Manipulation of Infantlike Traits Affects Perceived Cuteness of Infant

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Manipulation of Infantlike Traits Affects Perceived Cuteness of Infant Ethology Manipulation of Infant-Like Traits Affects Perceived Cuteness of Infant, Adult and Cat Faces Anthony C. Little School of Natural Sciences, University of Stirling, Stirling, UK Correspondence Abstract Dr. Anthony C. Little, School of Natural Sciences, University of Stirling, Stirling, Physical traits that are characteristic of human infants are referred to as FK9 4LA, UK. baby-schema, and the notion that these affect perception of cuteness and E-mail: [email protected] elicit care giving from adults has a long history. In this study, infant- similarity was experimentally manipulated using the difference between Received: March 6, 2012 adult and infant faces. Human infant, human adult and cat faces were Initial acceptance: May 14, 2012 manipulated to look more (human) infant-like or adult-like. The results Final acceptance: May 25, 2012 from the current study demonstrate the impact of infant-similarity on (L. Fusani) human adults’ perception of cuteness across the three different types of face. The type of face had a large impact on perceived cuteness in line with doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2012.02068.x the expected infant-similarity of the images. Infants and cats were cutest while adults were less cute. The manipulations of infant-similarity, how- ever, had similar effects on the perception of cuteness across all three types of face. Faces manipulated to have infant-like traits were rated as cuter than their equivalents manipulated to have adult-like traits. These data demonstrate that baby-like traits have a powerful hold over human perceptions and that these effects are not simply limited to infant faces. likely to enhance their own offspring’s survival Introduction (Bowlby 1969; Eibl-Eibesfeldt 1989), and the elicita- Physical traits that are characteristic of human tion of care giving has an obvious advantage to infants are referred to as ‘baby-schema’ (e.g. Lorenz the infant. Indeed, Struhsaker (1971) noted in 1943), and there is a long history to the notion that non-human primates that the loss of infant-like such traits might influence the perception of cute- characteristics as offspring age often coincides with ness and elicit care giving from adults. Darwin the subsiding of parental and protective responses. (1872) noted that something about infants prompts Images of infants certainly have powerful effects adults to care for them and that this is likely adap- on our behaviour and perception. For example, tive. Lorenz (1943) proposed seven characteristics individuals often initially smile in reaction to infants that all human infants possess that elicit a particular (Hildebrandt & Fitzgerald 1978; Schleidt et al. set of behaviours from human adults (such as care 1980). There is also a general preference for infant giving). Three of the seven are related to infant facial over adult images (Berman et al. 1975; Fullard & appearance, with infants typically possessing pre- Reiling 1976), and both adults and teenagers show dominance of the brain capsule (a large forehead), preferences for infants of more baby-like appearance large and low lying eyes, and a bulging cheek region (Gardner & Wallach 1965; Fullard & Reiling 1976; (Lorenz 1943). Human infants are entirely depen- Ritter et al. 1991). Other research has further dent on adults, and it is important to their survival shown that observers do respond to infant-like cues that we recognise infants and that infantile charac- (Todd et al. 1980). For example, Alley (1981) has teristics promote nurturing, care giving responses shown that more infantile facial profiles and more and suppress aggressive responses. Individuals rec- infantile body builds were judged to be cuter, ognising and acting on cues to infanthood would be more cuddly, and more defence provoking than Ethology 118 (2012) 775–782 © 2012 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 775 Infant-Likeness and Cuteness Across Faces A. C. Little more mature stimuli. Sternglanz et al. (1977) found Adults have been found to rate images of children up that in schematic faces, increased eye and forehead to 4.5 yr of age as more likeable and attractive if they size and smaller chin sizes were perceived as cuter possess baby-like features (Luo et al. 2011). The than the more mature looking reverses. Hildebrandt effects of infant-like facial features have been found & Fitzgerald (1979) showed adults pictures of real to extend into adulthood, however. Adults with infants and found that cuteness was related to short infant-like face traits are perceived in line with what and narrow noses, short and wide ears, and a nar- might be expected of infants. There exists a ‘baby- row face below the eyes. face’ stereotype (Berry & McArthur 1986) by which While much research has been conducted on baby- adult individuals whose faces most resemble infants schema, many studies have employed only simplified