Non-Parametric Bayesian Areal Linguistics

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Non-Parametric Bayesian Areal Linguistics Non-Parametric Bayesian Areal Linguistics Hal Daume´ III School of Computing University of Utah Salt Lake City, UT 84112 [email protected] Abstract what areal features are (is there a linear ordering of “borrowability” (Katz, 1975; Curnow, 2001) or is We describe a statistical model over linguis- that too prescriptive?), and what causes sharing to tic areas and phylogeny. Our model recov- take place (does social status or number of speakers ers known areas and identifies a plausible hi- play a role (Thomason, 2001)?). erarchy of areal features. The use of areas improves genetic reconstruction of languages In this paper, we attempt to provide a statistical both qualitatively and quantitatively according answer to some of these questions. In particular, to a variety of metrics. We model linguistic we develop a Bayesian model of typology that al- areas by a Pitman-Yor process and linguistic lows for, but does not force, the existence of linguis- phylogeny by Kingman’s coalescent. tic areas. Our model also allows for, but does not force, preference for some feature to be shared are- 1 Introduction ally. When applied to a large typological database Why are some languages more alike than others? of linguistic features (Haspelmath et al., 2005), we This question is one of the most central issues in his- find that it discovers linguistic areas that are well torical linguistics. Typically, one of three answers documented in the literature (see Campbell (2005) is given (Aikhenvald and Dixon, 2001; Campbell, for an overview), and a small preference for cer- 2006). First, the languages may be related “genet- tain features to be shared areally. This latter agrees, ically.” That is, they may have all derived from a to a lesser degree, with some of the published hi- common ancestor language. Second, the similarities erarchies of borrowability (Curnow, 2001). Finally, may be due to chance. Some language properties we show that reconstructing language family trees is are simply more common than others, which is of- significantly aided by knowledge of areal features. ten attributed to be mostly due to linguistic univer- We note that Warnow et al. (2005) have indepen- sals (Greenberg, 1963). Third, the languages may dently proposed a model for phonological change in be related areally. Languages that occupy the same Indo-European (based on the Dyen dataset (Dyen et geographic area often exhibit similar characteristics, al., 1992)) that includes notions of borrowing. Our not due to genetic relatedness, but due to sharing. model is different in that we (a) base our model on Regions (and the languages contained within them) typological features rather than just lexical patterns that exhibit sharing are called linguistic areas and and (b) we explicitly represent language areas, not the features that are shared are called areal features. just one-time borrowing phenomena. Much is not understood or agreed upon in the field 2 Background of areal linguistics. Different linguists favor differ- We describe (in Section 3) a non-parametric, hier- ent defintions of what it means to be a linguistic area archical Bayesian model for finding linguistic areas (are two languages sufficient to describe an area or and areal features. In this section, we provide nec- do you need three (Thomason, 2001; Katz, 1975)?), essary background—both linguistic and statistical— 593 Human Language Technologies: The 2009 Annual Conference of the North American Chapter of the ACL, pages 593–601, Boulder, Colorado, June 2009. c 2009 Association for Computational Linguistics for understanding our model. Finnic languages (especially Estonian and Livo- 2.1 Areal Linguistics nian). (Sometimes many more are included, such as: Belorussian, Lavian, Lithuanian, Norwegian, Old Areal effects on linguistic typology have been stud- Prussian, Polish, Romani, Russian, Ukranian.) ied since, at least, the late 1920s by Trubetzkoy, though the idea of tracing family trees for languages Ethiopia: Afar, Amharic, Anyuak, Awngi, Beja, goes back to the mid 1800s and the comparative Ge’ez, Gumuz, Janjero, Kefa, Sidamo, Somali, Ti- study of historical linguistics dates back, perhaps to gre, Tigrinya and Wellamo. Giraldus Cambrenis in 1194 (Campbell, In press). Needless to say, the exact definition and extent of A recent article provides a short introduction to both the actual areas is up to significant debate. More- the issues that surround areal linguistics, as well as over, claims have been made in favor of many lin- an enumeration of many of the known language ar- guistic areas not defined above. For instance, Dixon eas (Campbell, 2005). A fairly wide, modern treat- (2001) presents arguments for several Australian lin- ment of the issues surrounding areal diffusion is also guistic areas and Matisoff (2001) defines a South- given by essays in a recent book edited by Aikhen- East Asian language area. Finally, although “folk vald and Dixon (2001). The essays in this book pro- lore” is in favor