The Effects of Interbreeding on the Morphological and Genetic Attributes of Non-Human Primates / Beth Christine Laas

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Effects of Interbreeding on the Morphological and Genetic Attributes of Non-Human Primates / Beth Christine Laas The Effects of Interbreeding on the Morphological and Genetic Attributes of Non-human Primates / Beth Christine Laas "The Effects of Interbreeding on the Morphological and Genetic Attributes of Non-human Primates" By: Beth Christine Laas Anthropology Senior Thesis Dr. Martin Nickels, Advisor Spring 2001 Abstract Primatologists have been observing interbreeding in non-human primate species since the early 1960s. Often this interspecies breeding produces a mixture of morphological and genetic features in the hybrid offspring. The purpose of this paper is to address the effects this interspecies breeding has on the morphological, fertility and viability of the offspring. This paper will analyze observational data and available species studies on interbreeding between Papio hamadryas and Papio anubis in a natural environment, as well as the mating of a gibbon and siamang and a rhesus macaque and a baboon in captive situations. It will also examine the different explanations for this interbreeding as well as what perpetuates it. Through this data collection this paper will show that the different explanations of how interbreeding is perpetuated are both integral parts of this behavior. Introduction Interbreeding between different non-human primate species has always been of interest to primatologists and geneticists alike. The results of interbreeding are offspring that are intermediate between their parents in terms of morphology, genetic traits and behavior. In this paper I will analyze the data from existing journals and species studies of a number of different primate species, including two species of baboons in the wild, as well as the interbreeding of macaque species and a gibbon and siamang in a captive situation, in order to determine the effects of hybridization on the offspring. When examining hybridization between different species, certain questions arise such as whether there is a closer resemblance to one parent or the other, or if there is a possible advantage to being a hybrid rather than a more "pure" form. There are also two prevalent explanations as to why interbreeding occurs in the wild and how it is perpetuated. http://www.soa.ilstu.edu/anthropology/theses/laas/index.htm (1 of 15) [11/30/2007 1:20:42 PM] The Effects of Interbreeding on the Morphological and Genetic Attributes of Non-human Primates / Beth Christine Laas These two ideas are presented, and their appropriateness will be determined. The purpose of this paper is to evaluate these ideas and, in a broader sense, address the overall effects of interbreeding on nonhuman primates in general. I will be looking at several different hybrid crosses in order to analyze the morphological and genetic features found in the offspring of each. Morphological, genetic and behavioral attributes found in hybrid offspring are often shown to be a mix of both parents’ features (Moore, et. al.: 1999:120). This paper will analyze these crosses and evaluate their results in order to determine which species’ are more successful and why. In addition, I will also discuss any effects interbreeding has on the physical well being of the offspring, whether positive or negative. I expect to find that the hybrid offspring produced through interbreeding in the wild will exhibit a mixture of morphological and genetic traits inherited from each parent in the cross. Literature Review Interbreeding among nonhuman primate species has been well documented in the wild between two baboon species, Papio hamadryas and P. anubis. Explanations have been proposed as to why these two species keep interbreeding. Suggested explanations why these and other primates interbreed are related to their behavioral and morphological attributes. The observation of these two species interbreeding led to study of further inter-species breeding in captivity, such as the gibbon and siamang or the baboon and rhesus monkey. Initially, the physical attributes were studied, but over time behavioral attributes, social structure, genetic traits, and adaptations were studied also. Extensive observation of the hybrid offspring of the two baboon species in the Awash Valley in Ethiopia began in the early 1970s, particularly with Nagel's fieldwork. Nagel believed the primary motivation behind the interbreeding was the abduction of anubis females by hamadryas males (Nagel: 1973). This ties into Nagel's other idea: that the hybrid zone was fixed, rather than fluctuating. Nagel did not see environmental factors as playing a part in the groups interbreeding and felt that fixed ecological barriers kept the hybrid zone strictly demarcated. Phillips-Conroy and Jolly contradicted these hypotheses. While female abduction did occur, this in and of itself was "…not frequent enough to account for such rapid changes." (Phillips-Conroy, Jolly: 1986:346). They also found in their observations that "…the hybrid zone was in flux, not