ABSTRACT OR INTANGIBLE ENTITIES EG TIME 4AKEN ALL TOGETHER THESE lNDINGS SHOW THAT CONCEPTIONS OF EVEN SUCH FUNDAMENTAL DOMAINS AS TIME DIFFER DRAMATICALLY ACROSS CULTURES AND GROUPS 4HE RESULTS REVEAL SOME OF THE MECHA NISMS THROUGH WHICH LANGUAGES AND CULTURES HELP CONSTRUCT OUR BASIC NOTIONS OF TIME /NE OF THE GREAT MYSTERIES OF THE MIND IS HOW WE ARE ABLE TO THINK ABOUT THINGS WE CAN NEVER SEE OR TOUCH (OW DO WE COME TO REPRESENT AND REASON ABOUT ABSTRACT DOMAINS LIKE TIME JUSTICE OR IDEAS !LL OF OUR EXPERIENCE OF THE WORLD IS PHYSICAL ACCOMPLISHED THROUGH SENSORY PERCEPTION
    [Show full text]
  • Complexlang: a Compact Logical Experimental Language
    Complexlang: a COMPact Logical EXperimental LANGuage Kelvin M. Liu-Huang Carnegie Mellon University [email protected] Published in Proceedings of SIGBOVIK 2019 (15 March 2019) Informal revisions (27 March 2019, 29 April 2019) Abstract and learning barrier of natural languages, Natural languages trade compactness and mathematical expressions seem better in these ways. consistency for efficiency. Complexlang is an a priori, The tradeoff is, of course, that mathematical declarative, ideographic spoken/written language descriptions can be very elaborate or unwieldy. which attempts to construct/ground the semantic We attempt to address all these concerns by structure of both morphology and syntax from first constructing Complexlang. Language should ideally principles using tools provided by propositional logic, synchronize speech, writing, and comprehension in set theory, type theory, number theory, object- order to facilitate learning. Like aUI (Weilgart, 1979) oriented programming, metaphysics, linguistics, and and Arahau (Karasev, 2006), by infusing individual classical field theory. In doing so, we hypothesize letters with meaning and using phonemic that speakers may converse in Complexlang with little orthography, words have transparent and largely training and learn some math and science in the deterministic etymology; writing, speech, and process. meaning can all be inferred from each other, reducing ambiguity, speeding up learning, and even allowing efficient and deterministic creation of 1. Introduction neologisms. For simplicity, the orthography is simply Most previous constructed languages intended for the IPA symbols of the phonemes. Unlike aUI and human use set out to improve etymological integrity Arahau, Complexlang attempts to express semantics (Zamenhof, 1887), semantic clarity (Bliss, 1965; entirely through logic, specifically patterned after set Karasev, 2006; Weilgart, 1979; Quijada, 2004), theory, rather than metaphor, resulting in compact, consistency (Weilgart, 1979; Cowan, 1997; Quijada, transparent, and unambiguous expressions.
    [Show full text]
  • Languages and Worldview
    LANGUAGES AND WORLDVIEW Manon Allard-Kropp University of Missouri–St. Louis University of Missouri–St. Louis Languages and Worldview Manon Allard-Kropp This text is disseminated via the Open Education Resource (OER) LibreTexts Project (https://LibreTexts.org) and like the hundreds of other texts available within this powerful platform, it freely available for reading, printing and "consuming." Most, but not all, pages in the library have licenses that may allow individuals to make changes, save, and print this book. Carefully consult the applicable license(s) before pursuing such effects. Instructors can adopt existing LibreTexts texts or Remix them to quickly build course-specific resources to meet the needs of their students. Unlike traditional textbooks, LibreTexts’ web based origins allow powerful integration of advanced features and new technologies to support learning. The LibreTexts mission is to unite students, faculty and scholars in a cooperative effort to develop an easy-to-use online platform for the construction, customization, and dissemination of OER content to reduce the burdens of unreasonable textbook costs to our students and society. The LibreTexts project is a multi-institutional collaborative venture to develop the next generation of open-access texts to improve postsecondary education at all levels of higher learning by developing an Open Access Resource environment. The project currently consists of 13 independently operating and interconnected libraries that are constantly being optimized by students, faculty, and outside experts to supplant conventional paper-based books. These free textbook alternatives are organized within a central environment that is both vertically (from advance to basic level) and horizontally (across different fields) integrated.
    [Show full text]
  • How Does Our Language Shape the Way We Think?
    To arrive at the edge of the world's knowledge, seek out the most complex and sophisticated minds, put them in a room together, and have them ask each other the questions they are asking themselves. HOW DOES OUR LANGUAGE SHAPE THE WAY WE THINK? Lera Boroditsky [6.11.09] For a long time, the idea that language might shape thought was considered at best untestable and more often simply wrong. Research in my labs at Stanford University and at MIT has helped reopen this question. We have collected data around the world: from China, Greece, Chile, Indonesia, Russia, and Aboriginal Australia. What we have learned is that people who speak different languages do indeed think differently and that even flukes of grammar can profoundly affect how we see the world. Language is a uniquely human gift, central to our experience of being human. Appreciating its role in constructing our mental lives brings us one step closer to understanding the very nature of humanity. LERA BORODITSKY is an assistant professor of psychology, neuroscience, and symbolic systems at Stanford University, who looks at how the languages we speak shape the way we think. Humans communicate with one another using a dazzling array of languages, each differing from the next in innumerable ways. Do the languages we speak shape the way we see the world, the way we think, and the way we live our lives? Do people who speak different languages think differently simply because they speak different languages? Does learning new languages change the way you think? Do polyglots think differently when speaking different languages? These questions touch on nearly all of the major controversies in the study of mind.