of identifying a linguistic area in- vide a good introduction to the issues in the field. cluding English, French and certain Norse languages Campbell (2006) provides a critical survey of these (Norwegian, Swedish, Low Dutch, High German, and other hypotheses relating to areal linguistics. etc.), there are counter-arguments to this position There are several issues which are basic to the (Thomason, 2001) (see especially Case Study 9.8). study of areal linguistics (these are copied almost 2.1.2 Linguistic Features directly from Campbell (2006)). Must a linguistic Identifying which linguistic features are most eas- area comprise more than two languages? Must it ily shared “areally” is a long standing problem in comprise more than one language family? Is a sin- contact linguistics. Here we briefly review some of gle trait sufficient to define an area? How “nearby” the major claims. Much of this overview is adoped must languages in an area be to one another? Are from the summary given by Curnow (2001). some feature more easily borrowed that others? Haugen (1950) considers only borrowability as Despite these formal definitional issues of what far as the lexicon is concerned. He provided evi- constitutes a language area and areal features, most dence that nouns are the easiest, followed by verbs, historical linguists seem to believe that areal effects adjectives, adverbs, prepositions, etc. Ross (1988) play some role in the change of languages. corroborates Haugen’s analysis and deepens it to 2.1.1 Established Linguistic Areas cover morphology, syntax and phonology. He pro- Below, we list some of the well-known linguistic poses the following hierarchy of borrowability (eas- areas; Campbell (2005) provides are more complete iest items coming first): nouns > verbs > adjectives listing together with example areal features for these > syntax > non-bound function words > bound areas. For each area, we list associated languages: morphemes > phonemes. Coming from a “con- The Balkans: Albanian, Bulgarian, Greek, Mace- straints” perspective, Moravcsik (1978) suggests donian, Rumanian and Serbo-Croatian. (Sometimes: that: lexical items must be borrowed before lexi- Romani and Turkish) cal properties; inflected words before bound mor- South Asian: Languages belonging to the Dravid- phemes; verbal items can never be borrowed; etc. ian, Indo-Aryan, Munda, Tibeto-Burman families. Curnow (2001) argues that coming up with a rea- Meso-America: Cuitlatec, Huave, Mayan, Mixe- sonable hierarchy of borrowability is that “we may Zoquean, Nahua, Otomanguean, Tarascan, Tequist- never be able to develop such constraints.” Never- latecan, Totonacan and Xincan. theless, he divides the space of borrowable features North-west America: Alsea, Chimakuan, Coosan, into 15 categories and discusses the evidence sup- Eyak, Haida, Kalapuyan, Lower Chinook, Salishan, porting each of these categories, including: phonet- Takelman, Tlingit, Tsimshian and Wakashan. ics (rare), phonology (common), lexical (very com- The Baltic: Baltic languages, Baltic German, and mon), interjections and discourse markers (com- 594 mon), free grammatical forms (occasional), bound the Pitman-Yor process, the Nth customer sits at a grammatical forms (rare), position of morphology new table with probability proportional to α + Kd (rare), syntactic frames (rare), clause-internal syntax and sits at a previously occupied table k with proba- (common), between-clause syntax (occasional). bility proportional to # d, where # is the num- k − k ber of customers already seated at table k. Finally, 2.2 Non-parametric Bayesian Models with each table k we associate a parameter θk, with We treat the problem of understanding areal linguis- each θk drawn independently from G0. An impor- tics as a statistical question, based on a database of tant property of the Pitman-Yor process is that draws typological information. Due to the issues raised in from it are exchangable: perhaps counterintuitively, the previous section, we do not want to commit to the distribution does not care about customer order. the existence of a particular number of linguistic ar- The Pitman-Yor process induces a power-law dis- eas, or particular sizes thereof. (Indeed, we do not tribution on the number of singleton tables (i.e., the even want to commit to the existence of any linguis- number of tables that have only one customer). This tic areas.) However, we will need to “unify” the can be seen by noticing two things. In general, languages that fall into a linguistic area (if such a the number of singleton tables grows as (αN d). thing exists) by means of some statistical param- O When d = 0, we obtain a Dirichlet process with the eter. Such problems have been studied under the number of singleton tables growing as (α log N). name non-parametric models. The idea behind non- O parametric models is that one does not commit a pri- 2.2.2 Kingman’s Coalescent ori to a particularly number of parameters. Instead, Kingman’s coalescent is a standard model in pop- we allow the data to dictate how many parameters ulation genetics describing the common genealogy there are.
