American Psychologist

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

American Psychologist American Psychologist Public Skepticism of Psychology: Why Many People Perceive the Study of Human Behavior as Unscientific Scott O. Lilienfeld Online First Publication, June 13, 2011. doi: 10.1037/a0023963 CITATION Lilienfeld, S. O. (2011, June 13). Public Skepticism of Psychology: Why Many People Perceive the Study of Human Behavior as Unscientific. American Psychologist. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1037/a0023963 Public Skepticism of Psychology Why Many People Perceive the Study of Human Behavior as Unscientific Scott O. Lilienfeld Emory University Data indicate that large percentages of the general public and allied trends (see Redding & O’Donohue, 2009, and regard psychology’s scientific status with considerable Tierney, 2011, for recent discussions) have retarded the skepticism. I examine 6 criticisms commonly directed at the growth of scientific psychology. Others (e.g., Dawes, 1994; scientific basis of psychology (e.g., psychology is merely Lilienfeld, Lynn, & Lohr, 2003; Thyer & Pignotti, in press) common sense, psychology does not use scientific methods, have assailed the scientific status of large swaths of clinical psychology is not useful to society) and offer 6 rebuttals. I psychology, counseling psychology, and allied mental then address 8 potential sources of public skepticism to- health disciplines, contending that these fields have been ward psychology and argue that although some of these overly permissive of poorly supported practices. Still oth- sources reflect cognitive errors (e.g., hindsight bias) or ers (e.g., S. Koch, 1969; Meehl, 1978) have bemoaned the misunderstandings of psychological science (e.g., failure to at times painfully slow pace of progress of psychology, distinguish basic from applied research), others (e.g., psy- especially in the “softer” domains of social, personality, chology’s failure to police itself, psychology’s problematic clinical, and counseling psychology. public face) reflect the failure of professional psychology to Such criticisms have been ego bruising to many of us get its own house in order. I offer several individual and in psychology. At the same time, they have been valuable institutional recommendations for enhancing psychology’s and have spurred the field toward much needed reforms, image and contend that public skepticism toward psychol- such as greater emphasis on effect sizes and confidence ogy may, paradoxically, be one of our field’s strongest intervals in addition to (or in lieu of) tests of statistical allies. significance (Cohen, 1994; Meehl, 1978) and the develop- Keywords: psychology, skepticism, bias, psychotherapy, ment of criteria for, and lists of, evidence-based psycho- misconceptions, self-help logical interventions (Chambless & Hollon, 1998). Yet with the exception of criticisms of mental health practice, henever we psychologists dare to venture out- most of the concerns voiced by insiders probably overlap side of the hallowed halls of academia or our only minimally with those of outsiders. therapy offices to that foreign land called the Whatever the sources of the public’s skepticism of W psychology’s scientific status, it is clear that such doubts “real world,” we are likely at some point to encounter a puzzling and, for us, troubling phenomenon. Specifically, are not new (Benjamin, 2006)—nor is our field’s deep- most of us will inevitably hear the assertion from layper- seated insecurity about how outsiders view it. As Coon sons that psychology—which those of us within the pro- (1992) remarked when explaining psychology’s conspicu- fession generally regard as the scientific study of behavior, ous omission from Auguste Comte’s mid-19th-century hi- broadly construed—is in actuality not a science. Some erarchy of the sciences, outsiders go further, insinuating or insisting that much of It is well-known that whereas sociology sat at the peak of the modern psychology is pseudoscientific. Keith Stanovich Comtian hierarchy, psychology was not even on the pyramid, (2009) dubbed psychology the “Rodney Dangerfield of the having been deemed incapable of becoming a science because its sciences” (p. 175) in reference to the late comedian famous subject matter was unquantifiable and its methods mired in a for joking that “I don’t get no respect.” As Stanovich metaphysical morass. Psychology has never quite lived this down observed, “Most judgments about the field and its accom- and, as psychologists themselves like to say, has never recovered plishments are resoundingly negative” (p. 175). from its adolescent physics envy. (p. 143) Ironically, some of the harshest criticisms of contem- porary psychology’s scientific status have come from within the ranks of psychology itself (see Gergen, 1973; I thank Robin Cautin, David Baker, Steven Jay Lynn, William S. C. Hayes, 2004; Lilienfeld, 2010; Lykken, 1968, 1991; O’Donohue, and Rachel Winograd for their helpful comments on a draft G. A. Miller, 2004; Skinner, 1987). Specifically, many of this article and Rhea Farberman, Anthony Pratkanis, and Jim Clark for scholars within psychology have rued the extent to which sharing useful resources. