[Frontiers in Bioscience 8, E172-189, January 1, 2003] 172 ACUTE VERSUS CHRONIC FUNCTIONAL ASPECTS of UNILATERAL SPATIAL NEGLECT

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

[Frontiers in Bioscience 8, E172-189, January 1, 2003] 172 ACUTE VERSUS CHRONIC FUNCTIONAL ASPECTS of UNILATERAL SPATIAL NEGLECT [Frontiers in Bioscience 8, e172-189, January 1, 2003] ACUTE VERSUS CHRONIC FUNCTIONAL ASPECTS OF UNILATERAL SPATIAL NEGLECT Victor W. Mark Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of Alabama at Birmingham, 619 19th Street South, SRC 190, Birmingham AL TABLE OF CONTENTS 1. Abstract 2. Introduction 3. Primacy of unilateral neglect for disability 4. Functional manifestations of unilateral neglect 5. Neglect recovery and chronic aspects of neglect 6. The functionally valid assessment of neglect 7. Rehabilitation of neglect 7.1. Vestibular stimulation 7.2. Eye patching 7.3. Pharmacologic intervention 7.4. Prisms 8. Discussion 9. Acknowledgments 10. References 1. ABSTRACT 2. INTRODUCTION This article reviews the impact of unilateral Unilateral neglect (alternately termed spatial spatial neglect on daily living (“functional”) activities. Its neglect, hemispatial neglect, amorphosynthesis, hemi- disturbances on basic functional activities, such as feeding, inattention) has been recognized in humans since the early grooming, and locomotion, are easily identifiable. Patients 20th century (1). Definitions vary, but essentially the term with neglect frequently lack insight into their disorder and refers to pathologic spatial asymmetry in performance or do not initiate compensatory behaviors, which probably awareness. This lopsidedness entails a deficiency for action impedes recovery. Simple standard tests of neglect during or awareness that in most instances is contralateral to the visual exploration correlate with impaired recovery of side of brain injury. However, the individual with neglect functional skills acutely following brain injury. However, in addition may demonstrate overactivity in the opposite unilateral neglect resolves in most individuals, yet many direction (i.e., ipsilateral to the side of brain injury) (2-5). patients remain chronically impaired during daily living Neglect is considered to have an attentional basis, since it activities. This suggests that some other disorder associated may be overcome by cuing or increasing motivation (6-15). with neglect may contribute to the failure to regain functional independence. A candidate disorder is general Unilateral neglect is commonly recognized by (non-spatial) inattention. However, cognitive studies in physicians, psychologists, therapists, and family members stroke are biased toward assessing neglect and are usually in individuals with brain injury. Furthermore, unilateral insensitive to other disorders that may accompany stroke, inattention can interfere with routine daily living activities, such as general inattention and executive dysfunction. such as locomotion and feeding. This article will review the Therefore, the contribution of unilateral neglect toward impact of neglect on daily living activities (“functional functional status relative to diverse other cognitive activities”) both acutely and chronically following brain disorders after stroke is unclear. Treatments for unilateral injury. For this article, “functional” will refer broadly to neglect have been largely unsuccessful or impractical, or any voluntary activity that is involved with self-care, they were not evaluated in controlled studies. Intensive locomotion, social interaction, or self-fulfillment (e.g., practice of scanning appears to benefit, but this observation leisure pursuits). This is to be distinguished from activity needs to be replicated in a controlled manner. A recently on tasks that are considered to be experimental or developed treatment that involves wearing prisms to shift diagnostic, but without clear benefit to the individual (e.g., the view ipsilaterally has been associated with transfer of line bisection), or from primarily involuntary behaviors training effects to untreated spatial activities and prolonged (e.g., dreaming, reflexive withdrawal to pain). improvement of neglect. However, despite some promising lines of investigation in neglect rehabilitation, further Although investigational and clinical reports often regard research is required to understand where neglect stands in neglect as if it were a well-described and homogeneous relation to other cognitive disturbances that follow stroke phenomenon, investigators repeatedly have stressed that with respect to functional significance and recovery, to “neglect” comprises a heterogeneous array of disturbances, decide what disorders should be targeted for rehabilitation. as does “aphasia.” Inconsistencies may occur 172 Functional neglect Table 1. Literature citations associating specific cognitive from these studies on the primacy of unilateral neglect for disorders with impaired functional recovery following functional outcomes after stroke because of inconsistencies stroke in (1) the range of cognitive functions assessed, (2) the Cognitive disorder References specific cognitive tests used, and (3) the functional Unilateral neglect 26-42 assessments used. Moreover, the surfeit of studies on Depression 36, 43-49 neglect may reflect a clinical and investigational Aphasia 36, 37, 50-53 preoccupation with its bizarre aspects (e.g., drawing only Anosognosia (impaired insight 39, 54-56 half a flower, denial of limb ownership) or the ease of its into one’s own illness) assessment relative to other cognitive disorders. (For General inattention 57, 58 example, neglect is commonly assessed by measuring the Disorientation 59, 60 difference from true midpoint when subjects are asked to Limb apraxia 50, 61 bisect lines with a pen, or by counting the number of Memory