are seen as warmer, less likely to exhibit antisocial line drawings and schematic faces (Hueckstedt 1965; behaviour, more submissive, naive and irresponsible Sternglanz et al. 1977; Todd et al. 1980; Alley 1981; than those with more mature faces (Zebrowitz & McKelvie 1993). Other studies have examined natural Montepare 1992). Beyond humans, the impact of variation amongst infants, for example, using photo- baby-like features on preferences is apparent in the graphs of infants and correlating facial feature size appearance of pets, cartoon characters or even our with cuteness ratings (Hildebrandt & Fitzgerald 1979). preference for products such as teddy bears. For While both approaches have their merits, both also example, it has been noted that Walt Disney’s Mickey have drawbacks. Schematic stimuli sacrifice ecological Mouse has become increasingly more baby-like (Gou- validity for experimental control whereas real varia- ld 1979) and that teddy bears have changed in tion is prone to the presence of uncontrolled artefacts appearance from a long-snouted long-limbed bear to that may vary across stimuli. One recent study has become more baby-like in appearance (Hinde & Bar- re-examined these issues using computer graphic den 1985). Hinde & Barden (1985) have suggested techniques to manipulate specific aspects of the baby- that the teddy bear has evolved in this way by means schema in colour photographs of infants using a of artificial selection because of customer preference: manipulation based on anthropometric measure- People prefer baby-like characteristics and the bear ments to ensure realistic levels of transform. Manipu- makers cater to the tastes of their customers. Pets are lating faces to possess high (round face, high another example of how baby-like appearance can forehead, large eyes, small nose and mouth) and low generally impact on human perceptions. Lorenz (narrow face, low forehead, small eyes, large nose and (1950/1971) noted that that some breeds of dogs mouth) baby-schema, Glocker et al. (2009) found (Canis familiaris) have retained infant-like features that the high baby-schema images were seen as more into adulthood and several authors have noted the cute and more likely to elicit care giving than low selection for neotenous features in many breeds of pet baby-schema images, providing the first experimental cats (Felis catus) and dogs (Tuan 1984; Serpell 1986). evidence for baby-schema eliciting such perceptions Like Mickey Mouse and teddy bears, artificial selec- in photo-realistic stimuli. tion based on human preference for baby-like traits While not explicitly testing the effects of baby- could have directly resulted in a change in appearance schema, recent work has also examined the percep- of these pets. tion of cuteness on responses to infant face images. One study has examined how infant-like traits Using computer manipulation to manipulate cuteness influence the perception of attractiveness in cat and based on high and low scoring infant faces, it has been dog images. Faces of both cats and dogs containing shown that young women are more sensitive to dif- infant features were rated as more attractive than the ferences in infant cuteness than men or older women, faces that did not (Archer & Monton 2011). This at a post-menopausal age (Sprengelmeyer et al. study, however, used a limited number of stimuli 2009). Women’s sensitivity to cuteness has been fur- (two high and two low for each type of pet), and these ther demonstrated in studies in which women are were selected by the authors, with one of the images able to discriminate cues to cuteness in infant faces being actively manipulated while others were left better than men despite women and men being unchanged. The images were measured after selection equally capable of discriminating age and facial and differed on one aspect of infant schema – the ratio expression (Lobmaier et al. 2010). of the forehead to the middle of the eyes to the middle Infant-like features hold sway over our perceptions of the eyes to the base of the chin – indicating that the in domains beyond just infants. For example, several images did indeed differ in infant-like appearance. studies have addressed the impact on baby-like facial However, subjective selection could have led the traits on the perception of older children and adults. selection of attractive vs. unattractive images by the 776 Ethology 118 (2012) 775–782 © 2012 Blackwell Verlag GmbH A. C. Little Infant-Likeness and Cuteness Across Faces authors. Furthermore, alongside the difference in Stimuli measured ratio, it is clear the images were unmatched in other ways such as pose, expression and colour Original images traits. Adult human images were eight photographs (four The question asked of judges is likely to be impor- women, four men) of white individuals (aged tant. Some studies address perceived cuteness while between 18 and 25 yr) without spectacles or obvious others address attractiveness, and often the two terms facial hair randomly selected from a larger database. are treated as if they are equivalent.
