Memory Modifications and Ethical Implications
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Load more
Recommended publications
-
2010 Cancun, Mexico
Welcome to the NINETIETH ANNUAL CONVENTION of the WESTERN PSYCHOLOGICAL ASSOCIATION APRIL 22-25, 2010 at the Fiesta Americana Condesa Cancun The 90th meeting of the Western Psychological Association has: , The WPA Film Festival , Outstanding Invited Speakers , Special Programs for Students and Teachers , A Forum for Your Research Visit WPA at: www.westernpsych.org HOSTED BY 1 Dear Conference Attendees: On behalf of the University of Southern California, it is my great pleasure to welcome you to the 90th Annual Western Psychological Association Convention. USC, the Col- lege of Letters, Arts and Sciences, and the Department of Psychology are pleased to serve as sponsors of the annual meeting. I would especially like to thank WP A Presi- dent Stanley Sue, Executive Officer Chris Cozby, and Program Chair Steven Lopez for this opportunity. Located in Los Angeles, USC is one of the world’s leading private research universities. In the fall of 2009, USC enrolled 17,000 undergraduates, and 18,000 graduate and professional students. As a global university, the convention’s theme of diversity and its setting in Mexico are consistent with our multiple initiatives to address diversity issues within the United States. The Princeton Review has selected USC as one of 81 “Colleges with a Conscience” based on its outstanding record of involvement in the surrounding community with its large proportion of Latino Americans, African Americans and Asian Americans. In addition, USC enrolls more international students than any other U.S. university. Several mem- bers of the College’s Psychology Department are devoted to cross-national research in Korea, China, Rwanda, Finland, Sweden and Mexico, as well as multicultural research within the U.S. -
False Memories: What the Hell Are They For? 12 13 14 ERYN J
JOHN WILEY & SONS, LTD., THE ATRIUM, SOUTHERN GATE, CHICHESTER P019 8SQ, UK *** PROOF OF YOUR ARTICLE ATTACHED, PLEASE READ CAREFULLY *** After receipt of your corrections your article will be published initially within the online version of the journal. PLEASE NOTE THAT THE PROMPT RETURN OF YOUR PROOF CORRECTIONS WILL ENSURE THAT THERE ARE NO UNNECESSARY DELAYS IN THE PUBLICATION OF YOUR ARTICLE READ PROOFS CAREFULLY ONCE PUBLISHED ONLINE OR IN PRINT IT IS NOT POSSIBLE TO MAKE ANY FURTHER CORRECTIONS TO YOUR ARTICLE This will be your only chance to correct your proof Please note that the volume and page numbers shown on the proofs are for position only ANSWER ALL QUERIES ON PROOFS (Queries are attached as the last page of your proof.) List all corrections and send back via e-mail to the production contact as detailed in the covering e-mail, or mark all corrections directly on the proofs and send the scanned copy via e-mail. Please do not send corrections by fax or post CHECK FIGURES AND TABLES CAREFULLY Check sizes, numbering, and orientation of figures All images in the PDF are downsampled (reduced to lower resolution and file size) to facilitate Internet delivery. These images will appear at higher resolution and sharpness in the printed article Review figure legends to ensure that they are complete Check all tables. Review layout, titles, and footnotes COMPLETE COPYRIGHT TRANSFER AGREEMENT (CTA) if you have not already signed one Please send a scanned signed copy with your proofs by e-mail. Your article cannot be published unless we have received the signed CTA OFFPRINTS Free access to the final PDF offprint of your article will be available via Author Services only. -
Collective Memory from a Psychological Perspective
Int J Polit Cult Soc (2009) 22:125–141 DOI 10.1007/s10767-009-9057-9 Collective Memory from a Psychological Perspective Alin Coman & Adam D. Brown & Jonathan Koppel & William Hirst Published online: 26 May 2009 # Springer Science + Business Media, LLC 2009 Abstract The study of collective memory has burgeoned in the last 20 years, so much so that one can even detect a growing resistance to what some view as the imperialistic march of memory studies across the social sciences (e.g., Berliner 2005;Fabian1999). Yet despite its clear advance, one area that has remained on the sidelines is psychology. On the one hand, this disinterest is surprising, since memory is of central concern to psychologists. On the other hand, the relative absence of the study of collective memory within the discipline of psychology seems to suit both psychology and other disciplines of the social sciences, for reasons that will be made clear. This paper explores how psychology might step from the sidelines and contribute meaningfully to discussions of collective memory. It reviews aspects of the small literature on the psychology of collective memoryandconnectsthisworktothelargerscholarly community’sinterestincollectivememory. Keywords Social contagion . Memory restructuring . Collective memory . Collective forgetting General Comments Contextualizing the Study of Collective Memory Why not has psychology figured prominently in discussions of collective memory? For those in social science fields other than psychology, the methodological individualism of The first three authors contributed equally to this paper. The order in which they are listed reflects the throw of a die. A. Coman : J. Koppel : W. Hirst (*) The New School for Social Research, New York, NY 10011, USA e-mail: [email protected] A. -
Memory Specificity, but Not Perceptual Load, Affects Susceptibility to Misleading Information
