<<

CHAPTER 3 The Functions of and the Effects of E.H. During*, **, M. Kawai** *Division of , Stanford University, Redwood City, CA, USA **The Stanford Center for Sleep Sciences and Medicine, Redwood City, CA, USA

OUTLINE

Introduction 55 Cognitive Performance and the Effects of Sleep Deprivation 60 Sleep and Stress 57 Regulation of Appetite 58 Accidents and Safety in Acute and Chronic Sleep Deprivation 63 GH, PRL, TSH, GnRH, and Testosterone 58 Memory 63 Mood, Emotional Stability Insight and Creativity, Gist and Reward Circuits 58 Memory 65 Emotional Maturation 59 Immune Function 65 Dreaming 59 Conclusions 66 Regulation of Pain 60 References 66

INTRODUCTION

As , we spend approximately one third of our lives sleeping. Sleep is not only critical for physiological homeostasis and recovery also several essential metabolic, psychological, cognitive and immune functions. Sleep deprivation is a relative concept be- cause it compares actual with ideal sleep duration, which is not universally agreed upon, and may vary from person to person. Expert consensus from the American Academy of

Sleep and Neurologic Disease. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-804074-4.00003-0 Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 55 56 3. The Functions of Sleep and the Effects of Sleep Deprivation

Sleep Medicine has recently estimated that a minimum of 7 h of sleep is needed on aver- age to support optimal health in adults.1 The National Sleep Foundation has also published sleep duration recommendations for each age group.2 According to these recommendations, adults require 7–9 h of sleep, whereas teenagers require 8–10 h, and children ages 6–13 re- quire 9–11 h. Although 7 h may be sufficient for most adults, it may not be sufficient for other age groups, especially younger populations. Despite these recommendations, a recent report from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention estimates that over one third of the US population sleeps less than 7 h.3 Unfortunately, sleep is generally perceived by our society as a nonproductive state. This creates a double-bind situation in which the individual often opts to increase wake time over sleep, which paradoxically leads to reduced performance. Although many studies have demonstrated the adverse consequences of insufficient sleep, many individuals remain strikingly unaware of them. To understand sleep deprivation, it is necessary to first measure actual sleep duration. This can be accomplished in several ways. One simple method is to have the patient keep a sleep diary (Fig. 3.1), though this method involves some level of subjective interpretation and

FIGURE 3.1 An example of a 2-week sleep diary (American Academy of Sleep Medicine).

 Sleep and Stress 57

FIGURE 3.2 A typical actigraph recording for six consecutive days and nights. Arrowheads indicate the begin- ning and end of time in . The time with reduced activity is considered as a rough estimate of sleep time.4 recall, which may be impaired if the patient is sleep deprived. Actigraphy provides a more objective measurement by using an accelerometer to measure body movement (Fig. 3.2), and thus estimates sleep time and sleep efficiency. In clinical practice, however, measuring precise sleep duration is not always necessary, and self-reported sleep duration is often sufficient. Exceptions do exist, including patients with sleep-state misperception, shift workers, and professional drivers who may have a motive for misreporting.

SLEEP AND STRESS

Wakefulness is a state of high metabolic demand, due to the frequent need to adapt and respond to sensory inputs. In contrast, sleep is a state of energy conservation and recovery that contributes to the homeostasis of many physiological and psychological functions. This net economy in energy expenditure is more pronounced in nonrapid eye movement (NREM) sleep, as demonstrated by decreased cortical (prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, precuneus) and subcortical (thalami, basal ganglia and basal forebrain) glucose metabolism in regions that are usually hypermetabolic.5 This reduced metabolic state reaches its lowest point in slow wave sleep (SWS), showing a 40% global decrement.6 Sleep deprivation, in turn, results in hyperactivation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, as evidenced by the fact that adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH), corticotro- pin-releasing hormone (CRH) and glucocorticoid levels all increase after sleep deprivation. Sleep restriction to 4 h for 6 consecutive nights in young men results in increased levels of cortisol in the afternoon and early evening, as well as impaired glucose tolerance, a response similar to the response seen in those with noninsulin dependent diabetes.7 Stress and the metabolic consequences of sleep deprivation may increase glucocorticoid se- cretion, and it is often difficult to separate the two. This distinction is complicated by the fact that the sleep restriction of research protocols and in vivo ecological situations that can lead to sleep loss may both constitute a stress. Furthermore, insufficient sleep and stress have recipro- cal consequences, which may explain why chronic short sleepers have higher levels of cortisol when compared to chronic long sleepers.8 There is some data to suggest that this bidirection- al relationship may be regulated by hypocretin/orexin, and hypocretinergic projections are found in a broad network responsive to CRH, including the locus coeruleus, paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, and central amygdala.9

 58 3. The Functions of Sleep and the Effects of Sleep Deprivation REGULATION OF APPETITE

With the exception of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, which represents a relative hyper- metabolic state, sleep is a period of fasting and energy conservation.10 Leptin is a hormone that reflects the physiological state of satiety released by adipocytes in response to food in- take,11 while ghrelin is an appetite stimulant hormone. Sleep restriction results in decreased leptin12 and increased ghrelin serum levels,13,14 both of which result in increased food intake, with a predilection for energy-rich foods with high carbohydrate content.14 Chronic disruption of regular sleep patterns in shift workers is associated with higher body mass indices (BMI),15 perhaps due to the combined impairment of the HPA axis and appetite stimulation. A metaanalysis across all age groups demonstrated an inverse relation- ship between hours of sleep and BMI, with each hour of sleep reduction corresponding to a 0.35 kg/m2 increase in BMI.16 Increased levels of TNF-α have been associated with insulin resistance,17 linking sleep loss, insulin resistance and weight gain. Vgontzas et al.18 showed increased TNF-α levels after 1 week of mild sleep deprivation in male participants. Adiponectin has anti TNF-α properties and, similar to leptin, enhances insulin sensitivity.19 In addition, shorter sleep duration is as- sociated with decreased levels of adiponectine.20

GH, PRL, TSH, GnRH, AND TESTOSTERONE

The growth hormone (GH) axis is closely linked to SWS. GH plasma levels peak within minutes during each SWS period and remain significantly elevated during SWS through- out the night. Furthermore, pharmacological increases in SWS with ritanserin21 or gamma- hydroxybutyric acid (GHB)22 are associated with an increase in GH plasma levels. Prolactin (PRL) secretion is subject to inhibitory dopaminergic tone, which is higher during wakeful- ness. Shortly after , PRL levels increase and reach a twofold elevation during sleep, as compared to trough levels during wakefulness.23 Sleep exerts an inhibitory effect on thyroid-stimulating hormone (TSH) secretion,24 however, TSH secretion is more closely linked to the circadian rhythm than to sleep. TSH peaks in the evening and early night and reaches its trough in the afternoon. Nocturnal elevation of testosterone levels is temporally linked to the latency of the first REM period25 and is also observed during daytime .26 Conversely, experimental fragmentation of sleep, particularly REM sleep, leads to decreased testosterone secretion.27 In women, sleep has been associated with inhibition of the pulsatile gonadotropin-releasing hormone (GnRH) release in early follicular and early luteal phases.28

MOOD, EMOTIONAL STABILITY AND REWARD CIRCUITS

Sleep serves an important role in mood and emotional homeostasis. Although total acute sleep deprivation has shown to have a transient but robust antidepressive effect, likely via elevation of brain-derived neurotrophic factor BDNF29 and serotonin, taurine and tryptophan levels,30 prolonged sleep deprivation in turn has deleterious effects on depression and mood stability. Subjective perception of mood31 and severity of unipolar depression32 are signifi- cantly affected by even minimal misalignment between circadian and sleep–wake cycle.

