UC Berkeley Dissertations, Department of Linguistics

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

UC Berkeley Dissertations, Department of Linguistics UC Berkeley Dissertations, Department of Linguistics Title Distributing morphologically conditioned phonology: Three case studies from Guébie Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/38g4f01q Author Sande, Hannah L Publication Date 2017-07-01 eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California Distributing morphologically conditioned phonology: Three case studies from Gu´ebie by Hannah Leigh Sande A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the Graduate Division of the University of California, Berkeley Committee in charge: Professor Sharon Inkelas, Chair Professor Larry Hyman Professor Peter Jenks Professor Darya Kavitskaya Summer 2017 Distributing morphologically conditioned phonology: Three case studies from Gu´ebie Copyright 2017 by Hannah Leigh Sande 1 Abstract Distributing morphologically conditioned phonology: Three case studies from Gu´ebie by Hannah Leigh Sande Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics University of California, Berkeley Professor Sharon Inkelas, Chair The focus of this study is process morphology in Gu´ebie,an endangered Kru language spo- ken in C^oted'Ivoire. Unlike many primarily affixing morphological systems, much of the morphology in Gu´ebieinvolves root-internal phonological changes like tone shift and vowel replacement. For this reason, Gu´ebiedata have much to offer discussions of the interface be- tween morphology and phonology. Based on the Gu´ebiefacts presented here, I argue 1) that process morphology, where a non-concatenative phonological process is the sole exponent of a morpheme, is a subtype of morphologically conditioned phonology, and 2) that not all morphology involves underlying phonological items. I conclude that whether a morpheme triggers a phonological process is independent of whether a morpheme is associated with underlying phonological content. Instead, morphologically conditioned phonological pro- cesses are driven by phonological constraints, whose rankings are determined by particular morphosyntactic features present in the domain being phonologically evaluated. This study describes the phonology and morphology of Gu´ebie,focusing in particular on three case studies of morphologically conditioned phonological processes. These include phonologically determined noun class agreement, scalar tone shift, and vowel replacement. In each of these case studies we see evidence for specific interactions between not only morphology and phonology, but also syntax and phonology. On the morphological side, Gu´ebietonal morphology shows us that not every morpheme is associated with an underlying (abstract) phonological item. With respect to syntax, we see that domains of phonological evaluation in Gu´ebiemust be larger than a single word, but not larger than a syntactic phase. We also see phonological processes sensitive to both morphosyntax and lexical class in Gu´ebie, suggesting that any model of phonological grammar must be able to reference morphosyntactic and lexical information. By exploring morphological exponents across a language, we can narrow down the space of possible models that account for morphosyntactic interaction with phonology. i Contents Contents i 1 Introduction 1 1.1 Process morphology . 1 1.1.1 Process morphology and morphologically conditioned phonology . 1 1.1.2 Item versus process morphology . 3 1.1.3 Evidence from a highly process-based morphological system . 4 1.1.4 The proposed model . 5 1.2 Roadmap . 6 1.3 Background on Gu´ebie . 7 1.3.1 Language background . 7 1.3.2 The consultants and corpus . 8 2 Gu´ebiephonology and morphology 10 2.1 Introduction . 10 2.2 Gu´ebiephonology . 10 2.2.1 Segmental inventory . 11 2.2.1.1 Consonants . 11 2.2.1.2 Vowels . 13 2.2.2 Syllable structure . 15 2.2.3 Minimality . 20 2.2.4 Tone . 22 2.2.5 Vowel harmony . 28 2.3 Gu´ebiemorphology . 29 2.3.1 Affixes and compounding . 29 2.3.1.1 Nominal affixes . 29 2.3.1.2 Verbal affixes . 