Isochrony in English Speech: Its Statistical Validity and Linguistic Relevance

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Isochrony in English Speech: Its Statistical Validity and Linguistic Relevance W. JASSEM, D. R. HILL, I. H. WITTEN Isochrony in English Speech: its Statistical Validity and Linguistic Relevance 1. Introduction In her review of recent work on rhythm in English speech, Lehiste (1977) shows that there has been disappointingly little agreement among specialists as to the validity of the isochrony principle, ranging from fairly strong textbook affirmation, such as the oft-cited description by Pike (1945: 34) or a more up-to-date formulation by Ladefoged (1975: 102-103) through doubt and scepticism (e.g. O’Connor, 1965; Bolinger, 1965; Uldall, 1971) to downright rejection (e. g. Lea, 1974; Shen and Paterson, 1962). Her own experiments lead Lehiste (1973; 1975; 1977) to the conclusion that “... there were some aspects of the data that spoke for the presence of isochrony, and other aspects that spoke against it” (1977: 256). According to Lehiste, isochrony is most evident at the perceptual level, but seeing that the sensation of rhythmicality must reflect some properties of the signal, she also made measurements of the sound wave and produced evidence in favour of a certain measure of isochrony, which she related to syntax. The relation is an inverse one in the sense that if the (tendency for) isochrony is destroyed by “an increase in the interstress interval”, then this is a sign of the presence of a syntactic boundary. One might perhaps re-interpret Lehiste’s conclusions in a direct sense as assigning to isochrony a function of internal syntactic cohesion. One of the major merits of the extensive treatise on the rhythm of spoken English by Adams (1979) is a most exhaustive historical survey of previous work in the area, with laudable emphasis on the widely overlooked ideas of the early phoneticians. It transpires that much of the present-day thinking and argumentation on the rhythmicality of spoken English was anticipated in the preceding centuries, beginning with John Hart and Joshua Steele, the latter having strongly influenced Abercrombie’s (1964; 1973) theory, which will figure prominently further on in this paper. Also, in direct relation to one particular problem considered here, John Hart (Adams, 1973: 22-23) related ‘accent’ (equivalent to the early 20th century ‘stress’) to ‘melody and rhythmus’ rather than loudness and he was aware of rhythmical (accentual) units of speech such as ofanapple, fromtheCitie, “rediscovered” 30 years ago by one of the present writers (Jassem, 1949; 1952). 204 W. Jassem, D.R. Hill, I.H. Witten It is also noteworthy that a difference of opinion as to whether pauses in speech should be counted in the rhythmical structure of English can be traced to the late 18th and early 19th century, J. Steele supporting the pause and J. Odell opposing it (Adams, 1979: 29-30). 2. The Theory The general problem is so well-known that it will scarcely be necessary to expand on it. Hardly any present-day textbook of English phonetics (or phonology) fails to mention rhythmicality as reflected in the (approximate) isochrony ‘interstress intervals’. Some of the major, more specific, questions, are: 1. What are the relations between rhythmicality (and isochrony) in the acoustic signal and rhythmicality as a percept? 2. What are the appropriate measurement procedures to be applied to the speech wave which would test the hypothesis of rhythmicality? 3. What is the experimental design of appropriate perceptual tests? 4. How are the results of measurements and perceptual tests to be evaluated? 5. If the ‘isochrony effect’ is present, does it primarily affect the length of the entire syllables, or the duration of the constituent segments (phones)? 6. If isochrony is primarily reflected in the length of entire syllables, does it affect all the syllables of the rhythm unit (‘bar’, ‘foot’, or whatever) or only the accented (‘stressed’) syllables? 7. If isochrony is primarily reflected in the duration of the phonetic segments (phones), does it affect all the segments, or some of them only, and how? 8. Is rhythmicality equally effective in all styles of speech, or is it a dependent variable, being, for instance, more evident in verse reading, less so in prose reading, slow and deliberate speech, and perhaps least evident in fast, casual discourse? 9. Is rhythmicality an effect of accent, or is accent the effect of rhythmicality? 9a. If accent is the cause rather than the effect of rhythm, then which phonetic attributes are relevant for accentuation?1 10. If there is an isochrony effect, then (a) how can it be quantified, (b) how can the quantitative statement be used to describe the strength of the effect, and (c) how strong has the effect to be in order to be treated as phonologically relevant? 