Recommended publications
  • Teaching Visual Art with the Brain in Mind
    1 Teaching Visual Art with the Brain in Mind A thesis presented by Karen G. Pearson to the Graduate School of Education In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Education In the field of Education College of Professional Studies Northeastern University Boston, Massachusetts August 20, 2019 2 ABSTRACT Critical periods of perceptual development occur during the elementary and middle school years. Vision plays a major role in this development. The use of child development knowledge of Bruner, Skinner, Piaget and Inhelder coupled with the artistic thinking theories of Goldschmidt, Marshall, and Williams through and the lens of James J. Gibson and his ex-wife Eleanor J. framed the study. Sixteen 8-10-year-olds over eight one-hour weekly meetings focused on how they see and learn how to draw. The study demonstrated that the perception of the participants followed the development of the visual pathway as described in empirical neural studies. Salient features presented themselves first and then, over time, details such as space, texture, and finally depth can be learned over many years of development. The eye muscles need to build stamina through guided lessons that provide practice as well as a finished product. It was more important to focus on the variety of qualities of line, shape, and space and strategy building through solution finding and goal setting. Perceptual development indicators of how 8-10-year-old elementary students see and understand images will be heard from their voices. The results indicated that practice exercises helped participants build stamina that directly related to their ability to persist in drawing.
    [Show full text]
  • Paths from the Philosophy of Art to Everyday Aesthetics
    Paths from the Philosophy of Art to Everyday Aesthetics Edited by Oiva Kuisma, Sanna Lehtinen and Harri Mäcklin Paths from the Philosophy of Art to Everyday Aesthetics © 2019 Authors Cover and graphic design Kimmo Nurminen ISBN 978-952-94-1878-7 PATHS FROM THE PHILOSOPHY OF ART TO EVERYDAY AESTHETICS Eds. Oiva Kuisma, Sanna Lehtinen and Harri Mäcklin Published in Helsinki, Finland by the Finnish Society for Aesthetics, 2019 6 Contents 9 Oiva Kuisma, Sanna Lehtinen and Harri Mäcklin Introduction: From Baumgarten to Contemporary Aesthetics 19 Morten Kyndrup Were We Ever Modern? Art, Aesthetics, and the Everyday: Distinctions and Interdependences 41 Lars-Olof Åhlberg Everyday and Otherworldly Objects: Dantoesque Transfiguration 63 Markus Lammenranta How Art Teaches: A Lesson from Goodman 78 María José Alcaraz León Aesthetic Intimacy 101 Knut Ove Eliassen Quality Issues 112 Martta Heikkilä Work and Play – The Built Environments in Terry Gilliam’s Brazil 132 Kalle Puolakka Does Valery Gergiev Have an Everyday? 148 Francisca Pérez-Carreño The Aesthetic Value of the Unnoticed 167 Mateusz Salwa Everyday Green Aesthetics 180 Ossi Naukkarinen Feeling (With) Machines 201 Richard Shusterman Pleasure, Pain, and the Somaesthetics of Illness: A Question for Everyday Aesthetics 215 Epiloque: Jos de Mul These Boots Are Made for Talkin’. Some Reflections on Finnish Mobile Immobility 224 Index of Names 229 List of Contributors 7 OIVA KUISMA, SANNA LEHTINEN & HARRI MÄCKLIN INTRODUCTION: FROM BAUMGARTEN TO CONTEMPORARY AESTHETICS ontemporary philosopher-aestheticians
    [Show full text]
  • Yapp Dissertation Minor China
    UC Berkeley UC Berkeley Electronic Theses and Dissertations Title Minor China: Affect, Performance, & Contemporary China in the Global Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/7205c8fk Author Yapp, Hentyle Publication Date 2014 Peer reviewed|Thesis/dissertation eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California Minor China: Affect, Performance, & Contemporary China in the Global by Hentyle Taiwan Yapp A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Performance Studies and the Designated Emphasis in Women, Gender and Sexuality in the Graduate Division of the University of California, Berkeley Committee in charge: Professor Shannon Jackson, Chair Professor Mel Chen Professor Andrew Jones Professor Susan Kwan Spring 2014 1 Abstract Minor China: Affect, Performance, & Contemporary China in the Global by Hentyle Taiwan Yapp Doctor of Philosophy in Performance Studies and the Designated Emphasis in Women, Gender and Sexuality University of California, Berkeley Professor Shannon Jackson, Chair In our globalized moment, cultural production emerging from China and other non-Western locations has become of central concern for critical theory, art history, and cultural studies. In order to counteract previous decontextualized and over-universalizing discussions of contemporary Chinese art, most art history, theatre, and performance studies scholars have emphasized how art and culture emerged within specific historical and political contexts. However, by repeatedly relying on contextualization, the Chinese are reproduced as lacking imagination, contradictions, and complexity. By examining the historical emergence of this discourse, I demonstrate the limits of past approaches in order to explore other methodological possibilities. In contrast to other scholars who have situated contemporary Chinese performance and art within over-determined modes of contextualization, this dissertation locates alternative methodological possibilities in affect and feelings.