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Scott (a) excessive reliance on statistical significance testing, (b) O. Lilienfeld, Department of Psychology, Emory University, Room 473, the propensity of psychologists to posit vague theoretical Psychology and Interdisciplinary Sciences Building, 36 Eagle Row, At- entities that are difficult to test, and (c) political correctness lanta, GA 30322. E-mail: [email protected] Month 2011 ● American Psychologist 1 © 2011 American Psychological Association 0003-066X/11/$12.00 Vol. ●●, No. ●, 000–000 DOI: 10.1037/a0023963 ence (APS), and other professional organizations are right to rebut such misunderstandings when they arise. Yet, with the exception of the APA Science Directorate pamphlet, such efforts are usually reactive rather than proactive. Moreover, these efforts may meet with mixed and at best short-term success, because they largely neglect to appre- ciate the underlying reasons for policymakers’ skepticism. Understanding why many nonpsychologists are skep- tical of psychology is important for four reasons. First, such knowledge can forearm psychologists not only when field- ing questions from skeptical relatives at holiday dinners (“But isn’t what you’re studying pretty obvious?”) but, more crucially, when encountering resistance about psy- chological findings from students, therapy clients, and lay- persons. In this way, such knowledge can equip psycholo- gists with intellectual ammunition against misguided criticisms of their field. Second, such knowledge may allow psychologists to anticipate commonplace objections to psy- chological research from policymakers and help psycholo- gists explain the pragmatic and theoretical significance of Scott O. their research to outsiders. Third, such knowledge is valu- Lilienfeld able in its own right, because it sheds light on the psycho- logical sources of resistance to the scientific study of hu- man nature (see also Bloom & Weisberg, 2007, for a discussion of developmental precursors of resistance to Nevertheless, as a field, we have been reluctant to science). In this respect, it may help us to grasp why so examine the reasons for the widespread and longstanding many educated individuals find psychology to be unscien- public skepticism of psychology (but see Benjamin, 1986, tific. Fourth, such knowledge may aid psychologists in for a useful historical analysis), perhaps because we see crafting recommendations for counteracting public and little merit in these reasons. Nor have we invested much policymakers’ misunderstandings of psychology. effort in generating potential solutions for enhancing our In this article, I pose two overarching questions: (a) field’s public image. Are the negative views of psychology’s scientific status Yet we ignore negative lay perceptions of psychology held by many outsiders warranted? (b) What are the prin- at our peril. Public skepticism of psychology may render cipal sources of these views? I place primary emphasis on would-be mental health consumers reluctant to seek out our the psychological and sociological reasons for the public’s potentially valuable clinical services. Public skepticism skepticism of psychology’s scientific status. Nevertheless, I may have also contributed indirectly to psychology’s no- acknowledge that these negative attitudes have deep his- ticeable absence from some funding agencies’ lists of Sci- torical roots, including early 20th-century psychologists’ ence, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics (STEM) entanglements with the paranormal and spiritualism, which disciplines (Price, 2011). As a consequence of this lack of probably contributed to psychology’s less than glowing recognition, “Psychologists are often not eligible for tar- public image (Benjamin, 2006; Coon, 1992). I conclude by geted funding for education, professional training, and re- proposing several individual and institutional recommen- search that could contribute substantially to achieving dations for diminishing the widespread public skepticism STEM goals” (American Psychological Association [APA] of psychology as a scientific discipline. Before doing so, I 2009 Presidential Task Force Report on the Future of canvass data on the prevalence of the public’s skeptical Psychology as a STEM Discipline, 2010, p. 6), such as attitudes toward psychology’s scientific status, as such data programs for boosting psychology students’ science and offer us a glimpse of the magnitude
Recommended publications
  • Naturalism in the Philosophies of Dewey and Zhuangzi
