The Genesis of Music and Language Author(S): Bryan G. Levman Source: Ethnomusicology, Vol

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Genesis of Music and Language Author(S): Bryan G. Levman Source: Ethnomusicology, Vol The Genesis of Music and Language Author(s): Bryan G. Levman Source: Ethnomusicology, Vol. 36, No. 2, (Spring - Summer, 1992), pp. 147-170 Published by: University of Illinois Press on behalf of Society for Ethnomusicology Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/851912 Accessed: 15/07/2008 17:11 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=illinois. Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. JSTOR is a not-for-profit organization founded in 1995 to build trusted digital archives for scholarship. We work with the scholarly community to preserve their work and the materials they rely upon, and to build a common research platform that promotes the discovery and use of these resources. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. http://www.jstor.org VOL.36, No. 2 ETHNOMUSICOLOGY SPRING/SUMMER1992 The Genesis of Music and Language BRYANG. LEVMAN TORONTO,ONTARIO his article proposes to evince evidence in support of the hypothesis that language and music evolved out of a common "proto-faculty"which was primarilymusical in nature.1Because the subject of musical genesis and the relationship of music and language are topics which have been very controversial, it may well help to start by orienting the reader with a brief survey of the literature in the field. Authors have adopted three primary positions: (1) that language and music developed along separate paths and are in effect two completely different faculties, (2) that music developed out of language, or at least was chronologically later than language, and (3) that language developed out of music, or both developed from a common "proto-faculty." Implicit in the work of most glosso-geneticists2 is position one. This may be because these scholars-usually linguists, anthropologists, psycholo- gists, philosophers-know little about music, for though they often make the point that language can carry semantic meaning in intonation, most do not posit an evolutionary connection. In fact the vast majorityof investigators do not even consider the possible role of music in language development. Instead they see language as (1) primarilygestural in origin, either through bodily movements (Hewes 1983) or through the manipulation of the mouth and vocal tract as the gestural instrument (Foster 1983; Pulleyblank 1983); (2) the result of a primitive representational system which primeval humans developed to help navigate the environment (Bickerton 1990); (3) generated by forces for social cohesion and development like the need to communicate between intermarryinggroups and the discovery of tool-making technolo- gies (Livingstone 1983); (4) fundamentally mimetic, in that primitive humans imitated sounds in their environment (Plato 1937), perhaps to assure greater success in hunting (Fischer 1983); (5) fundamentally emotive and affective, with the first word-sounds originating in instinctive expressive declarations of species identification, warning calls, cries for help, and so forth (Von Raffler-Engel 1983). As will be demonstrated below, some of these views ? 1992 Board of Trustees of the University of Illinois 148 Ethnomusicology, Spring/Summer 1992 have important implications for the relationship of language and music; however, few scholars make an explicit connection between the two. One anthropologist (Livingstone 1973:25) has suggested that humans "could sing long before they could talk and that singing was in fact a prerequisite to speech and hence language,"but he laterrepudiated this position (1983:180); and two linguists, after detailed comparisons of the emotional patterns in speech intonation and music, have postulated a common origin for both faculties (Fonagy and Magdics 1963). There is also a significant body of literaturein the fields of ethology and child psychology which demonstrates the importance of articulated tone in communication and language acqui- sition which this paper will examine in support of its thesis. Position two, that music evolved out of language, was first promulgated by the sociologist Herbert Spencer (1857). His view was that the distinctive traits of song were simply the traits of emotional speech intensified and systematized. Thus the pitch, intervals, loudness, timbre, and rate of variation of the voice-all the modulating factors of the emotions-become exaggerated and transformed by the force of great emotion into song. Presumably, Spencer's position was influenced by Darwin's view that music originated from the love calls of primates during courtship; Darwin himself disagreed with Spencer's interpretations,maintaining the more narrow view that "musical notes and rhythm were first acquired by the male or female progenitors of mankind for the sake of charming the opposite sex" ([1871]1981 vol. 2:336), and that music then became firmly associated with the passions. Darwin thought that music probably came before language (ibid.:337), although he did not draw an explicit evolutionary link between the two faculties. Musicologists of the nineteenth and twentieth centuries adopt a variety of different views, though most seem to subscribe to position one or two: that music and