[Frontiers in Bioscience 8, E172-189, January 1, 2003] 172 ACUTE VERSUS CHRONIC FUNCTIONAL ASPECTS of UNILATERAL SPATIAL NEGLECT
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
[Frontiers in Bioscience 8, e172-189, January 1, 2003] ACUTE VERSUS CHRONIC FUNCTIONAL ASPECTS OF UNILATERAL SPATIAL NEGLECT Victor W. Mark Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of Alabama at Birmingham, 619 19th Street South, SRC 190, Birmingham AL TABLE OF CONTENTS 1. Abstract 2. Introduction 3. Primacy of unilateral neglect for disability 4. Functional manifestations of unilateral neglect 5. Neglect recovery and chronic aspects of neglect 6. The functionally valid assessment of neglect 7. Rehabilitation of neglect 7.1. Vestibular stimulation 7.2. Eye patching 7.3. Pharmacologic intervention 7.4. Prisms 8. Discussion 9. Acknowledgments 10. References 1. ABSTRACT 2. INTRODUCTION This article reviews the impact of unilateral Unilateral neglect (alternately termed spatial spatial neglect on daily living (“functional”) activities. Its neglect, hemispatial neglect, amorphosynthesis, hemi- disturbances on basic functional activities, such as feeding, inattention) has been recognized in humans since the early grooming, and locomotion, are easily identifiable. Patients 20th century (1). Definitions vary, but essentially the term with neglect frequently lack insight into their disorder and refers to pathologic spatial asymmetry in performance or do not initiate compensatory behaviors, which probably awareness. This lopsidedness entails a deficiency for action impedes recovery. Simple standard tests of neglect during or awareness that in most instances is contralateral to the visual exploration correlate with impaired recovery of side of brain injury. However, the individual with neglect functional skills acutely following brain injury. However, in addition may demonstrate overactivity in the opposite unilateral neglect resolves in most individuals, yet many direction (i.e., ipsilateral to the side of brain injury) (2-5). patients remain chronically impaired during daily living Neglect is considered to have an attentional basis, since it activities. This suggests that some other disorder associated may be overcome by cuing or increasing motivation (6-15). with neglect may contribute to the failure to regain functional independence. A candidate disorder is general Unilateral neglect is commonly recognized by (non-spatial) inattention. However, cognitive studies in physicians, psychologists, therapists, and family members stroke are biased toward assessing neglect and are usually in individuals with brain injury. Furthermore, unilateral insensitive to other disorders that may accompany stroke, inattention can interfere with routine daily living activities, such as general inattention and executive dysfunction. such as locomotion and feeding. This article will review the Therefore, the contribution of unilateral neglect toward impact of neglect on daily living activities (“functional functional status relative to diverse other cognitive activities”) both acutely and chronically following brain disorders after stroke is unclear. Treatments for unilateral injury. For this article, “functional” will refer broadly to neglect have been largely unsuccessful or impractical, or any voluntary activity that is involved with self-care, they were not evaluated in controlled studies. Intensive locomotion, social interaction, or self-fulfillment (e.g., practice of scanning appears to benefit, but this observation leisure pursuits). This is to be distinguished from activity needs to be replicated in a controlled manner. A recently on tasks that are considered to be experimental or developed treatment that involves wearing prisms to shift diagnostic, but without clear benefit to the individual (e.g., the view ipsilaterally has been associated with transfer of line bisection), or from primarily involuntary behaviors training effects to untreated spatial activities and prolonged (e.g., dreaming, reflexive withdrawal to pain). improvement of neglect. However, despite some promising lines of investigation in neglect rehabilitation, further Although investigational and clinical reports often regard research is required to understand where neglect stands in neglect as if it were a well-described and homogeneous relation to other cognitive disturbances that follow stroke phenomenon, investigators repeatedly have stressed that with respect to functional significance and recovery, to “neglect” comprises a heterogeneous array of disturbances, decide what disorders should be targeted for rehabilitation. as does “aphasia.” Inconsistencies may occur 172 Functional neglect Table 1. Literature citations associating specific cognitive from these studies on the primacy of unilateral neglect for disorders with impaired functional recovery following