Archives of Clinical 32 (2017) 876–887

The Neuropsychology of Huntington’s Disease Julie S. Snowden* Greater Manchester Centre, Salford Royal NHS Trust, Salford, UK Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/acn/article/32/7/876/4161107 by guest on 27 September 2021 Division of Neuroscience and Experimental Psychology, Faculty of Biology, Medicine and Health, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK

*Corresponding author at: Cerebral Function Unit, Greater Manchester Neuroscience Centre, Salford Royal NHS Trust, Salford M6 8HD, UK. Fax: +44-161-206-0388; Tel.: +44-161-206-2561. E-mail address: [email protected] (J.S. Snowden). Editorial Decision 25 August 2017; Accepted 4 September 2017

Abstract Huntington’s disease is an inherited, degenerative brain disease, characterized by involuntary movements, cognitive disorder and neuro- psychiatric change. Men and women are affected equally. Symptoms emerge at around 40 years, although there is wide variation. A rare juvenile form has onset in childhood or adolescence. The evolution of disease is insidious and structural and functional brain changes may be present more than a decade before symptoms and signs become manifest. The earliest site of pathology is the striatum and measures of striatal change correlate with neurological and cognitive markers of disease. Chorea and other aspects of the movement disorder are the most visible aspect of the disease. However, non-motor features have greatest affect on functional independence and quality of life, so require recognition and management. The evidence-base for non-pharmacological treatments in Huntington’s disease is currently limited, but recent intervention studies are encouraging.

Keywords: Huntington’s disease; Movement disorders; Subcortex; Basal ganglia; Congenital/genetic Disorders; Executive functions

Introduction and Epidemiology

Huntington’s disease (HD) is an inherited degenerative disorder of the brain, caused by an expansion in the number of CAG repeats in the huntingtin gene on chromosome 4 (Huntington’s Disease Collaborative Research Group, 1993). The mode of inheritance is autosomal dominant and is fully penetrant. Thus, children of an affected parent, both male and female, have a 50% risk of inheriting the faulty gene and gene carriers will develop symptoms of disease during a normal life span. The mean age at which symptoms and signs appear is around 40 years (Harper, 1991), but there is wide variation. In around 5% of cases onset is in childhood or teenage years, referred to as juvenile HD (Quarrell, 2014). At the other extreme, people may remain symptom-free until the seventh or eighth decade of life, referred to as late onset HD. The course of disease is insidiously progressive, the duration of illness from diagnosis to death being about 15–20 years. The insidious evolution of disease is important. Structural and functional brain changes and subtle cognitive, behavioral, and motor changes may begin years before the characteristic physical symptoms and signs are sufficiently manifest to warrant a clinical diagnosis of HD (Brandt, Shpritz, Codori, Margolis, & Rosenblatt, 2002; Paulsen et al., 2008; Paulsen, Smith, & Long, 2013; Stout et al., 2011; Tabrizi et al., 2009, 2012, 2013). The term “manifest HD” refers to “clinically evident” HD, which is not equivalent to “onset of the disease process”, which may be more than a decade earlier. HD occurs worldwide. The overall incidence and prevalence of disease is difficult to determine with accuracy. Like any strongly genetic condition there is geographical clustering so that epidemiological studies that sample from a limited geo- graphical area can result in underestimation or overestimation of overall prevalence. Nevertheless, it is clear that HD is most common in populations of European descent (Kay, Fisher, & Hayden, 2014), and genealogical studies have identified foun- ders from northern Europe, particularly the United Kingdom (Harper, 1992). Current figures for the UK, where prevalence has been most intensively investigated, suggest a prevalence rate in excess of 10 cases per 100,000, with higher than average

© The Author 2017. Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: [email protected]. doi:10.1093/arclin/acx086 Advance Access publication on 18 September 2017 J.S. Snowden / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 32 (2017); 876–887 877 prevalence in Scotland and northeastern England (Evans et al., 2013). The highest prevalence estimate reported in North America is 13.7 per 100,000 in the Canadian province of British Columbia (Fisher & Hayden, 2013), with substantially high- er prevalence in Caucasians than in other ethnic groups.

Neuropathology

HD is a degenerative brain disease, involving progressive atrophy of the brain. The major and earliest site of pathology is the neostriatum, which encompasses the caudate nucleus and putamen (Vonsattel & DiFiglia, 1998; Vonsattel, et al., 1985). The mutated huntingtin protein—the CAG repeat mutation—is assumed to have a toxic function causing neuronal death, the striatum being particularly vulnerable. More widespread brain atrophy is found over the disease course, reflecting loss of structural and functional connectivity between striatum and other parts of the brain. HD has provided a model for understand- Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/acn/article/32/7/876/4161107 by guest on 27 September 2021 ing the role of the basal ganglia in cognition.

Clinical Characteristics

HD gives rise to a triad of clinical features: motor, cognitive, and neuropsychiatric. The movement disorder is distinctive and is the hallmark of the disease. The most characteristic feature is chorea, rapid involuntary movements of the face, trunk, and limbs. Until recent years HD was known as Huntington’s chorea, reflecting that aspect of the movement disorder empha- sized by the eponymous George Huntington in his seminal description of the disease (Huntington, 1872). However, chorea is not the sole motor characteristic (Roos, 2014). People with HD also show dystonia, slow twisting movements of the limbs, as well as bradykinesia, slowed execution of movements, and limb rigidity, akin to that seen in Parkinson’s disease. These differ- ent motor features may co-occur, albeit with variable prominence in different individuals. Notably, juvenile HD is more often associated with prominent bradykinesia than with chorea (Hayden, 1981; Van Dijk, van der Velde, Roos, & Bruyn,1986). Choreiform movements may be striking to the external observer. Yet such movements are rarely a source of complaint to people affected with HD. Indeed, affected individuals report chorea to be less of a problem than do their caregivers (Simpson, Lovecky, Kogan, Vetter, & Yohrling, 2016) and studies have consistently shown reduced awareness of chorea in people with HD (Sitek et al., 2011; Snowden, Craufurd, Griffiths, & Neary,1998; Vitale et al., 2001). The cognitive and neuropsychiatric characteristics of disease are less immediately evident but their recognition is crucial. They contribute greatly to the affected person’s loss of functional independence and they have greatest impact on families (Beglinger et al., 2010; Hamilton et al., 2003; Marder et al., 2000; Mayeux, Stern, Herman, Greenbaum, & Fahn,1986; Nehl, Paulson, & Huntington Study Group, 2004; Ready, Mathews, Leserman, & Paulsen, 2008; Rothlind, Bylsma, Peyser, Folstein, & Brandt,1993; Simpson et al., 2016; Tabrizi et al., 2013). These features are considered below.

Neuropsychological Features

The salient changes in HD are in the domains of psychomotor and executive skills, memory, emotion processing, and social cognition.

Psychomotor Slowing

The earliest change and best predictor of disease progression is psychomotor slowing (Snowden, Craufurd, Griffiths, & Thompson, 2001; Snowden, Craufurd, Thompson, & Neary, 2002; Stout et al., 2011; Tabrizi et al., 2009, 2012, 2013). Slowing is demonstrated most commonly on timed tasks such as Stroop, Digit symbol substitution and Trail making (Snowden et al., 2001; Starkstein et al., 1992; Stout et al., 2012; Tabrizi et al., 2012, 2013). Cognitive slowing is found in the “pre-manifest” stages of HD (Foroud et al., 1995; Kirkwood et al., 1999; Maroof, Gross, & Brandt, 2011; Snowden et al., 2002; Stout et al., 2012; Tabrizi et al., 2012, 2013), and is reported to be a significant predictor of functional capacity in daily life (Eddy & Rickards, 2015a). Interestingly, the word reading component of the Stroop test is a more sensitive marker of change than the more demanding Interference component (Snowden et al., 2001; Tabrizi et al., 2012), attributed to a failure to “automatize” the simpler psychomotor task (Snowden et al., 2001). Psychomotor slowing has considerable practical impact in daily life. It has been found, for example, to be a significant predictor of driving cessation (Beglinger et al., 2012). 878 J.S. Snowden / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 32 (2017); 876–887

