A Pragmatic Theory of Discourse About the Self / by Ted

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

A Pragmatic Theory of Discourse About the Self / by Ted THE EXPRESSION OF THE SELF AND CONVERSATIONAL CONTEXT: A PRAGMATIC THEORY OF DISCOURSE ABOUT THE SELF Ted Wayne Altar M.A., University of Regina, 1983 THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY in the Department of PSYCHOLOGY Ted Wayne Altar 1993 SIMON FRASER UNIVERSITY July 1993 All rights reserved. This work may not be reproduced in whole or in part, by photocopy or other means, without permission of the author. APPROVAL Name: Ted Wayne Altar Degree: Doctor of Philosophy Title of ~hesis: The Expression of the Self and Conversational Context: A Pragmatic Theory of Discourse about the Self Examining Committee: Chair: Dr. James Marcia Profegsar 7 Dr. Anand Paranjpe, Professor Sdor Superwkmr - Dr. Bar- ~e~erstein, Associate Professor - Dr. Vito Modigliani, Professor. Dr,, Ti+0. alli ins on, Professor Emeritus Internal External Examiner - - Dr. Anthony Greenwald, Professor Department of Psychology University of Washington External Examiner Date Approved: PARTIAL COPYRIGHT LICENSE I hereby grant to Simon Fraser University the right to lend my thesis, project or extended essay (the title of which is shown below) to users of the Simon Fraser University Library, and to make partial or single copies only for such users or in response to a request from the library of any other university, or other educational institution, on its own behalf or for one of its users. I further agree that permission for multiple copying of this work for scholarly purposes may be granted by me or the Dean of Graduate Studies. It is understood that copying or publication of this work for financial gain shall not be allowed without my written permission. Title of Thesis/Project/Extended Essay The Expression of the Self and Conversational Context: A Pragmatic Theory of Discourse about the Self Author: - - (s i gnature) Ted Wayne Altar (name) ABSTRACT The first half of this dissertation provides a broad review and analysis of the concept of the self, the phenomenon of self-reference, and the use of metaphors in theorectical accounts of the self. The second half of the dissertation explores the role of the conversational context upon people's self-presentations of themselves, particularly the proportion of self-evaluative statements as elicited by open-ended probes for self- disquisition. Although the resurgence of interest in the study of the self has made for a greater awareness of the interpretative importance of context on a person's self- description, the implicit contextual effects of the experimenters' own choice of open- ended questions and enquiry into people's notions of themselves has not rece!.-ed due attention as part of a "collaborative interaction." The articulation of a person's self- concept as elicited by self-report instruments like the "Who are you?YWAY) or "tell us about yourself' open-ended questionnaires was here found to be highly variable with respect to minor re-wording of those questions or to the imagined situation in which those same questions are asked. A key finding of this study is that one can practically reverse previous research claims that at most only 10% of the "spontaneous self' is explicitly self-evaluative. From the perspective of a Gricean or pragmatic model of discourse interpretation, it is proposed that a person's self-related constructs are always interactive and guided by prototypic scenarios of discourse relevancy. Results were supportive of the prediction that in order to determine how the experimental question should be relevantly answered, participants in a psychology experiment must resort to the inference-making strategy of drawing upon situational prototypes of comparable questions found in everyday conversations. iii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I wish to convey deep thanks, and also acknowledge my considerable debt to, my supervisor Dr. hand Paranjpe, whose breadth of knowledge, scholarship and deep passion for interpersonal tolerance and understanding has been an inspiration to a great many students including me. Earnest gratitude also goes to my patient and forgiving committee members, Dr. Barry Beyerstein and Dr. Vito Modigliani whose support and exemplary qualities as outstanding teachers were much appreciated and needed. I am very grateful to the external members of the examining committee, Dr. Anthony Greenwald and Dr. Tom Mallinson, for their careful reading and thoughtful criticism of the dissertation. Dr. Greenwald was particularly helpful with crucial points of analysis. I wish also to thank Dr. Marian Scholtmeijer, Gira Bhatt, Cindy Letts and .i ,A1n McDonald who served during some stages of the research as my indispensable research assistants. Specifically, Gira Bhatt, Cindy Letts and John MacDonald helped with some of the collection of the data and Marian Sholtmeijer and Gira Bhatt both generously served as tireless judges for the