    Naturalism in the Philosophies of Dewey and Zhuangzi: The Live Creature and the Crooked Tree by Christopher C. Kirby A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor in Philosophy Department of Philosophy College of Arts and Sciences University of South Florida Co-Major Professor: John P. Anton Ph.D. Co-Major Professor: Martin Schönfeld Ph.D. Sidney Axinn Ph.D. Alexander Levine Ph.D. Date of Approval: December 12, 2008 Keywords: Pragmatism, Daoism, Metaphysics, Epistemology, Ethics © Copyright 2008 Christopher C. Kirby Dedication For P.J. – “Nature speaks louder than the call from the minaret.” (Inayat Khan, Bowl of Saki) Table of Contents List of Abbreviations ................................................................................................. ii Abstract ..................................................................................................................... iii Preface: West Meets East........................................................................................... 1 Dewey’s Encounter with China ............................................................................. 6 Chapter One: What is Naturalism? .......................................................................... 15 Naturalism and the Organic Point of View .......................................................... 16 Nature and the Language of Experience .............................................................. 22 Naturalistic Strategies in Philosophy ..................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Viscog Productions, Inc. | [email protected]
    VisCog Productions, Inc. www.viscog.com | [email protected] The Inattentional Blindness Collection VisCog Productions, Inc. www.viscog.com | [email protected] The Inattentional Blindness Collection This collection of videos, created by Professor Daniel Simons and his students, allows presenters to induce a striking failure of awareness known as inattentional blindness: When people focus attention on one thing, they often failure to notice unexpected objects, even a person in a gorilla suit. This collection includes the “invisible gorilla” selective attention test and many others. It is the only collection that includes the 2010 Monkey Business Illusion. All of the videos are stand-alone files that can be incorporated into your lectures and presentations. VisCog Productions, Inc. www.viscog.com | [email protected] Copyright The contents of this collection are copyrighted. The videos may be used for in-person presentations. All other rights are reserved. No resale VisCog Productions is the exclusive distributor for the videos in this collection, and they may not be re-sold or incorporated into other products. No broadcast or posting The videos may not be broadcast, posted online, distributed, included in other products, re-posted, uploaded to any server, or streamed online. If your presentation is recorded for distribution, the recording may not include the videos. Licensee only Only the licensee who purchased this collection may show these videos — they may not be provided to other users. If you are interested in a multiple-user license or a site license for your organization, please email us: [email protected]. VisCog Productions, Inc. www.viscog.com | [email protected] Contents and Instructions The Inattentional Blindness Collection includes 32 demonstration videos and 1 bonus video.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 1: Prescientific Psychology
    Prescientific Psychology 1 Start with these facts. Psychology is the most popular elective in American high schools today. Furthermore, psychology is one of the two or three most popular undergraduate majors in North American colleges. People cannot seem to get enough of psychol- ogy; it is everywhere today. It is the substance of movies, novels, computer games, social media, magazines, television shows, tabloid newspapers, radio talk shows, and music lyrics. Clearly, there is no shortage of public interest in psychology. People are interested in behavior—their own, their relatives, their neighbors, their cowork- ers, and even strangers who they know only through the media of books, magazines, or television shows such as soap operas, courtroom programs, game shows, situation comedies, dramas, and the so-called “reality” shows. There seems to be a never-ending fascination with human behavior that is perhaps inherent in human nature. It is likely that such an interest has afforded evolutionary advantages. An individual ’ s ability to understand and, better still, anticipate the behavior of others has survival value. Psy- chologists refer to this public interest in psychology as popular psychology. It isn ’ t psychology of the form that would be recognized by most psychologists as scientific psychology. Indeed, many psychologists would be embarrassed by any association with it. However, the public loves it, and it is their psychology. Psychology has existed, no doubt, from the very beginnings of human history. When hominids first walked erect on the earth, facing a life expectancy of perhaps 30 years, a life beset with hardships and dangers that could hardly be imagined today, these early individuals were in need of human comfort, of reassurance, of empa- thy, and of guidance.