31 2.3.1.3 Nasal harmony . 38 2.3.1.4 Genitives and compounds . 39 2.3.1.5 Summarizing concatenative morphology in Gu´ebie . 40 2.3.2 Non-concatenative morphology . 41 2.3.2.1 Phonologically determined agreement . 41 ii 2.3.2.2 Morphologically conditioned tone changes . 42 2.3.2.3 Tone replacement . 44 2.3.2.4 Vowel replacement . 45 2.3.3 Interim summary . 47 3 Phonologically determined agreement 48 3.1 Introduction . 48 3.2 Gu´ebiephonologically determined agreement . 49 3.2.1 Phonological agreement between nouns and pronouns . 50 3.2.2 Phonological agreement between nouns and modifiers . 56 3.3 An interface model of phonologically determined agreement . 57 3.3.1 Considering possible analyses . 57 3.3.2 The proposed model . 59 3.3.2.1 The syntactic structure . 61 3.3.2.2 The morphological structure . 66 3.3.2.3 The phonology . 67 3.4 Typological predictions . 72 3.4.1 Gu´ebiehuman pronouns . 73 3.4.2 Gu´ebieadjectives . 75 3.4.3 Other Kru languages . 76 3.4.3.1 Krahn . 76 3.4.3.2 Godi´e . 77 3.4.3.3 Vata . 78 3.4.3.4 Summary of Kru phonological agreement . 79 3.4.4 Bainuk . 79 3.4.5 Abu' . 81 3.5 Discussion . 82 3.6 Conclusion . 84 4 Scalar tone shift 85 4.1 Introduction . 85 4.2 Gu´ebiescalar tone shift . 86 4.3 Scalar shift in Distributed Morphology plus Cophonology Theory . 94 4.3.1 The syntax of verb movement in Gu´ebie . 94 4.3.1.1 Adverbs cannot intervene between Subject and Inflection . 95 4.3.1.2 Strict SubjVerb order . 96 4.3.1.3 Embedded clause order . 97 4.3.1.4 Movement blocked in the presence of an auxiliary . 98 4.3.1.5 Wh, Focus, Topic order . 99 4.3.1.6 Word order summary . 101 4.3.2 The morphological component . 102 4.3.3 Scalar tone cophonologies . 106 iii 4.3.3.1 The imperfective cophonology . 107 4.3.3.2 The elsewhere grammar . 111 4.3.4 Considering alternative analyses . 112 4.3.4.1 An Optimal Paradigms account . 112 4.3.4.2 Underlying representations . 113 4.3.4.3 Floating tones . 114 4.3.4.4 Floating tone features . 115 4.3.4.5 Interim summary: Underlying representations lack insight for scalar shift . 116 4.3.4.6 Extant Distributed Morphology tools . 116 4.3.4.7 Suppletive allomorphy . 117 4.3.4.8 Readjustment rules . 117 4.4 Other instances of tonal morphology in Gu´ebie. 118 4.4.1 Case marking via scalar tone shift . 118 4.4.2 Tone replacement in noun-noun compounds . 120 4.5 Conclusion . 122 5 Vowel alternations 124 5.1 Introduction . 124 5.2 Vowel reduction: CVCV to CCV . 125 5.2.1 The vowel reduction phenomenon . 125 5.2.2 Phonological properties of reducible words . 127 5.2.3 A MaxEnt-HG model of the distribution of reduction in Gu´ebie . 132 5.3 Vowel replacement . 135 5.3.1 Replacement in object contexts . 136 5.3.1.1 Phonological effects of object vowel replacement . 137 5.3.1.2 Neutralization due to vowel replacement . 138 5.3.1.3 The syntactic position of object markers . 139 5.3.1.4 Root vowel replacement with intervening affixes . 140 5.3.2 Vowel replacement in plural contexts . 141 5.3.2.1 /-A/ plural morpheme . 141 5.3.2.2 [-back, -round] plural morpheme . 142 5.3.3 No replacement in other morphosyntactic contexts . 144 5.3.3.1 Passive and Causative: No replacement, with ATR harmony 144 5.3.3.2 Definite and Nominalizer: No replacement, no ATR harmony 145 5.3.4 Interim summary: The distribution of vowel replacement . 146 5.4 Lexical specificity of vowel reduction and replacement . 149 5.4.1 Weak faithfulness to weakly encoded vowels . 149 5.4.2 The phonetics and phonology of weak vowels . 150 5.4.3 Neighborhood density effects . 153 5.4.4 Encoding strength summary . 155 5.5 A multiple-grammar constraint-based analysis . 156 iv 5.5.1 Object enclitic cophonology . 157 5.5.2 Plural cophonology . 161 5.5.2.1 Plural 1 . 161 5.5.2.2 Plural 2 . 164 5.5.3 Morphemes that fail to trigger vowel replacement . 169 5.6 Against an alternative approach . 171 5.6.1 Suppletive allomorphy . 171 5.6.2 Diacritics . 172 5.7 Conclusion . 173 6 Implications 175 6.1 What Gu´ebie tells us about process morphology . 175 6.1.1 Process morphology is morphologically conditioned phonology . 175 6.1.2 Not all morphology is item-based . ..