impairment 60, 62 omitted figures on cancellation tests, where subjects must Pathologic laughter and crying 61 cross out all figures of a certain kind.) Few studies that Motor impersistence 62 found disorders other than neglect to be importantly related Executive dysfunction 63 to outcome actually examined for neglect (56, 57, 60, 61). Numerals denote specific citations given in the References In addition, measures of functional ability are biased section. toward basic motoric activities, and they less reliably or easily assess social, leisure, or community-based activities in its severity (16-18) or even direction (i.e., to the left or (e.g., banking, driving, or other “instrumental” activities of right) within an individual patient (19-25), depending on daily living). Thus, Table 1 at best reflects the present the task or what specific stimuli are presented. (However, clinical and investigational appreciation for the impact of neglect on functional tasks is almost invariably neglect on a restricted set of functional activities. contralateral to the acute, major cerebral lesion.) Thus, the disturbances that are collectively referred to as “unilateral 4. FUNCTIONAL MANIFESTATIONS OF neglect” are considerably diverse and may have different UNILATERAL NEGLECT neurophysiologic bases. This frustrates developing standards for diagnosing and reporting neglect. However, Clinicians who treat brain-injured individuals the absence of standard definitions for neglect does not readily associate neglect with functional impairment. undermine the clinical importance it has for the Indeed, a disorder that seriously affects either the “input” or performance and recovery of functional activities. “output” aspects of directional spatial cognition might be expected a priori to impair any functional activity. 3. PRIMACY OF UNILATERAL NEGLECT FOR Consequently, the functional impact of neglect is almost DISABILITY without limit. Some of the more common or interesting aspects of functional neglect are described below. As would be expected with any cognitive impairment, functional disturbances may accompany In its most severe form, neglect may involve a neglect. However, neglect is especially important, since it strong gaze bias that is directed ipsilateral to a unilateral has been associated with impaired functional recovery hemispheric lesion. This finding, which has been termed following brain injury more often than any other cognitive “Vulpian’s sign” (64), is often associated with a marked disorder. Table 1 reflects the importance that clinical disinclination to orient or explore contralaterally, either investigators have attached to neglect following stroke. visually or manually. (For the remainder of this article, Although neglect may occur with nearly any illness that “ipsilateral” and “contralateral” will be used with respect to involves structural brain injury, it has most often been the location of the brain lesion that is presumed to be evaluated following stroke. (Stroke is defined as an responsible for the clinical disorder under discussion.) enduring neurologic disorder of abrupt onset that is Ipsilateral gaze bias nearly always appears when the brain presumed to be secondary to a pathologic alteration of injury is acute, and the brain lesion is generally large and regional blood flow, such as from hemorrhage or blood cortical, spanning most of a lobe or overlapping lobes. The vessel blockage.) This bias toward evaluating neglect impairment frustrates the most basic functional activities in following stroke is most likely due to the high prevalence the clinician-patient relationship, so that the patient fails to of stroke and its increasing incidence as the mean age of turn the head or eyes toward a clinician or family member populations increases, particularly in industrialized nations. who stands in contralateral space. The patient also often Table 1 cites the articles from the author’s own literature fails to find the contralateral hand with the ipsilateral
Recommended publications
  • Effect of Parietal Lobe Lesions on Saccade Targeting and Spatial Memory in a Naturalistic Visual Search Task Steven S
    Neuropsychologia 41 (2003) 1365–1386 Effect of parietal lobe lesions on saccade targeting and spatial memory in a naturalistic visual search task Steven S. Shimozaki a,∗, Mary M. Hayhoe a, Gregory J. Zelinsky b, Amy Weinstein c, William H. Merigan a, Dana H. Ballard a a Center for Visual Science, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA b Department of Psychology, State University of New York, Stony Brook, NY, USA c Department of Neurology, Strong Memorial Hospital, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA Received 8 November 2001; received in revised form 7 January 2003; accepted 7 January 2003 Abstract The eye movements of two patients with parietal lobe lesions and four normal observers were measured while they performed a visual search task with naturalistic objects. Patients were slower to perform the task than the normal observers, and the patients had more fixations per trial, longer latencies for the first saccade during the visual search, and less accurate first and second saccades to the target locations during the visual search. The increases in response times for the patients compared to the normal observers were best predicted by increases in the number of fixations. In order to investigate the effects of spatial memory on search performance, in some trials observers saw a preview of the search display. The patients appeared to have difficulty using previously viewed information, unlike normal observers who benefit from the preview. This suggests a spatial memory deficit. The patients’ deficits are consistent with the hypothesis that the parietal cortex has a role in the selection of targets for saccades, in memory for target location.