static" and that it was environmental factors, such as anubis' vulnerability to drought, that http://www.soa.ilstu.edu/anthropology/theses/laas/index.htm (2 of 15) [11/30/2007 1:20:42 PM] The Effects of Interbreeding on the Morphological and Genetic Attributes of Non-human Primates / Beth Christine Laas played the primary role in these two species interbreeding (Phillips-Conroy, Jolly: 1986:346). The two groups were not necessarily restricted to one particular ecological area. Phillips-Conroy and Jolly went even beyond disputing Nagel's explanations about this interbreeding. They had their own ideas that individuals with certain traits perpetuated this inter-group breeding. They thought that individuals whose phenotype resembled the recipient population, those who were otherwise more successful at entering "foreign" populations, or those who had a lower probability of breeding in their own population were maintaining this interbreeding between groups (Phillips-Conroy, Jolly: 1991:353). As for other interspecies crosses, there can be no question about the motivation behind them because they occurred in a captive situation. The gibbon and siamang mating as well as the many attempts at intergeneric crosses between Macaca and Papio all happened in zoos and captive situations (Hill: 1970:217). Any motivation behind this interbreeding would most likely come from being held in the same area together, or through human intervention. Nagel and Phillips-Conroy and Jolly did agree on the calculation of morphological hybrid index (MHI). Using seven different external features to find the MHI, Nagel discovered that there were no "pure" groups; rather there were varying degrees of "hamadryas-like" features and "anubis-like" features (Nagel: 1973). Social structure and behavior can often be affected by interbreeding. Certain innate behaviors of one species can often become confused with those of another species in a hybrid offspring. Sugawara studied the social structure of a group of wild baboon hybrids in the same region as Nagel, Phillips-Conroy and Jolly. What Sugawara found was that while the social structure in some of the study groups was closer to that of the anubis type, it did exhibit hamadryas characteristics as well, and vice versa (Sugawara: 1979:21-3). This finding was further backed up by Phillips-Conroy, et al. in 1991, when they categorized three different types of groups, all of which displayed a certain amount of admixture (Phillips-Conroy, et. al.: 1991:355-6). There were no absolutely "pure" anubis or hamadryas groups either in a morphological or behavioral sense. As far as individuals, according to Phillips-Conroy and Jolly, there is a correlation between individual behavior and morphology. According to their 1986 article, the extent to which hybrids exhibited hamadryas-like behavior was proportional to the "hamadryas-ness" of their appearance (Phillips-Conroy, Jolly: 1986:337). The morphology and behavior of an intergeneric hybrid was described by Moore et al. (1999). The result of a Macaca mulatta x Papio hamadryas cross, this offspring exhibited external features and behaviors of both parents, as well as a vocal sound that appeared to be a combination of both species. Its head, face http://www.soa.ilstu.edu/anthropology/theses/laas/index.htm (3 of 15) [11/30/2007 1:20:42 PM] The Effects of Interbreeding on the Morphological and Genetic Attributes of Non-human Primates / Beth Christine Laas and hair color was more like that of a macaque, while the body build resembled that of a baboon (Moore et al.: 1999:126). Behaviors such as the mantle shake and eyebrow raise were the same as in a baboon, but he lacked the teeth grinding and threat display common to this species. He also displayed fewer vocalizations than both of the parent species (Moore et al. 1999:121). This hybrid is the only long-term survivor of this type of cross, and most of the other infants died within 72 hours (Moore et al.: 1999:121). A cross between a gibbon and a siamang in captivity also produced an intergeneric hybrid. In this case, the offspring was also described as intermediate between each parent. The siabon displayed features that were found in both parents as well as a few that were not found in either. Wolkin and Myers suggested that since the siabon had more features in common with the siamang, but lacked the specialized adaptations (such as the laryngeal sac and alter-developing ischial callosities), that her features are more generalized and more closely resemble an ancestral hylobatid (the siamang is considered more generalized than the gibbon [p. 217]). Hybridization not only occurs between two species in a natural environment. It also has been documented between two different genera: Papio and Macaca. These are often less successful than interspecific crosses, but there is one documented case of a "rheboon" male that survived to reproductive age. "Baboons and macaques are closely related in phylogenetic terms and are quite genetically similar as well" (Moore, et. al.: 1999:119); however, this "rheboon" was the only survivor out of twenty-six attempted crosses and the individual was found to be sterile (Moore, et.