    [Show full text]
  • Theuse of Cuteness in Japanese Advertising
    SWEET SPOTS: THEUSE OF CUTENESS IN JAPANESE ADVERTISING Andreas RIESSLAND This article looks at Japanese society through its advertisements. To be more precise, it examines the utilization of a certain social value-.. cute­ ness - within advertising contexts. The focus is not so much on the per­ ception of 11 cuteIl advertising, i. e., on how an audience in Japan discerns and interprets signifiers of cuteness in ads, but on how advertisers in Ja­ pan utilize 11 cute" images for advertising purposes and to what ends. Advertisements, if properly analyzed, can provide useful hints as to what a sodety's values are. Through the idealized IIrealities" they display, they are suggestive of what is defined as desirable and positive. Converse­ ly, they also give clues about sodal restrictions and taboos through matters they avoid showing (such as the colour red in ads for sanitary pads). Look­ ing beyond the advertised commodity and investigating the IIsodal reali­ ties" displayed in the ad, we can get a pretty clear idea of what values and ideas are of relevance within a given sodety. What makes this possible is the fact, that contrary to the image of avant-garde and cutting-edge trend­ iness which ad makers try to promote for themselves, in their ads they tend to promote the conservative end of sodety's value spectrum (WERNICK 1991: 24). The reason for this is obvious: Evenwhenit targets a limited audience, an ad canachieve maximum acceptance withinits target group onlybynotgo­ ing against the sodal value system prevalent in this group. A certain dra­ matic toying with some of the sodal standards may be permissible and use­ ful for raising attention, but, on the whole, a consdous violation of audience expectations in advertising (such as Benetton's controversial poster cam­ paign) is extremely rare.
    [Show full text]
  • Softening Power: Cuteness As Organizational Communication Strategy in Japan and the West
    © 2018 Journal of International and Advanced Japanese Studies Vol. 10, February 2018, pp. 39-55 Master’s and Doctoral Programs in International and Advanced Japanese Studies Iain MACPHERSON & Teri Jane BRYANT Graduate School of Humanities and Social Sciences, University of Tsukuba Article Softening Power: Cuteness as Organizational Communication Strategy in Japan and the West Iain MACPHERSON MacEwan University, Faculty of Fine Arts and Communication, Assistant Professor Teri Jane BRYANT Haskayne School of Business, University of Calgary, Associate Professor Emerita This paper describes the use of cute communications (visual or verbal, and in various media) as an organizational communication strategy prevalent in Japan and emerging in western countries. Insights are offered for the use of such communications and for the understanding/critique thereof. It is first established that cuteness in Japan–kawaii–is chiefly studied as a sociocultural or psychological phenomenon, with too little analysis of its near-omnipresent institutionalization and conveyance as mass media. The following discussion clarifies one reason for this gap in research–the widespread conflation of ʻorganizational communication’ with advertising/branding, notwithstanding the variety of other messaging–public relations, employee communications, public service announcements, political campaigns–conveyed through cuteness by Japanese institutions. It is then argued that what few theorizations exist of organizational kawaii communications overemphasize their negative aspects or potentials, attributing to them both too much iniquity and too much influence. Outside of Japan studies, there is even less up-to-date scholarship on organizational cuteness, critical or otherwise. And there are no such studies at all, whether focused on Japan or elsewhere, that integrate intercultural insights.