Executive Skills

Executive difficulties in HD include problems in planning (Lawrence et al.,1996; Unschuld et al., 2013; Watkins et al., 2000), organization and sequencing (Snowden et al., 2001), cognitive flexibility and set shifting (Lawrence et al., 1996; Paulsen et al., 1995b; Watkins et al., 2000). In early studies, the Wisconsin Card Sorting test was commonly used to measure cognitive flexibility (Josiassen, Curry, & Mancall, 1983; Paulsen et al., 1995b; Pillon, Dubois, Ploska, & Agid, 1991; Weinberger, Berman, Iadarola, Driesen, & Zec, 1988), although its use has diminished in recent years. People with HD com- monly also show reduced performance on verbal fluency tasks (Henry, Crawford, & Phillips, 2005; Rohrer, Salmon, Wixted, & Paulsen,1999; Rosser and Hodges, 1994). Cognitive slowing as well as executive difficulties in strategic search are likely to contribute to low scores (Henry et al., 2005; Rohrer et al., 1999). A common practical difficulty observed in HD is in multi-tasking. In keeping with this, there is neuropsychological evi- dence of problems in attention (Georgiou, Bradshaw, Phillips, Bradshaw, & Chiu, 1995; Sprengelmeyer, Lange, & Homberg, Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/acn/article/32/7/876/4161107 by guest on 27 September 2021 1995). It is worth highlighting, however, that dual task difficulty in HD extends to tasks that in healthy individuals would be considered relatively undemanding of attention, such as bimanual motor tapping (Thompson et al., 2010) and walking concur- rently with carrying out a cognitive task (Delval et al., 2008). Such findings indicate that ostensibly “automatic” tasks require more conscious attention in people with HD.

Memory

Memory difficulties are commonly reported in HD and may be noticed by the affected person as well as by their relatives (Cleret de Langavant et al., 2013). Memory studies have shown proportionally poorer free recall than recognition memory and cued recall (Butters, Salmon, & Heindel,1994; Butters, Wolfe, Martone, Granholm, & Cermak,1985; Lundervold, Reinvang, & Lundervold,1994; Pillon, Deweer, Agid, & Dubois, 1993), more passive learning strategies in HD than controls (Lundervold et al., 1994), problems in source memory (Brandt, Bylsma, Aylward, Rothlind, & Gow, 1995) and in prospective memory (Nicoll et al., 2014) and relative preservation of retention from immediate to delayed recall. The profile of memory disturbances suggests a strong executive contribution to memory failures, in keeping with disruption to striatal-frontal pathways. Aside from problems in declarative memory (i.e. explicit memory for material previously presented), people with HD show problems in procedural memory (i.e. skill and habit learning). Difficulties have been demonstrated on tasks involving motor skill learning (Gabrieli, Stebbins, Singh, Willingham, & Goetz, 1997; Heindel, Butters, & Salmon, 1988), serial reac- tion time (Knopman & Nissen, 1991), and sequence learning (Thompson et al., 2010; Willingham & Koroshetz, 1993; Willingham, Koroshetz, & Peterson, 1996). Problems in procedural memory are consistent with the findings of difficulties in simultaneous execution of relatively low level, “automatic” tasks. The study of HD has been key to understanding of the role of the basal ganglia in memory (Salmon & Butters, 1995).

Emotion Processing and Social Cognition

There is substantial evidence that people with HD have difficulty processing facial expressions of emotion. Early reports suggested that the difficulty was particularly prominent for the emotion of disgust (Sprengelmeyer et al., 1996, 1997), although more recent studies have found deficits at least as great for other negative emotions, in particular anger and fear (Aviezer et al., 2009; Calder et al., 2010; Henley et al., 2008, 2012; Snowden et al., 2008). Impairments are cross modal, affecting recognition of vocal as well as facial emotions (Calder et al., 2010; Snowden et al., 2008). Difficulties in emotional expression as well as recognition have also been demonstrated (Trinkler, Cleret de Langavant, & Bachoud-Lévi, 2013). Emotion processing is important in HD, firstly because deficits appear at a very early stage of disease, including the preclini- cal phase (Johnson et al., 2007; Labuschagne et al., 2013; Tabrizi et al., 2009) and secondly, because they potentially contrib- ute to the social breakdown and reduction in sympathy and empathy that are common features of HD. Social breakdown in daily life encompasses difficulties with interpersonal relationships and reduced flexibility in adapting to the needs of others as well as impaired regulation of behavior in accordance with social conventions. Other aspects of social cognition may also be compromised. People with HD perform poorly on tests of “Theory of mind” that require attribution of intentions, beliefs, and mental states (Allain et al., 2011; Brune, Blank, Witthaus, & Saft, 2011; Eddy, Sira, & Rickards, 2012, 2014; Snowden et al., 2003), recognition of socially inappropriate behavior (Eddy et al., 2012) and sarcasm (Philpott, Andrews, Staios, & Churchyard, 2016). The precise interpretation of these findings and relationship between social cognitive and executive impairments is a subject of debate (Allain et al., 2011; Eddy et al., 2012; Philpott J.S. Snowden / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 32 (2017); 876–887 879 et al., 2016; Snowden et al., 2003). Nevertheless, the presence of such difficulties is likely to contribute to the social break- down in HD. The practical social implications are well illustrated by a study by Sprengelmeyer et al. (2016), which showed that people with HD have difficulty perceiving character traits such as trustworthiness and dominance. Some authors have demonstrated problems in social cognition in the “pre-manifest” stages of HD, before the development of motor symptoms (Adjeroud et al., 2016; Eddy & Rickards, 2015b).

Other Cognitive Domains

The domains outlined above are the most important from the perspective of characterizing the neuropsychological profile in HD. People with HD do not show frank aphasia, agnosia, or apraxia. Nevertheless, speech production becomes progres- fi sively less intelligible due to the motor disorder, they may have dif culty coping with complex syntax and impairments on Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/acn/article/32/7/876/4161107 by guest on 27 September 2021 language tasks may arise secondary to other cognitive difficulties (Podoll, Caspary, Lange, & Noth, 1988). In the visuospatial domain, people with HD have difficulty on high-level perceptual discrimination (Brouwers, Cox, Martin, Chase, & Fedio, 1984; Lawrence et al., 1996), perceptual integration (Bamford, Caine, Kido, Plassche, & Shoulson, 1989; Gomez Tortosa, del Barrio, Barroso, & Garcia Ruiz, 1996) and constructional (Bamford et al., 1989) tasks, which make executive demands. Spatially, people with HD show problems on tasks involving mental rotation or manipulation of information (Brouwers et al., 1984; Bylsma, Brandt, & Strauss, 1992; Mohr et al., 1991) and timed visual search (Labuschagne et al., 2016). Slowed visual search has been shown in the pre-manifest stages of disease and impaired mental rotation in people close to clinical onset (Labuschagne et al., 2016). From a practical perspective, perceptual and spatial problems in HD have not been identified as specific predictors of difficulties in activities such as driving, whereas psychomotor slowing is a strong predictor of driving cessation (Beglinger et al., 2012). Problems in executive function and cognitive decline have a detrimental effect on everyday functioning and level of inde- pendence of people with HD (Mayeux et al., 1986; Rothlind et al., 1993) and quality of life (Ready et al., 2008) and these problems have been described by caregivers as having the greatest impact (Simpson et al., 2016).

