Psychotherapy and the Pursuit of Happiness Ronald W

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Psychotherapy and the Pursuit of Happiness Ronald W Psychotherapy and the Pursuit of Happiness Ronald W. Dworkin Freudianism sits alongside Marxism and Darwinism in the pantheon of modern theories held to be so revelatory that they not only gained the adherence of Western intelligentsia but shaped the broader cul- ture. During the first half of the twentieth century, an air of intrigue and mystery hovered around Freud’s newly anointed practitioners. Psychotherapists occupied a strange universe, speaking in a language so incomprehensible but seemingly authoritative that it alternately awed and scared the average man on the street. Psychotherapy is no longer an intellectual movement today as it once was. But in the form of modern professional “caring,” it has assumed a new role, which is to provide a peculiar sort of substitute friendship — what we might call “artificial friendship” — for lonely people in a lonely age. To understand why this occurred and what it means for American culture, we must study the fractious history of the mental health field over the last six decades. It is a complicated story, with a staggering variety of terms, schools, leaders, and techniques, so any overview must neces- sarily leave out many important details. But from even just a synopsis of the conflicts that gave rise to today’s culture of psychotherapy — battles over who would hold the truest title to physician of the mind, tensions between scientists and clinicians, academics and professionals, elites and the public — we can see more clearly how psychotherapy has profoundly shaped the American conception of what happiness is and how we can achieve it. Disciplines in Conflict A mental health crisis erupted in the United States after the Second World War, touching not just returning soldiers but people from all walks of life. Alcoholism and juvenile delinquency became rampant. The number of patients admitted to hospitals and outpatient psychiatric Ronald W. Dworkin, M.D., Ph.D. is a senior fellow at the Hudson Institute and teaches in the George Washington University Honors Program. Spring 2012 ~ 69 Copyright 2012. All rights reserved. See www.TheNewAtlantis.com for more information. Ronald W. Dworkin clinics for mental health problems began to climb rapidly. There were not enough trained mental health personnel — the nation then had only a few thousand clinical psychologists — to deal with the problem. So the federal government responded by passing the National Mental Health Act (NMHA) in 1946, leading to the establishment a few years later of the National Institute of Mental Health and the provision of funding to train more psychotherapists. The new policy represented a genuine attempt to handle the crisis, yet it also brought to the surface important subcurrents and divisions within the mental health community, including skepticism among both academic psychologists and medically trained psychiatrists toward an elevated role for less-trained psychotherapists. Before proceeding, let us clarify some terminology. It is easy today to conflate psychologists and psychiatrists, psychoanalysis and psychothera- py, and the many other related words used by those who study the mind or seek to treat mental health problems. The vast multiplicity of terms can be daunting and, as we shall see, some definitions and distinctions have grown blurry with time. But if we use the words with care, the contours of our story will be clearer. Psychotherapy — the therapeutic treatment of individual mental and emotional problems — had fascinated the American people ever since Freud had visited the United States in 1909. Although as late as 1940 no more than four percent of the American population had actually under- gone some form of psychotherapy, the public held a generally fixed set of ideas about it, including the belief that psychoanalysis — the term used for Freudian psychotherapy, which most people did not distinguish from psychotherapy in general — was a true science. Psychiatrists are medical doctors; they are trained in biology and anato- my, and like other medical doctors can prescribe drugs. At the start of the twentieth century, most American psychiatrists had worked as superinten- dents in state mental hospitals and held a biological view of mental illness. Only a third of all psychiatrists offered psychoanalysis in private practice by the 1940s. Nevertheless, the great majority of psychoanalysts at the time were psychiatrists. The medical establishment had made a concerted effort to cleanse America of non-M.D. psychoanalysts, with the result that by 1953, 82 percent of the country’s psychoanalysts were psychiatrists. The government’s plan to boost rapidly the number of psychotherapists threat- ened both biology-minded