content analysis of a substantial amount of data. I also wish to make known my appreciation of the collegial friendship and debates with such capable and outstanding fellow students as Brian Grady, Chris Green, Gira Bhatt, Randy Tonks, Dave Eveleigh, Ross Powell, and many others. Of course, gratitude is owed to the Simon Fraser Department of Psychology and the Faculty of Graduate Studies for their financial and moral support. Finally, my deepest appreciation and love go to my companion, Marian Scholtmeijer, for her reading of the dissertation and thoughtful comments as a reader unfamiliar with this kind of research. TABLE OF CONTENTS I. THE CONCEPT OF THE RELATIONAL SELF 1 A. PREAMBLE 1 B. SELF-REFERENCE AND THE CONCEIT OF THE SELF 10 1. The Construction of Self-Reference in Folk Psychology 10 a. Descriptive import 15 b. Explanatory import 16 c. Ontic significations 17 d. Evaluative import 18 2. The Self in Academic Psychology and the Plurality of Self-concepts 20 11. METAPHOR AND UNDERSTANDING OF THE SELF 31 A. Previous Metaphors of the Self 37 1. Between the ape and the computer 37 2. Metaphors of the mirror, property, schema and text 4 3 111. CONVERSATION AS ACTION IN CONTEXT 52 A. THE APPROACH OF GRICEAN PRAGMATICS 55 IV. THE CONCEPTUAL SELF AS SUBJECT FOR DISCLOSURE 63 A. ASSUMITIONS OF THE WAY QUESTIONNAIRES AND THE ESSENTIALIST NOTION OF THE SELF 65 1. An Open-ended Measure of the Conceptual Self 65 2. The Assumptions of the WAY 66 3. McGuire's Notion of the Spontaneous Self 70 B. METHODOLOGICAL CRITIQUE OF THE WAY 7 1 C. THE RELEVANCY HYPOTHESIS 74 V. EXPERIMENT 1 78 A. METHOD 81 1. Materials and design 81 2. Coding scheme 83 3. Subjects 84 B. RESULTS 85 1. Discussion 88 VI. EXPERIMENT 2 89 A. METHOD 89 1. Materials and design 89 2. Procedure and Participants 92 B. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION 93 VII. EXPERIMENT 3 99 A. MATERIALS AND DESIGN 99 B. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION 99 VIII. EXPERIMENT 4 102 B. METHOD 103 1. Materials and Procedure 103 C. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION 104 IX. CONCLUSION 106 X. References 122 XI. Appendix: Questionnaires 142 LIST OF TABLES AND FIGURES table 1: key characteristics of self-discourse table 2: type of way question and corresponding situation where such a question is appropriate table 3: percentage of participants matching requests with situations table 4: mean inter-judge proportions of self-evaluations per person table 5: mean ranks of commonplaceness of requests & mean total responses table 6: experimental design: version of the question over situations table 7: mean proportion of evaluative/total responses within situations and way variants figure 1: mean proportion of evaluative responses per question over situations table 8: mean proportion of evaluative remarks I. THE CONCEPT OF THE RELATIONAL SELF A. PREAMBLE Not only do we experience ourselves in the world, we also endeavor to understand and give account of ourselves. The word self--either alone or as the first stem in over 200 hyphenated expressions like "self-abandonment," "self-abasing," "self-abhorrence," etc.--is a much-used word in the composing of such accounts and has come to stand for something we place great stock in and for the most part value highly. The established use of this word, as designating something of essential importance about ourselves, proves to be all the more singular when we are told that this same word became the kind of substantive it is today through a solecism arising from the use of the word "self' in print. "Selfes," as, for example, in the Old English "his selfes" or "as they wished for themselves" (Davis, 1974), was simply a pronominal adjective to indicate emphatically that the reference is to "him" and not, or not merely, to some other. In talking to another, one makes reference to oneself by this interlocutionary emphasis upon the speaker being the bearer, locus, or source of what is being said. This use survives in rare and ironic oblique cases, such as "Himself wants his tea." Possession or nominal identity was all that was indicated by this emphatic or reflexive function of selfe, as it is in today's compounds myselfj yourselfj and itself. But, as Peggy Rosenthal (1984) argues, "since the spelling was the same as if selfes were a genitive noun modified by his, selfes came to be seen in this case as a noun" (p. 9). What semantic weight this "spelling accident," as Rosenthal calls it, has come to possess since the 12th and 13th centuries! Critics like Mayo (1952) point to this curious history of the word in the English-speaking world and suggest that nothing substantial but only bad grammar and logic is being designated by any reference to a self. We have fallen into a Rylean "category mistake" in believing there to be reference to some private substantive when only a nominal or emphatic reference to the visible person is being made. We have made a false leap from intensives like itself, oneself, myself, etc., to the idea of a substantive Self.