Recommended publications
  • High Vowel Fricativization As an Areal Feature of the Northern Cameroon Grassfields
    High vowel fricativization as an areal feature of the northern Cameroon Grassfields Matthew Faytak WOCAL 8, | August 23, 2015 Overview High vowel fricativization is an areal or contact feature of the northern Grassfields, which carries implications for Niger-Congo reconstructions 1. What is (not) a fricativized vowel? 2. Where are they (not)? 3. Why is this interesting? Overview High vowel fricativization is an areal or contact feature of the northern Grassfields, which carries implications for Niger-Congo reconstructions 1. What is (not) a fricativized vowel? 2. Where are they (not)? 3. Why is this interesting? Fricative vowels Vowels with a fricative-like supralaryngeal constriction, which may or may not consistently result in audible fricative noise Not intended to encompass: • Devoiced or voiceless vowels Smith (2003) • Pre-/post-aspirated vowels Helgason (2002) Mortensen (2012) • Articulatory overlap at high speech rate • Wall noise sources in high vowels Shadle (1990) Prior mention Vowels that do fit one or both of the given definitions: • Apical vowels in Chinese Lee (2005) Feng (2007) Lee-Kim (2014) • Viby-i, G¨oteborges-i, etc. in Swedish Holmberg (1976) Engstrand et al. (2000) Sch¨otzet al. (2011) • Fricative vowels in Bantoid Medjo Mv´e(1997) Connell (2007) Articulatory evidence of constriction Olson and Meynadier (2015) Articulatory evidence of constriction z v " " High vowel fricativization (HVF) Fricative vowels arise from normal high vowels /i u/, often in tandem with raising of the next-lowest vowels Faytak (2014) i u e o i
    [Show full text]
  • Complexity As L2-Difficulty : Implications for Syntactic Change
    Erschienen in: Theoretical Linguistics ; 45 (2019), 3-4. - S. 183-209 https://dx.doi.org/10.1515/tl-2019-0012 Theoretical Linguistics 2019; 45(3-4): 183–209 George Walkden* and Anne Breitbarth Complexity as L2-difficulty: Implications for syntactic change https://doi.org/10.1515/tl-2019-0012 Abstract: Recent work has cast doubt on the idea that all languages are equally complex; however, the notion of syntactic complexity remains underexplored. Taking complexity to equate to difficulty of acquisition for late L2 acquirers, we propose an operationalization of syntactic complexity in terms of uninterpret- able features. Trudgill’s sociolinguistic typology predicts that sociohistorical situations involving substantial late L2 acquisition should be conducive to simplification, i.e. loss of such features. We sketch a programme for investigat- ing this prediction. In particular, we suggest that the loss of bipartite negation in the history of Low German and other languages indicates that it may be on the right track. Keywords: sociolinguistic typology, syntactic change, L2 acquisition, simplification, Interpretability Hypothesis 1 Context and big picture 1.1 Typology, complexity, and language change The traditional notion that all languages are equally complex, as expressed in Hockett (1958), has recently come under attack from a number of quarters. Notably, in his seminal work Sociolinguistic Typology, Trudgill (2011) has sug- gested that different types of sociolinguistic situation lead to differential sim- plification and complexification: for instance, long-term co-territorial language contact is predicted to lead to additive complexification, whereas short-term contact involving extensive adult second-language (L2) use is predicted to lead to simplification.