Recommended publications
  • Targets, Tactics, and Cooperation in the Play Fighting of Two Genera of Old World Monkeys (Mandrillus and Papio): Accounting for Similarities and Differences
    2019, 32 Heather M. Hill Editor Peer-reviewed Targets, Tactics, and Cooperation in the Play Fighting of Two Genera of Old World Monkeys (Mandrillus and Papio): Accounting for Similarities and Differences Kelly L. Kraus1, Vivien C. Pellis1, and Sergio M. Pellis1 1 Department of Neuroscience, University of Lethbridge, Canada Play fighting in many species involves partners competing to bite one another while avoiding being bitten. Species can differ in the body targets that are bitten and the tactics used to attack and defend those targets. However, even closely related species that attack and defend the same body target using the same tactics can differ markedly in how much the competitiveness of such interactions is mitigated by cooperation. A degree of cooperation is necessary to ensure that some turn-taking between the roles of attacker and defender occurs, as this is critical in preventing play fighting from escalating into serious fighting. In the present study, the dyadic play fighting of captive troops of 4 closely related species of Old World monkeys, 2 each from 2 genera of Papio and Mandrillus, was analyzed. All 4 species have a comparable social organization, are large bodied with considerable sexual dimorphism, and are mostly terrestrial. In all species, the target of biting is the same – the area encompassing the upper arm, shoulder, and side of the neck – and they have the same tactics of attack and defense. However, the Papio species exhibit more cooperation in their play than do the Mandrillus species, with the former using tactics that make biting easier to attain and that facilitate close bodily contact.
    [Show full text]
  • HAMADRYAS BABOON (Papio Hamadryas) CARE MANUAL
    HAMADRYAS BABOON (Papio hamadryas) CARE MANUAL CREATED BY THE AZA Hamadryas Baboon Species Survival Plan® Program IN ASSOCIATION WITH THE AZA Old World Monkey Taxon Advisory Group Hamadryas Baboon (Papio hamadryas) Care Manual Hamadryas Baboon (Papio hamadryas) Care Manual Published by the Association of Zoos and Aquariums in collaboration with the AZA Animal Welfare Committee Formal Citation: AZA Baboon Species Survival Plan®. (2020). Hamadryas Baboon Care Manual. Silver Spring, MD: Association of Zoos and Aquariums. Original Completion Date: July 2020 Authors and Significant Contributors: Jodi Neely Wiley, AZA Hamadryas Baboon SSP Coordinator and Studbook Keeper, North Carolina Zoo Margaret Rousser, Oakland Zoo Terry Webb, Toledo Zoo Ryan Devoe, Disney Animal Kingdom Katie Delk, North Carolina Zoo Michael Maslanka, Smithsonian National Zoological Park and Conservation Biology Institute Reviewers: Joe Knobbe, San Francisco Zoological Gardens, former Old World Monkey TAG Chair, SSP Vice Coordinator Hamadryas Baboon AZA Staff Editors: Felicia Spector, Animal Care Manual Editor Consultant Candice Dorsey, PhD, Senior Vice President, Conservation, Management, & Welfare Sciences Rebecca Greenberg, Animal Programs Director Emily Wagner, Conservation Science & Education Intern Raven Spencer, Conservation, Management, & Welfare Sciences Intern Hana Johnstone, Conservation, Management, & Welfare Sciences Intern Cover Photo Credits: Jodi Neely Wiley, North Carolina Zoo Disclaimer: This manual presents a compilation of knowledge provided by recognized animal experts based on the current science, practice, and technology of animal management. The manual assembles basic requirements, best practices, and animal care recommendations to maximize capacity for excellence in animal care and welfare. The manual should be considered a work in progress, since practices continue to evolve through advances in scientific knowledge.