language were separate faculties or that music developed out of language. Richard Wallaschek, for example, believes that music arose from a primary rhythmic impulse in humans which was first manifested in dance-play, the result of a "surplusvigour" exceeding the energies required or immediate needs (1891:375-76). In his view neither speech nor music originated one from the other, but both arose from an identical primitive stage (ibid.:383). ErnstNewman also asserts that music was independent of speech in origin, and maintains that humans possess a musical faculty that must have existed much earlier than speech in the order of time: "man certainly expressed his feelings in pure indefinite sound long before he had learned to agree with his fellows to attach certain meanings to certain stereotyped sounds" ([1905]1969:210-11). In examining the various theories of music origin up to 1930, musicologist Siegfried Nadel also rejects the Darwinian and Spencerian positions out of hand. Both Karl Bucher's view The Genesis of Music and Language 149 that music developed out of concerted rhythmical labour and CarlStumpfs position that music originated as a more efficient "acoustic sign language" for long distance communication are discussed and dismissed. Nadel's view is that music is an out-of-the-ordinary, supernatural language which has been superadded to speech by a process of what he calls "transference"- emotional experience carried over into artisticexpression. Music is therefore primarilya language of the gods and the demons, a language of invocation and exorcism, and Nadel makes the important point of the ubiquity of religious and ritualisticsongs in tribalcultures (1930). Among contemporary scholars, C. M. Bowra, echoing Wallaschek, believes that music was first manifested in the dance and that song developed by fitting standardized, formulaic sequences of (speech) sounds to pre-existing melodies (1962). Although music was post-linguistic, its motivation was primarily rhythmic, and only secondarily vocal (Wallaschek 1891, 1893). Curt Sachs originally suggested that music may have originated from speech or from emotion, calling the resulting styles logogenic and pathogenic (1943:41), but he later seems to change these views (1965:38). His dominant attitude is ironic detachment, rejecting all theories of musical origin as wrong or unprovable, and preferring to concentrate on primitive musics which are accessible for study (1948:1-2). This view is also shared byJohn Blacking who believes that music is a species-specific biological human impulse, separate from lan- guage, which is inseparable from the social context in which it develops (1973:55). Bruno Nettl hypothesizes that at one time humans had a kind of communication that shared elements of both language and music, and that the two articulatorymedia of contrastive vowels and pitches eventually took divergent evolutionary paths (1956:136-37, 1983:166). The evolution of music from speech is a position that is also attributed to Jean-Jacques Rousseau (Sachs 1943:19; Nadel 1930:535); however his view is closer to Nettl's, that music and speech have a common origin, a faculty which he believed to be primarily musical in nature. For Rousseau, the first words expressed the feelings of love, hate, pity, and anger, so that language was originally vital, singable, and passionate before it became simple and methodical ([1761]1966:12,chapter two). Primitivelanguage was sung, not spoken, its accents (pitch), quantity, and rhythm articulating the passions in an imitative, iconic fashion (ibid.:15, chapter four). Eventually language becomes more regular and less passionate, substituting ideas for feelings. Accent diminishes and consonantal articulation increases:
Recommended publications
  • PETER MARLER 24 February 1928 . 5 July 2014
    PETER MARLER BETHANY DANIELS / COLLEGE OF BIOLOGICAL SCIENCES UC DAVIS 24 february 1928 . 5 july 2014 PROCEEDINGS OF THE AMERICAN PHILOSOPHICAL SOCIETY VOL. 160, NO. 4, DECEMBER 2016 Marler.indd 437 11/30/2016 12:39:58 PM biographical memoirs ETER MARLER died on 5 July 2014. He pioneered the study of animal communication, which until his involvement had been P rather anecdotal. He set up a logical framework that looked at all aspects of the signals used (i.e., their physical structure, who produced them, in what context, and what responses they elicited). Answers to all of these questions then helped researchers infer what information the signals conveyed as well as the intention, or at any rate the benefit to the signaler. In the case of a male songbird singing in the spring, the message was species identity, individual identity, breeding condition, and marital status (e.g., single, shall welcome a mate). To other males, it stated, “This is my territory and I shall defend it.” In this manner, the same message could mean different things to different listeners. Marler came upon this area of interest while working on his first Ph.D. (University College, London, 1952), which involved vegetation surveys of potential nature reserves in Britain, France, and the Azores. In his spare time, he collected recordings of chaffinch song from the same localities. It was while doing this work that he realized the variability in chaffinch song was not random. For example, the song of chaffinches in the Azores was simpler than that in Britain or France.