 Dreaming 59

Sleep is also involved in the processing and modulation of emotional stimuli. One experi- ment required two groups of participants to rate the same set of photographs showing facial expressions of positive (happiness) and negative (fear, sadness, anger) emotions during two sessions, 5 h apart, with or without an intervening . In participants who napped, results showed lower ratings of negative fear and anger, and higher ratings of happiness between sessions. In addition, only those individuals who achieved REM sleep during their nap dis- played this modulation of affective reactivity.33 A study recorded skin conductance response (sweating) and corrugator supercilii muscle tone via electromyography (frowning reaction) to measure unconscious reactivity to negative emotional pictures. Results showed decreased responses (habituation) when retested 2.5 h later in the group allowed to nap, compared to the group requested to stay awake.34 Function MRI (fMRI) studies have demonstrated that the neurophysiological correlates of sleep-dependent emotional brain homeostasis may involve enhanced activity in the ventro- medial PFC, coupled with decreased amygdala activation.35 Reciprocally, sleep deprivation is associated with heightened amygdala activation in response to negative stimuli36 and exagger- ated appreciation of pleasure-evoking stimuli, correlating with increased activity in reward, and emotion-related circuits.37 Both findings are associated with decreased coupling on fMRI in medial and orbitofrontal cortices, areas known to be involved in behavioral inhibitory con- trol and appreciation of rewarding experiences.38 Sleep deprivation generally elevates expecta- tions of higher reward on gambling task, as nucleus accumbens activation increased with risky choices, and it attenuates neural responses to losses in the insular and orbitofrontal cortices.39 Taken together, these findings may explain how sleep-deprived individuals show higher risk- taking in reward-related situations with decreased insight into their negative consequences.40

EMOTIONAL MATURATION

Smiles during sleep in neonates consistently emerge before smiles during wakefulness, usually observed around 2 months of age. These spontaneous “Duchenne smiles”41 and other emotional expressions are associated with the active sleep of neonates, the equivalent of adult REM sleep, and can be seen as early as 13th week of gestation.42 These facial expressions of emotions during sleep have been hypothesized to foster complex motor programs, thereby facilitating the development of the emotional and social smile.43

DREAMING

Although activity and REM sleep have been closely associated, occur in all stages of sleep, as evidenced by persistent reports of dreams after reduction or suppression of REM sleep.44 The quality of dreams, however, differs between REM and NREM sleep. Dreams arising from NREM sleep are usually less emotionally charged, less vivid, and less complex when compared to dreams associated with REM sleep. With the exception of frightening images often reported in NREM , NREM mentations usually consist of images, events or situations of low emotional valence and do not have the narrative complexity of dreams arising from REM sleep.45

 60 3. The Functions of Sleep and the Effects of Sleep Deprivation

In each civilization, symbols and meanings have been associated with dream contents and have given rise to a wide body of explanatory models, from animistic, spiritual, and religious belief systems to psychoanalytical and neurocognitive theories. In the early 20th century, Sigmund Freud and Carl Jung put forth two psychoanalytical frameworks that have since become major theories of dreaming in our modern society. While Freud hypothesized that dreams are a window into our individual unconscious psyche,46 Jung emphasized their relation with universal archetypes that transcend individuals, culture, and beliefs.47 An al- ternative explanatory theory suggests that experiences in dreams could provide an offline environment for simulating situations similar to the real perceptual world. According to the threat simulation theory, dreams often contain challenging and novel situations (e.g., missing an important appointment, arriving late or forgetting to study for an exam, losing money or valuables, etc.) whose purpose might be to rehearse and improve our responses in similar waking conditions.48 This could explain the abundance of fear-related experiences in dreams, in some respect comparable to exposure therapy.48 Integrating neuroimaging, neurophysiological, and clinical evidence, the reward activa- tion model proposed by Perogamvros and Schwartz49 places the mesolimbic dopaminergic pathway at the center of a circuit involved in: (1) the generation of REM via activation of the sublaterodorsal nucleus of the pons; (2) dream contents; and (3) the offline activation and con- solidation of memories of high emotional or motivational values. Sex is a prominent theme in dreams. The prominent activity of the dopaminergic reward circuits in REM sleep described previously provides a neurophysiological explanation of the universality of sex- related dreams. On a psychodynamic level, Freud viewed our dreams as a free expression of unconscious wishes. On a neurophysiological level, sexual mentation in REM sleep may be facilitated by the specific dynamics of testosterone secretion during sleep. As described pre- viously, hormonal levels are coupled with the first nocturnal REM period, sustained during sleep,25 and also promoted during daytime naps.26 Beyond the interconnection between sexual dreams and testosterone levels, sleep has a potent effect on the activity of the neuroendocrine reproductive system and is ultimately implicated in multiple aspects of sexual satisfaction.50

REGULATION OF PAIN

Sleep may also be involved in the regulation of pain via involvement of the anterior cin- gulate cortex and limbic pathways, which are highly connected with the emotional neural network. Four studies have shown that sleep duration below 6 h is associated with increased subjective51–53 and objective54 measures of pain.

COGNITIVE PERFORMANCE AND THE EFFECTS OF SLEEP DEPRIVATION

A large body of neuropsychological literature has applied the paradigm of sleep depriva- tion to evaluate the neurocognitive functions of sleep. These studies have some limitations, namely that sleep deprivation itself can result in a stress response with multiple metabol- ic, neuroendocrine, behavioral and cognitive consequences. It is also difficult to delineate