    [Show full text]
  • The Consequences of Habitat Loss and Fragmentation on the Distribution, Population Size, Habitat Preferences, Feeding and Rangin
    The consequences of habitat loss and fragmentation on the distribution, population size, habitat preferences, feeding and ranging Ecology of grivet monkey (Cercopithecus aethiopes aethiops) on the human dominated habitats of north Shoa, Amhara, Ethiopia: A Study of human-grivet monkey conflict 1 Table of contents Page 1. Introduction 1 1.1. Background And Justifications 3 1.2. Statement Of The Problem 6 1.3. Objectives 7 1.3.1. General Objective 7 1.3.2. Specific Objectives 8 1.4. Research Hypotheses Under Investigation 8 2. Description Of The Study Area 8 3. Methodology 11 3.1. Habitat Stratification, Vegetation Mapping And Land Use Cover 11 Change 3.2. Distribution Pattern And Population Estimate Of Grivet Monkey 11 3.3. Behavioral Data 12 3.4. Human Grivet Monkey Conflict 15 3.5. Habitat Loss And Fragmentation 15 4. Expected Output 16 5. Challenges Of The Project 16 6. References 17 i 1. Introduction World mammals status analysis on global scale shows that primates are the most threatened mammals (Schipper et al., 2008) making them indicators for investigating vulnerability to threats. Habitat loss and destruction are often considered to be the most serious threat to many tropical primate populations because of agricultural expansion, livestock grazing, logging, and human settlement (Cowlishaw and Dunbar, 2000). Deforestation and forest fragmentation have marched together with the expansion of agricultural frontiers, resulting in both habitat loss and subdivision of the remaining habitat (Michalski and Peres, 2005). This forest degradation results in reduction in size or fragmentation of the original forest habitat (Fahrig, 2003). Habitat fragmentation is often defined as a process during which “a large expanse of habitat is transformed into a number of smaller patches of smaller total area, isolated from each other by a matrix of habitats unlike the original”.
    [Show full text]
  • Papio Hamadryas Hamadryas) Melissa C
    Bucknell University Bucknell Digital Commons Master’s Theses Student Theses Spring 2018 Social Relationships and Self-Directed Behavior in Hamadryas Baboons (Papio hamadryas hamadryas) Melissa C. Painter Bucknell University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.bucknell.edu/masters_theses Part of the Animal Studies Commons, and the Comparative Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Painter, Melissa C., "Social Relationships and Self-Directed Behavior in Hamadryas Baboons (Papio hamadryas hamadryas)" (2018). Master’s Theses. 200. https://digitalcommons.bucknell.edu/masters_theses/200 This Masters Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Theses at Bucknell Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Master’s Theses by an authorized administrator of Bucknell Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. I, Melissa Painter, do grant permission for my thesis to be copied. ii Acknowledgments I would like to thank my committee members, Dr. Z Morgan Benowitz-Fredericks and Dr. Regina Gazes, for their time, advice and support throughout the planning and execution of this project. I would also like to thank Meredith Lutz and Corinne Leard, both recent B.S. graduates from the Animal Behavior Program, for their assistance in navigating R to standardize my data and calculate the baboons’ dominance hierarchy. Thank you also to the caretakers at Bucknell’s Animal Behavior Laboratory, Mary Gavitt, Amber Hackenberg and Gretchen Long, for taking such great care of my study group and for being so accommodating when I asked to spend hours of undisturbed time with the baboons. Of course, I must also thank my fellow graduate students for enduring my multiple proposal and defense presentation rehearsals.