Recommended publications
  • 1 the Influence of Cognitive Factors on Category-Specific Phonology
    The Influence of Cognitive Factors on Category-Specific Phonology Jonathan Manker University of California, Berkeley A growing body of literature has documented an array of phonological patterns--- either static inventories or active rules--- which are sensitive to the grammatical category of the words they govern. Here I consider grammatical categories as including both syntactic classes such as nouns, verbs, and adjectives, and broad classes such as functional and content morphemes. A parallel body of literature has shown psycholinguistic and neurological differences in how different grammatical categories are treated. These independent observations in both the phonological and cognitive literature suggest a currently unexplored question: Are the observed category-specific phonological asymmetries the reflection of asymmetries in the mental organization or processing of these categories? A review of the literature reveals little solid evidence that there are either innate biases in category-specific phonology or that categories form neighborhoods that facilitate the spread of phonological patterns in the lexicon, when other possible factors, such as syntax and semantics, are considered. However, observations from the dual-stream model of speech perception suggest there may be emergent differences in how more predictable, functional categories are processed as opposed to less predictable, content categories. 1. Introduction In recent decades, linguists have begun to explore how many aspects of grammar interact with phonological patterns in language. In particular, studies coming from a wide range of approaches have documented and analyzed the effect of grammatical categories on sound patterns. Here I will use the term grammatical categories liberally to include classes based on syntactic function (nouns, verbs, adpositions, determiners, etc.), and the broader classes including content (nouns, verbs, adjectives) and functional morphemes (determiners, prepositions, inflectional affixes, etc.).
    [Show full text]
  • Liberian Studies Journal
    VOLUME XIV 1989 NUMBER 2 LIBERIAN STUDIES JOURNAL r 8 °W LIBERIA -8 °N 8 °N- MONSERRADO MARGIBI MARYLAND Geography Department 10 °W University of Pittsburgh at Johnstown 8oW 1 Published by THE LIBERIAN STUDIES ASSOCIATION, INC. PDF compression, OCR, web optimization using a watermarked evaluation copy of CVISION PDFCompressor Cover map: compiled by William Kory, cartography work by Jodie Molnar; Geography Department, University of Pittsburgh at Johnstown. PDF compression, OCR, web optimization using a watermarked evaluation copy of CVISION PDFCompressor VOLUME XIV 1989 NUMBER 2 LIBERIAN STUDIES JOURNAL Editor D. Elwood Dunn The University of the South Associate Editor Similih M. Cordor Kennesaw College Book Review Editor Dalvan M. Coger Memphis State University EDITORIAL ADVISORY BOARD Bertha B. Azango Lawrence B. Breitborde University of Liberia Beloit College Christopher Clapham Warren L. d'Azevedo Lancaster University University of Nevada Reno Henrique F. Tokpa Thomas E. Hayden Cuttington University College Africa Faith and Justice Network Svend E. Holsoe J. Gus Liebenow University of Delaware Indiana University Corann Okorodudu Glassboro State College Edited at the Department of Political Science, The University of the South PDF compression, OCR, web optimization using a watermarked evaluation copy of CVISION PDFCompressor CONTENTS THE LIBERIAN ECONOMY ON APRIL 1980: SOME REFLECTIONS 1 by Ellen Johnson Sirleaf COGNITIVE ASPECTS OF AGRICULTURE AMONG THE KPELLE: KPELLE FARMING THROUGH KPELLE EYES 23 by John Gay "PACIFICATION" UNDER PRESSURE: A POLITICAL ECONOMY OF LIBERIAN INTERVENTION IN NIMBA 1912 -1918 ............ 44 by Martin Ford BLACK, CHRISTIAN REPUBLICANS: DELEGATES TO THE 1847 LIBERIAN CONSTITUTIONAL CONVENTION ........................ 64 by Carl Patrick Burrowes TRIBE AND CHIEFDOM ON THE WINDWARD COAST 90 by Warren L.