 Cognitive Performance and the Effects of Sleep Deprivation 61 single-function deficits due to the fact that sleep deprivation affects multiple cognitive do- mains and behavioral aspects such as mood and affect, which determine the level of engage- ment for a given task. Other potential confounding factors are intersubject variability (het- erogeneity of susceptibility to sleep loss between individuals) and intrasubject variability (improvement of performance on a task can reflect learning effect).55 The psychomotor vigilance task (PVT) measures speed of responses to visual stimuli via a thumb-operated device and is not subject to learning effect. In the PVT, sleepiness manifests as or instability of the wake state. These translate to lapses of response (error of omission) or erroneous responses (error of commission), especially when the person requires significant effort in staying awake.56 In other words, this represents a balance between the homeostatic drive to fall asleep and a resistance from wake-promoting mechanisms.57 Re- gardless of the task, sleep deprivation generally results in a progressive degradation of the performance during extended tasks (fatigue effect) exacerbated by sleep loss.58,59 While tasks involving higher cognitive functions have been relatively less sensitive to sleep loss, possibly due to a higher level of engagement and compensatory effort,40 a large body of literature describes the consequences of sleep deprivation on sustained attention and other executive functions, procedural and episodic memory formation, and consolidation, as well as insight and creativity. In an experiment studying reaction time over 7 days of variable degrees of sleep restric- tion, sleep deprivation was shown to correlate with longer reaction time on PVT testing. Lon- ger reaction times were noted after only one night of sleep restriction (2 h of sleep reduction) compared to volunteers sleeping on average 8.5 h.60 Performance continued to diminish with each subsequent day and was directly correlated with the degree of sleep restriction. In the groups allowed to sleep for a maximum of 7 and 5 h, vigilance deficit stabilized after 5 nights, whereas in participants sleeping 3 h or less, vigilance continued to deteriorate in a linear fashion showing a twofold decrease compared to the control group at the end of the sleep restriction phase. After three nights with 8 h spent in bed, none of the sleep restriction groups returned to their baseline reaction times, suggesting that recovery from even mild sleep de- privation may last several days. This experiment also suggests that there may be an adaptive mechanism to mild and moderate chronic sleep deprivation (7 and 5 h groups) that may be insufficient to maintain performance levels in situations of severe chronic sleep deprivation (3 h group). Other studies have confirmed the finding that impairment in PVT testing increases in proportion to the degree of sleep deprivation (Fig. 3.3).61 Sleep deprivation is known to decrease insight into one’s own performance,40,62 and some studies suggest that in conditions of total sleep deprivation, wake promoting agents could be associated with decreased self-monitoring,63 possibly due to lack of compensatory effort when compared to placebo.64 However, some evidence suggests that subjective sleepiness does not correlate well with the magnitude of cognitive impairment. In an experiment con- ducted by Van Dongen et al.,61 PVT, working memory, cognitive throughput measured by the digit symbol substitution task65 and serial addition/subtraction tasks (SAST),66 demon- strated a comparable degree of impairment in conditions of acute total and mild chronic sleep deprivation (sleep restriction to 6 h over 14 days), supporting the concept of a “cumulative ” with regard to executive functions.61 After 14 days, although cognitive perfor- mance reached their lowest levels, participants in the 4 h and 6 h groups reported feeling only slightly sleepy on the Stanford sleepiness scale, suggesting that individuals subjected to

 62 3. The Functions of Sleep and the Effects of Sleep Deprivation

FIGURE 3.3 (A) PVT lapses and (B) sleepiness score after acute sleep deprivation and chronic sleep deprivation. Each panel displays group averages for subjects in the 8 h (◊), 6 h (h), and 4 h (○) chronic sleep period conditions across 14 days, and in the 0 h () sleep condition across 3 days.61 BL, Baseline chronic sleep deprivation could not reliably predict the degree of cognitive impairment on sleepiness alone. Based on the neurocognitive performances gathered across groups for the duration of the experiment, the authors estimated that the minimal 24 h requirement to attain a stable cognitive equilibrium is 3.8 h of continuous sleep. Below this critical threshold, the model diverges from a stable equilibrium and performance continues to degrade as seen in acute total sleep deprivation. This study demonstrates how relative resilience to sleepiness and stabilization of cognitive performance at a shifted new baseline may provide a false sense of coping with sleep deprivation. Recently, functional neuroimaging techniques, including fMRI and positron emission to- mography (PET), have been utilized to better understand the localization of cognitive dys- function in cases of acute sleep deprivation.67 Those studies investigate either increase or decrease of blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) measurements in fMRI or oxygen/glucose consumption in PET. The prefrontal cortex has been reported to be extremely vulnerable, showing decreased function in acute sleep deprivation.68 fMRI studies have demonstrated reduced activity in the prefrontal cortex after sleep deprivation in a serial subtraction task.69 However, in a verbal learning task, the prefrontal cortex and parietal cortex were more acti- vated after acute sleep deprivation.70 Interestingly, activation of the prefrontal cortex was posi- tively correlated with the degree of subjective sleepiness, whereas activation in the parietal cortex was positively associated with the preservation of near-normal verbal learning. These patterns of increase and decrease in cerebral activation most likely represent compensatory adaptations (CA). In CA, more activation in the prefrontal cortex with acute sleep deprivation indicates a compensation process in response to the increased homeostatic drive for sleep. The prefrontal cortex is involved in working memory, attention and executive function,71 functions known to be vulnerable to sleep deprivation. On the other hand, activation of the parietal lobe with sleep deprivation indicates an adaptive process to support the decreased function of other areas of the cortex. These patterns of CA have task-specific differences69,72 and indicate that sleep deprivation can result in a combination of dysfunction and compensatory hyperfunction in the brain. Level of difficulty modulate the cerebral response,73 and interindividual differences can con- tribute to the difficulty of using neuroimaging studies as a generalized parameter of sleepiness.

 Memory 63

It has been demonstrated in fMRI studies that activation of the frontoparietal region was more robustin participants found to be less vulnerable to the effects of sleep deprivation.74 This sug- gests that more activated CA may correlate with less vulnerability to sleep deprivation. As mentioned, several aspects of executive function are affected by sleep deprivation. Sleep deprivation results in difficulty determining the scope of a problem in cases of distracting in- formation and impairs divergent thinking and originality as measured on the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking.75,76 It also affects temporal memory, as shown by lower performance on tasks of recency (inability to recall the timing for recent events), even in conditions of preserved alertness with use of caffeine.40 In a working memory task under sleep deprivation, fMRI stud- ies demonstrated decreased activation in the parietal region, and increased activation in pre- frontal and thalamic regions in more complex tasks.77 This is another example of CA. Another function of sleep is to preserve flexible decision-making. Experiments show that acute and chronic sleep deprivation results in more rigid thinking and perseverative errors with poor appreciation of an updated situation despite intact critical reasoning,78,79 loss of focus to relevant cues,80,81 and increased risk taking,82 possibly due to reduced functioning of the ventro-medial PFC.39,83

ACCIDENTS AND SAFETY IN ACUTE AND CHRONIC SLEEP DEPRIVATION

Performance in lane variability of driving, or accuracy and reaction time after more than 16 h of sleep deprivation, has been reported to be similar to the performance in those with a blood alcohol content of 0.05–0.1%.84,85 However, when comparing factors of sleep deprivation and alcohol intoxication, subjects who were sleep deprived showed progressive deterioration through driving, whereas subjects who were intoxicated demonstrated stable impairment throughout the entire excercise.86 It has also been demonstrated that the circadian rhythm affects driving performance and risk of motor vehicle accidents. Connor et al.87 reported a fivefold higher incidence of motor vehicle accidents between 2 and 5 a.m. compared with other times of the day. Extending these findings, Lenné et al.,88 in a report on driving perfor- mance, especially speed control, showed deterioration in the early morning (2–5 a.m.) as well as between 2 and 5 p.m., corresponding to the mid-afternoon dip. These phenomena are more prominent in low stimulus situations, such as highway and rural driving.89 Sleep deprivation has been a major issue in the medical field. One of the most significant changes in modern medical training came about after the Libby Zion’s case, with the introduc- tion of the duty-hour regulations that helped to decrease the risk of medical errors by sleep- deprived physicians.90,91 One study demonstrated that the performance of sleep-deprived, postcall residents in vigilance, sustained attention and driving tasks is just as impaired as those with a blood alcohol concentration of 0.04–0.05%, the equivalent of 1–2 standard drinks.92

MEMORY

The successive stages of memory are encoding, consolidation/reconsolidation and retriev- al. Encoding and retrieval occur in the awake state, but adequate sleep improves all stages of memory formation, particularly consolidation and reconsolidation. Observation of improved