Recommended publications
  • Chapter 1: Prescientific Psychology
    Prescientific Psychology 1 Start with these facts. Psychology is the most popular elective in American high schools today. Furthermore, psychology is one of the two or three most popular undergraduate majors in North American colleges. People cannot seem to get enough of psychol- ogy; it is everywhere today. It is the substance of movies, novels, computer games, social media, magazines, television shows, tabloid newspapers, radio talk shows, and music lyrics. Clearly, there is no shortage of public interest in psychology. People are interested in behavior—their own, their relatives, their neighbors, their cowork- ers, and even strangers who they know only through the media of books, magazines, or television shows such as soap operas, courtroom programs, game shows, situation comedies, dramas, and the so-called “reality” shows. There seems to be a never-ending fascination with human behavior that is perhaps inherent in human nature. It is likely that such an interest has afforded evolutionary advantages. An individual ’ s ability to understand and, better still, anticipate the behavior of others has survival value. Psy- chologists refer to this public interest in psychology as popular psychology. It isn ’ t psychology of the form that would be recognized by most psychologists as scientific psychology. Indeed, many psychologists would be embarrassed by any association with it. However, the public loves it, and it is their psychology. Psychology has existed, no doubt, from the very beginnings of human history. When hominids first walked erect on the earth, facing a life expectancy of perhaps 30 years, a life beset with hardships and dangers that could hardly be imagined today, these early individuals were in need of human comfort, of reassurance, of empa- thy, and of guidance.
    [Show full text]
  • Final Thesis Shahram Rafieian Koupaei.Pdf
    Bangor University DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Dissociation, unconscious and social theory : towards an embodied relational sociology Rafieian Koupaei, Shahram Award date: 2015 Awarding institution: Bangor University Link to publication General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain • You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal ? Take down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Download date: 29. Sep. 2021 Dissociation, unconscious and social theory towards an embodied relational sociology Thesis submitted for examination for: PhD in Sociology and Social Policy Shahram Rafieian koupaei School of Social Sciences Bangor University 2015 I hereby declare that (i) the thesis is not one for which a degree has been or will be conferred by any other university or institution; (ii) the thesis is not one for which a degree has already been conferred by this University; (iii) the work for the thesis is my own work and that, where material submitted by me for another degree or work undertaken by me as part of a research group has been incorporated into the thesis, the extent of the work thus incorporated has been clearly indicated.