    [Show full text]
  • CURRICULUM VITAE Lester C. Loschky
    CURRICULUM VITAE Lester C. Loschky 8/27/12 Office address: Home address: Department of Psychology 1933 Bluestem Terrace 471 Bluemont Hall Manhattan, KS 66502 Kansas State University Phone: (785) 539-3411 Manhattan, KS 66056-5302 Phone: 785-532-6882 http://www.ksu.edu/psych/research_loschky. E-mail: [email protected] htm http://www.ksu.edu/psych/vcl/index.html EDUCATION 2003 PhD, Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL Thesis: Investigating perception and eye movement control in natural scenes using gaze-contingent multi-resolutional displays. 1989 MA, English as a Second Language, University of Hawaii at Manoa, Honolulu, HI Thesis: The effects of premodified input and negotiated interaction on second language comprehension and retention. 1986 BFA, Fine Arts, Columbia College, Columbia, MO AWARDS 2003 American Psychological Association (Division 3) New Investigator Award for Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 2002 Vision Sciences Society, Graduate Student First Author Travel Fund Award 1995 William Chandler Bagley Scholar, College of Education, University of Illinois at Urbana- Champaign 1990 Harry Whitten Prize for Scholarly Excellence (for best graduate research), Department of English as a Second Language, University of Hawaii at Manoa RESEARCH INTERESTS Broad: mind & brain, cognition, perception, memory, machine vision, human-computer interaction Specific: Scene Perception: scene gist acquisition, attention and eye guidance in scenes, eye movements and scene memory, resolution across the visual field Gaze-contingent Multi-Resolutional Displays: human factors of resolution drop-off functions and update delays, interaction between resolution levels and attention TEACHING INTERESTS Cognitive Psychology, Cognitive Science, Perception, Neuropsychology, Human Factors, Human- computer interaction, Introductory Psychology, Research Methods and Statistics Lester C.
    [Show full text]
  • NEUROCENTRISM Feld, Meca, & Sauvigné, in Press)
    NEUROCENTRISM feld, Meca, & Sauvigné, in press). In its Neurocentrism: Implications for Psychotherapy most extreme form, neurocentrism regards Practice and Research the CNS as essentially the only adequate level of analysis for conceptualizing and treating psychological phenomena. Scott O. Lilienfeld, Emory University The early 21st century is also awash in talk of psychological conditions as “brain Seth J. Schwartz and Alan Meca, University of Miami disorders.” For example, in a 2013 TEDx talk, Thomas Insel, director of the National Katheryn C. Sauvigné, Georgia State University Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), argued Sally Satel, American Enterprise Institute that “what we need conceptually to make progress here is to rethink these disorders [mental disorders] as brain disorders” (Insel, 2013; see also Insel & Cuthbert, 1989, SAMUEL GUZE, then one of the damental level of analysis—the brain. IN 2015). doyens of American psychiatry, laid down Hence, it is only at this level, Guze main- But is neurocentrism helpful in clarify- the gauntlet to his academic colleagues in a tained, that research will ultimately bear ing our thinking about the causes and provocative article, entitled “Biological fruit in understanding, treating, and pre- treatment of mental disorders? What are its Psychiatry: Is There Any Other Kind?”, venting mental afflictions. implications for psychotherapy practice published in a prestigious medical journal. Over a quarter of a century later, we find and research? On the opening page, Guze answered his ourselves confronting the same question own question with a resounding “no”: raised by Guze, but with respect to psy- The Long Swing of the Pendulum “There can be no such thing as a psychiatry chology.