    [Show full text]
  • Masakazu Konishi
    Masakazu Konishi BORN: Kyoto, Japan February 17, 1933 EDUCATION: Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan, B.S. (1956) Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan, M.S. (1958) University of California, Berkeley, Ph.D. (1963) APPOINTMENTS: Postdoctoral Fellow, University of Tübingen, Germany (1963–1964) Postdoctoral Fellow, Division of Experimental Neurophysiology, Max-Planck Institut, Munich, Germany (1964–1965) Assistant Professor of Biology, University of Wisconsin, Madison (1965–1966) Assistant Professor of Biology, Princeton University (1966–1970) Associate Professor of Biology, Princeton University (1970–1975) Professor of Biology, California Institute of Technology (1975– 1980) Bing Professor of Behavioral Biology, California Institute of Technology (1980– ) HONORS AND AWARDS (SELECTED): Member, American Academy of Arts and Sciences (1979) Member, National Academy of Sciences (1985) President, International Society for Neuroethology (1986—1989) F. O. Schmitt Prize (1987) International Prize for Biology (1990) The Lewis S. Rosenstiel Award, Brandeis University (2004) Edward M. Scolnick Prize in Neuroscience, MIT (2004) Gerard Prize, the Society for Neuroscience (2004) Karl Spencer Lashley Award, The American Philosophical Society (2004) The Peter and Patricia Gruber Prize in Neuroscience, The Society for Neuroscience (2005) Masakazu (Mark) Konishi has been one of the leaders in avian neuroethology since the early 1960’s. He is known for his idea that young birds initially remember a tutor song and use the memory as a template to guide the development of their own song. He was the fi rst to show that estrogen prevents programmed cell death in female zebra fi nches. He also pioneered work on the brain mechanisms of sound localization by barn owls. He has trained many students and postdoctoral fellows who became leading neuroethologists.
    [Show full text]
  • Peter Marler (1928–2014) Pioneering Interpreter of Animal Language
    COMMENT OBITUARY Peter Marler (1928–2014) Pioneering interpreter of animal language. hite-crowned sparrows to Japan to study macaques. Marler sing in distinct dialects himself spent time studying colobus in different parts of Cali- monkeys in Uganda and then, with Wfornia. African vervet monkeys use primatologist Jane Goodall, the social various alarm calls to signal different behaviour of chimpanzees in Tanza- dangers, such as snakes, mammalian nia. He had hoped to glean insights predators or birds of prey. Peter Robert on human language. He learned much Marler chronicled these phenomena about the signals that chimpanzees to establish ideas about how animals use, but felt that human language communicate. He hoped to find clues remained in its own class. about the biology of human language. Marler astutely realized that if he OF ROCKEFELLER ARCHIVE CENTER COURTESY How does language acquisition blend shifted his focus from language to innate knowledge and learning? vocal learning, then birds had much Marler, who died on 5 July, first to offer. Early on, he noticed that became interested in animal sounds songbirds were picky about what as a doctoral student in botany at sounds they imitated, and that choice University College London. Survey- often occurred during the first year ing potential nature reserves in Scot- of life. Moreover, the stages of vocal land, UK, he noticed that the song of learning were set. This explained why the chaffinch Fringilla coelebs changed from I was enthralled when I first heard Marler the dialects of his chaffinches disappeared valley to valley. This led to a second PhD, in lecture at Berkeley.
    [Show full text]
  • Vol. 16, No.3 August, 1971 NEWSLETTER ANIMAL BEHAVIOR
    Vol. 16, No.3 August, 1971 NEWSLETTER ANIMAL BEHAVIOR SOCIETY SECTION (DIVISION) ANIMAL BEHAVIOR AND SOCIOBIOLOGY Benjamin Dane, Editor Ecological Society of America Department of Biology, Tufts University American Society of Zoologists Medford, Massachusetts 02155 1971 AAAS MEETINGS: The 1971 AAAS meeting will be held on December 26 through 31 at Philadelphia. The deadline for submitting papers for this meeting is September 1, 1971. Please read and abide by the instructions on the ASZ Transmittal Form enclosed with this newsletter. The August Newsletter is being published early to ensure adequate time for the members to prepare and submit abstracts. Normally the transmittal forms are included with the May newsletter, but the forms were not printed in time this year. ABS will sponsor a symposium "Parental Ontogeny of Behavior" arranged by Gilbert Gottlieb, at this meeting. Do not forget to send the transmittal form to the Program Officer: C. Richard Terman, Department of Biology, William and Mary College, Williams­ burg, Va. 23185 by September 1, 1971. DELAY IN PUBLICATION OF ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR: Paul Siegel, Treasurer, has instructed me to inform the membership of the delay in receiving copies of ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR. The publishers are many months behind in publication due at least partially to the British mail strike. SECOND NATIONAL BIOLOGICAL CONGRESS: The Second National Biological Con­ gress will be held 23-26 October, 1971, at the Fountainebleau Hotel, Miami Beach, Florida. The theme of the congress follows that of the first, Man and Environment II. ANIMAL BEHAVIOR FILMS: Edwin Gould, Chairman of the Film Committee, has sent the following information about Animal Behavior Films.