    [Show full text]
  • Visuoconstructional Impairment: What Are We Assessing, and How Are We Assessing It?
    University of Rhode Island DigitalCommons@URI Open Access Dissertations 2004 Visuoconstructional Impairment: What Are We Assessing, and How Are We Assessing It? Jessica Somerville Ruffalo University of Rhode Island Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.uri.edu/oa_diss Recommended Citation Somerville Ruffalo, Jessica, "Visuoconstructional Impairment: What Are We Assessing, and How Are We Assessing It?" (2004). Open Access Dissertations. Paper 381. https://digitalcommons.uri.edu/oa_diss/381 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by DigitalCommons@URI. It has been accepted for inclusion in Open Access Dissertations by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@URI. For more information, please contact [email protected]. ... VISUOCONSTRUCTIONAL IMPAIRMENT: WHAT ARE WE ASSESSING, AND HOW ARE WE ASSESSING IT? BY JESSICA SOMERVILLE RUFFOLO A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PIDLOSOPHY IN PSYCHOLOGY THE UNIVERSITY OF RHODE ISLAND 2004 DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY DISSERTATON OF JESSICA SOMERVILLE RUFFOLO APPROVED: DEAN OF THE GRADUATE SCHOOL UNIVERSITY OF RHODE ISLAND 2004 ABSTRACT Visuoconstruction (VC) is a commonly-assessed neuropsychological domain that involves the ability to organize and manually manipulate spatial information to make a design. Tests used to measure VC are considered multifactorial in nature given their multiple demands (e.g., visuospatial, executive, motor), and therefore, interpretation of VC impairment can be difficult. Additionally, a wide variety of tests and methods are used to measure VC, further complicating interpretation of results. Although clinicians and researchers spend a great deal of time studying "VC," there has been much confusion about what it is, what is being measured, and how to best measure it.