    [Show full text]
  • History of Psychology
    The Psych 101 Series James C. Kaufman, PhD, Series Editor Department of Educational Psychology University of Connecticut David C. Devonis, PhD, received his doctorate in the history of psychology from the University of New Hampshire’s erstwhile pro- gram in that subject in 1989 with a thesis on the history of conscious pleasure in modern American psychology. Since then he has taught vir- tually every course in the psychology curriculum in his academic odys- sey from the University of Redlands in Redlands, California, and the now-closed Teikyo Marycrest University (formerly Marycrest College in Davenport, Iowa) to—for the past 17 years—Graceland University in Lamoni, Iowa, alma mater of Bruce Jenner and, more famously for the history of psychology, of Noble H. Kelly (1901–1997), eminent con- tributor to psychology’s infrastructure through his many years of ser- vice to the American Board of Examiners in Professional Psychology. Dr. Devonis has been a member of Cheiron: The International Society for the History of Behavioral and Social Sciences since 1990, a con- tributor to many of its activities, and its treasurer for the past 10 years. Currently he is on the editorial board of the American Psychological Association journal History of Psychology and is, with Wade Pickren, coeditor and compiler of the online bibliography History of Psychology in the Oxford Bibliographies Online series. History of Psychology 101 David C. Devonis, PhD Copyright © 2014 Springer Publishing Company, LLC All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or trans- mitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, without the prior permission of Springer Publishing Company, LLC, or authorization through payment of the appropriate fees to the Copyright Clearance Cen- ter, Inc., 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, 978-750-8400, fax 978-646-8600, [email protected] or on the Web at www.copyright.com.
    [Show full text]
  • History and Systems in Psychology Fall 2000
    PSYC 6066: History and Systems of Psychology Summer 2017 Professor: Mark E. Mattson, Ph.D. Office: Dealy 432c & LL819c Voicemail LC: 212-636-6356 E-mail: [email protected] Office hours: Monday and Wednesday 12:30-1:00 and other times by appointment Required textbook: Benjamin, Jr., L.T. (Ed.) (2009). A history of psychology: Original sources and contemporary research (3rd ed.). Malden MA: Blackwell Pub. Other required readings: download from ARES: password=mattson6066 Recommended books: Fancher, R. & Rutherford, A. (2017). Pioneers of psychology (5th ed.). New York: Norton. American Psychological Association. (2009). Publication manual of the American Psychological Association (6th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Goals of the course 1. Cover the history of psychology, through primary and secondary sources 2. Consider research methodology in history and psychology 3. Identify the major problems that have concerned psychologists 4. Practice research, writing, and presentation skills A satisfactory grade in the course (A-) will result from satisfactory performance on all four components: class participation, class presentations, a research project, and a final essay. 1. Class participation Come to class prepared to discuss/comment on the readings from Benjamin and ARES. Attendance is expected, with no more than one excused absence. 2. Class presentations The objective is to develop and make a formal presentation that teaches the class the essentials of one of the following history topics: women in psychology history of clinical psychology race and psychology counseling psychology psychometrics, especially psychological tests psychiatry/psychoanalysis applied psychology incl. forensic developmental psychology The due date for each topic is on the course schedule. Create and distribute a handout to the class and instructor.