    [Show full text]
  • Peter Marler Correspondence D-483
    http://oac.cdlib.org/findaid/ark:/13030/c88k7ddv No online items Inventory of the Peter Marler Correspondence D-483 University of California, Davis Library, Dept. of Special Collections 1st Floor, Shields Library, University of California 100 North West Quad Davis, CA 95616-5292 [email protected] URL: https://www.library.ucdavis.edu/archives-and-special-collections Inventory of the Peter Marler D-483 1 Correspondence D-483 Language of Material: English Contributing Institution: University of California, Davis Library, Dept. of Special Collections Title: Peter Marler Correspondence Creator: Marler, Peter. Identifier/Call Number: D-483 Physical Description: 20 linear feet Date (inclusive): 1960-2008 Abstract: Correspondence relating to Professor of Neurobiology, Physiology, and Behavior Peter Marler's research on animal communication and his involvement with professional organizations. Researchers should contact Archives and Special Collections to request collections, as many are stored offsite. Biography Professor of Neurobiology, Physiology, and Behavior, University of California, Davis (1989-1994). Scope and Contents Correspondence relating to Marler's research on animal communication and his involvement with professional organizations. Access Collection is open for research. Processing Information Liz Phillips encoded this finding aid with help from student assistant Aditi Sinha. Preferred Citation [Identification of item], Peter Marler correspondence, D-483, Archives and Special Collections, UC Davis Library, University of California, Davis. Publication Rights All applicable copyrights for the collection are protected under chapter 17 of the U.S. Copyright Code. Requests for permission to publish or quote from manuscripts must be submitted in writing to the Head of Special Collections. Permission for publication is given on behalf of the Regents of the University of California as the owner of the physical items.
    [Show full text]
  • Environmental Influences: Biology and Behavior Series
    ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES THIRD OF A SERIES ON BIOLOGY AND BEHAVIOR COPYRIGHT@ 196 8 BY THE ROCKEFELLER UNIVERSITY PRESS AND RUSSELL SAGE FOUNDATION LIBRARY OF CONGRESS CATALOG CARD NUMBER 68-56103 PRINTED. IN THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA Preface THERE IS a marked and growing concern among social scientists about the effects of environmental factors on biological mechanisms of behavior. One example is the recent work with small mammals on environmental stress and adrenal and gonadal functioning. Corre­ spondingly, many biological scientists call attention to the fact that a comprehensive human biology cannot omit one of man's more striking characteristics-his social nature. Although the interdepend­ ence of the organism and its environment has long been recognized, the systematic study of environmental effects on biological function­ ing and the behavioral consequences has received less attention than it deserves. On April 21-22, 1967, Russell Sage Foundation and The Rockefeller University sponsored a conference on environmental in­ fluences on behavior. The meetings were held in Caspary Audi­ torium on the Rockefeller campus in New York City. This was the last in a series of three conferences on biology and behavior. The aim of the series was to strengthen the dialogue between the biological and social sciences, and to stimulate a rapprochement between the two disciplines in order that future work in each field might be un­ dertaken in fuller recognition of the other. The present volume contains fifteen papers that were delivered at the two-day conference. The first volume was published in the fall of 1967 and dealt with the topic of neurophysiology and emotion.