    [Show full text]
  • The Post-Syntactic Morphology of the Albanian Pre-Posed Article: Evidence for Distributed Morphology
    The Post-Syntactic Morphology of the Albanian Pre-Posed Article: Evidence for Distributed Morphology Jochen Trommer Institut f¨urSemantische Informationsverarbeitung Universit¨atOsnabr¨uck D-49069 Osnabr¨uck Abstract The so-called "preposed article" (PA) is a functional morpheme that plays a central role in the morphosyntax of Albanian DPs but is poorly understood theoretically. In this paper I present an analysis of PAs morphology along the lines of Distributed Morphology (DM; Halle and Marantz, 1993). PA is taken to be a post-syntactically inserted agreement morpheme. It is argued that a correct account of the in- sertion contexts of PA and of its allomorphy presupposes reference to surface syntactic configurations thus supporting the conceptual frame- work of DM. The claim that PA is in certain instances a constitutive element of word formation (cf. e.g. Buchholz and Fiedler, 1987) is rejected. 1 Some Basic Empirical Facts 1.1 Occurrence The Albanian Preposed Article (PA) mainly occurs in two contexts1. It marks a dative DP as possessor of a complex DP and it occurs with most adjectives used attributively or predicatively: 1Besides other noun-modifying functional words (possessive pronouns, ordinal numbers, etc.) it also occurs with certain relative pronouns and nouns. For a more complete overview see (Buchholz and Fiedler, 1987, pg. 199) 1 (1) a. klas-a e kapitalist¨e-ve class-def PA capitalists-dat `The class of capitalists' b. vajz-a e bukur girl-def PA nice `the nice girl' c. vajz-a ¨esht¨e e bukur girl-def is PA nice `the girl is nice' While it functions as a grammatical marker in its first use, since it occurs with possessors perfectly regularly , and constitutes the only overt difference between dative verb complements and possessors, (2) a.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter Iv Research Finding Ii Types of Free and Bound
    98 CHAPTER IV RESEARCH FINDING II TYPES OF FREE AND BOUND MORPHEMES USED IN THE NEWS COLUMN OF THE JAKARTA POST ON TUESDAY EDITION VOL.31 APRIL 8, 2014 4.1 Theory of types of free morphemes and bound morphemes 4.1.1. Types of free morphemes Free morphemes are the morphemes that can stand alone as a word. According to George Yule (2006 : 63), he stated that free morphemes is the morphemes that can stand by themselves as single word, for example; cat, open, tour, in, them, because, when, etc. The morphemes can generally be identified as the set of separate english word forms such as basic nouns, adjectives, verbs, etc. There are two types of free morphemes, are they open class words and close class words. According to George Yule (2006 : 64), he stated that free morphemes divided into two categories, that are lexical morphemes and functional morphemes. These free morpheme are called lexical morphemes, such as nouns, verbs, adjectives, adverbs. some example are: girl (noun), man (noun), house (noun), tiger(noun), sad (adjective), long (adjective), open (verb), look (verb), follow (verb), break (verb). We can add new lexical morphemes to the language rather easily, so they are treated as an ‘open’ class of words. Other types of free morphemes are called functional morphemes, for example; and, but, when, because, on, near, above, in, the, that, it, them. This set consists largely of the funtional words in the language such as conjunctions, prepositions, articles, and pronouns. Because we almost never add new functional morpheme to the language, they are described as a ‘closed’ class of words.
    [Show full text]
  • Politics and Popular Culture: the Renaissance in Liberian Music, 1970-89
    POLITICS AND POPULAR CULTURE: THE RENAISSANCE IN LIBERIAN MUSIC, 1970-89 By TIMOTHY D. NEVIN A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FUFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA 2010 1 © 2010 Timothy Nevin 2 To all the Liberian musicians who died during the war-- (Tecumsey Roberts, Robert Toe, Morris Dorley and many others) Rest in Peace 3 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to thank my parents and my uncle Frank for encouraging me to pursue graduate studies. My father’s dedication to intellectual pursuits and his life-long love of teaching have been constant inspirations to me. I would like to thank my Liberian wife, Debra Doeway for her patience in attempting to answer my thousand and one questions about Liberian social life and the time period “before the war.” I would like to thank Dr. Luise White, my dissertation advisor, for her guidance and intellectual rigor as well as Dr. Sue O’Brien for reading my manuscript and offering helpful suggestions. I would like to thank others who also read portions of my rough draft including Marissa Moorman. I would like to thank University of Florida’s Africana librarians Dan Reboussin and Peter Malanchuk for their kind assistance and instruction during my first semester of graduate school. I would like to acknowledge the many university libraries and public archives that welcomed me during my cross-country research adventure during the summer of 2007. These include, but are not limited to; Verlon Stone and the Liberian Collections Project at Indiana University, John Collins and the University of Ghana at East Legon, Northwestern University, Emory University, Brown University, New York University, the National Archives of Liberia, Dr.