    [Show full text]
  • The Diachronic Emergence of Retroflex Segments in Three Languages* 1
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Hochschulschriftenserver - Universität Frankfurt am Main The diachronic emergence of retroflex segments in three languages* Silke Hamann ZAS (Centre for General Linguistics), Berlin [email protected] The present study shows that though retroflex segments can be considered articulatorily marked, there are perceptual reasons why languages introduce this class into their phoneme inventory. This observation is illustrated with the diachronic developments of retroflexes in Norwegian (North- Germanic), Nyawaygi (Australian) and Minto-Nenana (Athapaskan). The developments in these three languages are modelled in a perceptually oriented phonological theory, since traditional articulatorily-based features cannot deal with such processes. 1 Introduction Cross-linguistically, retroflexes occur relatively infrequently. From the 317 languages of the UPSI database (Maddieson 1984, 1986), only 8.5 % have a voiceless retroflex stop [ˇ], 7.3 % a voiced stop [Í], 5.3 % a voiceless retroflex fricative [ß], 0.9 % a voiced fricative [¸], and 5.9 % a retroflex nasal [˜]. Compared to other segmental classes that are considered infrequent, such as a palatal nasal [¯], which occurs in 33.7 % of the UPSID languages, and the uvular stop [q], which occurs in 14.8 % of the languages, the percentages for retroflexes are strikingly low. Furthermore, typically only large segment inventories have a retroflex class, i.e. at least another coronal segment (apical or laminal) is present, as in Sanskrit, Hindi, Norwegian, Swedish, and numerous Australian Aboriginal languages. Maddieson’s (1984) database includes only one exception to this general tendency, namely the Dravidian language Kota, which has a retroflex as its only coronal fricative.
    [Show full text]
  • 1. Introduction1 Geoffrey Haig and Geoffrey Khan
    1. Introduction1 Geoffrey Haig and Geoffrey Khan 1. Aims and scope of the volume This volume contains a series of descriptions of around 20 languages (or language groups), spoken across a region that includes most of eastern Turkey, western Iran and northern Iraq (see Fig. 1 for the locations), together with overview articles of sub-regions, and an appendix with selected lexical items from these languages. The region that we refer to here loosely as “Western Asia” is not clearly demar- cated, either politically or topographically, and requires a few words of explana- tion. Essentially it is linked to an ancient cultural core, namely the northern part of Mesopotamia, the upper catchment regions of the Tigris and Euphrates rivers. The book’s coverage goes beyond Mesopotamia in the narrower sense to include the surrounding mountainous regions of the Zagros in the southeast, and the elevated regions northwards and eastwards across the Anatolian plateau into the outliers of the Caucasus. These areas were all involved, at least economically and stra- tegically, in the succession of empires that arose in and around ancient Mesopo- tamia, beginning with the Sumerians in the third millennium BCE and continuing down to the Ottoman Empire into the dawn of the twentieth century. The sense of a common Kulturraum, while difficult to delineate precisely, is reflected by the growing recognition of linguistic parallels shared among the region’s languages, some of which we take up below. We have further divided the region into five sub-areas, each of which is treated in a section of the volume, with its own overview chapter.
    [Show full text]
  • Areal Features and Linguistic Reconstruction in Africa
    Areal features Hyman et al. Areal features and linguistic reconstruction in Introduction Africa Methodology The ALFA Project Tone database Larry M. Hyman1, Peter S. E. Jenks, Geoffrey Bacon, Nicolas Results 2 Tone Baier, Emily Clem , Matthew Faytak, Spencer Lamoureux, Vowel systems Florian Lionnet, John Merrill, Nicholas Rolle, and Hannah Sande3 Conclusion References [email protected], [email protected], [email protected] University of California, Berkeley 8th World Congress of African Linguistics Workshop 3: Areal Phenomena in Northern Sub-Saharan Africa 23 August 2015 Is Africa a linguistic area? Areal features Some scholars have claimed that Africa as a whole can be Hyman et al. considered a linguistic area Introduction Heine and Leyew (2008): 11 \African" linguistic properties Methodology Creissels et al. (2008): Africa forms a linguistic area based The ALFA Project on morphosyntactic data Tone database Results Others reject the idea of Africa as a single linguistic area, Tone but identify smaller linguistic areas on the continent Vowel systems Conclusion G¨uldemann(2008): The \Macro-Sudan