Neuropsychiatric Features

Apathy, irritability and depression are the most common and problematic neuropsychiatric symptoms in HD (Craufurd & Snowden, 2014). These symptoms are distinct and dissociable (Craufurd, Thompson, & Snowden, 2001; Kingma, van Duijn, Timman, van der Mast, & Roos, 2008; Naarding, Janzing, Eling, van der Werf, & Kremer, 2009; Rickards et al., 2011), and they follow different trajectories over the disease course (Thompson et al., 2012). Broadly, loss of motivation and drive is an early symptom, which becomes increasingly pervasive over the disease course. Irritability and loss of temper control worsen initially but subside in late stage disease, likely subsumed by apathy. Depressive symptoms may emerge at any time in the course of disease. The presence of apathy correlates with motor, cognitive and functional markers of disease progression (Baudic et al., 2006; Naarding et al., 2009; Thompson, Snowden, Craufurd, & Neary, 2002) and therefore it provides a marker of disease progression, even if not independent, whereas depression and irritability do not. People with HD rarely complain of apathy. Nevertheless, this symptom has a significant effect on functional disability and quality of life (Banaszkiewicz et al., 2012; Hamilton et al., 2003; Read et al., 2013). Importantly, from the perspective of pos- sible intervention, loss of motivation, initiative and spontaneity in HD has been noted to be situation-dependent, and may be reduced in the presence of stimulating input and structure (Caine & Shoulson, 1983; Caine, Hunt, Weingartner, & Ebert, 1978). Irritability and poor temper control are amongst the most troublesome behavioral features of HD and are typically assessed using standardized self-report or informant-based irritability scales (Craufurd et al., 2001; Reedeker et al., 2012). In one study cohort (Craufurd et al., 2001), clinically significant irritability was reported in more than 50%, verbal outbursts of temper in 40% and threatening behavior or violence in 22%. Studies have also shown higher levels of irritability in asymptomatic car- riers of the HD gene compared to non-carriers (Berrios et al., 2002; Julien et al., 2007). In these latter studies interviews were carried out prior to genetic testing so that differences could not be attributed to gene carriers’ knowledge of their genetic sta- tus. An increased frequency of irritability was seen up to 10 years before the onset of motor abnormalities. Irritability and poor temper control inevitably has a damaging effect on personal relationships, and so requires recognition and management. Treatment commonly involves the use of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, although the potential value of behavioral ap- proaches to management is recognized (van Duijn, 2010). Depressive symptoms, which may occur even in the prodromal phases of disease (Epping et al., 2016; Julien et al., 2007; van Duijn et al., 2008), may exacerbate apathy and social withdrawal, worsen cognitive performance (Smith, Mills, Epping, 880 J.S. Snowden / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 32 (2017); 876–887

Westervelt, & Paulsen, 2012), contribute to functional decline (Beglinger et al., 2010; Marder et al., 2000; Mayeux et al., 1986) and further impair quality of life (Read et al., 2013), exemplifying the need for recognition and treatment. Moreover, depressive symptoms may be associated with suicidal ideation (Hubers et al., 2013). An increased risk of suicide has been recognized since Huntington’s seminal description of the condition (Huntington, 1872). A recent study of more than two thousand HD mutation carriers, 98% with physical symptoms of HD, reported suicidal ideation in 8% (Hubers et al., 2013). Paulsen, Hoth, Nehl, Stierman and the Huntington Study Group (2005a) identified two particularly critical periods of vulnera- bility: the prodromal phase of disease, defined by the presence of mild neurological signs insufficient to make a firm diagnosis of HD, and when functional independence begins to diminish.

Neuropsychological Assessment Strategy Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/acn/article/32/7/876/4161107 by guest on 27 September 2021 With the advent of the genetic test, neuropsychological assessment now plays a relatively minor role in differential diagno- sis. A clinical diagnosis of HD is typically made on the basis of family history of HD, presence of the characteristic move- ment disorder and confirmed mutation on genetic testing. Nevertheless, neuropsychology has a crucial role in the identification of cognitive changes in the early phases of disease, in monitoring progression and in the evaluation of outcome of therapeutic interventions. Ideally, an assessment protocol should include each of those key components that have been found most sensitive to HD: psychomotor speed, memory, executive skills, and emotion recognition. However, consideration needs to be given to the pur- pose of the assessment and stage of illness. People in preclinical phases of disease may show only very subtle changes whereas people in the later stages of disease may be unable to tolerate lengthy assessments. Timed tasks are particularly valu- able because they elicit the earliest detectable changes in pre-manifest HD and are also most sensitive to change over the course of the disease (Snowden et al., 2001, 2002; Stout et al., 2011, 2012; Tabrizi et al., 2012). In keeping with such find- ings, the Huntington’s Disease Rating Scale (Huntington Study Group, 1996), a widely used screening instrument for HD, in- corporates a brief cognitive screen consisting of three tests, all of which are timed: verbal fluency, symbol digit modality, and Stroop. Similarly, the European Huntington’s disease network (Euro-HD), a European-wide network for research into Huntington’s disease, has adopted letter and category fluency, symbol-digit modality, and Stroop tests as “core” components of their recommended neuropsychological test battery, with additional elements where more extensive assessment is feasible. Some memory and executive measures are of value for cross-sectional assessment but show poorer sensitivity to change over time (Bachoud-Lévi et al., 2001; Snowden et al., 2001). One contributory factor is the effect of practice. It has been found (Bachoud-Lévi et al., 2001) that the practice effect is greatest between first and second assessment. In intervention stud- ies, the confounding effect can be mitigated if dual baseline assessments are introduced preceding the intervention. An additional consideration in evaluating cognition in HD is the contribution to performance of motor and neuropsychiat- ric/behavioral changes. Timed tasks that control for motor slowing are valuable because they provide information about cog- nitive speed that is independent of general slowing of motor responses. Apathy is a core behavioral change in HD that correlates with executive impairment (Thompson et al., 2002). The inclusion of informant-based behavioral measures, such as the Problems Behavior Assessment (Craufurd et al., 2001) can assist interpretation of cognitive test performance.

Laboratory and Radiographic Investigations

The genetic mutation responsible for HD was identified in 1993 (Huntington’s Disease Collaborative Research Group, 1993). Since then a clinical genetic test has become available to test for the CAG repeat expansion and is used routinely to confirm the clinical diagnosis of people who show symptoms of HD. The test, along with genetic counseling, is also available for “at risk” family members (i.e., offspring of an affected parent) who wish to know whether or not they have inherited the gene mutation. Some people prefer the certainty of knowing their gene status because this affords control of life choices, such as whether to have a family or to take promotion at work. On the other hand, the knowledge can lead to a variety of practical and emotional difficulties, ranging from difficulty in obtaining insurance cover to breakdown in interpersonal relationships with siblings who have a different genetic result from one’s own. The experience of predictive testing may be stressful and adjustment to test results can be difficult (Crozier, Robertson, & Dale, 2015; Meiser & Dunn, 2001). Around 80% of “at risk” people opt not to know (Meiser & Dunn, 2001). There has not, thus far, been a notable increase in uptake of the genetic test over the years that it has been available, and the reverse has even been reported (Bernhardt, Schwan, Kraus, Epplen, & Kunstmann, 2009). Nevertheless, the situation may change in the future as clinical therapeutic trials become available, for which confirmation of gene status may be a criterion for eligibility (Nance, 2017). J.S. Snowden / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 32 (2017); 876–887 881

The genetic test confirms whether an asymptomatic person will develop symptoms of HD but not when those symptoms will first appear. There is an inverse correlation between age at which disease becomes manifest and length of the CAG repeat expansion and formulae have been produced to predict age of clinical onset, i.e., when overt symptoms will begin (Langbehn, Brinkman, Falush, Paulsen, & Hayden, 2004; Zhang et al., 2011). Such prediction is insufficiently accurate to be helpful at an individual level for an asymptomatic person taking the genetic test. Nevertheless, it provides a valuable proxy of clinical onset for research studies. Major large-scale international research studies have been undertaken: the PREDICT-HD study in the USA (Paulsen et al., 2008) and TRACK-HD in Europe (Tabrizi et al., 2009, 2012, 2013) that have tracked people who have the HD gene mutation to establish the natural history of the disease. Brain imaging in HD shows atrophy, with structural and functional changes most prominent in the striatum (caudate nucleus and putamen). Striatal changes are present even in pre-manifest HD, i.e., before the emergence of overt clinical symp- toms and signs, (Andrews et al., 1999; Antonini et al., 1996; Aylward et al., 2004; Tabrizi et al., 2013), reinforcing the view that this is the earliest site of pathology. The availability of the genetic test means that neuroimaging is not essential for clini- Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/acn/article/32/7/876/4161107 by guest on 27 September 2021 cal diagnosis of HD. Nevertheless, it has been important in helping to understand the basis for the symptoms and signs in HD, and the natural history of the condition. Psychomotor speed in HD has been shown to correlate with measures of caudate atrophy (Bamford et al., 1989; Starkstein et al., 1992). There is evidence too of a relationship between executive test perfor- mance in HD and changes in the striatum, measured either by structural (Peinemann et al., 2005) or functional (Backman, Robins-Wahlin, Lundin, Ginovart, & Farde,1997; Lawrence et al., 1998) brain imaging. Interestingly, functional imaging studies in HD have demonstrated increased cortical recruitment in cognitive tasks (Georgiou-Karistianis et al., 2007), indicat- ing compensation for reduced striatal function.