    [Show full text]
  • Female Reproductive Strategies and the Ovarian Cycle in Hamadryas
    Femalereproductivestrategiesandtheovarian cycleinhamadryasbaboons AthesissubmittedinpartialrequirementoftheMaster’sDegreeof ScienceinConservationBiologyatVictoriaUniversityofWellington by R.E.Tobler ABSTRACT: Thisthesisexaminestherelationshipbetweensexualbehaviourandthe ovarian cycle in a group-living primate, Papio h. hamadryas . Of particular interest is whetherfemalesmodifytheirovariancycleinamannerthatisexpectedincreasetheir reproductivesuccess.Thestudywasconductedonacaptivecolonywheretheresident males (RM) had been vasectomised prior to start of the study resulting in all mature femalesundergoingrepeatedovariancyclingthroughoutthestudyperiod.Thismadethe analysisofsexualbehaviourrelativetofinescalechangesintheovariancyclepossible. One year of ovarian cycle data and 280 hours of behavioural data was collected via observationalsamplingduringthestudy.RMvasectomisationdidnotalterthearchetypal one male unit social structure nor the typical socio-spatial organisation of wild hamadryas populations. Females were found to be more promiscuous than in wild populations, however, presumably because of the confounding effect that the high number of simultaneously cycling females had on RM herding (Chapter 1). RMs dominatedcopulationsovertheoptimalconceptiveperiodoftheovariancycle,whilethe majorityofextra-OMUcopulationsoccurredoutsidethisperiodandwererarelysolicited by females. This pattern supports a dual paternity concentration/paternity confusion strategy,andnotfemalechoiceorfertilityinsurancestrategies(Chapter2).Femaleswere not found to synchronise
    [Show full text]
  • Intellectualism and Interesting Facts on Baboons (Papio Anubis Les.; Family: Cercopithecidae) (The Olive Baboons) in Yankari Game Reserve, Bauchi, Nigeria
    International Journal of Research Studies in Zoology (IJRSZ) Volume 3, Issue 2, 2017, PP 51-55 ISSN 2454-941X http://dx.doi.org/10.20431/2454-941X.0302004 www.arcjournals.org Intellectualism and Interesting Facts on Baboons (Papio anubis Les.; Family: Cercopithecidae) (the olive baboons) in Yankari Game Reserve, Bauchi, Nigeria Ukwubile Cletus Anes Department of Science Laboratory Technology, Biology Unit, School of Science and Technology, Federal Polytechnic Bali, Nigeria. Abstract: Baboons are the type of monkey that are found in African forests and the Arabians. There are five species of baboons worldwide which are distributed in different habitats such as tropical rainforests, savannas, open woodlands and semi-arid areas. A close observation made on baboons at the Yankari Game Reserve(YGR), showed that they feed on various foods which are the reason they are known as pests as well as they are scavengers on elephant's dung. Apart from poaching activity by humans, baboons at YGR are also threatened by loss of habitat due to regular predatory activity by a lion (Pathera leo) in the reserve on the baboons. Out of the five species, only one species (Papio anubis) are found in the large population at the Yankari Game Reserve. This increase in the population of the olive baboons at the YGR has become a source of concern to tourists and researchers who visit the reserve. Due to frequent visits by people to the reserve, the baboons has developed a high level of intellectualism as well as tricks to overcome the dominance by humans encroaching their habitat. Some of these behaviours are groupings, pretence, and acrobatics.
    [Show full text]
  • Theropithecus Gelada) on an Intact Afro-Alpine Grassland at Guassa, Ethiopia ______
    LONG-TERM RANGING PATTERNS OF WILD GELADA MONKEYS (THEROPITHECUS GELADA) ON AN INTACT AFRO-ALPINE GRASSLAND AT GUASSA, ETHIOPIA ____________________________________ A Thesis Presented to the Faculty of California State University, Fullerton ____________________________________ In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts in Anthropology ____________________________________ By Cha Moua Thesis Committee Approval: Associate Professor Peter J. Fashing, Chair Associate Professor Nga Nguyen, Department of Anthropology Associate Professor Elizabeth G. Pillsworth, Department of Anthropology Fall, 2015 ABSTRACT Long-term studies of animal ranging ecology are critical to understanding how animals utilize their habitat across space and time. Although gelada monkeys (Theropithecus gelada) inhabit an unusual, high altitude habitat that presents unique ecological challenges, no long-term studies of their ranging behavior have been conducted. To close this gap, I investigated the daily path length (DPL), annual home ranges (95%), and annual core areas (50%) of a band of ~220 wild gelada monkeys at Guassa, Ethiopia, from January 2007 to December 2011 (for total of n = 785 full-day follows). I estimated annual home ranges and core area using the fixed kernel reference (FK REF) and smoothed cross-validation (FK SCV) bandwidths, and the minimum convex polygon (MCP) method. Both annual home range (MCP - 2007: 5.9 km2; 2008: 8.6 km2; 2009: 9.2 km2; 2010: 11.5 km2; 2011: 11.6 km2) and core area increased over the 5-year study period. The MCP and FK REF generated broadly consistent, though slightly larger estimates that contained areas in which the geladas were never observed. All three methods omitted one to 19 sleeping sites from the home range depending on the year.