psychiatrists and psychiatrists practicing psycho- analysis. It was unlikely that the number of medically trained psychiatrists could be raised sufficiently to meet the demand for psychotherapists — in the 1950s there were only 10,000 psychiatrists in the United States, with 70 ~ The New Atlantis Copyright 2012. All rights reserved. See www.TheNewAtlantis.com for more information. Psychotherapy and the Pursuit of Happiness just 450 new graduates each year — so government support for psycho- therapy necessarily meant more non-M.D. therapists. Psychologists are not medical doctors; they pursue graduate training in psychology, generally obtaining an advanced non-medical degree. In the 1950s, the majority of psychologists were academics working in college labs. Indeed, the public idea of a psychologist was a man in a white coat testing rats in a maze. Treating unhappy people with psychotherapy was as foreign and threatening to these scientists as it was to the biologically oriented psychiatrists. Academic psychologists had spent decades cultivating the aura of the research scientist while deprecating the role of clinical psychologists — that is, those psychologists who actually worked with patients. A minority within psychology, consisting mostly of women and handicapped by a “nursing image,” clinical psychologists had spent forty years working under psychiatrists, performing tests on patients, but deferring to psy- chiatrists on diagnosis and treatment. Many of them were idealists who dreamed of a new social order with psychotherapy at its core, dreams inspired by Freud and his followers. Although academic psychologists didn’t begrudge the masses their therapists, they looked down on clinical psychologists, believing that the title of “psychologist” should be reserved for people who had been taught the scientific method in the finest schools. The notion that psychotherapists might be granted parity in the pub- lic mind with real professors of psychology was worrisome to these academics. Indeed, no group of mental health workers stood to benefit more from the federal government’s new mental-health policy than the clinical psychologists. Hoping to practice psychotherapy on their own, without restrictions or physician supervision, clinical psychologists saw oppor- tunity in the new push by the government to increase the numbers of psychotherapists. Inevitably, the NMHA brought psychiatrists and clinical psycholo- gists into conflict. No state laws forbade psychologists from practicing psychotherapy on their own, but the courts tended to interpret medical licensing statutes broadly, so a clinical psychologist practiced psychother- apy at some risk. Yet the increasing demand for mental health clinicians changed the dynamic. Clinical psychologists decided to test the limits of the law, and by the early 1950s, nine percent of clinical psychologists were practicing on their own. Psychiatrists fought back, arguing that psychologists lacked the train- ing to detect severe mental illness, while academic psychologists, who Spring 2012 ~ 71 Copyright 2012. All rights reserved. See www.TheNewAtlantis.com for more information. Ronald W. Dworkin resented the popularity of psychoanalysis, attacked the field by arguing that it was unscientific compared to the then-reigning paradigm of behav- iorism. Clinical psychologists, however, began working to popularize psychotherapy among the general public. During the 1950s, many major American newspapers began carrying regular columns on psychological lore, typically written by a psychologist. In popular women’s magazines like McCall’s and Cosmopolitan, psychologists demystified the therapeutic process. In 1958, a young Joyce Brothers, who held a Ph.D. in psychology from Cornell, hosted the first television show devoted to people’s emo- tional and psychological problems. The popular rise of psychotherapy was intertwined with the popular rise of psychoanalysis. Freudian ideas had been popular with intellectu- als and artists in the 1920s, but it was not until the 1950s, with the crisis in mental health, that they widely penetrated the public consciousness. Although few clinical psychologists of this era practiced Freudian psy- choanalysis, they distilled Freud’s ideas into buzzwords, which appeared frequently in their conversations, writings, and speeches, thereby tapping into the public consciousness and identifying with people’s concerns. Journalists of the time wrote about infatuation and subliminal influences. A social worker visiting a family would look for unhealthy parent-child relationships and refer to the Oedipus complex. A probation officer, even a truant officer, would discuss a juvenile delinquent’s family background in the context of aggression and compensation. Freudian concepts satu- rated popular movies, such as Marnie and The Three Faces of Eve.