Treatment

HD is a relentlessly progressive disorder that is currently incurable. Treatments have, until recently, largely comprised pharmacological symptomatic therapies for the alleviation of motor and mood-based symptoms. Tetrabenazine is a well- established treatment for involuntary movements, although it may increase the likelihood of depression in pre-disposed people (Kenney, Hunter, Meijia, & Jankovic, 2006) so needs to be used judiciously. SSRIs have been used for the treatment of irrita- bility (Groves et al., 2011). Currently, the evidence-base for non-pharmacological interventions in HD is limited. Assistive technology for cognition (ATC) refers to external aids that serve to compensate for a person’s cognitive difficulties. Two studies have shown that Talking Mats improved communication in people with relatively advanced HD who had poorly intelligible speech (Ferm, Sahlin, Sundin, & Hartelius, 2010; Hallberg, Mellgren, Hartelius, & Ferm, 2013). Notwithstanding this encouraging finding, evidence for the benefit of assistive technology use in HD remains scarce. Van Walsem, Howe, Frich and Andelic (2016) car- ried out a review of assistive technology use in 158 people with HD. The authors distinguished between self-generated, infor- mal use of mainstream products such as calendars, planners, cell phones and alarm clocks and formal use of computer software, specifically designed to support people with cognitive impairment. Around a third of participants used assistive tech- nology, with informal use occurring mainly in early-stage disease and formal use in moderate-stage disease. Notably, how- ever, a formal needs assessment for assistive technology had been carried out in only a third of participants and even fewer had received formal training for its use. No association was found between the use of assistive technology and improved qual- ity of life. The study by van Walsem et al. (2016) was descriptive rather than efficacy study. The authors highlighted the need for systematic investigation of the value of assistive technology in HD and its association with health-related quality of life. There are a priori grounds for thinking that assistive technology may be helpful. Memory inefficiencies, which include pro- blems in prospective memory (Nicoll et al., 2014), might potentially be alleviated by use of external prompts and memory aids. A potential rationale for cognitive intervention in HD comes from functional brain imaging findings, which suggest that neural compensation takes place in preclinical HD to preserve motor and cognitive performance. Cognitive interventions aim to support and enhance that neural compensation (Andrews, Dominguez, Mercieca, Georgiou-Kartistianis & Stout, 2015; Metzler-Baddeley et al., 2014; Papoutsi, Labuschagne, Tabrizi, & Stout, 2014). In a pilot study, Metzler-Baddeley et al. (2014) showed improved executive functions and white matter microstructure in ten people at varying stages of HD following a 2-month intervention involving rhythm exercises. Busse et al. (2013) too reported modest benefits from physical exercise training on cognition and mobility. Cruickshank et al. (2015) reported a multidisciplinary 9-month intervention study, involv- ing a program of physical exercises combined with cognitive exercises of verbal planning, memory and problem solving. The study group of 15 HD participants showed increased gray matter volumes on brain imaging that correlated with improved ver- bal learning performance at the end of the 9-month period. These findings offer a degree of optimism that interventions have 882 J.S. Snowden / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 32 (2017); 876–887

Table 1. Key empirically supported recommendations Assessment value References Findings Recommendations domain Diagnostic Lawrence et al. (1996), Paulsen et al. (1995b), Psychomotor slowing, executive, Include in test battery timed tasks such as Stroop, assessment of Snowden et al. (2001), Watkins et al. (2000), memory and emotion Trail making, Symbol Digit modalities test and manifest HD Henley et al. (2012) recognition impairments are verbal fluency as well as a memory test such as prominent features list learning and test of emotion recognition such as Ekman faces Diagnostic Maroof et al. (2011), Snowden et al. (2002), Psychomotor deficits are the Include Symbol Digit Modalities and Stroop tests assessment in Stout et al. (2012), Tabrizi et al. (2009) earliest detectable change in in assessment of gene carriers pre-manifest gene carriers HD Longitudinal Snowden et al. (2001), Stout et al. (2012), Psychomotor tasks are the most Symbol digit Modalities and Stroop tests are Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/acn/article/32/7/876/4161107 by guest on 27 September 2021 assessment – Tabrizi et al. (2012, 2013) sensitive markers of change useful outcome measures for clinical trials and sensitivity to over time intervention studies change over time Prediction of Sprengelmeyer et al. (1995), Georgiou et al. Difficulty with multi-tasking Encourage one thing at a time. Separate tasks into treatment (1995), Delval et al. (2008), Butters et al. including low level tasks manageable components. response (1985, 1994), Lundervold et al. (1994), Pillon Poorer free recall than cued Provide structure and external memory aids et al. (1993), Metzler-Baddeley et al. (2014), recall and recognition Multidisciplinary interventions can be helpful Andrews et al. (2015), Cruickshank et al. Adopts passive learning Use external stimulation to improve motivation (2015), Caine and Shoulson (1983), Caine strategies and engagement et al. (1978) Physical and cognitive exercises can improve cognitive function Apathy is reduced by stimulation Prediction of Eddy and Rickards (2015a), Mayeux et al. Cognitive slowing predicts Cognitive assessment is necessary as predictor of functional (1986), Rothlind et al. (1993), Simpson et al. reduced functional ability overall functional ability, quality of life and ability (2016), Tabrizi et al. (2012, 2013) Executive deficits impair care-giver impact functional ability Executive deficits have impact on caregivers Change in psychomotor scores predicts functional decline the potential for neural enhancement, which may delay clinical decline. Nevertheless, randomized controlled trials of cogni- tive intervention are required to establish the efficacy of this approach. Interventions in HD need to address behavioral/neuropsychiatric as well as cognitive problems. Apathy is a pervasive fea- ture that affects the quality of life. The finding of improvement with stimulating input and structure (Caine & Shoulson, 1983; Caine et al.,1978) mirrors clinical experience and has obvious implications for management. People with HD can benefit from external prompts, encouragement to participate in pleasurable activities and the provision of a structured environment. Irritability is another troubling feature of HD. There may, however, be predisposing or precipitating factors, and if recognized, aggressive outbursts can be circumvented. Education of family members is an important aspect of management of people with HD. When embarking on interventions with people who have HD, an important consideration is their reduced awareness of motor (Sitek et al., 2011; Snowden et al., 1998), cognitive/behavioral (Ho, Robbins, & Barker, 2006; Hoth et al., 2007; Sitek, Thompson, Craufurd, & Snowden, 2014), emotional, and functional (Hoth et al., 2007) changes.

Recommendations

Recommendations for neuropsychological assessment and intervention are summarized in Table 1. Timed psychomotor tasks need to be considered a core and necessary component of the assessment, regardless of stage of disease or the purpose of assessment. Executive, memory, and emotion recognition tests are valuable, particularly in manifest disease. Aside from cognitive assessment, evaluation needs to take account of behavioral/affective symptoms. Rating scales for behavioral symptoms of HD have undergone a systematic critique by an International study group, commissioned by the Movement Disorder Society (Mestre et al., 2016). “Suggested” scales specifically designed for use in HD include the Problems Behaviors Assessment for Huntington’s disease (PBA) (Craufurd et al., 2001) and Unified Huntington’s Disease J.S. Snowden / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 32 (2017); 876–887 883

Rating Scale, behavioral section (Huntington’s Study Group, 1996). The Apathy sub-scale of the PBA has been recommended by Tabrizi et al. (2012) as a behavioral measure for use in clinical trials. Interventions in HD need to take account of a number of characteristics.

