Extreme Polymorphism in a Y-Linked Sexually Selected Trait
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Heredity (2004) 92, 156–162 & 2004 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 0018-067X/04 $25.00 www.nature.com/hdy Extreme polymorphism in a Y-linked sexually selected trait AK Lindholm1,2, R Brooks2 and F Breden1 1Behavioural Ecology Research Group, Department of Biological Sciences, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada V5A 1S6; 2School of Biological, Earth and Environmental Sciences, University of New South Wales, Sydney NSW 2052, Australia Males of the livebearing fish, Poecilia parae, exhibit one of that the common morph is also the most successful morph in the most complex polymorphisms known to occur within terms of reproduction. However, dichotomous choice tests populations, whereas females are monomorphic. We de- show that two rare morphs are preferred by females over the scribe five distinct male colour morphs and an associated common morph. These results suggest that alternative male size dimorphism, and demonstrate through pedigree analysis mating strategies, sperm competition, overt male–male that the locus or loci controlling the male colour polymorph- competition, or other processes are overriding female ism is linked to the Y-chromosome. Field surveys from 1999 preferences in these populations. Furthermore, Y-linkage of to 2002 of nine populations in Guyana and Suriname, South the colour polymorphism in P. parae supports the hypothesis America, indicate that some morphs are consistently that heterogametic sex chromosomes harbour sexually abundant and others are rare, implying that the colour antagonistic traits beneficial to the heterogametic sex. polymorphism has important fitness consequences. By Heredity (2004) 92, 156–162, advance online publication, 21 rearing offspring of field-inseminated females, we showed January 2004; doi:10.1038/sj.hdy.6800386 Keywords: genetic polymorphism; Y-chromosome; Poecilia parae; sexual selection Introduction are often inherited on sex chromosomes rather than autosomes (Reinhold, 1998). Sexually antagonistic traits Understanding the maintenance of variation that occurs that benefit the heterogametic sex are thought to evolve in discrete morphs is a classic question in evolutionary more easily when linked to the heterogametic sex genetics (eg Fisher, 1931; Clarke, 1962). Several basic chromosome (Y or W), as they are not subject to selection questions must be answered in order to understand the in the other sex (Fisher, 1931; Rice, 1987). Thus, maintenance of such polymorphisms. To what extent are determining the genetic basis and linkage patterns of they environmentally or genetically controlled? If ge- sexually selected traits is essential to understanding their netically controlled, what are the detailed genetic evolution. mechanisms underlying the variation (eg, number of Here, we present the first detailed study of the loci, linkage patterns)? What are the temporal and spatial inheritance and geographic distribution of a sex-limited distributions of morphs? And what are the fitness discrete polymorphism in colour and size in males of the consequences of the different types? livebearing fish Poecilia parae. The morphs are sufficiently Models of sexual selection (eg Lande, 1981; Rice, 1984) distinct that two of them were originally named as and reproductive isolation (eg Kondrashov and Kondra- separate species: P. vivipara parae (Eigenmann, 1894) shov, 1999) have shown the importance of understanding and Acanthophacelus melanzonus (Eigenmann, 1909). the genetic basis of characters involved in these They were later classified as one species, P. parae, with processes, and therefore there has been much recent three male morphs: parae, melanzona and the unco- interest in the genetic architecture of sexually selected loured immaculata (Rosen and Bailey, 1963). Within traits (eg Reinhold, 1998; Ritchie and Phillips, 1998; the melanzona morph, blue and red variants have Lande and Wilkinson, 1999; Sinervo and Svensson, 2002). previously been noted from Guyana (Liley, 1963) and For example, linkage patterns may facilitate or constrain blue and yellow variants from French Guiana (Keith et al, genetic correlations underlying sexual selection pro- 2000). cesses (Lande and Wilkinson, 1999; Lindholm and We document five male morphs in P. parae and show Breden, 2002). Furthermore, fitness consequences such that all five morphs can occur within a single population. as those due to sexual selection can influence the linkage We use field surveys to show that distribution patterns patterns themselves. Sexually antagonistic traits, those within and between populations suggest strong fitness that are beneficial to one sex but detrimental to the other, effects. We then show that these morphs are probably under sexual selection by measuring variation in mating success in the wild, and the discrimination of males by Correspondence: A Lindholm, School of Biological, Earth and Environ- mental Sciences, University of New South Wales, Sydney NSW 2052, females on the basis of colour in the