Memory specificity, but not perceptual load, affects susceptibility to misleading information Francesca R. Farina1,2,* & Ciara M. Greene1 1School of Psychology, University College Dublin, Ireland. 2Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Ireland. Note: This paper has not yet been peer-reviewed. Please contact the authors before citing. Correspondence may be sent to Francesca Farina, Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Ireland. Email: [email protected] 1 Abstract The purpose of this study was to examine the role of perceptual load in eyewitness memory accuracy and susceptibility to misinformation at immediate and delayed recall. Despite its relevance to real-world situations, previous research in this area is limited. A secondary aim was to establish whether trait-based memory specificity can protect against susceptibility to misinformation. Participants (n=264) viewed a 1-minute video depicting a crime and completed a memory questionnaire immediately afterwards and one week later. Memory specificity was measured via an online version of the Autobiographical Memory Test (AMT). We found a strong misinformation effect, but no effect of perceptual load on memory accuracy or suggestibility at either timepoint. Memory specificity was a significant predictor of accuracy for both neutrally phrased and leading questions, though the effect was weaker after a one-week delay. Results suggest that specific autobiographical memory, but not perceptual load, enhances eyewitness memory and protects against misinformation. Keywords Perceptual load; memory specificity; eyewitness; misinformation. 2 General Audience Summary The misinformation effect is a memory impairment for a past event that occurs when a person is presented with leading information. Leading information can distort the original details of a memory and produce false memories. -
The Memory-Modifying Potential of Optogenetics and the Need for Neuroethics
Nanoethics https://doi.org/10.1007/s11569-020-00377-1 ORIGINAL RESEARCH PAPER The Memory-Modifying Potential of Optogenetics and the Need for Neuroethics Agnieszka K. Adamczyk & Przemysław Zawadzki Received: 21 December 2019 /Accepted: 30 September 2020 # The Author(s) 2020 Abstract Optogenetics is an invasive neuromodulation Keywords Optogenetics . Memory modification technology involving the use of light to control the technologies (MMTs) . Memory . Neuroethics . Safety . activity of individual neurons. Even though Moral obligations . Autonomy . Exploitation optogenetics is a relatively new neuromodulation tool whose various implications have not yet been scruti- nized, it has already been approved for its first clinical trials in humans. As optogenetics is being intensively What Is Optogenetics? investigated in animal models with the aim of develop- ing novel brain stimulation treatments for various neu- Combining genetic engineering with optics, optogenetics rological and psychiatric disorders, it appears crucial to enables the user not only to record but also to manipulate consider both the opportunities and dangers such ther- the activity of individual neurons in living tissue with the apies may offer. In this review, we focus on the use of light and to observe the effects of such manipula- memory-modifying potential of optogenetics, investi- tion in real time [1, 2]. To this end, neurons of interest are gating what it is capable of and how it differs from genetically modified to be made responsive to light, other memory modification technologies (MMTs). We which is done by means of inserting opsin genes (genes then outline the safety challenges that need to be ad- that express light-sensitive proteins). -
Noradrenergic Projections from the Locus Coeruleus to the Amygdala Constrain Fear Memory Reconsolidation Josue´ Haubrich1*, Matteo Bernabo2, Karim Nader1
RESEARCH ARTICLE Noradrenergic projections from the locus coeruleus to the amygdala constrain fear memory reconsolidation Josue´ Haubrich1*, Matteo Bernabo2, Karim Nader1 1Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Canada; 2Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Canada Abstract Memory reconsolidation is a fundamental plasticity process in the brain that allows established memories to be changed or erased. However, certain boundary conditions limit the parameters under which memories can be made plastic. Strong memories do not destabilize, for instance, although why they are resilient is mostly unknown. Here, we investigated the hypothesis that specific modulatory signals shape memory formation into a state that is reconsolidation- resistant. We find that the activation of the noradrenaline-locus coeruleus system (NOR-LC) during strong fear memory encoding increases molecular mechanisms of stability at the expense of lability in the amygdala of rats. Preventing the NOR-LC from modulating strong fear encoding results in the formation of memories that can undergo reconsolidation within the amygdala and thus are vulnerable to post-reactivation interference. Thus, the memory strength boundary condition on reconsolidation is set at the time of encoding by the action of the NOR-LC. Introduction New memories do not form instantly at the moment of an experience, but rather undergo a stabiliza- tion period during which they are gradually consolidated into a stable, long-term *For correspondence: memory (Asok et al., 2019). Later, recall may cause the memory to return to an unstable state (i.e. [email protected] destabilized) where it can be modified. Importantly, the memory must undergo a re-stabilization pro- cess called reconsolidation in order to persist (Haubrich and Nader, 2016). -
Beyond Disinformation: Deep Fakes and False