• People with HD require extra time to carry out everyday tasks. • People with HD are forgetful but do not have a classical amnesia. They have the potential to benefit from memory aids. • Tasks, such as walking and talking, which under normal circumstances might be regarded as relatively “automatic”, require more conscious attention in people with HD. They are more demanding of attentional resources. It is impor- tant for people with HD to focus on one activity at a time. • People with HD may not initiate activities but with encouragement can engage successfully in them and experience enjoyment. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/acn/article/32/7/876/4161107 by guest on 27 September 2021

Conflict of interest

None declared.

References

Adjeroud, N., Besnard, J., El Massiouri, N., Verny, C., Prudean, A., Scherer, C., et al. (2016). Theory of mind and empathy in preclinical and clinical Huntington’s disease. Social Cognitive and , 11,89–99. Allain, P., Havet-Thomassin, V., Verny, C., Gohier, B., Lancelot, C., Besnard, J., et al. (2011). Evidence for deficits on different components of theory of mind in Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychology, 25, 741–751. Andrews, T. C., Weeks, R. A., Turjanski, N., Gunn, R. N., Watkins, L. H. A., Sahakian, B., et al. (1999). Huntington’s disease progression: PET and clinical observations. Brain, 122, 2353–2363. Andrews, S. C., Dominguez, J. F., Mercieca, E. C., Georgiou-Karistianis, N., & Stout, J. C. (2015). Cognitive interventions to enhance neural compensation in Huntington’s disease. Neurodegenerative Disease Management, 5, 155–164. Antonini, A., Leenders, K. L., Spiegel, R., Meier, D., Vontobel, P., Weigell-Weber, M., et al. (1996). Striatal glucose metabolism and dopamine D2 receptor binding in asymptomatic gene carriers and patients with Huntington’s disease. Brain, 119, 2085–2095. Aviezer, H., Bentin, S., Hassin, R. R., Meschino, W. S., Kennedy, J., Grewal, S., et al. (2009). Not on the face alone: Perception of contextualized face ex- pressions in Huntington’s disease. Brain, 132, 1633–1644. Aylward, E. H., Sparks, B. F., Field, K. M., Yallapragada, V., Shpritz, B. D., Rosenblatt, A., et al. (2004). Onset and rate of striatal atrophy in preclinical Huntington disease. , 63,66–72. Bachoud-Lévi, A.-C., Maison, P., Bartolemeo, P., Boissé, M.-F., Dalla Barba, G., Ergis, A.-M., et al. (2001). Retest effects and cognitive decline in longitu- dinal follow-up of patients with early HD. Neurology, 56, 1052–1058. Backman, L., Robins-Wahlin, T. B., Lundin, A., Ginovart, N., & Farde, L. (1997). Cognitive deficits in Huntington’s disease are predicted by dopaminergic PET markers and brain volumes. Brain, 120, 2207–2217. Bamford, K. A., Caine, E. D., Kido, D. K., Plassche, W. M., & Shoulson, I. (1989). Clinical-pathologic correlation in Huntington’s disease: A neuropsycho- logical and computed tomography study. Neurology, 39, 796–801. Banaszkiewicz, K., Sitek, E. J., Rudzinska, M., Soltan, W., Slawek, J., & Szczudlik, A. (2012). Huntington’s disease from the patient, caregiver and physi- cian’s perspectives: Three sides of the same coin? Journal of Neural Transmission, 119, 1361–1365. Baudic, S., Maison, P., Dolbeau, G., Boisse, M. F., Bartolomeo, P., Dalla Barba, G., et al. (2006). Cognitive impairment related to apathy in early Huntington’s disease. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 21, 316–321. Beglinger, L. J., O’Rourke, J. J. F., Wang, C., Langbehn, D. R., Duff, K., Paulsen, J. S., et al. (2010). Earliest functional declines in Huntington disease. Research, 178, 414–418. Beglinger, L. J., Prest, L., Mills, J. A., Paulsen, J. S., Smith, M. M., Gonzalez-Alegre, P., et al. (2012). Clinical predictors of driving status in Huntington dis- ease. Movement Disorders, 27, 1146–1152. Bernhardt, C., Schwan, A.-M., Kraus, P., Epplen, J. T., & Kunstmann, E. (2009). Decreasing uptake of predictive testing for Huntington’s disease in a German centre: 12 years’ experience (1993–2004). European Journal of Human Genetics, 17, 295–300. Berrios, G. E., Wagle, A. C., Markova, I. S., Wagle, S. A., Rosser, A., & Hodges, J. R. (2002). Psychiatric symptoms in neurologically asymptomatic Huntington’s disease gene carriers: A comparison with gene negative at risk subjects. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 105, 224–230. Brandt, J., Bylsma, F. W., Aylward, E. H., Rothlind, J., & Gow, C. A. (1995). Impaired source memory in Huntington’s disease and its relation to basal gan- glia atrophy. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17, 868–877. Brandt, J., Shpritz, B., Codori, A. M., Margolis, R., & Rosenblatt, A. (2002). Neuropsychological manifestations of the genetic mutation for Huntington’s dis- ease in presymptomatic individuals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 918–924. Brouwers, P., Cox, C., Martin, A., Chase, T., & Fedio, P. (1984). Differential perceptuo-spatial impairment in Huntington’s disease and Alzheimer’s demen- tias. Archives of Neurology, 41, 1073–1076. Brune, M., Blank, K., Witthaus, H., & Saft, C. (2011). “Theory of mind” is impaired in Huntington’s disease. Movement Disorders, 26, 671–678. Busse, M., Quinn, L., Debono, K., Jones, K., Collett, J., Playle, R., et al. (2013). A randomized feasibility study of a 12-weel community-based exercise pro- gram for people with Huntington’s disease. Journal of Neurologic Physical Therapy, 37, 149–158. 884 J.S. Snowden / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 32 (2017); 876–887

Butters, N., Wolfe, J., Martone, M., Granholm, E., & Cermak, L. S. (1985). Memory disorders associated with Huntington’s disease: Verbal recall, verbal rec- ognition and procedural memory. Neuropsychologia, 23, 729–743. Butters, N., Salmon, D., & Heindel, W. C. (1994). Specificity of the memory deficits associated with basal ganglia function. Revue Neurologique, Paris, 150, 580–587. Bylsma, F. W., Brandt, J., & Strauss, M. E. (1992). Personal and extrapersonal orientation in Huntington’s disease patients and those at risk. Cortex, 28, 113–122. Caine, E. D., & Shoulson, I. (1983). Psychiatric syndromes in Huntington’s disease. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 728–733. Caine, E. D., Hunt, R. D., Weingartner, H., & Ebert, M. H. (1978). Huntington’s dementia. Clinical and neuropsychological features. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 377–384. Calder, A. J., Keane, J., Young, A. W., Lawrence, A. D., Mason, S., & Barker, R. A. (2010). The relation between anger and different forms of disgust: Implications for emotion recognition impairments in Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 48, 2719–2729. Cleret de Langavant, L., Fénelon, G., Benisty, S., Boissé, M.-F., Jacquemot, C., & Bachoud-Lévi, A.-C. (2013). Awareness of memory deficits in early stage Huntington’s disease. PLOS one, 8, e61676.