1 9a is only included here because the question has often been begged. But the answer is in fact a crucial premise in the theory of isochrony and rhythm. This has most fully been realized by Adams (1979), see especially her introduction and Chapter 6. Isochrony in English Speech 205 11. What are the relations, if any, between the rhythm units and syntax or morphology? The investigation reported on below proposes a method which may give partial answers to points 2 and 4. It also assumes a hypothesis related to point 7 and tests it statistically, leading to a possible answer to 10 a and b. Reference will also be made to point 11. 3. Two Specific Theories of English Speech Rhythm Two specific theories of the rhythm of spoken English have been proposed. The one, which will here be referred to as (A), was first put forward by Abercrombie (1964, 1973), and the other, referred to as (B), by Jassem (1952), (slightly modified in Jassem, 1980 and 1981).2 Apart from the fairly obvious postulate that both theories submit, viz. one of a tendency towards equality of interstress intervals, they have one important premise in common. They do not start off with any higher-order syntactic units of which the rhythm units would be constituents. In fact, the ‘beats’, or ‘bars’, or ‘feet’, etc. of either theory, though possibly correlated with syntactic entities, are independent of them.3 Abercrombie’s theory was further developed by Halliday (1970) and Witten (1977). The two theories may be summarized as follows: (A) Abercrombie 1. The rhythm unit, called FOOT, always begins with a stressed syllable, consequently any unstressed syllable follows a stressed one within the same Foot. All unstressed syllables may therefore be described as postaccentual (or postictic). 2. If any utterance begins with an unstressed syllable, a silent stress is posited, this being an abstraction manifested as zero sound, i.e. not materialized objectively, though real psychologically (subjectively). 3. A disyllabic foot is triple-timed and may be represented by one of the following structures: ∪ — (short-long), ∩ ∩ (medium-medium), or 2 Jassem 1980 and 1981 are recent editions of earlier works first published in 1954 and 1962 respectively. The modifications of the original 1952 version of (B) contained in these works were proposed long before the results of the present investigation were available, though they are largely borne out by it. 3 In this, as in other aspects, the position of both authors are drastically different from those assumed by the proponents of any variety of Generative Phonology. But it may be interesting to note that Jassem’s ‘rhythm units’ seem to coincide with Chomsky and Halle’s ‘phonological words’. The locution (utterance, tone-group, sentence, etc.) The book was in an unlikely place is analysed into three ‘phonological words’ (Chomsky and Halle 1968: 367-368): the-book was-in-an-unlikely place. Exactly the same division is obtained by applying the principles expounded in Jassem 1952. Abercrombie’s interpretation would be entirely different: the | book-was-in-an-un- | likely | place. 206 W. Jassem, D.R. Hill, I.H. Witten — ∪ (long-short). The original version of the theory did not discuss feet of more than two syllables, and this part of the theory was supplied by Witten (1977). This mora-based structure of rhythm was not insisted on by Halliday (1970). 4. The internal rhythmic structure of a Foot is inherently related to its segmental structure, e.g. (C)V1CV(C)4 and (C)V2(C)V(C) both produce ∩ ∩, etc. Abercrombie adds, however, that “the phonematic structure of the syllable may ... at times be quite irrelevant” (1964: 217). 5. There are certain relations between rhythm and syntax. For instance, the quantities depend on the presence of a word boundary” (ibid, p. 219). There is a rhythmic difference between a one-syllable word followed by an unstressed syllable of a word which is not directly related syntactically, e. g., (take) Grey to (London) as opposed to Greater (London)—and a word followed by an enclitic, e. g. take it, tell him. It is pointed out that enclitic treatment of monosyllables is “not entirely clear” (p. 221), some other cases of “rhythmic linking” being piece of, may there (be), etc.5 6. Stress is assumed (see, e. g., Abercrombie, 1967: 35; Ladefoged, 1975: 222) to be increased effort (or energy). The notion of stress is primary in relation to the notion of rhythm. (B) Jassem 1. English speech consists of two kinds of rhythm units: (a) Narrow Rhythm Units (NRU) and (b) Anacruses (ANA). For a given tempo, the length of a narrow rhythm unit depends on the number of syllables. This length is a constant for a monosyllabic rhythm unit and a given tempo, and may be denoted by Y As the number of syllables in a narrow rhythm unit increases, the length of the narrow rhythm unit (NRU) also increases, but not proportionately. A two-syllable NRU is longer than a monosyllabic one, but it is distinctly less than 2Y.