    [Show full text]
  • Birdman: [Dr. Peter Marler] Fulvio Bardossi
    Rockefeller University Digital Commons @ RU Rockefeller University Research Profiles Campus Publications Winter 1980 Birdman: [Dr. Peter Marler] Fulvio Bardossi Judith N. Schwartz Follow this and additional works at: http://digitalcommons.rockefeller.edu/research_profiles Part of the Life Sciences Commons Recommended Citation Bardossi, Fulvio and Schwartz, Judith N., "Birdman: [Dr. Peter Marler]" (1980). Rockefeller University Research Profiles. Book 1. http://digitalcommons.rockefeller.edu/research_profiles/1 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Campus Publications at Digital Commons @ RU. It has been accepted for inclusion in Rockefeller University Research Profiles by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons @ RU. For more information, please contact [email protected]. THE ROCKEFELLER UNIVERSITY ~ \1\1 \1\1 \~\ \~I \~ \~ RESEARCH --..._-- --\\1 \1\1 \~I \~ \~ PROFILES WINTER 1980 mingbirds-like human beings and apparently unlike any Bird111an other animals studied thus far-do learn their language. That is, they must hear and memorize the distinctive songs of their "What is so exciting to me about ethology," says Professor own species during a particular stage of their physical devel­ Peter Marler, "is that on the one hand it broaches behavioral opment in order to be able to reproduce them. The calls and problems complex enough to begin to give an inkling of the sounds of other animals may convey very specific messages, really deep issues, the ones that cognitive psychologists and philosophers worry about and, on the other hand, it's in touch with the machinery of the body-the physiology." Dr. Marler's base is The Rockefeller University's Field Research Center in Millbrook, New York, headquarters for a d,iverse program of investigations in ethology, the study of animal behavior - what he calls the area of zoology where "biology and psychology converge." At Millbrook, researchers ask questions about everything from bird migration to monkey alarm calls to bat "radar" to the "dances" of bees.
    [Show full text]
  • The Acoustic Features of Gorilla Double Grunts and Their Relation to Behavior
    American Journal of Primatology 33:31-50 (1994) The Acoustic Features of Gorilla Double Grunts and Their Relation to Behavior ROBERT M. SEYFARTH', DOROTHY L. CHENEY", ALEXANDER H. HARCOLJRT?, AND KELLY J. STEWART3 'Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 'Department of Biology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania .3Department of Anthropology, University of California, Davis, California Mountain gorillas (Gorilla gorilla beringei) give double-grunts to one an- other in a variety of situations, when feeding, resting, moving, or engaged in other kinds of social behavior. Some double-grunts elicit double-grunts in reply whereas others do not. Double-grunts are individually distinctive, and high-ranking animals give double-grunts at higher rates than others. There was no evidence, however, that the probability of eliciting a reply depended upon either the animals' behavior at the time a call was given or the social relationship between caller and respondent. The probability of eliciting a reply could be predicted from a double-grunt's acoustic features. Gorillas apparently produce at least two acoustically different subtypes of double-grunts, each of which conveys different information. Double-grunts with a low second formant (typically < 1600 Hz) are given by animals after a period of silence and frequently elicit vocal replies. Double-grunts with a high second formant (typically > 1600 Hz) are given by animals within 5 s of a call from another individual and rarely elicit replies. Q 1994 Wiley-Liss, Inc. Key words: mountain gorilla, social behavior, communication, vocaliza- tions INTRODUCTION When humans classify the calls of nonhuman primates by ear, they typically lump all similar sounding vocalizations within the same class, labeling them on- omatopoetically as coos, grunts, screams, barks, and so on.
    [Show full text]
  • The Origins of Music: Preface
    MUSPR 9/14/99 11:55 AM Page ix Preface The present volume is based on a workshop entitled “The Origins of Music” arranged by the Institute for Biomusicology in the Etruscan town of Fiesole outside Florence in late May 1997. As the workshop was the first international gathering held under the auspices of the Institute since its founding in 1995, a few words are in order concerning the Institute’s background. In 1982 one of us (NLW) published his dissertation Den musikaliska hjärnan (“The Musical Brain”) in Swedish, which was followed in 1991 by the book Biomusicology: Neurophysiological, Neuropsychological and Evolutionary Perspectives on the Origins and Purposes of Music. Both works gave expression to long-standing curiosity on the part of a musicologist regarding what light modern neuroscience might shed on questions such as the origins, evolutionary development, and purposes of music, questions that he felt were incompletely dealt with by his dis- cipline. Ever since his student days, this musicologist had been on a quest for a musicological paradigm to complement traditional approaches. He now hoped to find in modern biology what he had not found in Hume’s empiricism, in the logical empiricism of the Vienna and Chicago schools, or in the phenomenological trends that flourished in the 1940s and 1950s. Time was on his side. Since the Second World War, and more particularly in recent decades, the neurosciences and behavioral biology have made significant strides in areas relevant to the foundations of musicology. Thus there is now hope of gaining an understanding of the processes of musical cognition as well as biological factors that, together with cultural determinants, shaped mankind’s musical behavior and the rich global repertoire of musical structures it has produced.