    [Show full text]
  • The Adult Learner: a Neglected Species. INSTITUTION American Society for Training and Development, Madison, Wis
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 084 368 CE 000 509 AUTHOR Knowles, Malcolm TITLE The Adult Learner: A Neglected Species. INSTITUTION American Society for Training and Development, Madison, Wis. PUB DATE Apr 73 NOTE 207p. AVAILABLE FROM Gulf Publishing Company, P.O. Box 2608, Houston, TX 77001 ($7.95) EDRS PRICE MF-$0.65 HC Not Available from EDRS. DESCRIPTORS *Adult Education; *Adult Learning; Behavior Patterns; *Human Resources; Individual Development; Learning Characteristics; Learning Experience; Learning Motivation; *Learning Theories; Manpower Development; Organizational Development; Teaching Models; *Teaching Techniques; Training ABSTRACT Traditional theories of learning and the teaching practices resulting from them are reviewed. Most theories of adult learning are based on research into the learning of children, which in turn is founded upon theories of animal learning. These theories, formulated under laboratory conditions, are artificial at best--and not complex enough to apply to adult human beings. Emerging theories of adult learning, however, are Lased on the unique characteristics of adults as learners and result in differentiated educational practices. Human resource development (BED) is based on many of these newer theories and serves as a guideline for action. Knowles' andragogical theory is based on four assumptions which differ from those of pedagogy: (1) changes in self-concept,(2) the role of experience, (3) readiness to learn, and (4) orientation to learning. As a guideline for developing programs and for selecting andtraining teachers, the andragogical model of HRD is very applicab?e. Among the appendixes are "Is It Skinner or Nothing" and "An Approach to a Differential Psychology of the Adult Potential." There is a eleven-page bibliography.
    [Show full text]
  • History of Psychology
    The Psych 101 Series James C. Kaufman, PhD, Series Editor Department of Educational Psychology University of Connecticut David C. Devonis, PhD, received his doctorate in the history of psychology from the University of New Hampshire’s erstwhile pro- gram in that subject in 1989 with a thesis on the history of conscious pleasure in modern American psychology. Since then he has taught vir- tually every course in the psychology curriculum in his academic odys- sey from the University of Redlands in Redlands, California, and the now-closed Teikyo Marycrest University (formerly Marycrest College in Davenport, Iowa) to—for the past 17 years—Graceland University in Lamoni, Iowa, alma mater of Bruce Jenner and, more famously for the history of psychology, of Noble H. Kelly (1901–1997), eminent con- tributor to psychology’s infrastructure through his many years of ser- vice to the American Board of Examiners in Professional Psychology. Dr. Devonis has been a member of Cheiron: The International Society for the History of Behavioral and Social Sciences since 1990, a con- tributor to many of its activities, and its treasurer for the past 10 years. Currently he is on the editorial board of the American Psychological Association journal History of Psychology and is, with Wade Pickren, coeditor and compiler of the online bibliography History of Psychology in the Oxford Bibliographies Online series. History of Psychology 101 David C. Devonis, PhD Copyright © 2014 Springer Publishing Company, LLC All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or trans- mitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, without the prior permission of Springer Publishing Company, LLC, or authorization through payment of the appropriate fees to the Copyright Clearance Cen- ter, Inc., 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, 978-750-8400, fax 978-646-8600, [email protected] or on the Web at www.copyright.com.
    [Show full text]
  • History and Systems in Psychology Fall 2000
    PSYC 6066: History and Systems of Psychology Summer 2017 Professor: Mark E. Mattson, Ph.D. Office: Dealy 432c & LL819c Voicemail LC: 212-636-6356 E-mail: [email protected] Office hours: Monday and Wednesday 12:30-1:00 and other times by appointment Required textbook: Benjamin, Jr., L.T. (Ed.) (2009). A history of psychology: Original sources and contemporary research (3rd ed.). Malden MA: Blackwell Pub. Other required readings: download from ARES: password=mattson6066 Recommended books: Fancher, R. & Rutherford, A. (2017). Pioneers of psychology (5th ed.). New York: Norton. American Psychological Association. (2009). Publication manual of the American Psychological Association (6th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Goals of the course 1. Cover the history of psychology, through primary and secondary sources 2. Consider research methodology in history and psychology 3. Identify the major problems that have concerned psychologists 4. Practice research, writing, and presentation skills A satisfactory grade in the course (A-) will result from satisfactory performance on all four components: class participation, class presentations, a research project, and a final essay. 1. Class participation Come to class prepared to discuss/comment on the readings from Benjamin and ARES. Attendance is expected, with no more than one excused absence. 2. Class presentations The objective is to develop and make a formal presentation that teaches the class the essentials of one of the following history topics: women in psychology history of clinical psychology race and psychology counseling psychology psychometrics, especially psychological tests psychiatry/psychoanalysis applied psychology incl. forensic developmental psychology The due date for each topic is on the course schedule. Create and distribute a handout to the class and instructor.