 64 3. The Functions of Sleep and the Effects of Sleep Deprivation encoding capacity following sleep has been demonstrated in numerous studies,36,93,94 and ac- counted for by the “synaptic homeostasis” hypothesis:95,96 for the brain to adapt to the wide- spread synaptic long-term potentiation (strengthening of postsynaptic nerve cells responses to stimulations across synapses) resulting from the incessant formation of new memories, sleep-dependent mechanisms may downscale synaptic strengths to levels that are energeti- cally sustainable for the brain. Among all stages of sleep, slow-wave activity (SWA, 0.5–4 Hz) may contribute the most to this process.97,98 Sleep plays an active role in memory formation because it could provide a favorable neuro- chemical environment with sleep-specific neurophysiological events such as SWS, sleep spin- dles and hippocampal ripples in NREM sleep, and hippocampal theta activity in REM sleep. According to the dual process hypothesis,99 NREM100–102 and REM103–106 sleep have differential involvement in declarative (episodic and semantic) and nondeclarative memory (procedural and emotional memory) consolidation, respectively. Although reports are conflicting in older adults,107,108 SWS in young adults is positively correlated with declarative memory consolidation.108 Experimental enhancement of SWA via transcranial application of a 0.75 Hz oscillating potential is associated with improved declarative memory for word-pairs109 in young adults when compared to a sham stimula- tion group, indicating the specific involvement of SWA in declarative memory consolidation. Reactivation of hippocampal memory during SWS has been observed in both animal and human studies.110,111 This process involves synchronization between the and cortex, ultimately leading to synaptic consolidation. Studies in rats demonstrate a hippo- campal chronological “replay” during SWS following an experience of environment explo- ration (spatial learning)112 that are associated with hippocampal “ripples” (100–200 Hz) and sharp wave activity.113,114 In addition, coupling of hippocampal sharp-wave/ripple complexes with neocortical delta (1–4 Hz) slow oscillation patterns and spindles were demonstrated in rats during SWS.115 This dialog between the hippocampus and neocortex, mediated by thalamocortical sleep spindles, could facilitate transfer from the hippocampus to neocortical long-term storage sites.116 The specific implication of sleep spindles in memory consolidation and reconsolidation is supported by studies demonstrating a positive correlation between memory performance and spindle density, independent of the sleep stage.117–119 REM sleep may also enhance nondeclarative (procedural, priming) memory103–106 and the emotional modulation of memory.120,121 involves the acquisition of motor skills. Most common experimental designs have used tasks of finger-sequence tapping, mir- ror tracing or other forms of visuomotor learning. Procedural memory involves a wide set of structures, including the motor cortex (supplementary motor area), basal ganglia (striatum), thalamus and cerebellum. The hippocampus122 is involved to a lesser extent but may be im- plicated in more complex motor learning tasks.123 While selective NREM sleep deprivation is difficult without totally depriving an individual from sleep, selective REM sleep depriva- tion studies have supported the view that REM sleep may be involved in the formation of procedural memory.124 This has been supported by the finding of decreased performance on a learned finger-sequence tapping task following REM-suppressed sleep via acetylcholine receptor blockade.125 On the other hand, procedural memory is not affected by REM- suppressant antidepressant medications in both depressed and nondepressed patients.126–128 In addition, several studies have demonstrated that NREM sleep events such as SWA129–131 and spindles132–134 are associated with learning of even simple motor tasks. Finally, similar to the

 Immune Function 65 reactivation studies conducted with procedural memory, two studies reexposing participants to parts of a learned melody during SWS resulted in performance benefit.135,136 Memories associated with high emotional content are also more strongly associated with REM sleep.120,121,137 Activation of limbic areas and reward circuits are observed during REM sleep and dreams, and the amygdala may be involved in regulating hippocampal memory- encoding processes.138 Memories of high emotional valence may benefit from heightened sta- bilization during sleep. In normal conditions, emotional memory traces may be reactivated and remodeled each night during REM sleep. This process, repeated over time, results in strengthening the nonaffective “core,” or facts of the memory and, concomitantly, weakening the affective “layer” initially associated with it.139 This model, condensed by Walker et al.139 in the formula “sleep to remember and sleep to forget,” is supported by neuroimaging evidence of decreased amygdala activity during the repeated viewing of emotional pictures after an interval filled with sleep.35 NREM sleep may also play a role in the consolidation of emotional memory as pharmacological augmentation of sleep spindles with GHB or zolpidem may im- prove recall of highly arousing and negative stimuli.140 Further evidence stems from the ob- servation of odor-induced reactivation of fear memories in NREM sleep both in animals141 and humans.142 Despite this, there is lack of general agreement that NREM sleep, including sleep spindles, may be implicated in both declarative and nondeclarative memory, and REM sleep may be more specifically associated with emotional memory.

INSIGHT AND CREATIVITY, GIST MEMORY

Processes occurring during sleep can extract meaning and result in new insight, as sug- gested by historical anecdotes such as the discovery of the chemical structure of benzene or the periodic table of elements, which were reportedly sleep-induced breakthroughs.143 Some have hypothesized that memory consolidation during sleep could facilitate access to general rules or shortcuts, potentially leading to the solution of problems encountered during the wake state. Wagner et al.144 reported that 3 times more participants accessed a hidden rule for a number reduction task after sleep. Some mechanisms during memory consolidation may also promote its integration into a metalevel of cognition. This is illustrated by experiments showing that participants tend to create “false” memories of gist words that are not present on a word list task prior to sleep, but which pertain to the list of associated words (e.g., for white coat, stethoscope, hospital, nurse, thermometer, the gist word might be doctor).145,146 In addition, REM sleep may have a lasting effect on the ability to solve complex anagrams, as demonstrated by a 32% advantage on this task in participants awakened from REM sleep.147 This effect could be related to the prolongation of a REM-related “hyperassociative state,” a state promoting novel thinking and creative problem-solving skills. Conversely, the use of creative thinking has been associated with increased REM sleep during the following sleep period.148

IMMUNE FUNCTION

Sleep and circadian rhythm have a notable influence on the regulation of the immune sys- tem. Differentiated immune cells with immediate effector functions (natural killer cells and cy- totoxic T lymphocytes) peak during the wake period, while naive and central memory T cells,

 66 3. The Functions of Sleep and the Effects of Sleep Deprivation involved in the slowly evolving adaptive immune response, peak during sleep.149 SWS-related GH and prolactin (PRL) secretion and decreased antiinflammatory catecholamine, and corti- sol levels provide a favorable endocrine milieu for antigen presenting T cell interactions, with a shift toward T helper cell (Th) 1 cytokines, increase in Th cell proliferation, and migration of naive T cells to lymph nodes.149 Sleep restriction to 4 h results in increased plasma levels of in- terleukin 6 (IL-6)150 and C-reactive protein (CRP)151 within 10 days, and even mild sleep restric- tion to 6 h over 8 days leads to elevated levels of inflammatory cytokines.18 Elevations have been shown to be mild but consistent, resulting in a state of low-grade systemic inflammation within ranges increasing cardiovascular risk.152,153 Chronic sleep loss results in an increased risk of viral upper respiratory infections154 and a higher rate of infections in shift workers.155 Reports of decreased immune response to influenza virus vaccination in individuals suffering from chronic sleep loss156 further supports the view that, although associated with elevated markers of inflammation, sleep loss results in a functional state of immunodeficiency. There is little and conflicting data regarding quantity of sleep and cancer risk, and no evi- dence of an association with neurological tumors. Kakizaki et al.157 found an increased risk of breast cancer (hazard ratio = 1.62) associated with 6 h (compared to 7 h) of sleep; however, several large prospective cohort and case-control studies have failed to replicate this associa- tion. One prospective cohort study found sleep of less than 7 h to be associated with reduced ovarian cancer risk.158 Less than 6 h of sleep may be associated with colorectal adenoma,159 and both extreme short (≤5 h) and long (≥9 h) sleep durations may be associated with in- creased risk of colorectal cancer.160

CONCLUSIONS

In addition to its critical function in endocrine, metabolic, and immune regulation, sleep appears to play an active and important role in maintaining health, emotional well-being and neurocognitive performance during wakefulness. Lifestyle changes have been emphasized in recent years to prevent a wide range of diseases and promote health in general. We hope that in the future a similar effort will be made to stress the importance of sleep-related lifestyle changes that can improve both sleep quality and sleep quantity, not only for our patients but also for society at large.