    [Show full text]
  • Do English and Mandarin Speakers Think Differently About Time?
    Do English and Mandarin Speakers Think Differently About Time? Lera Boroditsky ([email protected]) Department of Psychology, 450 Serra Mall, Bldg. 420 Stanford, CA 94305 USA Abstract bounded events, unidirectional change, etc.) appear to be universal across cultures and languages. Do the languages we speak shape the ways we think? Boroditsky, (2001) demonstrated that speakers of English and However, there are many aspects of our concept of time Mandarin think differently about time. This work has recently that are not observable in the world. For example, does time been brought into question (January & Kako, 2007; Chen, move horizontally or vertically? Does it move forward or 2007). Here I present new evidence that again demonstrates a back, left or right, up or down? Does it move past us, or do difference between English and Mandarin speakers’ we move through it? All of these aspects are left unspecified construals of time. Both languages use horizontal and vertical in our experience with the world. They are however, spatial language to talk about time. For example, in English we might say that the best is ahead of us, or we may move a specified in our language —most often through spatial meeting up. In English, vertical metaphors are relatively metaphors. infrequent and horizontal metaphors predominate. In Across languages, people use spatial metaphors to talk Mandarin, both horizontal and vertical metaphors are about time. Whether they are looking forward to a brighter frequent. Importantly, vertical metaphors are much more tomorrow, proposing theories ahead of their time, or falling frequent in Mandarin than they are in English.
    [Show full text]
  • Language, Categories, Cognition
    LANGUAGE, CATEGORIES, AND COGNITION HONORS SEMINAR 01:090:293:01 Instructor PROF. KRISTEN SYRETT (k-syrett (at) ruccs.rutgers.edu) Day/Time 9:50-11:10 pm Tuesdays, Fridays Location Brett Hall Seminar Room (CAC) (RUID card required for access) Office Hours Tuesdays 11:30-12:15 pm (sign up through Sakai) and by appointment CORE CURRICULUM GOALS This course satisfies the SAS Core Goal: WCd (Discipline-Based Writing and Communication) Upon completion of this course, you will be able to (t) communicate effectively in modes appropriate to a discipline or area of inquiry [WCd] (u) evaluate and critically assess sources and use the conventions of attribution and citation correctly (v) analyze and synthesize information and ideas from multiple sources to generate new insights COURSE LEARNING GOALS The goals of the course are to • Engage you in a central debate at the heart of research in linguistics, cognitive psychology, and infant and child development • Equip you with arguing points on both side of the debate based on empirical research in these fields • Hone your skills in critiquing research, taking apart and delivering an argument, presenting research, and writing a review of research COURSE OVERVIEW How does the world we live in shape the language we acquire? What role does language play in shaping the way we think about the world? Does it lead us to inescapably conceptualize the world in a particular way, or does it serve as a guide, interacting with universal tendencies? How do linguistic, cognitive, and social processes constrain and affect each other throughout development and across language and cultures? In the early part of the 20th century, Edward Sapir argued that humans are “at the mercy” of their “language habits.” His student Benjamin Lee Whorf picked up on this idea and claimed that the concepts we acquire depend on our linguistic knowledge and practices.
    [Show full text]
  • How Language Shapes Thought Author(S): Lera Boroditsky Source: Scientific American, Vol
    How Language Shapes Thought Author(s): Lera Boroditsky Source: Scientific American, Vol. 304, No. 2 (February 2011), pp. 62-65 Published by: Scientific American, a division of Nature America, Inc. Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/26002395 Accessed: 02-10-2018 21:47 UTC REFERENCES Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article: https://www.jstor.org/stable/26002395?seq=1&cid=pdf-reference#references_tab_contents You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references. JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at https://about.jstor.org/terms Scientific American, a division of Nature America, Inc. is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Scientific American This content downloaded from 142.104.240.194 on Tue, 02 Oct 2018 21:47:44 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms This content downloaded from© 2011 142.104.240.194 Scientific American on Tue, 02 Oct 2018 21:47:44 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms Lera Boroditsky is an assistant professor of cognitive psychology at Stanford University and editor in chief of Frontiers in Cultural Psychology. Her lab conducts research around the world, focusing on mental representation and the efects of language on cognition.