Recommended publications
  • Chapter 1: Prescientific Psychology
    Prescientific Psychology 1 Start with these facts. Psychology is the most popular elective in American high schools today. Furthermore, psychology is one of the two or three most popular undergraduate majors in North American colleges. People cannot seem to get enough of psychol- ogy; it is everywhere today. It is the substance of movies, novels, computer games, social media, magazines, television shows, tabloid newspapers, radio talk shows, and music lyrics. Clearly, there is no shortage of public interest in psychology. People are interested in behavior—their own, their relatives, their neighbors, their cowork- ers, and even strangers who they know only through the media of books, magazines, or television shows such as soap operas, courtroom programs, game shows, situation comedies, dramas, and the so-called “reality” shows. There seems to be a never-ending fascination with human behavior that is perhaps inherent in human nature. It is likely that such an interest has afforded evolutionary advantages. An individual ’ s ability to understand and, better still, anticipate the behavior of others has survival value. Psy- chologists refer to this public interest in psychology as popular psychology. It isn ’ t psychology of the form that would be recognized by most psychologists as scientific psychology. Indeed, many psychologists would be embarrassed by any association with it. However, the public loves it, and it is their psychology. Psychology has existed, no doubt, from the very beginnings of human history. When hominids first walked erect on the earth, facing a life expectancy of perhaps 30 years, a life beset with hardships and dangers that could hardly be imagined today, these early individuals were in need of human comfort, of reassurance, of empa- thy, and of guidance.
    [Show full text]
  • History of Psychology
    The Psych 101 Series James C. Kaufman, PhD, Series Editor Department of Educational Psychology University of Connecticut David C. Devonis, PhD, received his doctorate in the history of psychology from the University of New Hampshire’s erstwhile pro- gram in that subject in 1989 with a thesis on the history of conscious pleasure in modern American psychology. Since then he has taught vir- tually every course in the psychology curriculum in his academic odys- sey from the University of Redlands in Redlands, California, and the now-closed Teikyo Marycrest University (formerly Marycrest College in Davenport, Iowa) to—for the past 17 years—Graceland University in Lamoni, Iowa, alma mater of Bruce Jenner and, more famously for the history of psychology, of Noble H. Kelly (1901–1997), eminent con- tributor to psychology’s infrastructure through his many years of ser- vice to the American Board of Examiners in Professional Psychology. Dr. Devonis has been a member of Cheiron: The International Society for the History of Behavioral and Social Sciences since 1990, a con- tributor to many of its activities, and its treasurer for the past 10 years. Currently he is on the editorial board of the American Psychological Association journal History of Psychology and is, with Wade Pickren, coeditor and compiler of the online bibliography History of Psychology in the Oxford Bibliographies Online series. History of Psychology 101 David C. Devonis, PhD Copyright © 2014 Springer Publishing Company, LLC All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or trans- mitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, without the prior permission of Springer Publishing Company, LLC, or authorization through payment of the appropriate fees to the Copyright Clearance Cen- ter, Inc., 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, 978-750-8400, fax 978-646-8600, [email protected] or on the Web at www.copyright.com.