References 1. Watson NF, Badr MS, Belenky G. Joint consensus statement of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine and Sleep Research Society on the recommended amount of sleep for a healthy adult: methodology and discussion. J Clin Sleep Med. 2015;11(8):931–952. 2. Hirshkowitz M, Whiton K, Albert SM. National Sleep Foundation’s updated sleep duration recommendations: final report. Sleep Heal. 2015;1(4):233–243. 3. Liu Y, Wheaton AG, Chapman DP, Cunningham TJ, Croft JB. Prevalence of healthy sleep duration among adults—United States, 2014. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2016;65(6):137–141. 4. Suratt PM, Barth JT, Diamond R. Reduced time in bed and obstructive sleep-disordered breathing in children are associated with cognitive impairment. Pediatrics. 2007;119(2):320–329. 5. Dang-Vu TT, Schabus M, Desseilles M, Sterpenich V, Bonjean M, Maquet P. Functional neuroimaging insights into the physiology of human sleep. Sleep. 2010;33(12):1589–1603. www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender. fcgi?artid=2982729&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract..

 REFERENCES 67

6. Maquet P. Functional neuroimaging of normal human sleep by positron emission tomography. J Sleep Res. 2000;9(3):207–231. 7. Spiegel K, Leproult R, Van Cauter E. Impact of sleep debt on metabolic and endocrine function. Lancet. 1999;354(9188):1435–1439. 8. Späth-Schwalbe E, Schöller T, Kern W, Fehm HL, Born J. Nocturnal adrenocorticotropin and cortisol secretion depends on sleep duration and decreases in association with spontaneous awakening in the morning. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1992;75(6):1431–1435. 9. Winsky-Sommerer R. Interaction between the corticotropin-releasing factor system and hypocretins (orexins): a novel circuit mediating stress response. J Neurosci. 2004;24(50):11439–11448. 10. Maquet P. Sleep function(s) and cerebral metabolism. Behav Brain Res. 1995;69(1–2):75–83. 11. Simon C, Gronfier C, Schlienger JL, Brandenberger G. Circadian and ultradian variations of leptin in normal man under continuous enteral nutrition: relationship to sleep and body temperature. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1998;83(6):1893–1899. 12. Mullington JM, Chan JL, Van Dongen HPA. Sleep loss reduces diurnal rhythm amplitude of leptin in healthy men. J Neuroendocrinol. 2003;15(9):851–854. 13. Taheri S, Lin L, Austin D, Young T, Mignot E. Short sleep duration is associated with reduced leptin, elevated ghrelin, and increased body mass index. PLoS Med. 2004;1(3):210–217. 14. Spiegel K, Tasali E, Penev P, Van Cauter E. Brief communication: Sleep curtailment in healthy young men is associated with decreased leptin levels, elevated ghrelin levels, and increased hunger and appetite. Ann Intern Med. 2004;141(11):846–850. 15. van Drongelen A, Boot CRL, Merkus SL, Smid T, van der Beek AJ. The effects of shift work on body weight change—a systematic review of longitudinal studies. Scand J Work Environ Heal. 2011;37(4):263–275. 16. Cappuccio FP, Taggart FM, Kandala NB. Meta-analysis of short sleep duration and obesity in children and adults. Sleep. 2008;31(5):619–626. 17. Hotamisligil GS. Inflammatory pathways and insulin action. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2003;27(suppl 3): S53–S55. 18. Vgontzas AN, Zoumakis E, Bixler EO. Adverse effects of modest sleep restriction on sleepiness, performance, and inflammatory cytokines.J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2004;89(5):2119–2126. 19. Guerre-Millo M. Adiponect: an, update. Diabetes Metab. 2008;34(1):12–18. 20. Kotani K, Sakane N, Saiga K. Serum adiponectin levels and lifestyle factors in Japanese men. Hear Vessel. 2007;22(5):291–296. 21. Gronfier C, Luthringer R, Follenius M. A quantitative evaluation of the relationships between growth hormone secretion and electroencephalographic activity during normal sleep and after enrichment in delta waves. Sleep. 1996;19(10):817–824. 22. Van Cauter E, Plat L, Scharf MB. Simultaneous stimulation of slow-wave sleep and growth hormone secretion by gamma-hydroxybutyrate in normal young men. J Clin Invest. 1997;100(3):745–753. 23. Van Cauter E, Aschoff J. Endocrine and other biological rhythms. In: DeGroot LJ, Jameson JL, eds. Endocrinology. Philadelphia, PA: Elseviers; 2006:341–372. 24. Brabant G, Prank K, Ranft U. Physiological regulation of circadian and pulsatile thyrotropin secretion in normal man and woman. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1990;70(2):403–409. 25. Luboshitzky R, Herer P, Levi M, Shen-Orr Z, Lavie P. Relationship between and testosterone secretion in normal men. J Androl. 1999;20(6):731–737. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10591612. 26. Axelsson J, Ingre M, Åkerstedt T, Holmbäck U. Effects of acutely displaced sleep on testosterone. J Clin Endocri- nol Metab. 2005;90(8):4530–4535. 27. Luboshitzky R, Zabari Z, Shen-Orr Z, Herer P, Lavie P. Disruption of the nocturnal testosterone rhythm by sleep fragmentation in normal men. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2001;86(3):1134–1139. 28. Filicori M, Santoro N, Merriam GR, Crowley WF. Characterization of the physiological pattern of episodic go- nadotropin secretion throughout the human menstrual cycle. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1986;62(6):1136–1144. 29. Schmitt K, Holsboer-Trachsler E, Eckert A. BDNF in sleep, , and sleep deprivation. Ann Med. 2016;3890:1–10. 30. Davies SK, Ang JE, Revell VL. Effect of sleep deprivation on the human metabolome. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014;111:10761–10766. 31. Danilenko KV, Cajochen C, Wirz-Justice A. Is sleep per se a zeitgeber in humans?. J Biol Rhythms. 2003;18(2):170–178.