    [Show full text]
  • Apraxia, Neglect, and Agnosia
    REVIEW ARTICLE 07/09/2018 on SruuCyaLiGD/095xRqJ2PzgDYuM98ZB494KP9rwScvIkQrYai2aioRZDTyulujJ/fqPksscQKqke3QAnIva1ZqwEKekuwNqyUWcnSLnClNQLfnPrUdnEcDXOJLeG3sr/HuiNevTSNcdMFp1i4FoTX9EXYGXm/fCfl4vTgtAk5QA/xTymSTD9kwHmmkNHlYfO by https://journals.lww.com/continuum from Downloaded Apraxia, Neglect, Downloaded CONTINUUM AUDIO INTERVIEW AVAILABLE and Agnosia ONLINE from By H. Branch Coslett, MD, FAAN https://journals.lww.com/continuum ABSTRACT PURPOSEOFREVIEW:In part because of their striking clinical presentations, by SruuCyaLiGD/095xRqJ2PzgDYuM98ZB494KP9rwScvIkQrYai2aioRZDTyulujJ/fqPksscQKqke3QAnIva1ZqwEKekuwNqyUWcnSLnClNQLfnPrUdnEcDXOJLeG3sr/HuiNevTSNcdMFp1i4FoTX9EXYGXm/fCfl4vTgtAk5QA/xTymSTD9kwHmmkNHlYfO disorders of higher nervous system function figured prominently in the early history of neurology. These disorders are not merely historical curiosities, however. As apraxia, neglect, and agnosia have important clinical implications, it is important to possess a working knowledge of the conditions and how to identify them. RECENT FINDINGS: Apraxia is a disorder of skilled action that is frequently observed in the setting of dominant hemisphere pathology, whether from stroke or neurodegenerative disorders. In contrast to some previous teaching, apraxia has clear clinical relevance as it is associated with poor recovery from stroke. Neglect is a complex disorder with CITE AS: many different manifestations that may have different underlying CONTINUUM (MINNEAP MINN) mechanisms. Neglect is, in the author’s view, a multicomponent disorder 2018;24(3,
    [Show full text]
  • And Its Use in Physical Activity
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 129 742 SP 010 455 TITLE Movement. Proceedings of Seventh Symposium in Psychomotor Learning and Sport Psychology. INSTITUTION Association of Physical Activity Professionals of Quebec, Montreal. PUB DATE Oct 75 NOTE 376p.; Partly in French; Proceedings of Canadian Psycho-Motor Learning and Sport Psychology Symposium (7th, Quebec, Canada, October 1975) .AVAILABLE FROMAssociation of Professlonals of Physical Activity of Quebec, 1415 Est Rue Jarry, Montreal, Quebec, Canada, H2E, 277 ($20.00) EDRS PRICE MF-$0.83 HC-$20.75 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS Athletes; Athletic Programs; *Athletics; Attitudes; Behavior; *Emotional Response; Memory; Morale; *MotivatiOn; Physical Activities; Physical Education; *Psychometrics; Psychomotor Objectives; *Psychomotor Skills; Response Node; Sportsmanship; *Time Perspective; Transfer of Training IDENTIFIERS *Sport Psychology ABSTRACT The symposium on the physical and psychomotor aspects of sports covered a variety of topics. Among those contained in.this report are: discussions on the taxonomies of sports; the time dimension of physical activity including ;esponse variables, judgment of temporal order, and duration discrimination; memory; information and its use in physical activity; trainitTand the transfer of __training to performance; the .academic discipline of sports; attitudes of the athlete; cooperation and competition in sports; 'anxiety and other emotions aroused in' athletics; the causes of success; motivation; the implications of participation.in sports activities; and other psycho-social aspects of sports. (JMF) *********************************************************************** * Documents acquired by ERIC include many informal unpublished * * materials not available from other sources. ERIC makes every effort * * to obtain.the best copy available. Nevertheless, items of marginal * * reproducibility are often encountered and this affects the quality * * of the microfiche and hardcopy reproductions ERIC makes available * * via the ERIC Document Reproduction Service (EDRS).
    [Show full text]
  • ED 266 190 TM 860 136 Kosslyn, Stephen M. TITLE Visual Hemispheric Specialization: a Computational Theory. Technical Report
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 266 190 TM 860 136 AUTilOR Kosslyn, Stephen M. TITLE Visual Hemispheric Specialization: A Computational Theory. Technical Report #7. INSTITUTION Harvard Univ., Cambridge, Mass. SPONS AGENCY National Inst. of Mental Health (DHHS), Rockville, MD.; Office of Naval Research, Arlington, Va. Personnel and Training Research Programs Office. PUB DATE 31 Oct 85 GRANT 141139478-01; N00014-83-K-0095; N00014-85-K-0291 NOTE 84p. PUB TYPE Reports - Research/Technical (143) EDRS PRICE MF01/PC04 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS Beuavior Patterns; *Cerebral Dominance; Conceptual Tempo; Elementary Secondary Education; Encoding (Psychology); *Lateral Dominance; Memory; Models; Pattern Recognition; *Perceptual Development; Reaction Time; Spatial Ability; Visualization; *Visual Perception IDENTIFIERS Brain Hemispheres; *Imaging; *Neuropsychology ABSTRACT Visual recognition, navigation, tracking, and imagery are posited to involve some of the same types of representations and processes. The first part of this paper develops a theory of some of the shared types of representations and processing modules. The theory is developed in light of neurophysiological and neuroanatomical data from non-human primates, computational constraints, and behavioral data from human subjects. In developing theories of some of the high-level computations that are performed by the visual system, three problems which must be solved by any visual system are considered: (1) position variability; (2) figure/ground segregation; and (3) non-rigid transformations. The second part of the paper develops a melhanism for the development of lateralization of visual function in the brain. The theory rests on a set of relatively simple, uncontroversial properties of the brain, including: (1) processing components; (2) ezarcise; (3) selectivity; (4) "central" bilateral control; and (5) reciprocal innervation.