    [Show full text]
  • Re-Evaluating Evidence for Linguistic Relativity: Reply to Boroditsky (2001) Q
    Cognition 104 (2007) 417–426 www.elsevier.com/locate/COGNIT Brief article Re-evaluating evidence for linguistic relativity: Reply to Boroditsky (2001) q David January a,*, Edward Kako b a University of Pennsylvania, USA b Swarthmore College, USA Received 1 February 2006; revised 30 May 2006; accepted 7 July 2006 Abstract Six unsuccessful attempts at replicating a key finding in the linguistic relativity literature [Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought?: Mandarin and English speakers’ con- ceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 1–22] are reported. In addition to these empirical issues in replicating the original finding, theoretical issues present in the original report are dis- cussed. In sum, we conclude that Boroditsky (2001) provides no support for the Whorfian hypothesis. Ó 2006 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Linguistic relativity; Whorfian hypothesis; Temporal cognition; Language and thought 1. Introduction Recently, the psychological community has seen renewed interest in the linguistic relativity hypothesis, the claim (Whorf, 1939/1956, 1941/1956) that the language one speaks structures how one thinks (for varying perspectives, see, e.g., Li & Gleitman, 2002; Levinson, Kita, Haun, & Rasch, 2002; Lowenstein & Gentner, 2005; Papafr- q This manuscript was accepted under the editorship of Jacques Mehler. * Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 215 898 0360; fax: +1 215 573 9247. E-mail address: [email protected] (D. January). 0010-0277/$ - see front matter Ó 2006 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.07.008 418 D. January, E. Kako / Cognition 104 (2007) 417–426 agou, Massey, & Gleitman, 2002 and the volume edited by Gentner & Goldin-Mead- ow, 2003).
    [Show full text]
  • Arxiv:1910.09729V1 [Cs.CL] 22 Oct 2019
    Grammatical Gender, Neo-Whorfianism, and Word Embeddings: A Data-Driven Approach to Linguistic Relativity Katharina Kann New York University, USA [email protected] Abstract 2012). Ultimately, Whorf went on to hypothesize that the Hopi perceive time differently as a result The relation between language and thought of their language’s grammar, kicking off a more has occupied linguists for at least a century. encompassing debate on the relation of language Neo-Whorfianism, a weak version of the con- troversial Sapir–Whorf hypothesis, holds that and thought and engendering one of the larger con- our thoughts are subtly influenced by the gram- troversies in linguistics to date (Deutscher, 2010). matical structures of our native language. One While influential, the strong version of what area of investigation in this vein focuses on has come to be known as the Sapir–Whorf2 hy- how the grammatical gender of nouns affects pothesis has been disproven. For instance, even the way we perceive the corresponding objects. though languages differ in the basic color terms For instance, does the fact that key is mascu- they employ, e.g., Korean has one word that rep- line in German (der Schlussel¨ ), but feminine in Spanish (la llave) change the speakers’ views resents both green and blue, the development of of those objects? Psycholinguistic evidence color terminology and perception is subject to uni- presented by Boroditsky et al.(2003, x4) sug- versalist constraints due to biology (Berlin and Kay, gested the answer might be yes: When asked 1969). Recent years, however, have witnessed a to produce adjectives that best described a key, resurgence of a milder strain of the hypothesis, al- German and Spanish speakers named more ternatively known as neo-Whorfianism or the weak stereotypically masculine and feminine ones, Sapir–Whorf hypothesis (Boroditsky, 2003).
    [Show full text]
  • In 2018 Linguapax Review
    linguapax review6 62018 Languages, Worlds and Action Llengües, mons i acció Linguapax Review 2018 Languages, Worlds and Actions Llengües, mons i acció Editat per: Amb el suport de: Generalitat de Catalunya Departament de Cultura Generalitat de Catalunya Departament d’Acció Exterior Relacions Institucionals i Transparència Secretaria d’Acció Exterior i de la Unió Europea Coordinació editorial: Alícia Fuentes Calle Disseny i maquetació: Maria Cabrera Callís Traduccions: Marc Alba / Violeta Roca Font Aquesta obra està subjecta a una llicència de Reconeixement-NoComercial-CompartirIgual 4.0 Internacional de Creative Commons CONTENTS - CONTINGUTS Introduction. Languages, Worlds and action. Alícia Fuentes-Calle 5 Introducció. Llengües, mons i acció. Alícia Fuentes-Calle Túumben Maaya K’aay: De-stigmatising Maya Language in the 14 Yucatan Region Genner Llanes-Ortiz Túumben Maaya K’aay: desestigmatitzant la llengua maia a la regió del Yucatán. Genner Llanes-Ortiz Into the Heimat. Transcultural theatre. Sonia Antinori 37 En el Heimat. Teatre transcultural. Sonia Antinori Sustaining multimodal diversity: Narrative practices from the 64 Central Australian deserts. Jennifer Green La preservació de la diversitat multimodal: els costums narratius dels deserts d’Austràlia central. Jennifer Green A new era in the history of language invention. Jan van Steenbergen 101 Una nova era en la història de la invenció de llengües. Jan van Steenbergen Tribalingual, a startup for endangered languages. Inky Gibbens 183 Tribalingual, una start-up per a llengües amenaçades. Inky Gibbens The Web Alternative, Dimensions of Literacy, and Newer Prospects 200 for African Languages in Today’s World. Kọ́lá Túbọ̀sún L’alternativa web, els aspectes de l’alfabetització i les perspectives més recents de les llengües africanes en el món actual.
    [Show full text]