    [Show full text]
  • History and Systems in Psychology Fall 2000
    PSYC 6066: History and Systems of Psychology Summer 2017 Professor: Mark E. Mattson, Ph.D. Office: Dealy 432c & LL819c Voicemail LC: 212-636-6356 E-mail: [email protected] Office hours: Monday and Wednesday 12:30-1:00 and other times by appointment Required textbook: Benjamin, Jr., L.T. (Ed.) (2009). A history of psychology: Original sources and contemporary research (3rd ed.). Malden MA: Blackwell Pub. Other required readings: download from ARES: password=mattson6066 Recommended books: Fancher, R. & Rutherford, A. (2017). Pioneers of psychology (5th ed.). New York: Norton. American Psychological Association. (2009). Publication manual of the American Psychological Association (6th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Goals of the course 1. Cover the history of psychology, through primary and secondary sources 2. Consider research methodology in history and psychology 3. Identify the major problems that have concerned psychologists 4. Practice research, writing, and presentation skills A satisfactory grade in the course (A-) will result from satisfactory performance on all four components: class participation, class presentations, a research project, and a final essay. 1. Class participation Come to class prepared to discuss/comment on the readings from Benjamin and ARES. Attendance is expected, with no more than one excused absence. 2. Class presentations The objective is to develop and make a formal presentation that teaches the class the essentials of one of the following history topics: women in psychology history of clinical psychology race and psychology counseling psychology psychometrics, especially psychological tests psychiatry/psychoanalysis applied psychology incl. forensic developmental psychology The due date for each topic is on the course schedule. Create and distribute a handout to the class and instructor.
    [Show full text]
  • Myths and Misconceptions About the Psychology of Adolescence and Intimate Relationships
    Psychology, 2019, 10, 2166-2176 https://www.scirp.org/journal/psych ISSN Online: 2152-7199 ISSN Print: 2152-7180 Myths and Misconceptions about the Psychology of Adolescence and Intimate Relationships Adrian Furnham1*, Simmy Grover2 1Department of Leadership and Organisational Behaviour, Norwegian Business School (BI), Nydalveien, Olso, Norway 2Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, UK How to cite this paper: Furnham, A., & Abstract Grover, S. (2019). Myths and Misconcep- tions about the Psychology of Adolescence This study examined the prevalence of psychological myths in two areas of and Intimate Relationships. Psychology, 10, psychology: Adolescence and Relationships. All 517 participants completed 2166-2176. two questionnaires in which they rated to what extent they thought various https://doi.org/10.4236/psych.2019.1015137 statements/facts about Adolescence and about Intimate Relationships were Received: November 29, 2019 True or False. A large number of these myths were rated as True (Definitely Accepted: December 24, 2019 or Partly). There were few significant demographic correlates of the total Published: December 27, 2019 correct score (determined by rating the myth as False). Implications are dis- cussed. Copyright © 2019 by author(s) and Scientific Research Publishing Inc. Keywords This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution International Psychology, Education, Myths, Misconceptions, Psychological Knowledge License (CC BY 4.0). http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ Open Access 1. Introduction There has been an academic interest in psychological myths and misconceptions for nearly a hundred years (Amsel, Baird, & Ashley, 2011; Brown, 1983; Furn- ham, 1992, 1993; Gaze, 2014; Hughes, Lyddy, & Lambe, 2013; Kowalski & Tay- lor, 2009; Nixon, 1925; Lamal, 1979; McKeachie, 1960; Taylor & Kowalski, 2004; Tupper & Williams, 1986; Vaughan, 1977).
    [Show full text]
  • Cinematic Psychotherapy Stereotypes
    Clinical Science Insights Cinematic Psychotherapy Stereotypes ByAdam Fisher, PhD to promote common misconceptions about psychotherapy Postdoctoral Clinical Fellow for dramatic effect (Lasky, 2006). For example, mental health treatment is commonly portrayed as ineffectual In perhaps the earliest on-screen fictional portrayal of a (Kemshal-Bell, 2010), and when it does work, it is shown mental health professional, a young woman was depicted as to be effective through love (e.g., Mozart and the Whale), being controlled by a hypnotist in the 1896 silent film Trilby. sexual boundary violations by the female professional Psychotherapists and other mental health professionals (e.g., The Prince of Tides) or other unethical behaviors have been portrayed in well over 5,000 films (Flowers & (Macfarlane, 2004; Otto & McGroarty, 2012; Wedding, Frizler, 2004), and across many genres including drama, Boyd, & Niemiec, 2010), or a “miraculous minute cure.” horror, musical, western, and even hardcore pornography The latter may foster unrealistic expectations for real-life (Greenberg, 2000). Indeed, 17% of the most popular films of patients, who tend to experience added distress when their the 1990s portrayed at least one mental health professional concerns are not as easily resolved (Orchowski, Spickard, & (Young, Boester, Whitt, & Stevens, 2008). McNamara, 2006). Mental health professionals have long been concerned Although some have argued that portrayals have about the potential negative effects of these portrayals become more balanced overall in the last decade in U.S. (Peterson, 2001; Schultz, 2004b), including concerns that (Niemiec & Wedding, 2006) and international cinema viewers will be less likely to seek help after viewing them (Niemiec, 2010), films from 1999 to 2010 portrayed (Stuart, 2006), that consumers’ perceptions of mental health mental health professionals acting unethically more often care will be negatively impacted (Vogel, Gentile, & Kaplan, than not (Otto & McGroarty, 2012).