    [Show full text]
  • Primate Cards
    #1 Agile Gibbon Hylobates agilis The agile gibbon, also known as the black- Distribution handed gibbon, is an Old World primate found in Indonesia on the island of Sumatra, Malaysia, and southern Thailand. They are an endangered species due to habitat destruction and the pet trade. They use their long arms to swing quickly from branch to branch (called “brachiating) and eat primarily fruit supplemented with leaves, flowers and insects. They live in monogamous pairs and raise their young for at least two years. #2 Allen's Swamp Monkey Allenopithecus nigroviridis Distribution The Allen's swamp monkey is an Old World primate that lives in swampy areas of central Africa. They can swim well, including diving to avoid danger from predators like raptors and snakes. Allen's swamp monkeys feed mostly on the ground and eat fruits, leaves, beetles and worms. They live together in large social groups of up to 40 individuals, and they communicate with each other using different calls, gestures and touches. They are hunted for their meat and are increasingly seen as household pets. #3 Angola Colobus Colobus angolensis The Angola colobus is an Old World primate that lives in rainforests along the Congo River in Distribution Burundi, Uganda, and parts of Kenya and Tanzania. They eat mostly leaves, supplemented with fruit and seeds. They are known as sloppy eaters, which together with their digestive system makes them important for seed dispersal. They live in groups of about 9 individuals, with a single dominant male and multiple females and their offspring. Females in the group are known to co-parent each others’ young, which are born completely white.
    [Show full text]
  • A Comparison of the Subspecific Divergence
    TITLE PAGE A COMPARISON OF THE SUBSPECIFIC DIVERGENCE OF TWO SYMPATRIC AFRICAN MONKEYS, PAPIO HAMADRYAS & CHLOROCEBUS AETHIOPS: MORPHOLOGY, ENVIRONMENT, DIET & PHYLOGENY BY JASON DUNN BSC HONS (ST AND.) PGCERT (HYMS) A THESIS SUBMITTED IN SATISFACTION OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR INCEPTION TO THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY IN THE HULL YORK MEDICAL SCHOOL SEPTEMBER 2011 THE UNIVERSITY OF HULL THE UNIVERSITY OF YORK THE HULL YORK MEDICAL SCHOOL 2 of 301 ABSTRACT The baboon and vervet monkey exhibit numerous similarities in geographic range, ecology and social structure, and both exhibit extensive subspecific variation corresponding to geotypic forms. This thesis compares these two subspecific radiations, using skull morphology to characterise the two taxa, and attempts to determine if the two have been shaped by similar selective forces. The baboon exhibits clinal variation corresponding to decreasing size from Central to East Africa, like the vervet. However West African baboons are small, unlike the vervet. Much of the shape variation in baboons is size-related. Controlling for this reveals a north-south pattern of shape change corresponding to phylogenetic history. There are significant differences between the chacma and olive baboon subspecies in the proportion of subterranean foods in the diet. No dietary differences were detected between vervet subspecies. Baboon dietary variation was found to covary significantly with skull variation. However, no biomechanical adaptation was detected, suggesting morphological constraint owing to the recent divergence between subspecies. Phylogeny correlates with morphology to reveal an axis between northern and southern taxa in baboons. In vervets C. a. sabaeus is the most morphologically divergent, which with other evidence, suggests a West African origin and radiation east and south, in contrast with a baboon origin in southern Africa.
    [Show full text]
  • Final Report on Environment Enhancement to Promote the Psychological Well-Being of Nonhuman Primates
    Enviromental enhancement -- Non -Human Primates FINAL REPORT ON ENVIRONMENT ENHANCEMENT TO PROMOTE THE PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING OF NONHUMAN PRIMATES U. S. Department of Agriculture Animal and Plant Health Inspection Service Animal Care Riverdale, MD July 15, 1999 This Final Report contains the scientific basis for the Draft Policy and the methods we used in developing the Draft Policy. To view the Draft Policy and request for comments that was published in the Federal Register on July 15, 1999, click on Pdf TABLE OF CONTENTS I. INTRODUCTION AND PROJECT HISTORY A. Background on Evaluation of the Performance-Based Standard for Nonhuman Primates B. Team Methods C. Results of Surveys and Interviews II. PROMOTING PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING A. Intent and Language of the Animal Welfare Act B. Community Response C. Other Nations and Societies D. Difficulties Inherent in Measuring Psychological Well-being E. Species-Typical Behavior (STB) III. CRITICAL ELEMENT CONCEPT IV. LITERATURE REVIEW AND DISCUSSION A. Social Grouping B. Social Needs of Infants C. Structure and Substrate D. Foraging Opportunities E. Manipulanda F. Consideration of Sensory Stimulation G. Consideration of Novelty and Control V. REFERENCES AND OTHER SOURCES http://www.nal.usda.gov/awic/enrichment/Enviromental_Enhancement_NonHuman_Primates.htm[8/6/2015 1:02:44 PM] Enviromental enhancement -- Non -Human Primates A. References B. Other Sources APPENDIX A. 9 CFR Section 3. Environment Enhancement to Promote Psychological Well-Being of Nonhuman Primates APPENDIX B. Glossary APPENDIX C. Sample Species Information Sheets I. INTRODUCTION AND PROJECT HISTORY This report provides Animal and Plant Health Inspection Service (APHIS) Animal Care employees, the facilities they regulate, and the public with a policy on environment enhancement to promote the psychological well-being of nonhuman primates.