Beyond disinformation: deep fakes and false memory implantation Erin Morrow Emory University Neuroscience and Behavioral Biology Undergraduate [email protected] Disclosures: The author discloses their employment with the International Neuroethics Society and thus access to other contest submissions. General Audience / Neuroethics Essay Contest, 2021 Beyond disinformation: deep fakes and false memory implantation Most social media users are intimately familiar with labels on their timelines and feeds for content deemed ‘manipulated media.’ Twitter rolled out new policies last year to address photos or videos found to be “significantly and deceptively altered or fabricated” (Roth & Achuthan, 2020). The first such content to be flagged on this platform showed then-presidential candidate Joe Biden appearing to state “We can only re-elect Donald Trump.” In reality, this Kansas City speech was deceptively spliced, edited, and thus taken out of context—but by the time this post received its warning label, it had already garnered over five million views (Chambers, 2020). Yet, misleadingly cropped video is not the only form of deceptive content being popularized. Also within the jurisdiction of this new regulation is an emerging form of media: deep fakes. These artificial intelligence-generated creations, often in the form of videos that deceptively portray public figures, have emerged within the past decade as a force of disinformation to be reckoned with (Liv & Greenbaum, 2020; Resnick, 2018). Such videos can now manipulate the facial eXpressions and speech of these figures with minimal distortion (Resnick, 2018). Deep fakes of public figures have the potential to influence discourse and decision-making in the realms of politics, public health, and more through a powerful cognitive system: memory. -
The University of Chicago Sentient Atmospheres A
THE UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO SENTIENT ATMOSPHERES A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF THE DIVISION OF THE HUMANITIES IN CANDIDACY FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY DEPARTMENT OF ENGLISH LANGUAGE AND LITERATURE BY JEFFREY HAMILTON BOGGS CHICAGO, ILLINOIS JUNE 2016 Copyright © 2016 by Jeffrey Hamilton Boggs All rights reserved Table of Contents ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ........................................................................................................... iv INTRODUCTION .......................................................................................................................... 1 I. ATMOSPHERE IN LATE LATE JAMES ............................................................................... 21 II. “WE HAD THE AIR”: THE ATMOSPHERIC FORM OF THE VIETNAM WAR .............. 48 III. THIS IS AIR: THE ATMOSPHERIC POLITICS OF DAVID FOSTER WALLACE ......... 87 IV. NARRATING THE ANTHROPOCENE: THE ATMOSPHERIC COMEDY ................... 120 CODA: PLANETARY AIR ....................................................................................................... 150 BIBLIOGRAPHY ....................................................................................................................... 160 iii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I am grateful to the members of my dissertation committee for their unwavering support throughout the course of my academic progress. Bill Brown embodies the very best of the intellectual culture of the University of Chicago. He combines a big mind with a highly refined sensibility, exquisite taste -
The Amygdala, Fear and Reconsolidation
Digital Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Dissertations from the Faculty of Social Sciences 140 The Amygdala, Fear and Reconsolidation Neural and Behavioral Effects of Retrieval-Extinction in Fear Conditioning and Spider Phobia JOHANNES BJÖRKSTRAND ACTA UNIVERSITATIS UPSALIENSIS ISSN 1652-9030 ISBN 978-91-554-9863-4 UPPSALA urn:nbn:se:uu:diva-317866 2017 Dissertation presented at Uppsala University to be publicly examined in Gunnar Johansson salen, Blåsenhus, von Kraemers allé 1A, Uppsala, Friday, 12 May 2017 at 13:00 for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. The examination will be conducted in English. Faculty examiner: Emily Holmes (Karolinska institutet, Institutionen för klinisk neurovetenskap; University of Oxford, Department of Psychiatry). Abstract Björkstrand, J. 2017. The Amygdala, Fear and Reconsolidation. Neural and Behavioral Effects of Retrieval-Extinction in Fear Conditioning and Spider Phobia. Digital Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Dissertations from the Faculty of Social Sciences 140. 72 pp. Uppsala: Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis. ISBN 978-91-554-9863-4. The amygdala is crucially involved in the acquisition and retention of fear memories. Experimental research on fear conditioning has shown that memory retrieval shortly followed by pharmacological manipulations or extinction, thereby interfering with memory reconsolidation, decreases later fear expression. Fear memory reconsolidation depends on synaptic plasticity in the amygdala, which has been demonstrated in rodents using both pharmacological manipulations and retrieval-extinction procedures. The retrieval-extinction procedure decreases fear expression also in humans, but the underlying neural mechanism have not been studied. Interfering with reconsolidation is held to alter the original fear memory representation, resulting in long-term reductions in fear responses, and might therefore be used in the treatment of anxiety disorders, but few studies have directly investigated this question. -
Can Implicit Post-Event Information Influence Explicit Eyewitness Memory?