Belt" is a References linguistic area Clements and Rialland (2008): Africa can be divided into 6 phonological areas Creissels (2015): West Africa is a linguistic area based on morphosyntactic tone and other features. More detailed study of the linguistic geography of Africa is needed in order to identify both macro- and micro-areas (Heine and Nurse, 2008) Goals of this talk Areal features Hyman et al. Introduction Based on data from 243 tone systems and 74 vowel Methodology systems, we argue that Africa as a whole does not form a The ALFA Project unified linguistic area Tone database Results Likewise, these data do not provide evidence for the Tone Vowel systems Macro-Sudan Belt as a unified phonological area Conclusion Instead, we find evidence for smaller linguistic micro-areas References South of Lake Chad The Nigeria/Cameroon border The Ghana/Togo/Burkina Faso cluster The Kru/Mande Zone Overview Areal features Hyman et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Areal Features of English in Ireland (Hickey 2012)
    ‘Areal features of English in Ireland’, in: Raymond Hickey (ed.) 2012. Areal Features of the Anglophone World. Berlin: de Gruyter Mouton, pp. 79-107. Areal features of English in Ireland Raymond Hickey University of Duisburg and Essen 1. Introduction The notion of linguistic area (from German Sprachbund, lit. ‘language federation’) is one which is often invoked when dealing with languages which share features and are found in a geographically contiguous area. Some of these areas have gained general acceptance in linguistic literature as the evidence for them is quite convincing. These include the Pacific North-West, Meso-America, the Balkans, the Baltic area, Arnhem Land in Northern Australia, Southern India (with Indic / Dravidian languages) to mention just some of the better known instances. Scholars concerned with linguistic areas, vary considerably in their approach and following Campbell (1998: 300) one can distinguish at least two basic orientations in the field: 1) circumstantianlist where authors are content with listing the various features of a putative area and 2) historicist where authors attempt to show that the shared features diffused historically through the languages of an area. The second approach is necessary to avoid the pitfall of raising features which may have arisen by chance to the level of defining characteristics of an area. Furthermore, it is essential to accord relative weight to features under consideration. Those which are typologically unspectacular cannot be appealed to when defining an area. For instance any set of languages or varieties which show a palatalisation of velars – and just that – could hardly be regarded as forming a linguistic area as this is a very general feature.
    [Show full text]
  • Schiffman, Harold F. TITLE Language and Society in South Asia. Final Report
    DOCUMEKT RESUNE ED 127 806 PL 007 948 AUTHOR Shapiro, Michael C.; Schiffman, Harold F. TITLE Language and Society in South Asia. Final Report. INSTITUTION Institute of International Studies (DHEW/OE), Washington, D.C. BUREAU NO BR-110012HH PUB DATE Sep 75 CONTRACT OEC-0-74-2093 NOTE 380p. EDRS PRICE MF-$C.83 Hc-$20.75 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS *Asian Studies; *Bilingualism; Burmese; Cultural Context; *Dialects; Dialect Studies; Dravidian Languages; Language Classification; *Language Variation; Linguistic Borrowing; Multilingualism; Regional Dialects; Social Dialects; *Sociolinguistics; Tibetan IDENTIFIERS *Asia (South); *Code Switching; Indo Aryan Languages; Munda Languages; Tibeto Burman Languages ABSTRACT This work attempts to provide an overview of liuguistic diversity in South Asia and to place this diversity in a cultural context. The work tries to describe the current state of knowledge concerning socially conditioned language variation in the subcontinent. Each of five major language families contains numerous mutually intelligible and unintelligible dialects. Different dialects of a language may be required for 'written and spoken use and for different social groups. Bilingualism and multilingualism are common for communication between groups. Language choice is important for education, politics, radio and television. Chapter 2 of this book enumerates criteria used in the taxonomy of language forms, discussing a number of theories of dialect formation from the points of view of linguistic innovation and diffusion of linguistic change. Chapter 3 surveys literature on classification of South Asian languages. Chapter 4 considers South Asia as a distinct linguistic area and Chapter 5 evaluates literature on South Asian social dialects. Chapter 6 examines linguistic codes encompassing elements from more than one autonomous language.