    [Show full text]
  • Functional Categories and Maturation: The
    FUNCTIONAL CATEGORIES AND MATURATION: THE PREFUNCTIONAL STAGE OF LANGUAGE ACQUISITION Ianthi-Maria Tsimpli Thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Ph.D. University College London M ay 1992 l ProQuest Number: 10610950 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a com plete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. uest ProQuest 10610950 Published by ProQuest LLC(2017). Copyright of the Dissertation is held by the Author. All rights reserved. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States C ode Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. ProQuest LLC. 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106- 1346 For Alexia ABSTRACT The aim of this thesis is to provide a theory of language acquisition within the Principles and Parameters framework of Generative Grammar. In Chapter 1 ,1 outline the syntactic theory I adopt which assumes that functional categories determine crosslinguistic variation in terms of parameterisation. In the model of the grammar presented, the set of functional categories is argued to constitute an independent module in the Language Faculty, the Functional Module. This is also referred to as the UG lexicon on the ground that it consists of categories that belong to the grammar proper. Substantive categories are assumed to be included in the Mental lexicon which is part of an independent module of the mind/brain.
    [Show full text]
  • The Phonology of Possession
    Piggott, Glyne; Lisa Travis; Heather Newell The Phonology of Possession The first goal of this talk is to demonstrate that phonological distinctions between Inalienable (INAL) and Alienable (AL) constructions in synthetic possessives are remarkably consistent cross- linguistically. In AL constructions, the marker of agreement (AGRPOSS) with the possessor (POSS) that appears on the possessed noun has the properties of a clitic; it is phonologically and morphologically ‘distant’ from the noun. In INAL constructions, on the other hand, these markers have a tendency to behave like true affixes; they are inseparable from the noun and are phonologically ‘close’. This morpho-syntactic closeness is widely discussed in the syntactic literature as being due to a featural specification ([+relational], following Barker 1995) on the INAL noun, but the parallel phonological closeness has received little attention in the phonological literature. The proposed [+rel] feature on the INAL noun entails that it must itself enter into a relationship with a POSS argument, while an AL noun’s status with POSS must be mediated by a functional morpheme, F (null or overt). Examples of this contrast will be offered from languages such as (1) Ojibwe (Eastern Algonquian), (2) Nvikh (isolate/Paleosiberian), (3) Akan (Niger- Congo), and languages such as Mangap-Mbula (Oceanic), Lango (Nilotic), Cupeño (Uto-Aztec) and Nanti (Arawakan). (1) a. no:komis 'my grandmother' b. nidogima:m ‘my leader’ ni-o:komis 1P-grandmother ni-o:gima:-im 1P-leader-POSS’ (2) a. phnaχ ‘one’s own eyes’ b. phinaχ ‘one’s own bed’ h h h p -naχ 1P-eyes p -!-naχ p -POSS-naχ (3) a.