laboratory. Finally, Australia. E-mail: [email protected] we use pedigree analyses to determine the genetic basis Received 13 January 2003; accepted 10 June 2003 and linkage pattern of male colour morphs. Polymorphism in a Y-linked sexually selected trait AK Lindholm et al 157 Methods To determine the genetic basis and linkage pattern of loci underlying colour morphs, we analysed pedigrees of Field surveys males and females from Georgetown and their descen- P. parae occurs along the coast from Guyana to the mouth dants. We assigned mates to virgin daughters and of the Amazon River in Brazil (Rosen and Bailey, 1963). isolated and reared resulting offspring as above. To document the frequency of morphs and their geographic distribution, we searched for P. parae near Dichotomous mate choice tests the coast in ditches draining into rivers in Guyana and Dichotomous choice tests were conducted in one of two Suriname in four separate sets of field surveys (Guyana: identical glass aquaria in the morning from 22 June to 30 June–August 1999, November 2000, January–February August 1999 in Georgetown at the laboratory of the 2002; Suriname: May 2000). P. parae were only found in guesthouse of the Smithsonian Research Institute. Each mud-bottomed ditches of a variety of sizes that served as aquarium had four compartments: a central compart- sewerage outlets. We considered different drainage ment (23  30 cm2) and two end compartments systems into a river as different sampling sites. Adult (11  30 cm2), one of which was divided to make two males and females were captured with dip nets or a equal sized compartments (11 Â15 cm2), one for each test 3m 1.5 m seine net, and male colour patterns were male. The test female in the middle compartment was scored. We noted potential predators, based on Wine- able to see both males at one end of the aquarium at the miller et al (1990) and Endler (1978). At the end of each same time. At the compartment at the other end of the sampling period, fish were either released or taken into tank were two companion females who were housed captivity. there for days at a time and provided the test female an opportunity to school with other females rather than with only males. Males did not direct courtship towards Variation in morph mating success and investigation of these females. genetic basis of colour morphs Each aquarium contained 20 l of rainwater to a depth Male and female P. parae were transported to Simon of 21 cm, and a tablespoon of sea salt. Rainwater was Fraser University, Canada from Georgetown, Guyana in used to provide standard experimental conditions, as the 1999 and from Suriname in 2000. To determine the colour of water collected from the field sites varied distribution of morphs among offspring of females that between days and between study sites. A layer of tan had mated in the wild, we isolated females in 5 l tanks for gravel lined the bottom of the tanks. Differences in the 3–5 months to allow them to produce offspring from field background exterior to the tanks were minimized by inseminations (poeciliids can store sperm for several covering the glass with translucent waxed paper, except months; Constantz, 1989). Surviving females were then for the glass of the companion female compartment, assigned a mate. All offspring in a brood were through which the behaviour of the test female and transferred to a new tank after birth. Prior to sexual males was observed. Lighting was provided by natural maturity (full gonopodial development), sons were daylight and an Aquari-lux FL-20 full-spectrum aqua- separated from daughters to prevent uncontrolled rium light suspended 37 cm above the water surface and inseminations. Male phenotypes were scored when centred between the two tanks, which were placed 11 cm colours developed, upon sexual maturity. Males were apart. scored as ‘immaculata’ if they did not develop colour 2 At the start of a choice test, a test female, a parae male months after sexual maturity. In no case did males and an equal-sized (within 1 mm in standard length) initially scored as immaculata later develop colour. All opponent male were placed into their respective com- fish were kept on a natural daylight schedule and fed partments (see Figure 1 for photographs of males). The with TetraMin fish flakes and brine shrimp nauplii. parae male was placed into the right compartment (with Figure 1 Male and female Poecilia parae from Guyana: (a) red melanzona, (b) yellow melanzona, (c) blue melanzona, (d) parae, (e) immaculata and (f) female. The immaculata male was photographed 12 months after capture as a sexually mature male. Heredity Polymorphism in a Y-linked sexually selected trait AK Lindholm et al 158 respect to the observer) in half of the trials. Opaque and Table 1 Standard length of male morphs plus ANOVA test statistic clear glass partitions divided the two male compart- ments, and also the male compartments and the test Morph N Standard length F df P female compartment. After an acclimation period of Mean SD 10 min, the opaque partitions were removed, and the behaviour of males and the female observed for 10 min.