    [Show full text]
  • Cuteness": the Aesthetics of Social Relations in a New Postwar Japanese Order
    CAPITALIZING ON "CUTENESS": THE AESTHETICS OF SOCIAL RELATIONS IN A NEW POSTWAR JAPANESE ORDER Leila MADGE In the fall of 1988, Yoshida Kenji, the president of the San'yo Sago Bank in Okayama, made headline news in the economics section of leading Japa­ nese newspapers. Unlike most bank presidents appearing in the news at that time, Yoshida was not defending himself or his bank from accusations of wrongdoings. He had made the national news because of the new name he had chosen for his bank: Tomato Bank. The occasion for the new name was the bank's planned change in status from a savings and loan associa­ tion to a commercial bank following a recent reform in Japanese banking law. The hoopla that arose around the name "Tomato," along with the bank's bright red logo, was related to the associations that it conjured up in many Japanese minds, associations that somehow seemed incongruent with the image that banking institutions have traditionally held in Japan. Although there was some initial opposition among bank employees, who felt that the name Tomato sounded more appropriate for an agricul­ tural cooperative than for a bank, the public response seemed only posi­ tive. Although the conversion was not to take place for six months, Sanyo Sago was inundated with inquiries from people all over the country wish­ ing to open accounts at the new Tomato Bank. In interviews about the un­ expected popularity of the previously little-known prefectural bank, Yoshida explained that he thought it important to send the message that he was "breaking the status quo." A crucial element in the delivery of this message was the use of a name that would be perceived by the public as kawaii (cute, as translated in the Japan Times, 21 October 1988: 11).
    [Show full text]
  • Bridging the Art Museum and the Middle School Math Classroom
    June 2, 2013 15:41 Journal of Mathematics and the Arts jmabasedfp Journal of Mathematics and the Arts Vol. 00, No. 00, January 2013, 1–16 The Fusion Project: Bridging the Art Museum and the Middle School Math Classroom a b Benjamin Wells ⇤ and Philip Wagner aDepartment of Mathematics, University of San Francisco, 2130 Fulton Street, San Francisco CA 94117, USA; bThe Fusion Project, Davis CA, USA (Version: v4.1, for the USF Fusion Project 2013 Summer Institute. c 2013 Benjamin Wells) The Fusion Project is a distinct program of the University of San Francisco’s College of Arts and Sciences with the support of the School of Education. It was created by Philip Wagner and is directed at USF by Benjamin Wells. With the support of the Fine Arts Museums of San Francisco, its goal is to bring art to the math classroom and math students to the art museum, enhancing existing curricula in order to improve basic and advanced skills, standard-oriented test scores, and students’ interest in mathematics. Keywords: mathematics education, math in art museums; art in math classrooms; museum-school mathematics collaboration AMS Subject Classification: 97D40, 97B50, 97U99 1. History and background In the new century, the United States government recognized the need to improve knowledge and skills in language and mathematics, and so the US authorized the No Child Left Behind (NCLB) program in 2002. Under this program the performance of public schools has been judged on standardized test scores in language and mathematics. Review of the well-publicized testing results over the years indicates that that California students rank below their peers in other states and US students rank below their fellows in other countries based on similar testing.