    [Show full text]
  • Nominal Plurality in Languages of the Greater Hindukush
    Nominal plurality in languages of the Greater Hindukush Noa Lange Department of Linguistics Bachelor’s Degree Project 15 HE credits General linguistics Kandidatprogrammet i lingvistik Spring term 2015 Supervisor: Henrik Liljegren Examinator: Henrik Liljegren Expert reviewer: Ljuba Veselinova Nominal plurality in languages of the Greater Hindukush Noa Lange Abstract The Greater Hindukush is an area that stretches from northwestern–central Afghanistan, through Khyber Pakhtunkhwa and Gilgit–Baltistan of Pakistan and to Kashmir in northwestern India. It is home to some fifty languages of various genera including Indo-Aryan, Iranian, Nuristani, Tibeto- Burman, Turkic and the isolate Burushaski. It has long been of interest in areal typology, and there has been some discussion of phonological and lexical features shared between the languages, presumed to have evolved due to language contiguity and contact. The purpose of the present study is to research a grammatical feature, namely the plural marking of common nouns, in a selection of languages spoken in the Greater Hindukush area, and to discuss its salience as an areal or sub-areal feature. Several grammars have been consulted in the extraction and analysis of all relevant information on nominal plurality in the languages. The results indicate a correlation between stem modification as a means of marking nouns for plural, as well as optionality in the overt plural marking, and the contiguity of some languages. Conclusively, stem change as a method of expression displays some moderate degree of areality, while optionality of nominal plural marking in particular is suggested as a sub-areal feature of the area. Keywords Nominal plurality, areal typology, Hindukush, obligatoriness, optionality Nominal pluralitet i språk som talas i Greater Hindukush Noa Lange Sammanfattning Greater Hindukush är ett område som sträcker sig från nordvästra–centrala Afghanistan, genom Khyber Pakhtunkhwa och Gilgit–Baltistan i Pakistan, till Kashmir i nordvästra Indien.
    [Show full text]
  • Languages in Sicily Between the Classical Age and Late Antiquity: a Case of Punctuated Equilibrium? Marta Capano (Università Degli Studi Di Napoli L’Orientale)
    Languages in Sicily between the Classical Age and Late Antiquity: a case of punctuated equilibrium? Marta Capano (Università degli Studi di Napoli L’Orientale) Abstract Although late Roman Sicily is clearly represented by the ancient authors as a multilingual environment, the 20th–century scientific debate has proposed two divergent descriptions of the Sicilian linguistic landscape. While some scholars denied a deep Latinization under the Roman Empire, the increasing evidence of Latin inscriptions led others to hypothesize the decline of Greek. In the last decades, new approaches to bilingualism and linguistic contact, applied to antiquity, have demonstrated that many languages frequently coexist for a long time. Multilingualism has always characterized Sicily, but, before the Roman conquest, all minority languages had gradually disappeared, and the diatopic and dialectal variation of Greek converged towards a slightly Doric κοινά. As we can see from the epigraphic evidence, Roman Sicily was fully Greek–Latin bilingual until the end of the 5th century, and the two languages influenced each other. Latin and Greek epigraphs show similar onomastic material and phonological and morphological features, as well as a number of shared set phrases (mostly from Latin). These data are consistent with the first phase of Dixon’s theory of “punctuated equilibrium”, namely the equilibrium, since the two populations had a similar population, lifestyle and religious beliefs and, although Romans ruled over Sicily, Greek language and culture never lost their prestige. Even though the quantity of Greek evidence is not stable over the course of the 5th century, Sicily ultimately displays a situation of equilibrium until the end of the 5th century.
    [Show full text]
  • Myth of the Caucasian Sprachbund: the Case of Ergativity
    The myth of the Caucasian Sprachbund: The case of ergativity. Kevin Tuite, Université de Montréal and ANU Research Centre for Linguistic Typology Département d’anthropologie Université de Montréal Case postale 6128, Succursale centre-ville Montréal, Québec H3C 3J7, Canada [email protected] 1. Introduction. The study of linguistic areas, or “Sprachbünde”, owes a great deal to the important research of Murray B. Emeneau on the geographical distribution of linguistic features among the languages of India and its neighboring countries. In the introduction he wrote to a collection of his essays, Emeneau observes, with deserved pride, that his work has “introduced the Indian linguistic area … as a worthy partner of the Balkans or the Caucasus” (Emeneau, 1980). I hope that the distinguished Sankritist and Dravidologist will not take it amiss if I use a quotation from his writings to introduce a paper in which I will argue that the Caucasus, whatever it might be in geolinguistic terms, is not a Sprachbund like the Balkans or, for that matter, the Indian subcontinent. 1.1. Trubetzkoy on Sprachbünde. The term Sprachbund was introduced to the linguistic world seventy years ago (although it had appeared in a tract on cultural themes by Trubetzkoy five years earlier (Toman, 1995: 204)), on the occasion of the 1st International Congress of Linguists in April 1928. At a session devoted to the “Établissement et délimination des termes techniques”, the phonologist Nikolai Trubetzkoy proposed that, in order avoid “Missverständnisse und Fehler” in the classification of languages, a distinction be made between Sprachfamilien and Sprachbünde: Gruppen, bestehend aus Sprachen, die eine große Ähnlichkeit in syntaktischer Hinsicht; eine Ähnlichkeit in den Grundsätzen des morphologischen Baues aufweisen; und eine große Anzahl gemeinsamer Kulturwörter bieten, manchmal auch äussere Ähnlichkeit im Bestande der Lautsystem, — dabei aber keine systematischen Lautentsprechungen, Myth of the Caucasian Sprachbund (K.