    [Show full text]
  • August 1986 Vol. 31, No. 3
    NEWSLETTER Vol. 31, No.3 August, 1986 Animal Behavior Society A quarterly publication Terrv Christenson, ABS Secretary Jav Hansc/le, Associate Editor, ABS Ne'wsletter Department oj Psychology, Tlllane University, New Orleans. LA 70118 NEW ABS FELLOW BALLOT ENCLOSED FOR ABS ELECTION Devra Kleiman has been elected Fellow of the Society. Devra Biographic information on the candidates for Second President­ served as ABS President during the 1983-84 term. She is Elect, Secretary, and Member-at-Large and information currently the Assistant Director for Research at the National concerning the Constitutional amendment can be found on pages Zoological Park in Washington D,C, 4 and 5 of this Newsletter, BALLOT IS ON PAGE 5. Please respond; generally only a small percentage of the membership ALLEE STUDENT COMPETITION AWARD votes. The winner of the 1986 Allee competition for the best student NATURE CONSERVANCY CALL FOR HELP paper presented at the annual convention was Mary Victoria McDonald, Dept of Zoology, University of Florida for the paper Your help is needed to increase tfie amount of protected dry entitled "Vocalizations and reproductive behavior in seaside tropical forest in Guanacaste province of Costa Rica. In total, sparrows". The runner-up was Eldridge S, Adams, Department only 147 square miles of this unique forest type are protected, of Zoology, University of California at Berkeley, A total of 15 Currently, 39 square miles are protected m Guanacaste, Allee papers were presented in competition at the Tucson however, more area in the moister upland is needed to buffer the meetings. severely threatened dry forest from normal environmental flucuations.
    [Show full text]
  • J NATIONAL SCIENCE
    j NATIONALSCIENCE FOU NSF4Zb 1’ I National Science Foundation Fourteenth Annud Report for the F&d Year Eded June 30, 1964 For 881e by the Superintendent of Documents. IJ. 8. Qovernment Printing C&ze Washington, D. C., 20402 - Price 45 centa LETTEROF TRANSMITTAL WASHINGTON, D.C. January 25, 1965. MY DEAR MR. PRESIDENT: I have the honor to transmit herewith the Annual Report for Fiscal Year 1964 of the National Science Foundation for submission to the Congress as required by the National Science Foundation Act of 1950. Respectfully, LELAND J. HAWORTH Director, National Science Foundation. The Honorable The President of the United States. iu CONTENTS PW# . LETTER OF TRANSMITTAL ..................................... 111 THE DIRECTOR’S STATEMENT. ................................. vii PROGRAM ACTIVITIES OF THE NATIONAL SCIENCE FOUNDATION. ........................................ 1 INS~TUT~~~NAL PR00RAMs. ................................... 3 Science Development Program. ........................... 3 Institutional Grants for Science. ........................... 5 Graduate Science Facilities. .............................. 8 SUPPORT OF SCIENTIFIC RESEARCH ............................. 13 Basic Research Projects. .................................. 14 Mathematical, Physical, and Engineering Sciences. ....... 18 Biological and Medical Sciences. ....................... 27 Social Sciences. ...................................... 35 National Research Centers. ............................... 39 National Radio Astronomy Observatory ................. 40
    [Show full text]
  • Notes and References
    Notes and References Caveat: The Dangers of Behavioral Biology The contents of this book are known to be dangerous. I do not mean that in the sense that all ideas are potentially dangerous. Specifically, ideas about the biological basis of behavior have encouraged political tendencies and movements later regretted by all decent people and condemned in school histories. Why, then, purvey such ideas? Because some ideas in behavioral biology are true—among them, to the best of my knowledge, the ones in this book—and the truth is essential to wise action. But that does not mean that these ideas cannot be distorted, nor that evil acts cannot arise from them. I doubt, in fact, that what I say can prevent such distortion. Political and social movements arise from worldly causes, and then seize whatever congenial ideas are at hand. Nonethe- less, I am not comfortable in the company of scientists who are content to search for the truth and let the consequences accumulate as they may. I therefore recount here a few pas- sages in the dismal, indeed shameful history of the abuse of behavioral biology, in some of which scientists were willing participants. The first episode is recounted in William Stanton’s The Leopard’s Spots: Scientific Atti- tudes Toward Race in America, 1815–59 (Chicago: University of Chicago, 1960). Such names as Samuel George Morton, George Robins Gliddon, and Josiah Clark Nott mean little to present-day students of anthropology, but in the difficult decades between the death of Jeffer- son and the Civil War, they founded the American School of Anthropology.
    [Show full text]