Craufurd, D., & Snowden, J. S. (2014). and neuropsychology. In Bates G. P., Tabrizi S., & Jones L. (Eds.), Huntington’s disease (4th ed., Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/acn/article/32/7/876/4161107 by guest on 27 September 2021 pp. 36–65). New York: Oxford University Press. Craufurd, D., Thompson, J., & Snowden, J. S. (2001). Behavioural changes in Huntington’s disease: The Problem Behaviours Assessment (2001). Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology and Behavioural Neurology, 14, 219–226. Crozier, S., Robertson, N., & Dale, M. (2015). The psychological impact of predictive genetic testing for Huntington’s disease: A systematic review of the lit- erature. Journal of Genetic Counseling, 24,29–39. Cruickshank, T. M., Thompson, J. A., Dominguez, D., Reyes, A. P., Bynevelt, M., Georgiou-Karistianis, N., et al. (2015). The effect of multidisciplinary rehabilitation on brain structure and cognition in Huntington’s disease: An exploratory study. Brain and Behaviour, 5, e00312. Delval, A., Krystkowiak, P., Delliaux, M., Dujardin, K., Blatt, J. L., Destee, A., et al. (2008). Role of attentional resources on gait performance in Huntington’s disease. Movement Disorders, 23, 684–689. Eddy, C. M., & Rickards, H. E. (2015a). Cognitive deficits predict poorer functional capacity in Huntington’s disease: But what is being measured? Neuropsychology, 29, 268–273. Eddy, C. M., & Rickards, H. E. (2015b). Theory of mind can be impaired prior to motor onset in Huntington’s disease. Neuropschology, 29, 792–798. Eddy, C. M., Sira, M. S., & Rickards, H. E. (2012). Is Huntington’s disease associated with deficits in theory of mind? Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 126, 376–383. Eddy, C. M., Sira, M. S., & Rickards, H. E. (2014). Putting things into perspective: The nature and impact of theory of mind impairment in Huntington’s dis- ease. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 264, 697–705. Epping, E. A., Kim, J. I., Craufurd, D., Brashers-Krug, T. M., Anderson, K. E., McCusker, E., et al. (2016). Longitudinal psychiatric symptoms in prodromal Huntington’s disease: A decade of data. American Journal of Psychiatry, 173, 184–192. Evans, S. J., Douglas, I., Rawlins, M. D., Wexler, N. S., Tabrizi, S. J., & Smeeth, L. (2013). Prevalence of adult Huntington’s disease in the UK based on diagnoses recorded in general practice records. Journal of Neurology, and Psychiatry, 84, 1156–1160. Ferm, U., Sahlin, A., Sundin, L., & Hartelius, L. (2010). Using Talking Mats to support communication in persons with Huntington’s disease. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 45, 523–536. Fisher, E. R., & Hayden, M. R. (2013). Multisource ascertainment of Huntington’s disease in Canada: Prevalence and population risk. Movement Disorders, 29, 105–114. Foroud, T., Siemers, E., Kleindorfer, D., Bill, D. J., Hodes, M. E., Norton, J. A., et al. (1995). Cognitive scores in carriers of Huntington’s disease gene com- pared to noncarriers. Annals of Neurology, 37, 657–664. Gabrieli, J. D. E., Stebbins, G. T., Singh, J., Willingham, D. B., & Goetz, C. G. (1997). Intact mirror-tracing and impaired rotary-pursuit skill learning in pa- tients with Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychology, 11, 272–281. Georgiou, N., Bradshaw, J. L., Phillips, J. G., Bradshaw, J. A., & Chiu, E. (1995). The Simon effect and attention deficits in Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome and Huntington’s disease. Brain, 118, 1305–1318. Georgiou-Karistianis, N., Sritharan, A., Farrow, M., Cunnington, R., Stout, J., Bradshaw, J., et al. (2007). Increased cortical recruitment in Huntington’s dis- ease using a Simon task. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1791–1800. Gomez Tortosa, E., del Barrio, A., Barroso, T., & Garcia Ruiz, P. J. (1996). Visual processing disorders in patients with Huntington’s disease and asymptom- atic carriers. Journal of Neurology, 243, 286–292. Groves, M., van Duijn, E., Anderson, K., Craufurd, D., Edmondson, M., Goodman, N., et al. (2011). An international survey-based algorithm for the pharma- cologic treatment of irritability in Huntington’s disease. Huntington’s disease. PLoS Currents, 3, RRN1259. Hallberg, L., Mellgren, E., Hartelius, L., & Ferm, U. (2013). Talking Mats in a discussion group for people with Huntington’s disease. Disability & Rehabilitation Assistive Technology, 8,67–76. Hamilton, J. M., Salmon, D. P., Corey-Bloom, J., Gamst, A., Paulsen, J. S., Jerkins, S., et al. (2003). Behavioural abnormalities contribute to functional decline in Huntington’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 74, 120–122. Harper, P. S. (1991). The natural history of Huntington’s disease. In Harper P. S. (Ed.), Huntington’s disease (pp. 127–139). London: W.B. Saunders Company Ltd.. Harper, P. S. (1992). The epidemiology of Huntington’s disease. Human Genetics, 89, 365–376. Hayden, M. (1981). Huntington’s chorea (pp. 78–81). Berlin: Springer. Heindel, W. C., Butters, N., & Salmon, D. P. (1988). Impaired learning of a motor skill in patients with Huntington’s disease. Behavioural Neuroscience, 102, 141–147. Henley, S. M., Wild, E. J., Hobbs, N. Z., Warren, J. D., Frost, C., Scahill, R. I., et al. (2008). Defective emotion recognition in early HD is neuropsycholo- gically and anatomically generic. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2152–2160. Henley, S. M., Novak, M. J. U., Frost, C., King, J., Tabrizi, S. J., & Warren, J. D. (2012). Emotion recognition in Huntington’s disease: A systematic review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 237–253. J.S. Snowden / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 32 (2017); 876–887 885

Henry, J. D., Crawford, J. R., & Phillips, L. H. (2005). A meta-analytic review of verbal fluency deficits in Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychology, 19, 243–252. Ho, A. K., Robbins, A. O., & Barker, R. A. (2006). Huntington’s disease patients have selective problems with insight. Movement Disorders, 21, 385–389. Hoth, K. F., Paulsen, J. S., Moser, D. J., Tranel, D., Clark, L. A., & Bechara, A. (2007). Patients with Huntington’s disease have impaired awareness of cogni- tion, emotional and functional abilities. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29, 365–376. Hubers, A. A. M., van Duijn, W., Roos, R. A. C., Craufurd, D., Rickards, H., Landwehrmeyer, G. B., et al. (2013). Suicidal ideation in a European Huntington’s disease population. Journal of Affective Disorders, 151, 248–258. HuntingtonG.1872On choreaMedical and Surgical Reporter26320321 Huntington’s disease Collaborative Research Group.. (1993). A novel gene containing a trinucleotide repeat that is expanded and unstable on Huntington’s disease chromosomes. Cell, 72, 971–983. Huntington Study Group. (1996). Unified Huntington’s Disease Rating Scale: Reliability and consistency. Movement Disorders, 11, 136–142. Johnson, S. A., Stout, J. C., Solomon, A. C., Langbehn, D. R., Aylward, E. H., Cruce, C. B., et al. (2007). Beyond disgust: Impaired recognition of negative emotions prior to diagnosis in Huntington’s disease. Brain, 130, 1732–1744.