functional outcomes after stroke because of inconsistencies stroke in (1) the range of cognitive functions assessed, (2) the Cognitive disorder References specific cognitive tests used, and (3) the functional Unilateral neglect 26-42 assessments used. Moreover, the surfeit of studies on Depression 36, 43-49 neglect may reflect a clinical and investigational Aphasia 36, 37, 50-53 preoccupation with its bizarre aspects (e.g., drawing only Anosognosia (impaired insight 39, 54-56 half a flower, denial of limb ownership) or the ease of its into one’s own illness) assessment relative to other cognitive disorders. (For General inattention 57, 58 example, neglect is commonly assessed by measuring the Disorientation 59, 60 difference from true midpoint when subjects are asked to Limb apraxia 50, 61 bisect lines with a pen, or by counting the number of Memory impairment 60, 62 omitted figures on cancellation tests, where subjects must Pathologic laughter and crying 61 cross out all figures of a certain kind.) Few studies that Motor impersistence 62 found disorders other than neglect to be importantly related Executive dysfunction 63 to outcome actually examined for neglect (56, 57, 60, 61). Numerals denote specific citations given in the References In addition, measures of functional ability are biased section. toward basic motoric activities, and they less reliably or easily assess social, leisure, or community-based activities in its severity (16-18) or even direction (i.e., to the left or (e.g., banking, driving, or other “instrumental” activities of right) within an individual patient (19-25), depending on daily living). Thus, Table 1 at best reflects the present the task or what specific stimuli are presented. (However, clinical and investigational appreciation for the impact of neglect on functional tasks is almost invariably neglect on a restricted set of functional activities. contralateral to the acute, major cerebral lesion.) Thus, the disturbances that are collectively referred to as “unilateral 4. FUNCTIONAL MANIFESTATIONS OF neglect” are considerably diverse and may have different UNILATERAL NEGLECT neurophysiologic bases. This frustrates developing standards for diagnosing and reporting neglect. However, Clinicians who treat brain-injured individuals the absence of standard definitions for neglect does not readily associate neglect with functional impairment. undermine the clinical importance it has for the Indeed, a disorder that seriously affects either the “input” or performance and recovery of functional activities. “output” aspects of directional spatial cognition might be expected a priori to impair any functional activity. 3. PRIMACY OF UNILATERAL NEGLECT FOR Consequently, the functional impact of neglect is almost DISABILITY without limit. Some of the more common or interesting aspects of functional neglect are described below. As would be expected with any cognitive impairment, functional disturbances may accompany In its most severe form, neglect may involve a neglect. However, neglect is especially important, since it strong gaze bias that is directed ipsilateral to a unilateral has been associated with impaired functional recovery hemispheric lesion. This finding, which has been termed following brain injury more often than any other cognitive “Vulpian’s sign” (64), is often associated with a marked disorder. Table 1 reflects the importance that clinical disinclination to orient or explore contralaterally, either investigators have attached to neglect following stroke. visually or manually. (For the remainder of this article, Although neglect may occur with nearly any illness that “ipsilateral” and “contralateral” will be used with respect to involves structural brain injury, it has most often been the location of the brain lesion that is presumed to be evaluated following stroke. (Stroke is defined as an responsible for the clinical disorder under discussion.) enduring neurologic disorder of abrupt onset that is Ipsilateral gaze bias nearly always appears when the brain presumed to be secondary to a pathologic alteration of injury is acute, and the brain lesion is generally large and regional blood flow, such as from hemorrhage or blood cortical, spanning most of a lobe or overlapping lobes. The vessel blockage.) This bias toward evaluating neglect impairment frustrates the most basic functional activities in following stroke is most likely due to the high prevalence the clinician-patient relationship, so that the patient fails to of stroke and its increasing incidence as the mean age of turn the head or eyes toward a clinician or family member populations increases, particularly in industrialized nations. who stands in contralateral space. The patient also often Table 1 cites the articles from the author’s own literature fails to find the contralateral hand with the ipsilateral