    [Show full text]
  • TANREND 2015/2016 Elektív És Fakultatív Tárgyak
    Pécsi Tudományegyetem Általános Orvostudományi Kar ÁLTALÁNOS ORVOS SZAK TANREND 2015/2016 Elektív és fakultatív tárgyak PTE ÁOK Általános orvos szak – Elektív és fakultatív modul - tantárgyleírások - 2015/2016-os tanév 1. szemeszter OOE-AKF _____ Alapfogalmak és közlési funkciók az angol orvosi szaknyelvben ________________________________ 8 OOE-BFA _____ A biofizika fizikai alapjai ______________________________________________________________ 10 OOE-BO1 _____ Biofizika szemináriumok 1. ____________________________________________________________ 12 OOE-ELH _____ Elhízás - a modern kor „járványa” _______________________________________________________ 13 OOE-ESO _____ Egészség-szociológia _________________________________________________________________ 15 OOE-IKG _____ Interkulturelle Kommunikation in Gesundheitsberufen _______________________________________ 17 OOE-JNK _____ Jelnyelvi kommunikáció az orvosi praxisban ______________________________________________ 19 OOE-KRM ____ Krisenmanagement bei Studium um Arbeit im Bereich Medizin _______________________________ 21 OOE-MS1 _____ Molekuláris sejtbiológiai kísérletek I. ____________________________________________________ 23 OOE-N06 _____ Orvosi terminológia __________________________________________________________________ 25 OOE-ORE _____ Orvosi rehabilitáció a gyakorlatban ______________________________________________________ 27 OOE-SMA _____ Szakszövegalkotás és szaknyelvi műfajok az angol orvosi szaknyelvben _________________________ 29 OOE-SSS _____ Sprache, Spracherwerb,
    [Show full text]
  • The Persuaders: Nonbehavioristic Psychologists
    The Persuaders 271 high as well when the community is persuaded to change and it accepts a successful Persuader as a guide for some time. Practically all the "great names" in the history of science can be viewed as successful Persuaders. This chapter presents two apparently successful Persuaders and two relatively unsuccessful ones. At any one time there are probably many The Persuaders: unsuccessful Persuaders in a scientific community. They may be said to represent the seeds of potential change; but most of these seeds fall Nonbehavioristic Psychologists on arid ground. The chances of success for any Persuader is small. Yet without the presence of such people, the scientific community would be Whoso would be a man must be a non-conformist. -RALPH WALDOEMERSON left with no coherent program of development when its current course faltered. Timing is a critical ingredient in the process of successful persua- ~ion.In psychology as elsewhere, a receptive audience is one that has almost arrived at the same conclusion by itself. Thus, Plans and the Struc- Although behaviorism unquestionably dominated scientific psychology ture ofBehiuiio~,by George A. Miller, Eugene Galanter, and Karl Pribram, until well into the 1950s, there were numerous cross-currents even in had a significant impact on the cognitive revolution, in part because it the heyday of behaviorism. I have already pointed to social psychology appeared in 1960, just as many psychologists were preparing to think as one field in which many of the major developments in the cognitive more cognitively. Had it bee$ published five years earlier, it would have framework were foreshadowed (Chapter 4).