 68 3. The Functions of Sleep and the Effects of Sleep Deprivation

32. Hasler BP, Buysse DJ, Kupfer DJ, Germain A. Phase relationships between core body temperature, melatonin, and sleep are associated with depression severity: further evidence for circadian misalignment in non-seasonal depression. Psychiatry Res. 2010;178(1):205–207. 33. Gujar N, McDonald SA, Nishida M, Walker MP. A role for REM sleep in recalibrating the sensitivity of the hu- man brain to specific emotions.Cereb Cortex. 2011;21(1):115–123. 34. Pace-Schott EF, Shepherd E, Spencer RMC. Napping promotes inter-session habituation to emotional stimuli. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2011;95(1):24–36. 35. Van Der Helm E, Yao J, Dutt S, Rao V, Saletin JM, Walker MP. REM sleep depotentiates amygdala activity to previous emotional experiences. Curr Biol. 2011;21(23):2029–2032. 36. Yoo S-S, Hu PT, Gujar N, Jolesz Fa, Walker MP. A deficit in the ability to form new human memories without sleep. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10(3):385–392. 37. Gujar N, Yoo S-S, Hu P, Walker MP. Sleep deprivation amplifies reactivity of brain reward, networks, biasing the appraisal of positive emotional experiences. J Neurosci. 2011;31(12):4466–4474. 38. Wallis JD. Orbitofrontal cortex and its contribution to decision-making. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2007;30:31–56. 39. Venkatraman V, Chuah YML, Huettel Sa, Chee MWL. Sleep deprivation elevates expectation of gains and at- tenuates response to losses following risky decisions. Sleep. 2007;30(5):603–609. 40. Harrison Y, Horne JA. The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making: a review. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2000;6(3):236–249. 41. Ekman P. An argument for basic emotions. Cogn Emot. 1992;6(3):169–200. 42. Messinger D, Dondi M, Nelson-Goens GC, Beghi A, Fogel AA, Simion F. How sleeping neonates smile. Dev Sci. 2002;5(1):48–54. 43. Dondi M, Messinger D, Colle M. A New perspective on neonatal smiling: differences between the judgments of expert coders and naive observers. Infancy. 2007;12(3):235–255. 44. Oudiette D, Dealberto MJ, Uguccioni G. Dreaming without REM sleep. Conscious Cogn. 2012;21(3):1129–1140. 45. Fosse R, Stickgold R, Hobson JA. Brain-mind states: reciprocal variation in thoughts and hallucinations. Psychol Sci. 2001;12(1):30–36. 46. Freud S. The Interpretation of Dreams. New York: The Macmillan Company; 1913. doi:10.1037/10561-004. 47. Jung CG. Memories, Dreams, Reflections. Jaffé A, ed. New York, NY: Vintage Books; 1989. 48. Valli K, Revonsuo A. The threat simulation theory in light of recent empirical evidence: a review. Am J Psychol. 2009;122(1):17–38. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19353929. 49. Perogamvros L, Schwartz S. The roles of the reward system in sleep and dreaming. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36(8):1934–1951. 50. Andersen ML, Alvarenga TF, Mazaro-Costa R, Hachul HC, Tufik S. The association of testosterone, sleep, and sexual function in men and women. Brain Res. 2011;1416:80–104. 51. Hamilton Na, Catley D, Karlson C. Sleep and the affective response to stress and pain. Health Psychol. 2007;26(3):288–295. 52. Buxton OM, Hopcia K, Sembajwe G. Relationship of sleep deficiency to perceived pain and functional limita- tions in hospital patient care workers. J Occup Environ Med. 2012;54(7):851–858. 53. Edwards RR, Almeida DM, Klick B, Haythornthwaite JA, Smith MT. Duration of sleep contributes to next-day pain report in the general population. Pain. 2008;137(1):202–207. 54. Roehrs Ta, Harris E, Randall S, Roth T. Pain sensitivity and recovery from mild chronic sleep loss. Sleep. 2012;35(12):1667–1672. 55. Dorrian J, Rogers NL, Dinges DF. Psychomotor vigilance performance: neurocognitive assay sensitive to sleep loss. In: Kushida C, ed. Sleep Deprivation. New York: Marcel Dekker; 2005:39–70. 56. Lim J, Dinges DF. Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2008;1129:305–322. 57. Goel N, Rao H, Durmer JS, Dinges DF. Neurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation. Semin Neurol. 2009;29(4):320–339. 58. Bjerner B. Alpha depression and lowered pulse rate during delayed actions in a serial reaction test: a study of sleep deprivation. Acta Physiol Scand. 1949;19(suppl 65):1–93. 59. Wilkinson RT. Sleep deprivation: performance tests for partial and selective sleep deprivation. In: Abt L, ed. Progress in Clinical Psychology. New York, NY: Grune; 1969:28–43. 60. Belenky G, Wesensten NJ, Thorne DR. Patterns of performance degradation and restoration during sleep re- striction and subsequent recovery: a sleep dose-response study. J Sleep Res. 2003;12(1):1–12.

 REFERENCES 69

61. Van Dongen HP, Maislin G, Mullington JM, Dinges DF. The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose- response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2003;26(2):117–126. 62. Dinges DF, Kribbs NB. Performing while sleepy: effects of experimentally-induced sleepiness. In: Monk TH, ed. Sleep, Sleepiness and Performance. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons; 1991:97–128. 63. Baranski JV, Pigeau RA. Self-monitoring cognitive performance during sleep deprivation: effects of modafinil, d-amphetamine and placebo. J Sleep Res. 1997;6(2):84–91. 64. Bard EG, Sotillo C, Anderson AH, Thompson HS, Taylor MM. The DCIEM map task corpus: spontaneous dia- logue under sleep deprivation and drug treatment. Speech Commun. 1996;20(1–2):71–84. 65. Wechsler D. Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1981. 66. Thorne DR, Genser SG, Sing HC, Hegge FW. The Walter Reed performance assessment battery. NeurobehavToxi- colTeratol. 1985;7(4):415–418. 67. Dang-Vu TT, Desseilles M, Petit D, Mazza S, Montplaisir J, Maquet P. Neuroimaging in sleep medicine. Sleep Med. 2007;8(4):349–372. 68. Horne JA. Human sleep, sleep loss and behaviour. Implications for the prefrontal cortex and psychiatric disor- der. Br J Psychiatry. 1993;162:413–419. 69. Drummond SP, Brown GG, Stricker JL, Buxton RB, Wong EC, Gillin JC. Sleep deprivation-induced reduction in cortical functional response to serial subtraction. Neuroreport. 1999;10(18):3745–3748. 70. Drummond SP, Brown GG, Gillin JC, Stricker JL, Wong EC, Buxton RB. Altered brain response to verbal learn- ing following sleep deprivation. Nature. 2000;403(6770):655–657. 71. Jones K, Harrison Y. Frontal lobe function, sleep loss and fragmented sleep. Sleep Med Rev. 2001;5(6):463–475. 72. Mu Q, Nahas Z, Johnson KA. Decreased cortical response to verbal working memory following sleep depriva- tion. Sleep. 2005;28:55–67. 73. Drummond SPA, Brown GG, Salamat JS, Gillin JC. Increasing task difficulty facilitates the cerebral compensa- tory response to total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2004;27:445–451. 74. Mu Q, Mishory A, Johnson KA. i in. Decreased brain activation during a working memory task at rested base- line is associated with vulnerability to sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2005;28:433–446. 75. Wimmer F, Hoffmann R, Bonato R, Moffitt A. The effects of sleep deprivation on divergent thinking and atten- tion processes. J Sleep Res. 1992;1(4):223–230. 76. Horne JA. Sleep loss and “divergent” thinking ability. Sleep. 1988;11(6):528–536. 77. Chee MWL, Choo WC. Functional imaging of working memory after 24 hr of total sleep deprivation. J Neurosci. 2004;24:4560–4567. 78. Harrison Y, Horne JA. One night of sleep loss impairs innovative thinking and flexible decision making. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 1999;78(2):128–145. 79. Herscovitch J, Stuss D, Broughton R. Changes in cognitive processing following short-term cumulative partial sleep deprivation and recovery oversleeping. J Clin Neuropsychol. 1980;2(4):301–319. 80. Norton R. The effects of acute sleep deprivation on selective attention. Br J Psychol. 1970;61(2):157–161. 81. Blagrove M, Alexander C, Horne Ja. The effects of chronic sleep reduction on the performance of cognitive tasks sensitive to sleep deprivation. Appl Cogn Psychol. 1995;9:21–40. 82. Harrison Y, Horne JA. Sleep loss affects risk-taking. J Sleep Res. 1998;7(suppl 2):113. 83. Womack SD, Hook JN, Reyna SH, Ramos M. Sleep loss and risk-taking behavior: a review of the literature. Behav Sleep Med. 2013;11(5):343–359. 84. Williamson AM, Feyer AM. Moderate sleep deprivation produces impairments in cognitive and motor perfor- mance equivalent to legally prescribed levels of alcohol intoxication. Occup Environ Med. 2000;57(10):649–655. 85. Dawson D, Reid K. Fatigue, alcohol and performance impairment. Nature. 1997;388(6639):235. 86. Hack MA, Choi SJ, Vijayapalan P, Davies RJO, Stradling JR. Comparison of the effects of sleep deprivation, alcohol and obstructive sleep apnoea (OSA) on simulated steering performance. Respir Med. 2001;95(7): 594–601. www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6WWS-45V29JK-9/2/7dceb05ba44e7d4ed40c1dcebf9067d0. 87. Connor J, Norton R, Ameratunga S. Driver sleepiness and risk of serious injury to car occupants: population based case control study. BMJ. 2002;324(7346):1125. 88. Lenné MG, Triggs TJ, Redman JR. Time of day variations in driving performance. Accid Anal Prev. 1997;29(4):431–437.