    [Show full text]
  • Exploring the Effects of Cuteness on Meat Consumption Janis H
    TO SWEET TO EAT 1 Too Sweet to Eat: Exploring the Effects of Cuteness on Meat Consumption Janis H. Zickfeld*, Jonas R. Kunst University of Oslo Sigrid M. Hohle Simula Research Laboratory In press at Appetite. This paper is not the copy of record and may not exactly replicate the authoritative document published in Appetite. Author Note Janis H. Zickfeld and Jonas R. Kunst, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Norway; Sigrid M. Hohle, Simula Research Laboratory, Oslo, Norway. *Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Janis H. Zickfeld, Postboks 1094 Blindern, 0317 Oslo, Norway. E-mail: [email protected]. TO SWEET TO EAT 2 Abstract Although daily meat consumption is a widespread habit, many individuals at the same time put a high value on the welfare of animals. While different psychological mechanisms have been identified to resolve this cognitive tension, such as dissociating the animal from the consumed meat or denying the animal’s moral status, few studies have investigated the effects of the animal’s appearance on the willingness to consume its meat. The present article explored how the perception of cuteness influences hypothetical meat consumption. We hypothesized that cuter animals would reduce the willingness to consume meat, and that this relationship would be mediated by empathy felt towards the animal. Across four pre- registered studies sampling 1074 US and Norwegian participants, we obtained some support for this prediction in the US but to a lesser degree in Norway. However, in all studies an indirect mediation effect of cuteness on meat consumption going through empathy towards the animal was observed.
    [Show full text]
  • INTEGRATED COLLABORATIVE MATHEMATICS Charity M
    - INTEGRATED COLLABORATIVE MATHEMATICS Charity M Tanaleon B.A., University of California, Davis, 2002 PROJECT Submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS in EDUCATION (Curriculum and Instruction) at CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY, SACRAMENTO SUMMER 2009 C 2009 Charity M Tanaleon ALL RIGHTS RESERVED ii - INTEGRATED COLLABORATIVE MATHEMATICS A Project by Charity M Tanaleon Approved by: Committee Chair D/ Julta Lambating au¢t~ .2 6(, 024 q Date 6/ iii L I *Student: Charity M. Tanaleon I certify that this student has met the requirements for format contained in the University format manual, and that this project is suitable for shelving in the Library and credit is to be awarded for the Project. 3raduate Coordinator . Julita Lambating Date v Department of Teacher Education iv Abstract of INTEGRATED COLLABORATIVE MATHEMATICS by Charity M Tanaleon Currentmathematical instructionalpractices remain unchanged, allowing educational practicesand curriculum to remain shallow, undemanding and diffuse in content coverage (NationalResearch Council, 2001). The standardizedmath curriculums are basal programsthat utilize a spiral approach to instruction, often presenting concepts as isolatedor unrelated skills. These concepts present themselves too quickly and in a specified order with educatorsfocusing on basic skills due to the amplified stress of high stakes testing and accountability. These curriculums lack relevance to students' lives, inhibitingeffective brainfunctioning and covering more math topics than other countries. The sources of data used in this project include achievement comparisonsfrom internationalassessments such as the Programfor InternationalAssessment (PISA) and Trends in InternationalMathematics and Science Study (TIMSS). This project grounds itself in constructivist theories of Piaget, Dewey, and Vygotsky, as well as brain research and observational studies examining variationsof problem based learning (PBL) incorporatedinto the classroom.
    [Show full text]
  • Neural and Behavioral Responses to Attractiveness in Adult and Infant Faces
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by St Andrews Research Repository Neural and Behavioral Responses to Attractiveness in Adult and Infant Faces Amanda C. Hahn1 & David I. Perrett2 1 University of Glasgow, Institute of Neuroscience and Psychology 2 University of Saint Andrews, School of Neuroscience and Psychology Corresponding Authors: Amanda C. Hahn Institute of Neuroscience and Psychology, University of Glasgow 58 Hillhead Street Glasgow, G12 8QB Email: [email protected] David I. Perrett School of Neuroscience and Psychology, University of Saint Andrews St Mary’s Quad Saint Andrews, KY16 9JP Email: [email protected] ABSTRACT Facial attractiveness provides a very powerful motivation for sexual and parental behavior. We therefore review the importance of faces to the study of neurobiological control of human reproductive motivations. For heterosexual individuals there is a common brain circuit involving the nucleus accumbens, the medial prefrontal, dorsal anterior cingulate and the orbitofrontal cortices that is activated more by attractive than unattractive faces, particularly for faces of the opposite sex. Behavioral studies indicate parallel effects of attractiveness on incentive salience or willingness to work to see faces. Both work/effort and brain activation to the sight of opposite sex attractiveness is more pronounced in men than women, perhaps reflecting the greater importance assigned to physical attractiveness by men when evaluating a potential mate. Studies comparing heterosexual and homosexual observers indicate the orbitofrontal cortex and mediodorsal thalamus are more activated by faces of the desired sex than faces of the less preferred sex, independent of observer gender or sexual orientation.