    [Show full text]
  • Some Affixes Are Roots, Others Are Heads
    Nat Lang Linguist Theory (2018) 36:45–84 DOI 10.1007/s11049-017-9372-1 Some affixes are roots, others are heads Ava Creemers1 · Jan Don2 · Paula Fenger3 Received: 20 May 2014 / Accepted: 22 March 2017 / Published online: 5 July 2017 © The Author(s) 2017. This article is published with open access at Springerlink.com Abstract A recent debate in the morphological literature concerns the status of derivational affixes. While some linguists (Marantz 1997, 2001; Marvin 2003) con- sider derivational affixes a type of functional morpheme that realizes a categorial head, others (Lowenstamm 2015; De Belder 2011) argue that derivational affixes are roots. Our proposal, which finds its empirical basis in a study of Dutch derivational affixes, takes a middle position. We argue that there are two types of derivational affixes: some that are roots (i.e. lexical morphemes) and others that are categorial heads (i.e. functional morphemes). Affixes that are roots show ‘flexible’ categorial behavior, are subject to ‘lexical’ phonological rules, and may trigger idiosyncratic meanings. Affixes that realize categorial heads, on the other hand, are categorially rigid, do not trigger ‘lexical’ phonological rules nor allow for idiosyncrasies in their interpretation. Keywords Derivational affixes · Distributed morphology · Stress-behavior · Categorial flexibility · Phasal spell-out B J. Don [email protected] A. Creemers [email protected] P. Fenger [email protected] 1 Department of Linguistics, University of Pennsylvania, 3401-C Walnut Street, Suite 300, C Wing, Philadelphia, PA 19104-6228, USA 2 Department of Dutch Linguistics, Universiteit van Amsterdam, Spuistraat 134, 1012 VB Amsterdam, The Netherlands 3 University of Connecticut, Department of Linguistics, 365 Fairfield Way, Unit-1145, Room 368, Storss, CT, 06269, USA 46 A.
    [Show full text]
  • Extended Exponence by Enrichment: Argument Encoding in German, Archi, and Timucua
    Extended Exponence by Enrichment: Argument Encoding in German, Archi, and Timucua Gereon Muller¨ ∗ May 5, 2006 1 Introduction This paper is concerned with a principled approach to cases of extended (or multiple) exponence, as postulated in Matthews (1972, 82) for Greek verb in- flection (perfect forms in particular), and in Matthews (1974, 149) for English verb inflection (among other phenomena). Extended exponence characterizes those cases of morphological realization where a single morpho-syntactic prop- erty seems to be expressed by more than one exponent (i.e., inflection marker, in the cases to be considered here). Extended exponence raises a problem in Distributed Morphology. The solution that I propose centers around a new type of rule that applies to syntactic operations before morphological realiza- tion takes place, and that I call enrichment. Enrichment rules are complemen- tary to well-established impoverishment rules. They make it possible to account for extended exponence without invoking a concept of secondary exponence via contextual features. The empirical evidence that I discuss comes mainly from the interaction of argument encoding and number/person marking in German, Archi, and Timucua (with case-marking of nouns in the first two cases, and agreement morphology on verbs in the latter), but I also address verb inflection in Tamazight Berber. Let me begin by introducing the phenomenon of extended exponence on the basis of case-marked plural forms of nouns in German. As shown in (1), plural can be marked twice on nouns in dative (dat) contexts in German (see, e.g., Eisenberg (2000), Wiese (2000)). The inflected nouns in (1-ac) involve the co- occurrence of a plural marker (er in (1-a), which is common for neuter stems such as Kind (`child'), and e in (1-c), which is the unmarked plural marker for masculine stems like Tisch (`table')), and a dat plural marker n.
    [Show full text]
  • East Benue-Congo
    East Benue-Congo Nouns, pronouns, and verbs Edited by John R. Watters language Niger-Congo Comparative Studies 1 science press Niger-Congo Comparative Studies Chief Editor: Valentin Vydrin (INALCO – LLACAN, CNRS, Paris) Editors: Larry Hyman (University of California, Berkeley), Konstantin Pozdniakov (INALCO – LLACAN, CNRS, Paris), Guillaume Segerer (LLACAN, CNRS, Paris), John Watters (SIL International, Dallas, Texas). In this series: 1. Watters, John R. (ed.). East Benue-Congo: Nouns, pronouns, and verbs. 2. Pozdniakov, Konstantin. The numeral system of Proto-Niger-Congo: A step-by-step reconstruction. East Benue-Congo Nouns, pronouns, and verbs Edited by John R. Watters language science press John R. Watters (ed.). 2018. East Benue-Congo: Nouns, pronouns, and verbs (Niger-Congo Comparative Studies 1). Berlin: Language Science Press. This title can be downloaded at: http://langsci-press.org/catalog/book/190 © 2018, the authors Published under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 Licence (CC BY 4.0): http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ ISBN: 978-3-96110-100-9 (Digital) 978-3-96110-101-6 (Hardcover) DOI:10.5281/zenodo.1314306 Source code available from www.github.com/langsci/190 Collaborative reading: paperhive.org/documents/remote?type=langsci&id=190 Cover and concept of design: Ulrike Harbort Typesetting: Sebastian Nordhoff, John R. Watters Illustration: Sebastian Nordhoff Proofreading: Ahmet Bilal Özdemir, Andrew Spencer, Felix Hoberg, Jeroen van de Weijer, Jean Nitzke, Kate Bellamy, Martin Haspelmath, Prisca Jerono, Richard Griscom, Steven Kaye, Sune Gregersen, Fonts: Linux Libertine, Libertinus Math, Arimo, DejaVu Sans Mono Typesetting software:Ǝ X LATEX Language Science Press Unter den Linden 6 10099 Berlin, Germany langsci-press.org Storage and cataloguing done by FU Berlin Contents Preface iii 1 East Benue-Congo John R.