Recommended publications
  • Prosody – Introductory
    THE PHONOLOGY OF RHONDDA VALLEYS ENGLISH. (Rod Walters, University of Glamorgan, 2006) 4. PROSODY – INTRODUCTORY (Readers unfamiliar with the prosodic terms used will find explanations in the Glossary.) 4.1 Aims of RVE Research Prosody (stress, rhythm, intonation etc) contributes strongly towards the ‘melody’ of Welsh English accents. Chapter 5 will attempt to describe the main features of RVE prosody. Like t'Hart, Collier, and Cohen of the Institute for Perception Research in Eindhoven (1990: 2-6), the approach will be primarily ‘from the phonetic level of observation’.35 Comments will be made concerning the functions / meanings of the prosodic forms identified, but the researcher is very aware that the links between meaning and prosody are seldom straightforward: (1) discerning the meaning of an utterance is a matter of pragmatic interpretation in which many factors beside prosody need to be taken into account (propositional content of the lexis-grammar, full context of the situation, speakers’ body language etc) (2) several prosodic features, e.g. voice quality, loudness and intonation, may be operative at the same time and therefore difficult to disentangle (3) the same prosodic feature may be involved in ‘doing’ more than one thing at the same time – for example a given pitch movement may be simultaneously involved in accentuation and demarcation (4) nearly all prosodic features – including pitch level – are subject to gradient variation and can be used to carry signals that many analysts would consider paralinguistic rather than linguistic (cf discussion in Ladd 1996: 33-41). Due to such factors, suggestions as to the meanings of RVE prosodic forms will be tentative and restricted to discourse functions such as the segmenting, structuring and highlighting of information.
    [Show full text]
  • A Contrastive Study Farah Hafedh Ibrahim College of Arts,Mustansiriyah University, Baghdad, Iraq Email : [email protected]
    36 Syllables in English and Arabic : A contrastive Study Farah Hafedh Ibrahim College of Arts,Mustansiriyah University, Baghdad, Iraq Email : [email protected] Abstract The present study investigates syllables in both English and Arabic for the sake of revealing the similarities and differences between them in these two languages. The syllable is regarded as the basic unit or the building block of speech that has attracted the attention of English and Arab phoneticians. The present study tackles the nature of English and Arabic syllables, sheds light on some of the theories that described them as well as classifying them into types and describing their structure. The study arrives at the conclusion that there are many differences between syllables in English and syllables in Arabic. These differences concern the way syllables are viewed, their types and their structure. However, both languages consider the syllable to be the basic unit of speech. Phonetically, syllables are described in English as consisting of a centre which has little or no obstruction to airflow preceded and followed by great obstruction; whereas in Arabic, syllables are phonetically described as chest pulses. Keywords: Syllable; Syllable Nature; Syllable Type; Structure Introduction The term syllable, in English language, is not easy to define since it can be defined phonetically, or phonologically or both phonetically and phonologically. Starting with Ladefoged (2006:242) who describes the syllable as “the smallest unit of speech. Every utterance must contain at least one syllable”. He (ibid.) further states that speech is composed of segments such as vowels or consonants and these segments are considered to be aspects of the syllable.