Josiassen, R. C., Curry, L. M., & Mancall, E. L. (1983). Development of neuropsychological deficits in Huntington’s disease. Archives of Neurology, 40, Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/acn/article/32/7/876/4161107 by guest on 27 September 2021 791–796. Julien, C. L., Thompson, J. C., Wild, S., Yardumian, P., Snowden, J. S., Turner, G., et al. (2007). Psychiatric disorders in preclinical Huntington’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 78, 939–943. Kay, C., Fisher, E., & Hayden, M. R. (2014). Epidemiology. In Bates G. P., Tabrizi S., & Jones L. (Eds.), Huntington’s Disease (4th ed., pp. 131–164). Oxford University Press. Kenney, C., Hunter, C., Meijia, N., & Jankovic, J. (2006). Is history of depression a contraindication to treatment with tetrabenazine? Clinical , 29, 259–264. Kingma, E. M., van Duijn, E., Timman, R., van der Mast, R. C., & Roos, R. A. C. (2008). Behavioural problems in Huntington’s disease using the Problems Behaviours Assessment. General Hospital Psychiatry, 30, 155–161. Kirkwood, S. C., Siemers, E., Stout, J. C., Hodes, M. E., Conneally, P. M., Christian, J. C., et al. (1999). Longitudinal cognitive and motor changes among presymptomatic Huntington disease gene carriers. Archives of Neurology, 56, 563–568. Knopman, D., & Nissen, M. J. (1991). Procedural learning is impaired in Huntington’s disease: Evidence from the serial reaction time task. Neuropsychologia, 29, 245–254. Labuschagne, I., Jones, R., Callaghan, J., Whitehead, D., Dumas, E. M., Say, M. J., et al. (2013). Emotional face recognition deficits and medication effects in pre-manifest through stage-II Huntington’s disease. Psychiatry Research, 207, 118–126. Labuschagne, I., Mulick Cassidy, A., Schahill, R. I., Johnson, E. J., Rees, E., O’Regan, A., et al. (2016). Visuospatial processing decifits linked to posterior brain regions in premanifest and early stage Huntington’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 22, 595–608. Langbehn, D. R., Brinkman, R. R., Falush, D., Paulsen, J. S., & Hayden, M. R. (2004). A new model for prediction of the age of onset and penetrance for Huntington’s disease based on CAG length. Clinical Genetics, 65, 267–277. Lawrence, A. D., Sahakian, B. J., Hodges, J. R., Rosser, A. E., Lange, K. W., & Robbins, T. W. (1996). Executive and mnemonic functions in early Huntington’s disease. Brain, 119, 1343–1355. Lawrence, A. D., Weeks, R. A., Brooks, D. J., Andrews, T. C., Watkins, L. H., Harding, A. E., et al. (1998). The relationship between striatal dopamine receptor binding and cognitive performance in Huntington’s disease. Brain, 121, 1343–1355. Lundervold, A. J., Reinvang, I., & Lundervold, A. (1994). Characteristic patterns of verbal memory function in patients with Huntington’s disease. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 35,38–47. Marder, K., Zhao, H., Myers, R. H., Cudkowicz, M., Kayson, E., Kieburtz, K., et al. (2000). Rate of functional decline in Huntington’s disease. Huntington Study Group. Neurology, 54, 452–458. Maroof, D. A., Gross, A. L., & Brandt, J. (2011). Modeling longitudinal change in motor and cognitive processing speed in presymptomatic Huntington’s dis- ease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33, 901–909. Mayeux, R., Stern, Y., Herman, A., Greenbaum, L., & Fahn, S. (1986). Correlates of early disability in Huntington’s disease. Annals of Neurology, 20, 727–731. Meiser, B., & Dunn, S. (2001). Psychological effect of genetic testing for Huntington’s disease: An update of the literature. Western Journal of Medicine, 174, 336–340. Metzler-Baddeley, C., Cantera, J., Coulthard, E., Rosser, A., Jones, D. K., & Baddeley, R. J. (2014). Improved executive function and collosal white matter microstructure after rhythm exercise in Huntington’s disease. Journal of Huntington’s Disease, 3, 273–283. Mestre, A., van Duijn, E., Davis, A. M., Bachoud-Lévi, A.-C., Busse, M., Anderson, K. E., et al. (2016). Rating scales for behavioral symptoms in Huntington’s disease: Critique and recommendations. Movement Disorders, 31, 1466–1478. Mohr, E., Brouwers, P., Claus, J. J., Mann, U. M., Fedio, P., & Chase, T. N. (1991). Visuospatial cognition in Huntington’s disease. Movement Disorders, 6, 127–132. Naarding, P., Janzing, J. G. E., Eling, P., van der Werf, S., & Kremer, B. (2009). Apathy is not depression in Huntington’s disease. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical , 21, 266–270. Nance, M. A. (2017). Genetic counseling and testing for Huntington’s disease: A historical review. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 174B,75–92. Nehl, C., & Paulsen, J. S., Huntington Study Group. (2004). Cognitive and psychiatric aspects of Huntington disease contribute to functional capacity. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 192,72–74. Nicoll, D. R., Pirogovsky, E., Woods, S. P., Holden, H. M., Filoteo, J. V., Gluhm, S., et al. (2014). “Forgetting to remember” in Huntington’s disease: A study of laboratory, semi-naturalistic, and self-perceptions of prospective memory. Journal of the Neuropsychological Society, 20, 192–199. Papoutsi, M., Labuschagne, I., Tabrizi, S. J., & Stout, J. C. (2014). The cognitive burden in Huntington’s disease: Pathology, phenotype, and mechanisms of compensation. Movement Disorders, 29, 673–683. Paulsen, J. S., Hoth, K. F., Nehl, C., & Stierman, L., the Huntington Study Group. (2005a). Critical periods of suicide risk in Huntington’s disease. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 725–731. 886 J.S. Snowden / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 32 (2017); 876–887

Paulsen, J. S., Salmon, D. P., Monsch, A. U., Butters, N., Swanson, M. R., & Bondi, M. W. (1995b). Discrimination of cortical from subcortical dementias on the basis of memory and problem-solving tests. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51,48–58. Paulsen, J. S., Langbehn, D. R., Stout, J. C., Aylward, E., Ross, C. A., Nance, M., et al. (2008). Detection of Huntington’s disease decades before diagnosis: The Predict-HD study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 79, 874–880. Paulsen, J. S., Smith, M. M., & Long, J. D. (2013). Cognitive decline in prodromal Huntington disease: Implications for clinical trials. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 84, 1233–1239. Peinemann, A., Schuller, S., Pohl, C., Jahn, T., Weindl, A., & Kassubek, J. (2005). Executive dysfunction in early stages of Huntington’s disease is associated with striatal and insular atrophy: A neuropsychological and voxel-based morphometric study. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 239,11–19. Philpott, A. L., Andrews, S. C., Staios, M., & Churchyard, A. (2016). Emotion evaluation and social inference impairments in Huntington’s disease. Journal of Huntington’s Disease, 5, 175–183. Pillon, B., Dubois, B., Ploska, A., & Agid, Y. (1991). Severity and specificity of cognitive impairment in Alzheimer’s, Huntington’s and Parkinson’s disease and progressive supranuclear palsy. Neurology, 41, 634–643. Pillon, B., Deweer, B., Agid, Y., & Dubois, B. (1993). Explicit memory in Alzheimer’s, Huntington’s and Parkinson’s diseases. Archives of Neurology, 50,