    [Show full text]
  • Can Psychology Become a Science?
    Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 281–288 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Can psychology become a science? Scott O. Lilienfeld Department of Psychology, Emory University, Room 473, 36 Eagle Row, Atlanta 30322, Georgia article info abstract Article history: I am profoundly grateful to Tom Bouchard for helping me learn to think scientifically. Scientific thinking, Received 3 December 2009 which is characterized by a set of safeguards against confirmation bias, does not come naturally to the Received in revised form 16 January 2010 human species, as the relatively recent appearance of science in history attests. Even today, scientific Accepted 25 January 2010 thinking is in woefully short supply in many domains of psychology, including clinical psychology and Available online 23 February 2010 cognate disciplines. I survey five key threats to scientific psychology – (a) political correctness, (b) radical environmentalism, (c) the resurrection of ‘‘common sense” and intuition as arbiters of scientific truth, Keywords: (d) postmodernism, and (e) pseudoscience – and conclude that these threats must be confronted directly Psychology by psychological science. I propose a set of educational and institutional reforms that should place Science Pseudoscience psychology on firmer scientific footing. Postmodernism Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Psychotherapy 1. Can psychology become a science? groups, they would need to possess above zero validity for at least one subgroup. When I entered graduate school in psychology at the University Of course, a naïve graduate student can perhaps be forgiven for of Minnesota in the Fall of 1982, I was a bright-eyed, bushy-tailed such logical errors, especially one embarking on his training nearly 21 year-old eager to learn about the mysteries of the mind.
    [Show full text]
  • History and Systems in Psychology Fall 2000
    PSYC 6066: History and Systems of Psychology Summer 2020 Professor: Mark E. Mattson, Ph.D. Office: DE432c & LL609j Voicemail LC: 212-636-6356 E-mail: [email protected] Office hours: Monday and Wednesday 12:30-1:00 and other times by appointment Required textbook: Benjamin, Jr., L.T. (Ed.) (2009). A history of psychology: Original sources and contemporary research (3rd ed.). Malden MA: Blackwell Pub. Other required readings: download from ARES: password=mattson6066 Recommended books: Fancher, R. & Rutherford, A. (2017). Pioneers of psychology (5th ed.). New York: Norton. American Psychological Association. (2009). Publication manual of the American Psychological Association (6th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Goals of the course 1. Cover the history of psychology, through primary and secondary sources 2. Consider research methodology in history and psychology 3. Identify the major problems that have concerned psychologists 4. Practice research, writing, and presentation skills A satisfactory grade in the course (A-) will result from satisfactory performance on all four components: class participation, class presentations, a research project, and a final essay. 1. Class participation Come to class prepared to discuss/comment on the readings from Benjamin and ARES. Attendance is expected, with no more than one excused absence. 2. Class presentations The objective is to develop and make a formal presentation that teaches the class the essentials of one of the following history topics: women in psychology history of clinical psychology race and psychology counseling psychology psychometrics, especially psychological tests psychiatry/psychoanalysis applied psychology incl. forensic developmental psychology The due date for each topic is on the course schedule. Create and distribute a handout to the class and instructor.