    [Show full text]
  • A Behavioural Science Framework for Understanding Kawaii and Cuteness
    EAPC 2 (1) pp. 79–95 Intellect Limited 2016 East Asian Journal of Popular Culture Volume 2 Number 1 © 2016 Intellect Ltd Article. English language. doi: 10.1386/eapc.2.1.79_1 HIROSHI NITTONO Osaka University The two-layer model of ‘kawaii’: A behavioural science framework for understanding kawaii and cuteness ABSTRACT KEYWORDS ‘Kawaii’ is one of the most popular words in contemporary Japan and is recognized baby schema as representative of Japanese pop culture. It is often translated into English as ‘cute’, cognition but a subtle difference of nuance seems to exist between the two words. In this arti- culture cle, a framework for research on kawaii from a behavioural science perspective is cuteness put forward. After introducing the dictionary definition, history and current usage emotion of kawaii, this article reports survey results of Japanese students and office work- psychology ers about their attitudes towards kawaii. These findings and past psychological and behavioural science research lead to a two-layer model that consists of kawaii as an emotion and kawaii as a social value. This model postulates that the basis of kawaii is a positive emotion related to the social motivation of watching for and staying with preferable persons and objects, which is typically observed in affection towards babies and infants, but not limited to them. This culturally non-specific, biological trait has been appreciated and fostered in Japan by certain characteristics of Japanese culture. Because previous research on cuteness has been almost exclusively associated with 79 eeajpc2.1.indbajpc2.1.indb 7799 111/04/161/04/16 110:540:54 PPMM Hiroshi Nittono infant physical attractiveness and baby schema, using the relatively fresh, exotic word ‘kawaii’ may be helpful to describe this broader psychological concept.
    [Show full text]
  • How Product Cuteness Influence Consumers' Perception in Negative
    Cute Scam: HowPoster product Title cuteness influence consumers' perception in negative ways FirstnameYoun Soo, LastnameKim1, Kwanho1, William, Suk 2Hedgcock2 1 Tippie College of Business,Business, UniversityDepartment of ofIowa, Marketing, 2 Department University of Marketing, of Iowa, 2 UniversityDepartment of ofIowa Marketing, Korea University IntroductionAbstract or Abstract ResultsStimuli ConclusionsDouble Mediation Results InsertToday ,introduction many companies or abstract have textinvested here. theirPlease resource keep general in Resource Quality textproduct to a minimumdesign. They of 36pt. believe that product design can result Allocation in positive result. However, product aesthetics can negatively influence consumers' quality perception and purchase Purchase intention. When consumers face cute products, they infer Aesthetics that those products have lower product functionality. This is Intention because of consumers' lay theory of zero-sum heuristics; consumers assume that superiority of one product dimension, cute design, is compensated by inferiority on Indirect Effect SE 95% CI another dimension, functionality. The double mediation Predicted results also support consumers’ perception of firm’s resource Portable Battery Portable Battery -.46 .11 -.71 to -.29 allocation and product attribute trade-off. Charger(Control) Charger(Experiment) Pathway Reverse Model .07 .06 -.02 to .21 Cuteness Results Being attractive in adorable or endearing way (Hellen & Sääksjärvi N=202 (M=36.7, Female=94) References 2011) Manipulation Check: Control (M=3.39, SD=1.59) < Experiment (M=5.34, SD=1.57) • kindchenschema (baby schema) collection of cute features of • Newman, G. E., Gorlin, M., & Dhar, R. (2014). When going green newborns such as bulging forehead, large eyes, and rounded backfires: How firm intentions shape the evaluation of socially cheeks Quality Perception beneficial product enhancements.
    [Show full text]