    [Show full text]
  • PCS AP Psych 0280
    A Planned Course of Study for Advanced Placement Psychology ASHS Course # 0280 Abington School District Abington, Pennsylvania September, 2016 a. Objectives Students will demonstrate the appropriate level of proficiency in each of the following areas: History and Approaches • Recognize how philosophical and physiological perspectives shaped the development of psychological thought . • Describe and compare different theoretical approaches in explaining behavior: — structuralism, functionalism, and behaviorism in the early years; — Gestalt, psychoanalytic/psychodynamic, and humanism emerging later; — evolutionary, biological, cognitive, and biopsychosocial as more contemporary approaches . • Recognize the strengths and limitations of applying theories to explain behavior . • Distinguish the different domains of psychology (e .g ., biological, clinical, cognitive, counseling, developmental, educational, experimental, human factors, industrial–organizational, personality, psychometric, social) . • Identify major historical figures in psychology (e .g ., Mary Whiton Calkins, Charles Darwin, Dorothea Dix, Sigmund Freud, G . Stanley Hall, William James, Ivan Pavlov, Jean Piaget, Carl Rogers, B . F . Skinner, Margaret Floy Washburn, John B . Watson, Wilhelm Wundt) Research Methods • Differentiate types of research (e .g ., experiments, correlational studies, survey research, naturalistic observations, case studies) with regard to purpose, strengths, and weaknesses . • Describe how research design drives the reasonable conclusions that can be
    [Show full text]
  • Thinking About Trance Over a Century: the Making of a Set of Impasses
    Thinking about Trance Over a Century: The Making of a Set of Impasses The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters Citation Harrington, Anne. 2016. Thinking about Trance Over a Century: The Making of a Set of Impasses. In Hypnosis and meditation: Towards an integrative science of conscious planes, eds. Michael Lifshitz and Amir Raz. New York: Oxford University Press. Published Version https://global.oup.com/academic/product/hypnosis-and- meditation-9780198759102?cc=us&lang=en&# Citable link http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:33439060 Terms of Use This article was downloaded from Harvard University’s DASH repository, and is made available under the terms and conditions applicable to Open Access Policy Articles, as set forth at http:// nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:dash.current.terms-of- use#OAP Part 2 Philosophical, historical, and cultural perspectives Chapter 2 Thinking about trance over a century The making of a set of impasses Anne Harrington Abstract Despite differences in methods and (usually) goals, both hypnosis and meditation involve an unusual state of awareness, generally known as “trance.” Yet, the idea of trance, as an object of scholarly and scientific study, turns out to have been marked, historically, by confusion and controversy. Is trance one thing or many things? A regression to a pathological, primitive state or ascent to an elevated state? Noisy or quiet? Biological or social? Meditation researchers, hypnosis researchers, and anthropologists (interested in phenomena like shamanism and spirit possession) have all, historically, struggled with questions like these in surprisingly similar ways.
    [Show full text]
  • Myths and Misconceptions About the Psychology of Adolescence and Intimate Relationships
    Psychology, 2019, 10, 2166-2176 https://www.scirp.org/journal/psych ISSN Online: 2152-7199 ISSN Print: 2152-7180 Myths and Misconceptions about the Psychology of Adolescence and Intimate Relationships Adrian Furnham1*, Simmy Grover2 1Department of Leadership and Organisational Behaviour, Norwegian Business School (BI), Nydalveien, Olso, Norway 2Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, UK How to cite this paper: Furnham, A., & Abstract Grover, S. (2019). Myths and Misconcep- tions about the Psychology of Adolescence This study examined the prevalence of psychological myths in two areas of and Intimate Relationships. Psychology, 10, psychology: Adolescence and Relationships. All 517 participants completed 2166-2176. two questionnaires in which they rated to what extent they thought various https://doi.org/10.4236/psych.2019.1015137 statements/facts about Adolescence and about Intimate Relationships were Received: November 29, 2019 True or False. A large number of these myths were rated as True (Definitely Accepted: December 24, 2019 or Partly). There were few significant demographic correlates of the total Published: December 27, 2019 correct score (determined by rating the myth as False). Implications are dis- cussed. Copyright © 2019 by author(s) and Scientific Research Publishing Inc. Keywords This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution International Psychology, Education, Myths, Misconceptions, Psychological Knowledge License (CC BY 4.0). http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ Open Access 1. Introduction There has been an academic interest in psychological myths and misconceptions for nearly a hundred years (Amsel, Baird, & Ashley, 2011; Brown, 1983; Furn- ham, 1992, 1993; Gaze, 2014; Hughes, Lyddy, & Lambe, 2013; Kowalski & Tay- lor, 2009; Nixon, 1925; Lamal, 1979; McKeachie, 1960; Taylor & Kowalski, 2004; Tupper & Williams, 1986; Vaughan, 1977).