 70 3. The Functions of Sleep and the Effects of Sleep Deprivation

89. Reimer B, D’Ambrosio LA, Coughlin JF. Secondary analysis of time of day on simulated driving performance. J Safety Res. 2007;38(5):563–570. 90. The Libby Zion case. Ann Intern Med. 1991;115(12):985–986. 91. Asch DA, Parker RM. The Libby Zion case. One step forward or two steps backward?. N Engl J Med. 1988;318(12):771–775. 92. Arnedt JT, Owens J, Crouch M, Stahl J, Carskadon Ma. Neurobehavioral performance of residents after heavy night call vs after alcohol ingestion. JAMA. 2005;294(9):1025–1033. 93. McDermott CM, LaHoste GJ, Chen C, Musto A, Bazan NG, Magee JC. Sleep deprivation causes be- havioral, synaptic, and membrane excitability alterations in hippocampal neurons. J Neurosci. 2003;23(29):9687–9695. 94. Mander BA, Santhanam S, Saletin JM, Walker MP. Wake deterioration and sleep restoration of human learning. Curr Biol. 2011;21(5). 95. Tononi G, Cirelli C. Sleep and synaptic homeostasis: a hypothesis. Brain Res Bull. 2003;62(2):143–150. 96. Tononi G, Cirelli C. Sleep and the price of plasticity: from synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consoli- dation and integration. Neuron. 2014;81(1):12–34. 97. Van Der Werf YD, Altena E, Schoonheim MM. Sleep benefits subsequent hippocampal functioning. Nat Neuro- sci. 2009;12(2):122–123. 98. Massimini M, Tononi G, Huber R. Slow waves, synaptic plasticity and information processing: insights from transcranial magnetic stimulation and high-density EEG experiments. Eur J Neurosci. 2009;29(9):1761–1770. 99. Ackermann S, Rasch B. Differential effects of non-REM and REM sleep on memory consolidation?. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 2014;14(2). 100. Yaroush R, Sullivan MJ, Ekstrand BR. Effect of sleep on memory. II. Differential effect of the first and second half of the night. J Exp Psychol. 1971;88(3):361–366. 101. Marshall L, Born J. The contribution of sleep to hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11(10):442–450. 102. Gais S, Born J. Declarative memory consolidation: mechanisms acting during human sleep. Learn Mem. 2004;11(6):679–685. 103. Plihal W, Born J. Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on declarative and procedural memory. J Cogn Neurosci. 1997;9(4):534–547. 104. Wagner U, Gais S, Born J. Emotional memory formation is enhanced across sleep intervals with high amounts of rapid eye movement sleep. Learn Mem. 2001;8(2):112–119. 105. Plihal W, Born J. Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on priming and spatial memory. Psychophysiology. 1999;36(5):571–582. 106. Verleger R, Schuknecht SV, Jaskowski P, Wagner U. Changes in processing of masked stimuli across early- and late-night sleep: a study on behavior and brain potentials. Brain Cogn. 2008;68(2):180–192. 107. Mander BA, Rao V, Lu B. Prefrontal atrophy, disrupted NREM slow waves and impaired hippocampal-­ dependent memory in aging. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16(3):357–364. 108. Scullin MK. Sleep, memory, and aging: the link between slow-wave sleep and episodic memory changes from younger to older adults. Psychol Aging. 2013;28(1):105–114. 109. Marshall L, Helgadóttir H, Mölle M, Born J. Boosting slow oscillations during sleep potentiates memory. Nature. 2006;444(7119):610–613. 110. O’Neill J, Pleydell-Bouverie B, Dupret D, Csicsvari J. Play it again: reactivation of waking experience and mem- ory. Trends Neurosci. 2010;33(5):220–229. 111. Peigneux P, Laureys S, Fuchs S. Are spatial memories strengthened in the human hippocampus during slow wave sleep?. Neuron. 2004;44(3):535–545. 112. Wilson MA, McNaughton BL. Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. Science. 1994;265(5172):676–679. 113. Buzsáki G. Hippocampal sharp waves: their origin and significance.Brain Res. 1986;398(2):242–252. 114. Nádasdy Z, Hirase H, Czurkó A, Csicsvari J, Buzsáki G. Replay and time compression of recurring spike se- quences in the hippocampus. J Neurosci. 1999;19(21):9497–9507. 115. Sirota A, Csicsvari J, Buhl D, Buzsáki G. Communication between neocortex and hippocampus during sleep in rodents. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003;100(4):2065–2069. 116. Mölle M, Born J. Slow oscillations orchestrating fast oscillations memory consolidation. Prog Brain Res. 2011;193:93–110.