    [Show full text]
  • Part 1: Introduction to The
    PREVIEW OF THE IPA HANDBOOK Handbook of the International Phonetic Association: A guide to the use of the International Phonetic Alphabet PARTI Introduction to the IPA 1. What is the International Phonetic Alphabet? The aim of the International Phonetic Association is to promote the scientific study of phonetics and the various practical applications of that science. For both these it is necessary to have a consistent way of representing the sounds of language in written form. From its foundation in 1886 the Association has been concerned to develop a system of notation which would be convenient to use, but comprehensive enough to cope with the wide variety of sounds found in the languages of the world; and to encourage the use of thjs notation as widely as possible among those concerned with language. The system is generally known as the International Phonetic Alphabet. Both the Association and its Alphabet are widely referred to by the abbreviation IPA, but here 'IPA' will be used only for the Alphabet. The IPA is based on the Roman alphabet, which has the advantage of being widely familiar, but also includes letters and additional symbols from a variety of other sources. These additions are necessary because the variety of sounds in languages is much greater than the number of letters in the Roman alphabet. The use of sequences of phonetic symbols to represent speech is known as transcription. The IPA can be used for many different purposes. For instance, it can be used as a way to show pronunciation in a dictionary, to record a language in linguistic fieldwork, to form the basis of a writing system for a language, or to annotate acoustic and other displays in the analysis of speech.
    [Show full text]
  • Morphological Sources of Phonological Length
    Morphological Sources of Phonological Length by Anne Pycha B.A. (Brown University) 1993 M.A. (University of California, Berkeley) 2004 A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the Graduate Division of the University of California, Berkeley Committee in charge: Professor Sharon Inkelas (chair) Professor Larry Hyman Professor Keith Johnson Professor Johanna Nichols Spring 2008 Abstract Morphological Sources of Phonological Length by Anne Pycha Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics University of California, Berkeley Professor Sharon Inkelas, Chair This study presents and defends Resizing Theory, whose claim is that the overall size of a morpheme can serve as a basic unit of analysis for phonological alternations. Morphemes can increase their size by any number of strategies -- epenthesizing new segments, for example, or devoicing an existing segment (and thereby increasing its phonetic duration) -- but it is the fact of an increase, and not the particular strategy used to implement it, which is linguistically significant. Resizing Theory has some overlap with theories of fortition and lenition, but differs in that it uses the independently- verifiable parameter of size in place of an ad-hoc concept of “strength” and thereby encompasses a much greater range of phonological alternations. The theory makes three major predictions, each of which is supported with cross-linguistic evidence. First, seemingly disparate phonological alternations can achieve identical morphological effects, but only if they trigger the same direction of change in a morpheme’s size. Second, morpheme interactions can take complete control over phonological outputs, determining surface outputs when traditional features and segments fail to do so.
    [Show full text]
  • English Vowels Basic Vowel Description Tense And
    Main vowel symbols of GA and WCE Front Central Back* Height [i] ‘beat’ [u] ‘boot’ higher high English vowels [I] ‘bit’ [U] ‘book’ lower high [e] ‘bait’ [o] ‘boat’ higher mid [E] ‘bet’ [´] ‘sofa’ [ç] ‘ bought, GA’ lower mid Narrow transcription [! ] ‘but’ [Å] ‘Bob, bought WCE’ higher low [Q] ‘bat’ [A] ‘Bob, GA’ lower low •Back vowels except [A] are rounded; the rest are unrounded • [Å] is described as lower low in your text and IPA. I’ll take either. It is ROUNDED • [e] and [o] are the first part (nucleus) of the diphthongs [ej] and [ow] 1 3 Basic vowel description Tense and Lax • The basic descriptors for vowels are HAR • English phonology traditionally makes the – Height distinction between tense and lax vowels – Advancement – This is not phonetically well-defined as a single – Rounding characteristic • It is useful to subdivide each height class into a – You just need to learn which vowels are classed as tense and lax ‘higher’ and ‘lower’ subdivision – You should learn all the heights in the next chart • This distinction based mainly on phonotactics – Phonotactics is the description of which sounds can occur together in a legal word or syllable of a language 2 4 Duration patterns tense and lax Occurrence of TENSE Vs vowels • Vowels called ‘tense’ occur freely at the ends of • Tense vowels are longer than lax vowels of the one syllable words same general height class i, ej, u, ow, Å ( A and ç in GA) /i/ longer than /I/ /u/ longer than /U/ • Also tense : aj, aw, çj /ej/ longer than /E/ • Examples : • The tense back vowels /ow/ and /Å/ (both /A/ and – ‘bee’, ‘bay’, ‘too’, ‘tow’ , ‘law’ ( ‘spa’ and ‘law’ in GA) /ç/ in GA) are longer than the lax central /! / • An exception to the ‘lax vowels shorter than tense’ is /Q/ – It is often as long as any other vowel 5 7 Length of tense v.