    [Show full text]
  • Cinematic Psychotherapy Stereotypes
    Clinical Science Insights Cinematic Psychotherapy Stereotypes ByAdam Fisher, PhD to promote common misconceptions about psychotherapy Postdoctoral Clinical Fellow for dramatic effect (Lasky, 2006). For example, mental health treatment is commonly portrayed as ineffectual In perhaps the earliest on-screen fictional portrayal of a (Kemshal-Bell, 2010), and when it does work, it is shown mental health professional, a young woman was depicted as to be effective through love (e.g., Mozart and the Whale), being controlled by a hypnotist in the 1896 silent film Trilby. sexual boundary violations by the female professional Psychotherapists and other mental health professionals (e.g., The Prince of Tides) or other unethical behaviors have been portrayed in well over 5,000 films (Flowers & (Macfarlane, 2004; Otto & McGroarty, 2012; Wedding, Frizler, 2004), and across many genres including drama, Boyd, & Niemiec, 2010), or a “miraculous minute cure.” horror, musical, western, and even hardcore pornography The latter may foster unrealistic expectations for real-life (Greenberg, 2000). Indeed, 17% of the most popular films of patients, who tend to experience added distress when their the 1990s portrayed at least one mental health professional concerns are not as easily resolved (Orchowski, Spickard, & (Young, Boester, Whitt, & Stevens, 2008). McNamara, 2006). Mental health professionals have long been concerned Although some have argued that portrayals have about the potential negative effects of these portrayals become more balanced overall in the last decade in U.S. (Peterson, 2001; Schultz, 2004b), including concerns that (Niemiec & Wedding, 2006) and international cinema viewers will be less likely to seek help after viewing them (Niemiec, 2010), films from 1999 to 2010 portrayed (Stuart, 2006), that consumers’ perceptions of mental health mental health professionals acting unethically more often care will be negatively impacted (Vogel, Gentile, & Kaplan, than not (Otto & McGroarty, 2012).
    [Show full text]
  • Can Psychology Become a Science?
    Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 281–288 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Can psychology become a science? Scott O. Lilienfeld Department of Psychology, Emory University, Room 473, 36 Eagle Row, Atlanta 30322, Georgia article info abstract Article history: I am profoundly grateful to Tom Bouchard for helping me learn to think scientifically. Scientific thinking, Received 3 December 2009 which is characterized by a set of safeguards against confirmation bias, does not come naturally to the Received in revised form 16 January 2010 human species, as the relatively recent appearance of science in history attests. Even today, scientific Accepted 25 January 2010 thinking is in woefully short supply in many domains of psychology, including clinical psychology and Available online 23 February 2010 cognate disciplines. I survey five key threats to scientific psychology – (a) political correctness, (b) radical environmentalism, (c) the resurrection of ‘‘common sense” and intuition as arbiters of scientific truth, Keywords: (d) postmodernism, and (e) pseudoscience – and conclude that these threats must be confronted directly Psychology by psychological science. I propose a set of educational and institutional reforms that should place Science Pseudoscience psychology on firmer scientific footing. Postmodernism Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Psychotherapy 1. Can psychology become a science? groups, they would need to possess above zero validity for at least one subgroup. When I entered graduate school in psychology at the University Of course, a naïve graduate student can perhaps be forgiven for of Minnesota in the Fall of 1982, I was a bright-eyed, bushy-tailed such logical errors, especially one embarking on his training nearly 21 year-old eager to learn about the mysteries of the mind.
    [Show full text]