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Drawing Conclusions
    Drawing Conclusions: An Exploration of the Cognitive and Neuroscientific Foundations of Representational Drawing Rebecca Susan Chamberlain Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology Thesis submitted to UCL for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy July 2013 1 Declaration ‘I, Rebecca Susan Chamberlain confirm that the work presented in this thesis is my own. Where information has been derived from other sources, I confirm that this has been indicated in the thesis.’ 2 Publications The work in this thesis gave rise to the following publications: Chamberlain, R., McManus, I. C., Riley. H., Rankin, Q., & Brunswick, N. (2013) Local processing enhancements in superior observational drawing are due to enhanced perceptual functioning, not weak central coherence. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66 (7), 1448-66. Chamberlain, R. (2012) Attitudes and Approaches to Observational Drawing in Contemporary Artistic Practice. Drawing Knowledge. TRACEY Drawing and Visualisation Research. Chamberlain, R., McManus, I. C., Riley. H., Rankin, Q., & Brunswick, N. (In Press) Cain’s House Revisited and Revived: Extending Theory and Methodology for Quantifying Drawing Accuracy. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Art. 3 Acknowledgements My first thanks extend to my supervisor and mentor, Professor Chris McManus, for his infectious enthusiasm for research and for the countless words of wisdom (academic and non-academic) he has given me over the last six years. I would like to thank my collaborators at The Royal College of Art and Swansea Metropolitan University, Howard Riley and Qona Rankin, who have been so generous with their time, resources and thoughts on the artistic mind. This research also wouldn’t have been possible without the hundreds of art and design students who were willing to take time out of their hectic lives to participate in my studies and discuss their experiences and reflections with me.
    [Show full text]
  • Parietal Lobes: Evolution, Neuroanatomy, and Function
    Parietal Lobes: Evolution, Neuroanatomy, and Function Charles J. Vella, PhD May 3, 2017 Intent of this lecture Parietal lobe syndromes are mostly domain of neurology, not NP But these syndromes will affect NP performance on multiple tests There are lots of anatomical references: you can always use the pdf online as reference text There are lots of unusual, sometimes rare, syndromes My objective is to have you be aware of when parietal lobe functioning is involved in something you are seeing clinically in a patient The discovery of the Parietal Lobe Franciscus Sylvius (1614-1672) Physician, physiologist, anatomist, and chemist Professor at the University of Leidon (1641) Was in the chemistry lab at the University when he discovered the deep cleft (Sylvian Fissure). History of Parietal Lobe Discovery In 1874 Bartholow recorded odd sensation from legs on stimulating post central gyrus through skull wounds Djerine – alexia , agraphia -- angular gyrus lesion Hugo Liepmann--- ideomotor & ideational apraxia in (L) sided lesion Cushing in 1909 --- Electrical stimulation in conscious human beings –– mainly tactile hallucinations; first homunculus Critchley (1953) – 1st monograph on “ The Parietal Lobes” Siegel et al. (2003) – “The Parietal Lobes” Macdonald Critchley: Parietal Lobes Parietal Lobes No independent existence as anatomical / physiological unit Operates in conjunction with brain as a whole Strategically situated between other lobes Greater variety of clinical manifestations than rest of the hemisphere Ignored in
    [Show full text]
  • Website Lecture 3 Visuospatial Deficits and Visual Agnosia