City University of New York (CUNY) CUNY Academic Works Student Theses John Jay College of Criminal Justice Spring 6-2017 Can Implicit Post-Event Information Influence Explicit yE ewitness Memory? Hang Sze Chau CUNY John Jay College, [email protected] How does access to this work benefit ou?y Let us know! More information about this work at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu/jj_etds/15 Discover additional works at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu This work is made publicly available by the City University of New York (CUNY). Contact: [email protected] Running Head: UNCONSCIOUS PROCESSING OF POST-EVENT INFORMATION 1 Can Implicit Post-Event Information Influence Explicit Eyewitness Memory? Hang-Sze Chau John Jay College of Criminal Justice City University of New York UNCONSCIOUS PROCESSING OF POST-EVENT INFORMATION 2 Table of Contents Introduction 4 Misinformation Effect 5 Source Monitoring 7 Implicit Learning 9 Methods 13 Results 19 Discussion 23 Reference 30 Appendix 37 UNCONSCIOUS PROCESSING OF POST-EVENT INFORMATION 3 Abstract This study examines whether unconscious -
Impairing Existing Declarative Memory in Humans by Disrupting Reconsolidation
Impairing existing declarative memory in humans by disrupting reconsolidation Jason C. K. Chana,b,1 and Jessica A. LaPagliaa aDepartment of Psychology and bNeuroscience Interdisciplinary Program, Iowa State University, Ames, IA 50014 Edited by Robert Desimone, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, and approved April 23, 2013 (received for review October 23, 2012) During the past decade, a large body of research has shown that through a memory test, the results almost unequivocally show memory traces can become labile upon retrieval and must be re- that retrieval actually makes the original memory less, not more, stabilized. Critically, interrupting this reconsolidation process can susceptible to interference (17–20). In sum, scant evidence exists abolish a previously stable memory. Although a large number of to support the idea that declarative memory undergoes recon- studies have demonstrated this reconsolidation associated amne- solidation upon retrieval. sia in nonhuman animals, the evidence for its occurrence in hu- Distinct neural systems subserve the formation and retrieval of mans is far less compelling, especially with regard to declarative fear, motor, and declarative memory (21). Whereas memories memory. In fact, reactivating a declarative memory often makes it acquired via fear conditioning or motor sequence learning can be more robust and less susceptible to subsequent disruptions. Here relatively localized neurologically (22), the encoding and retrieval we show that existing declarative memories can be selectively of declarative memories rely on a more distributed network (23– impaired by using a noninvasive retrieval–relearning technique. In 27). This complexity might be one reason why declarative mem- six experiments, we show that this reconsolidation-associated am- ory is particularly resistant to treatments designed to disrupt re- nesia can be achieved 48 h after formation of the original memory, consolidation. -
Remembering the Father of Cognitive Psychology - Association for Psychological Science 12-05-01 2:20 PM
Remembering the Father of Cognitive Psychology - Association for Psychological Science 12-05-01 2:20 PM Remembering the Father of Cognitive Psychology Ulric Neisser (1928-2012) Ulric (Dick) Neisser was the “father of cognitive psychology” and an advocate for ecological approaches to cognitive research. Neisser was a brilliant synthesizer of diverse thoughts and findings. He was an elegant, clear, and persuasive writer. Neisser was also a relentlessly creative researcher, constantly striving to invent methods to explore important questions. Throughout his career, Neisser remained a champion of the underdog and an unrepentant revolutionary — his goal was to push psychology in the right direction. In addition, Dick was a lifelong baseball fan, a challenging mentor, and a good friend. With the publication of Cognitive Psychology (1967), Neisser brought together research concerning perception, pattern recognition, attention, problem solving, and remembering. With his usual elegant prose, he emphasized both information processing and constructive processing. Neisser always described Cognitive Psychology as an assault on behaviorism. He was uncomfortable with behaviorism because he considered behaviorist assumptions wrong and because those assumptions limited what psychologists could study. In Cognitive Psychology, he did not explicitly attack behaviorism, but instead presented a compelling alternative. The book was immediately successful. Researchers working on problems throughout the field saw a unified theory that connected their Ulric Neisser research to this approach. Because Neisser first pulled these areas together, he was frequently referred to and introduced as the “father of cognitive psychology.” As the champion of underdogs and revolutionary approaches, however, Neisser was uncomfortable in such a role. In many ways, Cognitive Psychology was the culmination of Neisser’s own academic journey to that point.