    [Show full text]
  • Lexical and Functional Decomposition in Syntax: a View from Phonology
    Poznań Studies in Contemporary Linguistics 47(2), 2011, pp. 399–425 © School of English, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań, Poland doi:10.2478/psicl-2011-0023 LEXICAL AND FUNCTIONAL DECOMPOSITION IN SYNTAX: A VIEW FROM PHONOLOGY DRAGANA ŠURKALOVIĆ Center for Advanced Study in Theoretical Linguistics (CASTL), University of Tromsø [email protected] ABSTRACT In recent years many lexical elements in the syntactic tree have been decomposed into formal features forming part of the functional sequence. This paper explores the effects of this change on the syntax–phonology interface, addressing two problems for language modularity and proposing that the Lexicon be the locus of communication between the two modules. The first issue is the sensitivity of prosody to edges of syntactic constitu- ents and to lexical elements and projections but not to functional ones (cf. Selkirk 1995; Truckenbrodt 1999, 2007 inter alia). Lexicon subcategorisation is offered as the solution (cf. Bye 2006; Paster 2005). The second issue is the prosodic marking of Information Structure. In Prosodic Phonology, constraints (Align-F, Stress-Focus) “see” these syntac- tic features, which is undesirable if modularity is to be maintained. This paper uses the Nanosyntactic view that features are merged into the tree individually, and suggests that Lexical entries for e.g. F and CT features in English are suprasegmental affixes pairing a H* tone with F feature or a L+H*L-H% contour with a CT feature. KEYWORDS : Modularity; syntax–phonology interface; prosody; information structure; lexicon. 1. Introduction The term “modularity” as it is used in this paper refers to the notion that the language computation system consists of three independent modules, syntax, phonology and semantics.
    [Show full text]
  • Analytic and Synthetic: Typological Change in Varieties of European Languages
    1 In: Buchstaller, Isabelle & Siebenhaar, Beat (eds.) 2017. Language Variation – European Perspectives VI: Selected papers from the 8th International Conference on Language Variation in Europe (ICLaVE 8), Leipzig 2015. Amsterdam: Benjamins. Analytic and synthetic: Typological change in varieties of European languages MARTIN HASPELMATH & SUSANNE MARIA MICHAELIS 1. The macro-comparative perspective: Language typology and language contact Since the early 19th century, linguists have sometimes tried to understand language change from a broader perspective, as affecting the entire character of a language. Since A.W. von Schlegel (1818), it has been commonplace to say that Latin was a SYNTHETIC language, while the Romance languages are (more) ANALYTIC, i.e. make more use of auxiliary words and periphrastic constructions of various kinds. In this paper, we adopt a macro-comparative perspective on language variation in Europe, corresponding to our background in world-wide typology of contact languages (Michaelis et al. 2013) and general world-wide typology (Haspelmath et al. 2005). While variationist studies typically ask for patterns of variation within a single language, we ask whether there is a “big picture” in addition to all the details, in the tradition of A.W. von Schlegel. In particular, the topic of this paper is the replacement of synthetic patterns by analytic patterns that has interested typologists and historical linguists since the 19th century and that has recently been the focus of some prominent research in variationist studies of English and English-lexified creoles (Szmrecsanyi 2009; Kortmann & Szmrecsanyi 2011; Siegel et al. 2014; Szmrecsanyi 2016). By way of a first simple illustration, consider the examples in Table 1, where the symbol “>>” means that the newer pattern on the right-hand side competes with and tends to replace the older pattern on the left-hand side.
    [Show full text]