    [Show full text]
  • Six-Way Syllable Sort Objective the Student Will Identify Syllables in Words
    Phonics Syllable Patterns P.034 Six-Way Syllable Sort Objective The student will identify syllables in words. Materials Header cards (Activity Master P.034.AM1) Word cards (Activity Master P.034.AM2a - P.034.AM2f) Note: Two syllable words ending in consonant-le should use consonant-le as the target syllable. Activity Students will sort words by syllable types. 1. Place header cards face up on a flat surface. Shuffle the word cards and place face down in a stack. 2. Taking turns, students select the top card from the stack, read the word, and identify what type of syllable it is (e.g., “tennis, closed syllables” or “loop, vowel pair syllable”). 3. Place in column under matching header card. 4. Point to and read words in entire column starting with header card. 5. Continue until all cards are sorted. 6. Peer evaluation Extensions and Adaptations Make more words and sort. Sort fewer patterns and/or words. Record words. 2006 The Florida Center for Reading Research (Revised July, 2007) 2-3 Student Center Activities: Phonics Phonics P.034.AM1 Six-Way Syllable Sort flat she closed syllables open syllables snake sharp vowel-consonant-e syllables r-controlled syllables read sample vowel pair syllables consonant-le syllables header cards 2-3 Student Center Activities: Phonics 2006 The Florida Center for Reading Research (Revised July, 2007) Phonics Six-Way Syllable Sort P.034.AM2a witness bobbin grand suffix tennis camp panic happen closed syllable word cards 2006 The Florida Center for Reading Research (Revised July, 2007) 2-3 Student Center Activities:
    [Show full text]
  • 49 Stress-Timed Vs. Syllable- Timed Languages
    TBC_049.qxd 7/13/10 19:21 Page 1 49 Stress-timed vs. Syllable- timed Languages Marina Nespor Mohinish Shukla Jacques Mehler 1Introduction Rhythm characterizes most natural phenomena: heartbeats have a rhythmic organization, and so do the waves of the sea, the alternation of day and night, and bird songs. Language is yet another natural phenomenon that is charac- terized by rhythm. What is rhythm? Is it possible to give a general enough definition of rhythm to include all the phenomena we just mentioned? The origin of the word rhythm is the Greek word osh[óp, derived from the verb oeí, which means ‘to flow’. We could say that rhythm determines the flow of different phenomena. Plato (The Laws, book II: 93) gave a very general – and in our opinion the most beautiful – definition of rhythm: “rhythm is order in movement.” In order to under- stand how rhythm is instantiated in different natural phenomena, including language, it is necessary to discover the elements responsible for it in each single case. Thus the question we address is: which elements establish order in linguistic rhythm, i.e. in the flow of speech? 2The rhythmic hierarchy: Rhythm as alternation Rhythm is hierarchical in nature in language, as it is in music. According to the metrical grid theory, i.e. the representation of linguistic rhythm within Generative Grammar (cf., amongst others, Liberman & Prince 1977; Prince 1983; Nespor & Vogel 1989; chapter 43: representations of word stress), the element that “establishes order” in the flow of speech is stress: universally, stressed and unstressed positions alternate at different levels of the hierarchy (see chapter 40: stress: phonotactic and phonetic evidence).