    [Show full text]
  • HANDBOOK of PSYCHOLOGY: VOLUME 1, HISTORY of PSYCHOLOGY
    HANDBOOK of PSYCHOLOGY: VOLUME 1, HISTORY OF PSYCHOLOGY Donald K. Freedheim Irving B. Weiner John Wiley & Sons, Inc. HANDBOOK of PSYCHOLOGY HANDBOOK of PSYCHOLOGY VOLUME 1 HISTORY OF PSYCHOLOGY Donald K. Freedheim Volume Editor Irving B. Weiner Editor-in-Chief John Wiley & Sons, Inc. This book is printed on acid-free paper. ➇ Copyright © 2003 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc., Hoboken, New Jersey. All rights reserved. Published simultaneously in Canada. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning, or otherwise, except as permitted under Section 107 or 108 of the 1976 United States Copyright Act, without either the prior written permission of the Publisher, or authorization through payment of the appropriate per-copy fee to the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc., 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, (978) 750-8400, fax (978) 750-4470, or on the web at www.copyright.com. Requests to the Publisher for permission should be addressed to the Permissions Department, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 111 River Street, Hoboken, NJ 07030, (201) 748-6011, fax (201) 748-6008, e-mail: [email protected]. Limit of Liability/Disclaimer of Warranty: While the publisher and author have used their best efforts in preparing this book, they make no representations or warranties with respect to the accuracy or completeness of the contents of this book and specifically disclaim any implied warranties of merchantability or fitness for a particular purpose. No warranty may be created or extended by sales representatives or written sales materials.
    [Show full text]
  • American Psychologist
    American Psychologist Public Skepticism of Psychology: Why Many People Perceive the Study of Human Behavior as Unscientific Scott O. Lilienfeld Online First Publication, June 13, 2011. doi: 10.1037/a0023963 CITATION Lilienfeld, S. O. (2011, June 13). Public Skepticism of Psychology: Why Many People Perceive the Study of Human Behavior as Unscientific. American Psychologist. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1037/a0023963 Public Skepticism of Psychology Why Many People Perceive the Study of Human Behavior as Unscientific Scott O. Lilienfeld Emory University Data indicate that large percentages of the general public and allied trends (see Redding & O’Donohue, 2009, and regard psychology’s scientific status with considerable Tierney, 2011, for recent discussions) have retarded the skepticism. I examine 6 criticisms commonly directed at the growth of scientific psychology. Others (e.g., Dawes, 1994; scientific basis of psychology (e.g., psychology is merely Lilienfeld, Lynn, & Lohr, 2003; Thyer & Pignotti, in press) common sense, psychology does not use scientific methods, have assailed the scientific status of large swaths of clinical psychology is not useful to society) and offer 6 rebuttals. I psychology, counseling psychology, and allied mental then address 8 potential sources of public skepticism to- health disciplines, contending that these fields have been ward psychology and argue that although some of these overly permissive of poorly supported practices. Still oth- sources reflect cognitive errors (e.g., hindsight bias) or ers (e.g., S. Koch, 1969; Meehl, 1978) have bemoaned the misunderstandings of psychological science (e.g., failure to at times painfully slow pace of progress of psychology, distinguish basic from applied research), others (e.g., psy- especially in the “softer” domains of social, personality, chology’s failure to police itself, psychology’s problematic clinical, and counseling psychology.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychology Loses Academic Clout As Therapists Turn Substitute Friends
    Psychology loses academic clout as therapists turn substitute friends The Sydney Morning Herald | September 7, 2012 Tanveer Ahmed Sydney Morning Herald columnist The Black Dog Institute published its first index of national happiness recently and Australians scored 77 out of 100, which was interpreted as meaning about three‐quarters of us are happy with our lives. Not bad at all. Economists are also fond of measuring wellbeing. But the very idea that we can rely on a subjective measurement of how we feel at a random point is a new one. And it's the result of a trend over several decades. In the history of happiness as an idea, the main tension has been between thinkers who hold a life must meet some objective standard to be considered happy, and those who hold that happiness is merely the subjective state of being pleased with one's life. In ancient times, happiness was deemed a transcendent, almost godlike state, attainable only by the few. Today, however, the concept has become democratised: anyone can be happy. It is more about feeling good than being good. The catch is, with happiness supposedly in the reach of everyone, it is pursued with a frenzy that, perversely, often gives rise to its opposite: unease, discontent, even guilt. The nature of the psychological sciences and the way they have been sold to the masses in the past 50 years has been critical in this transition ‐ in particular, the central place of the modern clinical psychologist as confidant to patients. Today, counsellors are the new friends.