    Visuospatial deficits and visual agnosia Masud Husain Dept Experimental Psychology & Nuffield Dept Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford 1 Visuospatial deficits Often associated with right posterior parietal (dorsal visual stream) damage or dysfunction § Visual disorientation often with visual mislocalization and gaze apraxia § Constructional apraxia § Spatial working memory deficits § Optic ataxia | associated with right (or left) superior parietal lesions § Visual extinction |associated with right (or left) parietal lesions § Neglect syndrome | more severe and long-lasting with right inferior parietal lesions § Topographical disorientation | of the egocentric variety (see later) In contrast, left parietal damage is often associated with language dysfunction, verbal working memory deficits, dyscalculia and limb apraxia 2 Visual disorientation Gordon Holmes (1918) Visual disorientation When asked to touch an object in front of him he would grope hopelessly. He could not count coins set before him. He had difficulty in seeing more than one item at a time and would bump into objects. Emphasized a disorder of visual space perception Gordon Holmes (1918) Bálint’s syndrome Bálint put greater emphasis on inattention and visually guided misreaching § Patient could no longer judge where things were. Felt unsafe to cross roads. § Looked straight ahead, unware of objects on either side (bilateral inattention) § Bálint called it “psychic paralysis of gaze” (effectively a gaze apraxia) § Could only report one object at a time (simultagnosia) § Misreached to visual objects (optic ataxia) § Post-mortem: large bilateral strokes involving parietal lobes 8 Bálint (1917) Balint-Holmes' syndrome 119 shown by Holmes’ patients. They could move their eyes in any direction spontaneously, in response to a sudden sound, or towards a part of their own body that had been named or touched by the examiner (with the exception of patient 5), but failed when requested to search for a visual target or to direct their gaze to a stimulus suddenly appearing in the peripheral field.
    [Show full text]
  • APRAXIA, AGNOSIAS, and HIGHER VISUAL FUNCTION ABNORMALITIES Jdwgreene V25
    J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.2005.081885 on 16 November 2005. Downloaded from APRAXIA, AGNOSIAS, AND HIGHER VISUAL FUNCTION ABNORMALITIES JDWGreene v25 J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2005;76(Suppl V):v25–v34. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2005.081885 ognitive neurology deals mainly with disorders of memory (for example, is the patient’s poor memory due to early dementia or to anxiety/depression?) or language (as in stroke). It Cshould be remembered, however, that other areas of cognition may be selectively impaired. This review will cover disorders of perception and of higher order motor output, both in terms of pathological loss and pathological gain of function. c PERCEPTION AND ITS DISORDERS A patient must be conscious in order to perceive the world around them. An exploration of consciousness is outside the remit of this article though recently reviewed by others.12The patient must also have the capacity to attend selectively in order to focus on one part of the sensorium. Perceptual processing is then necessary to identify what is being perceived through the various sensory modalities (namely vision, hearing, touch, smell, taste), thus allowing access to semantic knowledge and through this understanding of the environment. Initially, perceptual information is basic and modality specific, but as it is processed by higher order centres, meaning is ascribed to percepts, and information becomes multi-modal (fig 1). Ultimately, semantic knowledge is accessed using the various sensory streams. For example, if standing in the path of an oncoming train, basic perception will involve visual information, hearing the train coming, and feeling vibration from the ground.
    [Show full text]
  • Selective Deficit of Mental Visual Imagery with Intact Primary Visual
    cortex 44 (2008) 109–118 available at www.sciencedirect.com journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cortex Research report Selective deficit of mental visual imagery with intact primary visual cortex and visual perception Valentina Moroa,*, Giovanni Berlucchib, Jason Lerchc, Francesco Tomaiuolod,e and Salvatore M. Agliotid,f,1 aDepartment of Psychology and Culture Anthropology, University of Verona, Italy bDepartment of Neurological and Visual Sciences, Physiology Section, University of Verona, Italy cBrain Imaging Centre, Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada dIRCCS Fondazione S. Lucia, Roma, Italy eAuxilium Vitae Volterra SpA, Volterra, Pisa, Italy fDepartment of Psychology, University of Rome ‘‘La Sapienza’’, Roma, Italy article info abstract Article history: There is a vigorous debate as to whether visual perception and imagery share the same Received 1 March 2006 neuronal networks, whether the primary visual cortex is necessarily involved in visual im- Reviewed 7 April 2006 agery, and whether visual imagery functions are lateralized in the brain. Two patients with Revised 6 June 2006 brain damage from closed head injury were submitted to tests of mental imagery in the Accepted 9 June 2006 visual, tactile, auditory, gustatory, olfactory and motor domains, as well as to an extensive Action editor Jane Riddoch testing of cognitive functions. A computerized mapping procedure was used to localize the Published online 17 November 2007 site and to assess the extent of the lesions. One patient showed pure visual mental imagery deficits in the absence of imagery deficits in other sensory domains as well as in the motor Keywords: domain, while the other patient showed both visual and tactile imagery deficits.