    [Show full text]
  • A Phonetic Study of Length and Duration in Kyrgyz Vowels
    Swarthmore College Works Linguistics Faculty Works Linguistics 2019 A Phonetic Study Of Length And Duration In Kyrgyz Vowels Nathaniel Ziv Stern , '20 Jonathan North Washington Swarthmore College, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://works.swarthmore.edu/fac-linguistics Part of the Linguistics Commons Let us know how access to these works benefits ouy Recommended Citation Nathaniel Ziv Stern , '20 and Jonathan North Washington. (2019). "A Phonetic Study Of Length And Duration In Kyrgyz Vowels". Proceedings Of The Workshop On Turkic And Languages In Contact With Turkic. Volume 4, 119-131. DOI: 10.3765/ptu.v4i1.4577 https://works.swarthmore.edu/fac-linguistics/256 This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 License. This work is brought to you for free by Swarthmore College Libraries' Works. It has been accepted for inclusion in Linguistics Faculty Works by an authorized administrator of Works. For more information, please contact [email protected]. 2019. Proceedings of the Workshop on Turkic and Languages in Contact with Turkic 4. 119–131. https://doi.org/10.3765/ptu.v4i1.4577 A phonetic study of length and duration in Kyrgyz vowels Nathaniel Ziv Stern & Jonathan North Washington∗ Abstract. This paper examines the phonetic correlates of the (phonological) vowel length contrast in Kyrgyz to address a range of questions about the nature of this contrast, and also explores factors that affect (phonetic) duration in short vowels. Measurement and analysis of the vowels confirms that there is indeed a significant duration distinction be- tween the Kyrgyz vowel categories referred to as short and long vowels.
    [Show full text]
  • Teach Vowel Team Syllable Type- There Are Six Types of Syllables: 1
    Vowel Team Syllables Information In this intervention group, the following skills are taught: Teach Vowel Team Syllable Type- There are six types of syllables: 1. A closed syllable ends in a consonant. The vowel has a short vowel sound, as in the word bat. 2. A vowel-consonant-e syllable is typically found at the end of a word. The final e is silent and makes the next vowel before it long, as in the word name. 3. An open syllable ends in a vowel. The vowel has a long vowel sound, as in the first syllable of apron. 4. An r-controlled syllable contains a vowel followed by the letter r. The r controls the vowel and changes the way it is pronounced, as in the word car. 5. A vowel team syllable has two vowels next to each other that together say a new sound, as in the word south. 6. A consonant +l-e syllable is found in words like handle, puzzle, and middle. Read Words With ai,ay- fail, aim, de/tail, con/tain, re/frain, day, tray, dis/play, pay/ment, hay/stack Spelling Patterns for Long a (a, ai, ay, a-e, eigh, ea)- eight, save, a/pron, hay, neigh/bor, sub/way, train, pro/claim, re/frig/er/ate, ba/gel, great, break/ing Read Words With ee,ey- they, grey, en/trée, fi/an/cée Spelling Patterns for Long e (e, ee, ea, ie, y, ey, e-e)- ba/by, key, me, be, feed, creek, thief, pier, re/lief, bee/tle, bun/ny, cra/zy, mon/ey, Pete, bum/ble/bee Read Words With oa,oe,ue- Oa and oe say the long o sound as in boat, and toe.
    [Show full text]
  • Reading Competency, Speech Rate and Rhythm
    Ariel Cuadro, Alvaro Mailhos, Ignacio Estevan, and Francisco Martínez-Sánchez Psicothema 2021, Vol. 33, No. 2, 222-227 ISSN 0214 - 9915 CODEN PSOTEG Copyright © 2021 Psicothema doi: 10.7334/psicothema2020.80 www.psicothema.com Reading Competency, Speech Rate and Rhythm Ariel Cuadro1, Alvaro Mailhos2, Ignacio Estevan2, and Francisco Martínez-Sánchez3 1 Universidad Católica del Uruguay, 2 Universidad de la República, and 3 Universidad de Murcia Abstract Resumen Background: There is increasing evidence defi ning reading competency Competencia Lectora, Fluidez y Ritmo. Antecedentes: cada vez más in terms of accuracy, speed and prosody, as well as interest in gaining evidencias defi ne la competencia lectora en términos de precisión, better understanding of the interrelation as a function of prosodic velocidad y prosodia; así hay interés en comprender esa relación en features. This study aims to analyze the relationship between reading función de los rasgos prosódicos que se consideren. Este estudio analiza competency, in terms of accuracy and speed of written word recognition, la relación entre precisión y velocidad de reconocimiento de la palabra and two attributes related to prosody in oral reading of texts: speech rate escrita y dos atributos relacionados con la prosodia en la lectura oral de and rhythm. Method: Oral reading of a narrative text by 141 third and textos: la velocidad y el ritmo del habla. Método: se analizó la lectura oral fourth grade Spanish-speaking students was analyzed using an automated de un texto narrativo de 141 estudiantes de habla hispana de tercer y cuarto acoustic speech procedure and their reading competency was assessed. grado de primaria mediante un procedimiento acústico automatizado del Results: Reading profi ciency was associated with a lower proportion habla, evaluada su competencia lectora.