    [Show full text]
  • The Phonetics and Phonology of Retroflexes Published By
    The Phonetics and Phonology of Retroflexes Published by LOT phone: +31 30 253 6006 Trans 10 fax: +31 30 253 6000 3512 JK Utrecht e-mail: [email protected] The Netherlands http://wwwlot.let.uu.nl/ Cover illustration by Silke Hamann ISBN 90-76864-39-X NUR 632 Copyright © 2003 Silke Hamann. All rights reserved. The Phonetics and Phonology of Retroflexes Fonetiek en fonologie van retroflexen (met een samenvatting in het Nederlands) Proefschrift ter verkrijging van de graad van doctor aan de Universiteit Utrecht op gezag van de Rector Magnificus, Prof. Dr. W.H. Gispen, ingevolge het besluit van het College voor Promoties in het openbaar te verdedigen op vrijdag 6 juni 2003 des middags te 4.15 uur door Silke Renate Hamann geboren op 25 februari 1971 te Lampertheim, Duitsland Promotoren: Prof. dr. T. A. Hall (Leipzig University) Prof. dr. Wim Zonneveld (Utrecht University) Contents 1 Introduction 1 1.1 Markedness of retroflexes 3 1.2 Phonetic cues and phonological features 6 1.3 Outline of the dissertation 8 Part I: Phonetics of Retroflexes 2 Articulatory variation and common properties of retroflexes 11 2.1 Phonetic terminology 12 2.2 Parameters of articulatory variation 14 2.2.1 Speaker dependency 15 2.2.2 Vowel context 16 2.2.3 Speech rate 17 2.2.4 Manner dependency 19 2.2.4.1 Plosives 19 2.2.4.2 Nasals 20 2.2.4.3 Fricatives 21 2.2.4.4 Affricates 23 2.2.4.5 Laterals 24 2.2.4.6 Rhotics 25 2.2.4.7 Retroflex vowels 26 2.2.5 Language family 27 2.2.6 Iventory size 28 2.3 Common articulatory properties of retroflexion 32 2.3.1 Apicality 33 2.3.2 Posteriority
    [Show full text]
  • Lenition in Irish English
    In: Alison Henry, Martin Ball and Margaret MacAliskey (eds) Papers from the International Conference on Language in Ireland. Belfast Working Papers in Language and Linguistics, 13. Belfast: University of Ulster, 1996, 173-93. Lenition in Irish English Raymond Hickey Essen University Abstract. Consonant weakening (lenition) is a feature of both Irish and Irish English which reaches back far in the histories of both languages. Its manifestation and its system status is quite different in both cases so that any simple transfer theory will fail to account for lenition in Irish English. The present paper will be primarily concerned with describing the phonological conditions for consonant weakening in Irish English and with putting it in the broader perspective of similar phenomena in other varieties of English and finally with addressing the question of its genesis within Irish English, above all in the south of the country. Attention with be paid to its distribution in urban and rural varieties of the Republic and to its extent in colloquial and less- received registers of Irish English. Introduction There is general acceptance that the term ‘lenition’ refers to phonetic weakening, i.e. an increase in segmental sonority diachronically or occasionally as a morphological device as in Irish and other Celtic languages. Lenition is a phenomenon which is common in many languages which usually manifests itself as a shift from stop to fricative or a shift from voiceless to voiced with obstruents. Lenition normally consists of several steps and historically a language may exhibit a shift from stop to zero via a number of intermediary stages.
    [Show full text]