374–379. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/acn/article/32/7/876/4161107 by guest on 27 September 2021 Podoll, K., Caspary, P., Lange, H. W., & Noth, J. (1988). Language functions in Huntington’s disease. Brain, 111, 1475–1503. Quarrell, O. W. J. (2014). Juvenile Huntington’s disease. In Bates G. P., Tabrizi S. J., & Jones L. (Eds.), Huntington’s disease (4th ed., pp. 66–85). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Read, J., Jones, R., Owen, G., Leavitt, B. R., Coleman, A., Roos, R. A. C., et al. (2013). Quality of Life in Huntington’s disease: A comparative study investi- gating the impact for those with pre-manifest and early manifest disease and their partners. Journal of Huntington’s Disease, 2, 159–175. Ready, R. E., Mathews, M., Leserman, A., & Paulsen, J. S. (2008). Patient and caregiver quality of life in Huntington’s disease. Movement Disorders, 23, 721–726. Reedeker, N., Bouwens, J. A., Giltay, E. J., Le Mair, S. E., Roos, R. A. C., van der Mast, R. C., et al. (2012). Irritability in Huntington’s disease. Psychiatry Research, 200, 813–818. Rickards, H., De Souza, J., van Walsem, M., van Duijn, E., Simpson, S. A., Squitieri, F., et al. (2011). Factor analysis of behavioural symptoms in Huntington’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 82, 411–412. Rohrer, D., Salmon, D. P., Wixted, J. T., & Paulsen, J. S. (1999). The disparate effects of Alzheimer’s disease and Huntington’s disease on semantic memory. Neuropsychology, 13, 381–388. Roos, R. A. C. (2014). Clinical neurology. In Bates G. P., Tabrizi S. J., & Jones L. (Eds.), Huntington’s disease (4th ed., pp. 25–35). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Rosser, A. E., & Hodges, J. R. (1994). Initial letter and semantic category fluency in Alzheimer’s disease, Huntington’s disease and progressive supranuclear palsy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 57, 1389–1394. Rothlind, J. C., Bylsma, F. W., Peyser, C., Folstein, S. E., & Brandt, J. (1993). Cognitive and motor correlates of everyday functioning in early Huntington’s disease. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 181, 194–199. Salmon, D., & Butters, N. (1995). Neurobiology of skill and habit learning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 5, 184–190. Simpson, J. A., Lovecky, D., Kogan, J., Vetter, L., & Yohrling, G. J. (2016). Survey of the Huntington’s disease patient and caregiver community reveals most impactful symptoms and treatment needs. Journal of Huntington’s Disease, 5, 395–403. Sitek, E. J., Sołtan, W., Wieczorek, D., Schinwelski, M., Robowski, P., Reilmann, R., et al. (2011). Self-awareness of motor dysfunction in patients with Huntington’s disease in comparison to Parkinson’s disease and cervical dystonia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 788–795. Sitek, E. J., Thompson, J. C., Craufurd, D., & Snowden, J. S. (2014). Unawareness of deficits in Huntington’s disease. Journal of Huntingtons Disease, 3, 125–135. Smith, M. M., Mills, J. A., Epping, E. A., Westervelt, H. J., & Paulsen, J. S. (2012). Depressive symptom severity is related to poorer cognitive performance in prodromal Huntington disease. Neuropsychology, 26, 664–669. Snowden, J. S., Craufurd, D., Griffiths, H. L., & Neary, D. (1998). Awareness of involuntary movements in Huntington disease. Archives of Neurology, 55, 801–805. Snowden, J. S., Craufurd, D., Griffiths, H. L., & Thompson, J. (2001). Longitudinal evaluation of cognitive disorder in Huntington’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7,33–44. Snowden, J. S., Craufurd, D., Thompson, J., & Neary, D. (2002). Psychomotor, executive and memory function in preclinical Huntington’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 133–145. Snowden, J. S., Austin, N. A., Sembi, S., Thompson, J. C., Craufurd, D., & Neary, D. (2008). Emotion recognition in Huntington’s disease and frontotempor- al dementia. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2638–2649. Snowden, J. S., Gibbons, Z. C., Blackshaw, A., Doubleday, E., Thompson, J., Craufurd, D., et al. (2003). Social cognition in frontotemporal dementia and Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 41, 688–701. Sprengelmeyer, R., Lange, H., & Homberg, V. (1995). The pattern of attentional deficits in Huntington’s disease. Brain, 118, 145–152. Sprengelmeyer, R., Young, A. W., Calder, A. J., Karnat, A., Lange, H., Homberg, V., et al. (1996). Loss of disgust. Perception of faces and emotions in Huntington’s disease. Brain, 119, 1647–1665. Sprengelmeyer, R., Young, A. W., Sprengelmeyer, A., Calder, A. J., Rowland, D., Perrett, D., et al. (1997). Recognition of facial expression: Selective impairment of specific emotions in Huntington’s disease. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 839–879. Sprengelmeyer, R., Young, A. W., Baldas, E.-M., Ratheiser, I., Sutherland, C. A. M., Muller, H.-P., et al. (2016). The neuropsychology of first impressions: Evidence from Huntington’s disease. Cortex, 85, 100–115. Starkstein, S. E., Brandt, J., Bylsma, F., Peyser, C., Folstein, M., & Folstein, S. E. (1992). Neuropsychological correlates of brain atrophy in Huntington’s disease: A magnetic resonance imaging study. , 34, 487–489. Stout, J. C., Jones, R., Labuschagne, I., O’Regan, A. M., Say, M. J., Dumas, E. M., et al. (2012). Evaluation of longitudinal 12 and 24 month cognitive outcomes in premanifest and early Huntington’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 83, 687–694. J.S. Snowden / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 32 (2017); 876–887 887

Stout, J. C., Paulsen, J. S., Queller, S., Solomon, A. C., Whitlock, K. B., Campbell, J. C., et al. (2011). Neurocognitive signs in prodromal Huntington dis- ease. Neuropsychology, 25 (1), 1–14. Tabrizi, S. J., Langbehn, D. R., Leavitt, B. R., Roos, R. A. C., Durr, A., Craufurd, F., et al. (2009). Biological and clinical manifestations of Huntington’s dis- ease in the longitudinal TRACK-HD study: Cross-sectional analysis of baseline data. The Lancet Neurology, 8, 791–801. Tabrizi, S. J., Reilmann, R., Roos, R. A., Durr, A., Leavitt, B., Owen, G., et al. (2012). Potential endpoints for clinical trials in premanifest and early Huntington’s disease in the TRACK-HD study: Analysis of 24 month observational data. The Lancet Neurology, 11,42–53. Tabrizi, S. J., Scahill, R. I., Owen, G., Durr, A., Leavitt, B. R., Roos, R., et al. (2013). Predictors of phenotypic progression and disease onset in premanifest and early-stage Huntington’s disease in the TRACK-HD study: Analysis of 36-month observational data. The Lancet Neurology, 12, 637–649. Thompson, J. C., Harris, J., Sollom, A. C., Stopford, C. L., Howard, E., Snowden, J. S., et al. (2012). Longitudinal evaluation of neuropsychiatric symptoms in Huntington’s disease. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 24,53–60. Thompson, J. C., Snowden, J. S., Craufurd, D., & Neary, D. (2002). Behavior in Huntington’s disease: Dissociating cognition-based and mood-based changes. Journal of Neuropsychiatry & Clinical Neuroscience, 14,37–43. Thompson, J. C., Poliakoff, E., Sollom, A. C., Howard, E., Craufurd, D., O’Boyle, D. J., et al. (2010). Automaticity and attention in Huntington’s disease:

When two hands are not better than one. Neuropsychologia, 48, 171–178. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/acn/article/32/7/876/4161107 by guest on 27 September 2021 Trinkler, I., Cleret de Langavant, L., & Bachoud-Lévi, A. C. (2013). Joint recognition-expression impairment of facial emotions in Huntington’s disease despite intact understanding of feelings. Cortex, 49, 549–558. Unschuld, P. G., Liu, X., Shanahan, M., Margolis, R. L., Bassett, S. S., Brandt, J., et al. (2013). Prefrontal executive function associated coupling relates to Huntington’s disease stage. Cortex, 49, 2661–2673. Van Dijk, J. G., van der Velde, E. A., Roos, R. A. C., & Bruyn, G. W. (1986). Juvenile Huntington disease. Human Genetics, 75, 235–239. Van Duijn, E. (2010). Treatment of irritability in Huntington’s disease. Current Treatment Options in Neurology, 12, 424–433. Van Duijn, E., Kingma, E. M., Timman, R., Zitman, F. G., Tibben, A., Roos, R. A. C., et al. (2008). Cross-sectional study on prevalences of psychiatric dis- orders in mutation carriers of Huntington’s disease compared with mutation-negative first-degree relatives. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69, 1804–1810. Van Walsem, M. R., Howe, E. I., Frich, J. C., & Andelic, N. (2016). Assistive technology for cognition and health-related quality of life in Huntington’s dis- ease. Journal of Huntington’s Disease, 5, 261–270. Vitale, C., Pellecchia, M. T., Grossi, D., Fragassi, N., Cuomo, T., Di Maio, L., et al. (2001). Unawareness of dyskinesias in Parkinson’s and Huntington’s dis- eases. Neurological Science, 22, 105–106. Vonsattel, J. P., & DiFiglia, M. (1998). Huntington disease. Journal of and Experimental Neurology, 57, 369–384. Vonsattel, J. P., Myers, R. H., Stevens, T. J., Ferrante, R. J., Bird, E. D., & Richardson, E. P. (1985). Neuropathological classification of Huntington’s disease. Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology, 44, 559–577. Watkins, L. H., Rogers, R. D., Lawrence, A. D., Sahakian, B. J., Rosser, A. E., & Robbins, T. W. (2000). Impaired planning but intact decision making in early Huntington’s disease: Implications for specific fronto-striatal pathology. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1112–1125. Weinberger, D. R., Berman, K. F., Iadarola, M., Driesen, N., & Zec, R. F. (1988). Prefrontal cortical blood flow and cognitive function in Huntington’s dis- ease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 51,94–104. Willingham, D. B., & Koroshetz, W. J. (1993). Evidence of dissociable motor skills in Huntington’s disease patients. Psychobiology, 21, 173–182. Willingham, D. B., Koroshetz, W. J., & Peterson, E. W. (1996). Motor skills have diverse neural bases: Spared and impaired skill acquisition in Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychology, 10, 315–321. Zhang, Y., Long, J. D., Mills, J. A., Warner, J. H., Lu, W., & Paulsen, J. S. (2011). Indexing disease progression at study entry with individuals at-risk for Huntington disease. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 156, 751–763.