    [Show full text]
  • (Mandarin), Phonology Of
    1 Chinese (Mandarin), Phonology of (For Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics, 2nd edition, Elsevier Publishing House) San Duanmu, University of Michigan, MI USA February 2005 Chinese is the first language of over 1,000 million speakers. There are several dialect families of Chinese (each in turn consisting of many dialects), which are often mutually unintelligible. However, there are systematic correspondences among the dialects and it is easy for speakers of one dialect to pick up another dialect rather quickly. The largest dialect family is the Northern family (also called the Mandarin family), which consists of over 70% of all Chinese speakers. Standard Chinese (also called Mandarin Chinese) is a member of the Northern family; it is based on the pronunciation of the Beijing dialect. There are, therefore, two meanings of Mandarin Chinese, one referring to the Northern dialect family and one referring to the standard dialect. To avoid the ambiguity, I shall use Standard Chinese (SC) for the latter meaning. SC is spoken by most of those whose first tongue is another dialect. In principle, over 1,000 million people speak SC, but in fact less than 1% of them do so without some accent. This is because even Beijing natives do not all speak SC. SC has five vowels, shown in Table 1 in IPA symbols (Chao 1968, Cheng 1973, Lin 1989, Duanmu 2002). [y] is a front rounded vowel. When the high vowels occur before another vowel, they behave as glides [j, , w]. [i] and [u] can also follow a non-high vowel to form a diphthong. The mid vowel can change frontness and rounding depending on the environment.
    [Show full text]
  • Rhythm in Language
    Rhythm in Language Patel Chapter 3.3 Rhythm Ch. 3b 1 Introduc;on • In many languages, it is normal to produce the syllables of an u?erance with differing degrees of prominence. x x x x x She wrote her novels with a blue pen she inherited x from her aunt. • The most important physical correlates of prominence are duraon, pitch movement, vowel quality and loudness. • Most sec;ons below will treat prominence as a binary quan;ty referred to as “stress.” • Not all languages have lexical stress. – Tone languages with stress (Mandarin) and without (Cantonese) – Pitch accent languages with stress (Swedish) and without (Japanese) Rhythm Ch. 3b 2 Periodicity and typology • Kenneth Pike 1945 – Syllable-;med languages: e.g. Spanish • Syllables mark off roughly equal temporal intervals – Stress-;med languages: e.g. English • Roughly equal temporal intervals between stresses. x x x x The teacher is interested in buying some books. x x x x Big bales are fought daily. • To achieve evenly ;med feet, speakers would stretch or compress syllables to fit into the typical foot duraon. Rhythm Ch. 3b 3 Periodicity and typology cont. • Abercrombie 1967 – Each syllable is associated with a contrac;on of muscles associated with exhalaon, and some contrac;ons are especially strong (chest pulses, stress pulses). – In each language one or the other occurred rhythmically, and rhythm is equated with periodicity. • Stress-;med languages: English, Russian, Arabic. • Syllable-;med languages: French, Telegu, Yoruba • Mora-;med languages: Japanese – A unit smaller than a syllable, usually consis;ng of a consonant and vowel, but some;mes containing only a consonant.
    [Show full text]