    [Show full text]
  • Viscog Productions, Inc. | [email protected] Viscog Productions, Inc
    VisCog Productions, Inc. www.viscog.com | [email protected] VisCog Productions, Inc. www.viscog.com | [email protected] First released in 2003, Surprising Studies of Visual Awareness (Vol 1) includes videos and demos that can be used to induce failures of awareness in an audience. It has been used by presenters throughout the world, in a wide range of training, teaching, and presentation contexts. To accommodate decreasing use of DVD drives in computers, in 2016 we replaced the physical version of our DVDs with downloadable versions. VisCog Productions, Inc. www.viscog.com | [email protected] Important restrictions see www.viscog.com/policies.html for details • You may burn the digital contents to one DVD disc or flash drive for your own use in presentations. • The DVD contents may not be provided to other users— they are licensed to the individual who purchased them. If you are interested in a site license for your organization, please email us: [email protected]. • The DVD contents may be used only for in-person presentations. The DVD contents may not be broadcast, posted online, or distributed. They may not be be re-posted, uploaded to any server, or streamed online. If your presentation is recorded for distribution, the recording may not include the contents of this DVD. • The DVD and its contents may not be re-sold. VisCog Productions is the exclusive distributor for these DVDs, and they may not be sold or incorporated into other products by anyone else. • The DVD and its contents are copyrighted. The license for the DVD permits use of the DVD for in-person presentations, but all other rights are reserved.
    [Show full text]
  • The Invisible Gorilla
    the invisible gorilla • And Other Ways Our Intuitions Deceive Us • by Christopher Chabris and Daniel Simons, 2010, CROWN: New York, NY. • Summary by Douglas W. Green, EdD • [email protected] For More Go To DrDougGreen.Com 1 If you like this summary, buy this book. Tuesday, June 22, 2010 Who are these guys? • Christopher Chabris: Psychology professor at Union College in upstate New York • Daniel Simons: Psychology professor at the University of Illinois For More Go To DrDougGreen.Com 2 If you like this summary, buy this book. Tuesday, June 22, 2010 Six Everyday Illusions • The Invisible Gorilla is about six everyday illusions that profoundly influence our lives. They are the illusions of attention, memory, confidence, knowledge, cause, and potential. These are distorted beliefs we hold about our minds that are not just wrong, but wrong in dangerous ways. Once you know about these illusions, you will view the world differently and think about it more clearly. You will recognize when people are taking advantage of illusions in an attempt to obfuscate or persuade. Seeing through these veils will help connect you with reality. (Dr. Doug: If you have a brain, you should buy this book. Here is the Amazon.Com link. http://amzn.to/cB2H2u ) For More Go To DrDougGreen.Com 3 If you like this summary, buy this book. Tuesday, June 22, 2010 What I Left Out • Here are some things that I did not discuss in my summary in order to give my readers further incentive to buy this book. • The book starts with a gripping story of a crime that will suck you in.
    [Show full text]
  • Myths and Misconceptions About the Psychology of Adolescence and Intimate Relationships
    Psychology, 2019, 10, 2166-2176 https://www.scirp.org/journal/psych ISSN Online: 2152-7199 ISSN Print: 2152-7180 Myths and Misconceptions about the Psychology of Adolescence and Intimate Relationships Adrian Furnham1*, Simmy Grover2 1Department of Leadership and Organisational Behaviour, Norwegian Business School (BI), Nydalveien, Olso, Norway 2Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, UK How to cite this paper: Furnham, A., & Abstract Grover, S. (2019). Myths and Misconcep- tions about the Psychology of Adolescence This study examined the prevalence of psychological myths in two areas of and Intimate Relationships. Psychology, 10, psychology: Adolescence and Relationships. All 517 participants completed 2166-2176. two questionnaires in which they rated to what extent they thought various https://doi.org/10.4236/psych.2019.1015137 statements/facts about Adolescence and about Intimate Relationships were Received: November 29, 2019 True or False. A large number of these myths were rated as True (Definitely Accepted: December 24, 2019 or Partly). There were few significant demographic correlates of the total Published: December 27, 2019 correct score (determined by rating the myth as False). Implications are dis- cussed. Copyright © 2019 by author(s) and Scientific Research Publishing Inc. Keywords This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution International Psychology, Education, Myths, Misconceptions, Psychological Knowledge License (CC BY 4.0). http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ Open Access 1. Introduction There has been an academic interest in psychological myths and misconceptions for nearly a hundred years (Amsel, Baird, & Ashley, 2011; Brown, 1983; Furn- ham, 1992, 1993; Gaze, 2014; Hughes, Lyddy, & Lambe, 2013; Kowalski & Tay- lor, 2009; Nixon, 1925; Lamal, 1979; McKeachie, 1960; Taylor & Kowalski, 2004; Tupper & Williams, 1986; Vaughan, 1977).