    [Show full text]
  • Baboons, Drills and Mandrills
    Baboons, Drills And Mandrills Baboons– are terrestrial monkeys found in open or rocky areas, including open woodland, savannah, grassland, and rocky hills in Africa. They have doglike faces and walk on all fours. Baboons are boisterous, cunning, and often fierce animals that have been known to raid human settlements for food and occasionally attack humans to defend themselves. Baboons are very smart and noisy. Five species are known; the hamadryas baboon; the western baboon; the olive baboon; (known to the ancient Egyptians as Anubis); the yellow baboon; the chacma baboon. Baboons are among the largest of the monkeys. Depending on the species, baboons measure between 20 and 45 inches in body length and weigh between 30 and 88 pounds. Olive baboon Hamadryas Baboon- female Olive baboons live in groups or "troops" as they are often called, ranging in size from 15 to 150 baboons. The friendships between boys and girl baboons include relaxed grooming sessions, traveling and hunting for food together during the day, sleeping near each other, help defend them from bullies, and help each other in caring for babies. When two boys meet each other for the first time, the greeting begins when one male approaches another with a rapid wal , looking directly at the other male, smacking his lips, squinting, laying his ears flat against his head, and finally showing his bottom. One unusual thing seen in young olive baboons in Nigeria is their ability to swim and dive. They have been seen swimming with their faces under water in a river, and diving from trees that hang over the river.
    [Show full text]
  • Baboons: Possible Relationships to Life History and Behavior Jay R
    Cerebrospinal Fluid Monoaminergic Metabolites Differ in Wild Anubis and Hybrid (Anubis hamadryas) Baboons: Possible Relationships to Life History and Behavior Jay R. Kaplan, Ph.D., Jane Phillips-Conroy, Ph.D., M. Babette Fontenot, D.V.M., M.S., Ph.D., Clifford J. Jolly, Ph.D., Lynn A. Fairbanks, Ph.D., and J. John Mann, M.D. This article reports monoaminergic metabolite males characterized by hamadryas admixture had [homovanillic acid (HVA), 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5- significantly higher concentrations of HVA, 5-HIAA, and HIAA), and 3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol (MHPG)], MHPG (p , .05, respectively), a result possibly driven by values from the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) of 27 wild differences in serotonergic activity. These data provide baboons (Papio hamadryas) aged 40 to 140 months. initial evidence that variation in central monoaminergic Animals were either anubis, or anubis with hamadryas activity, as indicated by CSF monoamine metabolite admixture; males of the latter subspecies generally have a concentrations, may reflect differences in behavior and life reduced tendency to disperse from their natal groups. history that have taxonomic and, perhaps, evolutionary Overall, the values and interrelationships among the CSF significance. [Neuropsychopharmacology 20: 517–524, monoamine metabolites resembled data reported from 1999] © 1999 American College of closely related, captive-housed animals. For example, age Neuropsychopharmacology. Published by Elsevier was significantly correlated with HVA concentrations (r 5 Science Inc. 260, p , .05), but not with the other metabolites. Notably, KEY WORDS: Primates; Serotonin; Dopamine; There is increasing interest in the neurobiologic factors Norepinephrine; CSF underlying variation in social behavior, both among in- dividuals and between species (Clarke and Boinski 1995; Young et al.
    [Show full text]