 REFERENCES 71

117. Schabus M, Gruber G, Parapatics S. Sleep spindles and their significance for declarative memory consolidation. Sleep. 2004;27(8):1479–1485. 118. Eschenko O, Mölle M, Born J, Sara SJ. Elevated density after learning or after retrieval in rats. J Neurosci. 2006;26(50):12914–12920. 119. Piosczyk H, Holz J, Feige B. The effect of sleep-specific brain activity versus reduced stimulus interference on declarative memory consolidation. J Sleep Res. 2013;22(4):406–413. 120. Wagner U, Fischer S, Born J. Changes in emotional responses to aversive pictures across periods rich in slow- wave sleep versus rapid eye movement sleep. Psychosom Med. 2002;64:627–634. 121. Groch S, Wilhelm I, Diekelmann S, Born J. The role of REM sleep in the processing of emotional memories: Evidence from behavior and event-related potentials. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2013;99:1–9. 122. Albouy G, King BR, Maquet P, Doyon J. Hippocampus and striatum: Dynamics and interaction during acquisi- tion and sleep-related motor sequence memory consolidation. Hippocampus. 2013;23(11):985–1004. 123. Peigneux P, Laureys S, Delbeuck X, Maquet P. Sleeping brain, learning brain. The role of sleep for memory sys- tems. Neuroreport. 2001;12(18):A111–A124. 124. Smith C. Sleep states and memory processes in humans: procedural versus declarative memory systems. Sleep Med Rev. 2001;5(6):491–506. 125. Rasch B, Gais S, Born J. Impaired off-line consolidation of motor memories after combined blockade of cholin- ergic receptors during REM sleep-rich sleep. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34(7):1843–1853. 126. Vertes RP, Eastman KE. The case against memory consolidation in REM sleep. Behav Brain Sci. 2000;23(6):867–876. 127. Siegel JM. The REM sleep: memory consolidation hypothesis. Science. 2001;294(5544):1058–1063. 128. Rasch B, Pommer J, Diekelmann S, Born J. Pharmacological REM sleep suppression paradoxically improves rather than impairs skill memory. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12(4):396–397. 129. Huber R, Felice Ghilardi M, Massimini M, Tononi G. Local sleep and learning. Nature. 2004;430(6995):78–81. 130. Aeschbach D, Cutler AJ, Ronda JM. A role for non-rapid-eye-movement sleep homeostasis in perceptual learn- ing. J Neurosci. 2008;28(11):2766–2772. 131. Landsness EC, Crupi D, Hulse BK. Sleep-dependent improvement in visuomotor learning: a causal role for slow waves. Sleep. 2009;32(10):1273–1284. 132. Walker MP, Brakefield T, Morgan A, Hobson JA, Stickgold R. Practice with sleep makes perfect: sleep-depen- dent motor skill learning. Neuron. 2002;35(1):205–211. 133. Nishida M, Walker MP. Daytime naps, motor memory consolidation and regionally specific sleep spindles. PLoS One. 2007;2(4). 134. Fogel SM, Smith CT, Cote KA. Dissociable learning-dependent changes in REM and non-REM sleep in declara- tive and procedural memory systems. Behav Brain Res. 2007;180(1):48–61. 135. Schönauer M, Geisler T, Gais S. Strengthening procedural memories by reactivation in sleep. J Cogn Neurosci. 2013;26(1):143–153. 136. Antony JW, Gobel EW, O’Hare JK, Reber PJ, Paller KA. Cued memory reactivation during sleep influences skill learning. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15(8):1114–1116. 137. Nishida M, Pearsall J, Buckner RL, Walker MP. REM sleep, prefrontal theta, and the consolidation of human emotional memory. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19(5):1158–1166. 138. McGaugh JL. The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2004;27:1–28. 139. Walker MP, van der Helm E. Overnight therapy? The role of sleep in emotional brain processing. Psychol Bull. 2009;135(5):731–748. 140. Kaestner EJ, Wixted JT, Mednick SC. Pharmacologically increasing sleep spindles enhances recognition for negative and high-arousal memories. J Cogn Neurosci. 2013;25(10):1597–1610. 141. Rolls A, Makam M, Kroeger D, Colas D, de Lecea L, Heller HC. Sleep to forget: interference of fear memories during sleep. Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18(11):1166–1170. 142. Hauner KK, Howard JD, Zelano C, Gottfried JA. Stimulus-specific enhancement of fear extinction during slow- wave sleep. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16(11):1553–1555. 143. Stickgold R, Walker M. To sleep, perchance to gain creative insight?. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8(5):191–192. 144. Wagner U, Gais S, Haider H, Verleger R, Born J. Sleep inspires insight. Nature. 2004;427:352–355. 145. Payne JD, Schacter DL, Propper RE. The role of sleep in false memory formation. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2009;92(3):327–334.

 72 3. The Functions of Sleep and the Effects of Sleep Deprivation

146. Diekelmann S, Born J, Wagner U. Sleep enhances false memories depending on general memory performance. Behav Brain Res. 2010;208(2):425–429. 147. Walker MP, Liston C, Hobson JA, Stickgold R. Cognitive flexibility across the sleep-wake cycle: REM-sleep enhancement of anagram problem solving. Cogn Brain Res. 2002;14(3):317–324. 148. Lewin I, Gombosh D. Increase in REM time as a function of the need for divergent thinking. In: Levin PKW, ed. Sleep: Physiology, Biochemistry, Psychology, Pharmacology, Clinical Implications. Basel: Karger; 1973:399–403. 149. Besedovsky L, Lange T, Born J. Sleep and immune function. Pflugers Arch Eur J Physiol. 2012;463(1):121–137. 150. Haack M, Sanchez E, Mullington JM. Elevated inflammatory markers in response to prolonged sleep restriction are associated with increased pain experience in healthy volunteers. Sleep. 2007;30(9):1145–1152. www.pubmed- central.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=1978405&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract. 151. Meier-Ewert HK, Ridker PM, Rifai N. Effect of sleep loss on C-reactive protein, an inflammatory marker of cardiovascular risk. J Am Coll Cardiol. 2004;43(4):678–683. 152. Mullington JM, Simpson NS, Meier-Ewert HK, Haack M. Sleep loss and inflammation. Best Pract Res Clin Endo- crinol Metab. 2010;24(5):775–784. 153. Mullington JM, Haack M, Toth M, Serrador JM, Meier-Ewert HK. Cardiovascular, inflammatory, and metabolic consequences of sleep deprivation. Prog Cardiovasc Dis. 2009;51(4):294–302. 154. Cohen S, Doyle WJ, Alper CM, Janicki-Deverts D, Turner RB. Sleep habits and susceptibility to the common cold. Arch Intern Med. 2009;169(1):62–67. 155. Mohren DCL, Jansen NWH, Kant IJ, Galama J, van den Brandt PA, Swaen GMH. Prevalence of common infec- tions among employees in different work schedules. J Occup Environ Med. 2002;44(11):1003–1011. 156. Spiegel K, Sheridan JF, Van Cauter E. Effect of sleep deprivation on response to immunization. JAMA. 2002;288(12):1471–1472. 157. Kakizaki M, Kuriyama S, Sone T. Sleep duration and the risk of breast cancer: the Ohsaki Cohort Study. BR J Cancer. 2008;99(9):1502–1505. 158. Weiderpas E, Sandin S, Inoue M. Risk factors for epithelial ovarian cancer in Japan—results from the Japan Public Health Center-based Prospective Study cohort. Int J Oncol. 2012;40(1):21–30. 159. Thompson CL, Larkin EK, Patel S, Berger NA, Redline S, Li L. Short duration of sleep increases risk of colorectal adenoma. Cancer. 2011;117(4):841–847. 160. Jiao L, Duan Z, Sangi-Haghpeykar H, Hale L, White DL, El-Serag HB. Sleep duration and incidence of colorectal cancer in postmenopausal women. Br J Cancer. 2013;108(1):213–221.