    [Show full text]
  • Cinematic Psychotherapy Stereotypes
    Clinical Science Insights Cinematic Psychotherapy Stereotypes ByAdam Fisher, PhD to promote common misconceptions about psychotherapy Postdoctoral Clinical Fellow for dramatic effect (Lasky, 2006). For example, mental health treatment is commonly portrayed as ineffectual In perhaps the earliest on-screen fictional portrayal of a (Kemshal-Bell, 2010), and when it does work, it is shown mental health professional, a young woman was depicted as to be effective through love (e.g., Mozart and the Whale), being controlled by a hypnotist in the 1896 silent film Trilby. sexual boundary violations by the female professional Psychotherapists and other mental health professionals (e.g., The Prince of Tides) or other unethical behaviors have been portrayed in well over 5,000 films (Flowers & (Macfarlane, 2004; Otto & McGroarty, 2012; Wedding, Frizler, 2004), and across many genres including drama, Boyd, & Niemiec, 2010), or a “miraculous minute cure.” horror, musical, western, and even hardcore pornography The latter may foster unrealistic expectations for real-life (Greenberg, 2000). Indeed, 17% of the most popular films of patients, who tend to experience added distress when their the 1990s portrayed at least one mental health professional concerns are not as easily resolved (Orchowski, Spickard, & (Young, Boester, Whitt, & Stevens, 2008). McNamara, 2006). Mental health professionals have long been concerned Although some have argued that portrayals have about the potential negative effects of these portrayals become more balanced overall in the last decade in U.S. (Peterson, 2001; Schultz, 2004b), including concerns that (Niemiec & Wedding, 2006) and international cinema viewers will be less likely to seek help after viewing them (Niemiec, 2010), films from 1999 to 2010 portrayed (Stuart, 2006), that consumers’ perceptions of mental health mental health professionals acting unethically more often care will be negatively impacted (Vogel, Gentile, & Kaplan, than not (Otto & McGroarty, 2012).
    [Show full text]
  • Simons (1999) Gorillas in Our Midst. Sustained Inattentional Blindness
    Perception, 1999, volume 28, pages 1059 ^ 1074 DOI:10.1068/p2952 Gorillas in our midst: sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events Daniel J Simons, Christopher F Chabris Department of Psychology, Harvard University, 33 Kirkland Street, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA; e-mail: [email protected] Received 9 May 1999, in revised form 20 June 1999 Abstract. With each eye fixation, we experience a richly detailed visual world. Yet recent work on visual integration and change direction reveals that we are surprisingly unaware of the details of our environment from one view to the next: we often do not detect large changes to objects and scenes (`change blindness'). Furthermore, without attention, we may not even perceive objects (`inattentional blindness'). Taken together, these findings suggest that we perceive and remember only those objects and details that receive focused attention. In this paper, we briefly review and discuss evidence for these cognitive forms of `blindness'. We then present a new study that builds on classic studies of divided visual attention to examine inattentional blindness for complex objects and events in dynamic scenes. Our results suggest that the likelihood of noticing an unexpected object depends on the similarity of that object to other objects in the display and on how difficult the priming monitoring task is. Interestingly, spatial proximity of the critical unattended object to attended locations does not appear to affect detection, suggesting that observers attend to objects and events, not spatial
    [Show full text]