    [Show full text]
  • Press Materials
    ABOUT THE BOOK A novel of passion and ambition based on the life of one of the most controversial scientists—and mothers—of the 20th century In 1920, when she graduated from Vassar College with a degree in psychology, Rosalie Rayner took a coveted position at the Johns Hopkins research lab to assist charismatic John B. Watson, the man who pioneered behaviorist psychology. Together, Watson and Rayner conducted experiments on hundreds of babies to prove behavior- ist principles of nurture over nature. One such experiment was the incredibly controversial “Little Albert” study, in which they fear-conditioned an infant. Watson and Rayner also embarked on a scandalous affair that cost them both their jobs. The Watsons’ parenting book, Psychological Care of Infant and Child, which emphasized emotional detachment, was a bestseller, and affected the upbringings of generations of American children—but Rosalie, now a mother herself, had to confront its tenets personally. With Behave, Andromeda Romano-Lax offers a fictional biography of Rosalie Rayner Watson, a woman whose lab work is now widely repudiated but who in her time was at the cutting edge of parenting psychology. Both moving and horrifying, Behave is a thought-provok- ing and compelling novel about the meaning of motherhood. Behave: A Novel Andromeda Romano-Lax On sale February 23, 2016 Published by Soho Press Distributed to the trade by Penguin Random House Publisher Services Hardcover: 978-1-61695-653-0 | Ebook: 978-1-61695-654-7 Audiobook available in CD and digital formats from Recorded Books ABOUT THE AUTHOR Born in 1970 in Chicago, Andromeda Romano-Lax worked as a freelance journalist and travel writer before turning to fiction.
    [Show full text]
  • Why Many Clinical Psychologists Are Resistant to Evidence-Based Practice: Root Causes and Constructive Remedies☆
    Clinical Psychology Review 33 (2013) 883–900 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Clinical Psychology Review Why many clinical psychologists are resistant to evidence-based practice: Root causes and constructive remedies☆ Scott O. Lilienfeld a,⁎, Lorie A. Ritschel b, Steven Jay Lynn c, Robin L. Cautin d, Robert D. Latzman e a Department of Psychology, Emory University, United States b Department of Psychiatry, Emory University School of Medicine, United States c Department of Psychology, Binghamton University, United States d Department of Psychology, Manhattanville College, United States e Department of Psychology, Georgia State University, United States HIGHLIGHTS • Evidence-based practice does not equal empirically supported therapy. • Evidence concerning clinicians' attitudes toward evidence-based practice is reviewed. • Sources of professionals' resistance to evidence-based practice are examined. • Misconceptions regarding evidence-based practice are delineated. • Recommendations for addressing resistance to evidence-based practice are outlined. article info abstract Available online 8 April 2013 Psychotherapists are taught that when a client expresses resistance repeatedly, they must understand and address its underlying sources. Yet proponents of evidence-based practice (EBP) have routinely ignored Keywords: the root causes of many clinical psychologists' reservations concerning the use of scientific evidence to Evidence-based practice inform clinical practice. As a consequence, much of the resistance to EBP persists, potentially
    [Show full text]