    [Show full text]
  • Bálint's Syndrome: Anatomoclinical Findings and Electrooculographic Analysis in a Case
    Bálint’s syndrome Laure Pisella, Audrey Vialatte, Aarlenne Zein, Yves Rossetti To cite this version: Laure Pisella, Audrey Vialatte, Aarlenne Zein, Yves Rossetti. Bálint’s syndrome. Handbook of Clinical Neurology 3 rd Series, In press. hal-03007997 HAL Id: hal-03007997 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-03007997 Submitted on 16 Nov 2020 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Handbook of Clinical Neurology 3rd Series Bálint’s syndrome Laure Pisella, Audrey Vialatte, Aarlenne Zein Khan & Yves Rossetti [email protected] Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Lyon, France Phone number: (+33) 4 72 91 34 00 Abstract: We start by review the evolution of Bálint syndrome, covering the various interpretations of it over time. We then develop a novel integrative view in which we propose that the various symptoms, historically reported and labelled by various authors, result from a core mislocalization deficit. This idea is in accordance with our previous proposal that the core deficit of Bálint syndrome is attentional (Pisella et al. 2009, 2013, 2017) since covert attention improves spatial resolution in visual periphery (Yeshurun and Carrasco 1998); a deficit of covert attention would thus increase spatial uncertainty and thereby impair both visual object identification and visuo-motor accuracy.
    [Show full text]
  • Cerebral Localization
    CEREBRAL LOCALIZATION PARIETAL LOBE SIGNS December 18,2013 PARIETAL LOBE DR UTKARSH BHAGAT DR UTKARSH File:Gray726,727.svg 2 December 18,2013 BROADMANN AREAS DR UTKARSH BHAGAT DR UTKARSH wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/0/09 3 December 18,2013 PARIETAL LOBE ¢ Special high order sensory organ ¢ Locus of transmodal ( intersensory) integration BHAGAT DR UTKARSH ¢ Awareness of one’s body & its relation to extra personal space and of objects in the environment to each other. 4 December 18,2013 LESIONS OF PARIETAL LOBE Post central gyrus DR UTKARSH BHAGAT DR UTKARSH ¢ Simple somatosensory disturbances ¢ Contra lateral sensory loss (object recognition > position sense > touch > pain and temperature, vibration); tactile extinction ¢ Contra lateral pain, paresthesias Mesial aspect (cuneus) ¢ Transcortical sensory aphasia? (dominant hemisphere) ¢ Attentional disorder 5 December 18,2013 LESIONS OF PARIETAL LOBE ¢ In general, left hemisphere is dominant for thought and DR UTKARSH BHAGAT DR UTKARSH reasoning, analytic and mathematical skills the right hemisphere is dominant for tasks requiring spatial and constructional skills, as well as for directed attention and body image 6 December 18,2013 LESIONS OF PARIETAL LOBE Lateral aspect (superior and inferior parietal lobules) ¢ Dominant hemisphere DR UTKARSH BHAGAT DR UTKARSH ¢ Parietal apraxia (higher lesion) ¢ Finger agnosia ¢ Acalculia ¢ Right-left disorientation ¢ Literal alexia (supramarginal gyrus) ¢ Conduction aphasia 7 December 18,2013 LESIONS OF PARIETAL LOBE ¢ Nondominant hemisphere ¢ Anosognosia DR UTKARSH BHAGAT DR UTKARSH ¢ Hemispatial neglect (sensory inattention) ¢ Constructional apraxia ¢ Dressing apraxia ¢ Loss of topographical memory ¢ Allesthesia ¢ Hemisomatognosia ¢ Asymbolia for pain 8 December 18,2013 ANOSOGNOSIA ¢ Patients fail to recognize the hemiplegic limbs as belonging to them (anosognosia) DR UTKARSH BHAGAT DR UTKARSH ¢ and confabulate when asked whom they belong to (they often ascribe them to the examiner: somatoparaphrenia).
    [Show full text]