The Concept of Conscious Pleasure in the History of Modern American Psychology

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Concept of Conscious Pleasure in the History of Modern American Psychology University of New Hampshire University of New Hampshire Scholars' Repository Doctoral Dissertations Student Scholarship Winter 1989 The concept of conscious pleasure in the history of modern American psychology David C. Devonis University of New Hampshire, Durham Follow this and additional works at: https://scholars.unh.edu/dissertation Recommended Citation Devonis, David C., "The concept of conscious pleasure in the history of modern American psychology" (1989). Doctoral Dissertations. 1593. https://scholars.unh.edu/dissertation/1593 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Scholarship at University of New Hampshire Scholars' Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Doctoral Dissertations by an authorized administrator of University of New Hampshire Scholars' Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. INFORMATION TO USERS The most advanced technology has been used to photograph and reproduce this manuscript from the microfilm master. UMIfilms the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type of computer printer. The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and-there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand comer and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each original is also photographed in one exposure and is included in reduced form at the back of the book. Photographs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced xerographically in this copy. Higher quality 6" x 9" black and white photographic prints are available for any photographs or illustrations appearing in this copy for an additional charge. Contact UMI directly to order. University Microfilms International A Ben & Howell Information Com pany 300 North Zeeb Road. Ann Arbor. Ml 48106-1346 USA 313 761-4700 800 521-0600 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Order Number 9022638 The concept of conscious pleasure in the history of modern American psychology Devonis, David C., Ph.D. University of New Hampsliire, 1989 UMI 300 N. Zeeb Rd. Ann Arbor, MI 48106 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. TOE CONCEPT OF CONSCIOUS PLEASURE IN THE HISTORY OF MODERN AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGY BY DAVID C. DEVONIS B. A., Temple University, 1982 M. A., University of New Hampshire, 1986 DISSERTATION Submitted to the University of New Hampshire in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Psychology December, 1989 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. This dissertation has been examined and approved. Dissertation Director Dr. David Leary Dean of the Faculty of Arts & Sciences University of Richmond Dr. John Limber, Associate Professor Department of Psychology University of New Hampshire Dr. Kathleen McCartney, Assistant Professor Department of Psychology University of New Hampshire Dr. Michael Sokal, Professor of History Department of Humanities Worcester Polytechnic Institute Dr. Donald Wilcox, Professor Department of History University of New Hampshire Date Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. TABLE OF CONTENTS DEDICATION................................................................................................................ iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ...........................................................................................v ABSTRACT ............................................................................................................... vii CHAPTER PAGE 1. THE DESCENT OF PLEASURE .................................................................. 1 2. THE QUANTIFICATION OF PLEASURE ................................................. 43 3. THE QUALIFICATION OF PLEASURE .................................................... 73 4. THE PROMOTION OF PLEASURE ..........................................................122 5. TOE PLACE OF PLEASURE IN TOE HISTORY OF PSYCHOLOGY.... 159 REFERENCE NOTES ............................................................................................... 177 LIST OF REFERENCES ............................................................................................189 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. To Deborah Ann Cardinale and to the memory of the late Dr. Anne W. Sandoval. iv Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Sometimes only when one reaches the end of what appears a lonely journey does one realize just how many sympathetic persons were met along the way. The members of my doctoral committee were invariably precise in their criticisms, which were delivered always in a helpful and generous spirit. I hope they will all recognize those parts which are better for their advice. Ms. Sande Webster, at some personal sacrifice, read the manuscript near its completion. Her accurate historical and stylistic sense was particularly necessary at that juncture, and she deserves more than mere thanks. Several other individuals provided material aid in various forms. Many benefits, tangible and intangible, resulted from conversations with members of the Psychology Department at the University of New Hampshire: Dr. Victor Benassi; Dr. George Haslerud; Dr. Robert Mair, Dr. John Mayer, Dr. Carolyn Mebert, Dr. John Nevin; and Dr. William Woodward. Dr. John Cerullo, cf the University of New Hampshire at Manchester, has been a constant source of support both to me personally and to the whole psychological community at Durham. Beyond these members of my own community, others elsewhere were generous in their communication of information about individuals and events mentioned in this work: Dr. Gerald Cupchik of the University of Toronto; Dr. Maiy Henle of the New School for Social Research; Dr. Paul Meehl of the University of Minnesota, Dr. Larry Smith of the University of Maine at Orono; and Dr. Hans Wallach of Swarthmore College. Also, I must express my gratitude to two individuals in particular, Dr. Jerome Kagan of Harvard University and Dr. Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi of the University of Chicago, who at important times during the development and writing of this work provided great encouragement. V Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. I count it a privilege to have associated with the members of the Wellesley Colloquium for the History of Psychology over the past several years. To Dr. Otto Marx, Dr. Laurel Furumoto, Dr. John Chirban, and Dr. Steven Heims, and to its other constant and variable members, thank you for creating and maintaining a community in which students in this field can develop intellectual self-respect The graduate student community at the University of New Hampshire is, I think, unique in its closeness. My fellow student colleagues, during my years there, maintained a sense of mutuality, trust, and friendship conducive to creative and productive thought. I thank you all for this over the years. I would like also to offer special thanks to Kevin Fleming, Sandy Rutter, John Calabrese, and Beth Jordan for their particular contributions to the present work. Institutional support for this work was provided by the University of New Hampshire in many forms: teaching assistanships at both the Durham and Manchester campuses of the University, a Summer Teaching Fellowship in 1985, the Manchester Teaching Fellowship in 1987-88, and a Dissertation Year Fellowship in 1988-89. I am grateful to The Graduate School of the University, and especially Dean Raymond Erickson, for their efforts on my behalf and on behalf of the graduate program there during increasingly difficult times. Finally, not least, I would like to acknowledge the assistance of the following members of the library staff at the University of New Hampshire: Nancy Langlois, Charles Cragin, John Wiggins, and Reyna Hart. Also, I thank the staff of the Baker and Dana Libraries of Dartmouth College. And, further, thanks to Dr. John Popplestone and the staff of the Archives of the History of Psychology at the University of Akron and Mrs. Bertha Maslow, through whom permission has been granted to quote from the papers of A. H. Maslow. vi Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. ABSTRACT THE CONCEPT OF CONSCIOUS PLEASURE IN THE HISTORY OF MODERN AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGY by David C. Devonis University of New Hampshire, December, 1989 The concept of pleasure is acknowledged by historians of psychology
Recommended publications
  • 'Introspectionism' and the Mythical Origins of Scientific Psychology
    Consciousness and Cognition Consciousness and Cognition 15 (2006) 634–654 www.elsevier.com/locate/concog ‘Introspectionism’ and the mythical origins of scientific psychology Alan Costall Department of Psychology, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth, Hampshire PO1 2DY, UK Received 1 May 2006 Abstract According to the majority of the textbooks, the history of modern, scientific psychology can be tidily encapsulated in the following three stages. Scientific psychology began with a commitment to the study of mind, but based on the method of introspection. Watson rejected introspectionism as both unreliable and effete, and redefined psychology, instead, as the science of behaviour. The cognitive revolution, in turn, replaced the mind as the subject of study, and rejected both behaviourism and a reliance on introspection. This paper argues that all three stages of this history are largely mythical. Introspectionism was never a dominant movement within modern psychology, and the method of introspection never went away. Furthermore, this version of psychology’s history obscures some deep conceptual problems, not least surrounding the modern conception of ‘‘behaviour,’’ that continues to make the scientific study of consciousness seem so weird. Ó 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Introspection; Introspectionism; Behaviourism; Dualism; Watson; Wundt 1. Introduction Probably the most immediate result of the acceptance of the behaviorist’s view will be the elimination of self-observation and of the introspective reports resulting from such a method. (Watson, 1913b, p. 428). The problem of consciousness occupies an analogous position for cognitive psychology as the prob- lem of language behavior does for behaviorism, namely, an unsolved anomaly within the domain of an approach.
    [Show full text]
  • Bernard Tursky (1918-2012)
    UC Irvine UC Irvine Previously Published Works Title Bernard Tursky (1918-2012). Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/0t0857rg Journal Psychophysiology, 51(11) ISSN 0048-5772 Author Jamner, Larry D Publication Date 2014-11-01 DOI 10.1111/psyp.12338 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California Psychophysiology, 51 (2014), 1059–1060. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright © 2014 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12338 In Memoriam Bernard Tursky (1918–2012) LARRY D. JAMNER, Ph.D. Department of Psychology and Social Behavior University of California Irvine Psychophysiology and our Society lost one of its original greats when Bernard Tursky passed away on September 13, 2012 at the age of 93. Bernard Tursky was an academic trail blazer and pioneer extraordinaire. Raised in New York City, Bernard immigrated to the U.S. with his grandparents and brothers at the age of three. Circumstances demanded that he cease his formal education after the eighth grade in order to support his family. Bernie or “Profes- sor” as I affectionately referred to him represented as close an embodiment of the self-taught “Renaissance Man” I have ever met or will likely ever come across again. To enter Bernie’s office was to enter a veritable library of classic and contemporary texts, and reference manuals on topics such as electrical engineering, classic psychology, political science, medical sciences, physiology, psy- chiatry, systems theory, and mathematics. His academic career began as a technical engineer at MIT, and as his reputation grew as an ingenious developer of laboratory equipment and programming, he was recruited by the Massachusetts Mental Health Center.
    [Show full text]
  • Jeremy Bentham, Werner Stark, and 'The Psychology of Economic Man'
    Jeremy Bentham, ‘The Psychology of Economic Man’, and Behavioural Economics Michael Quinn (Bentham Project, UCL. [email protected]) Résumés English Francais § 1 briefly reviews first the received interpretation of Bentham, which sees him as having had little to do with the development of economics (excepting some passing mentions which recognize his deployment of the concept of utility or his reduction of human motivation to self-interest, and perhaps a note on his discussion of the concept of diminishing marginal utility); and second, the manner in which he applies his concept of rationality to political economy. In § 2, the central thesis of the paper is presented: it is argued that an examination of his insights into the psychology of individual choice supplies good reasons to identify him as an intellectual godfather of behavioural economics. In keeping with the normativity of his concept of rationality, Bentham would maintain that the way in which traditional economics continues to ignore the gulf between its model of human decision-making and the facts of human psychology weakens its usefulness both as a science and as a guide to public policy. Bentham anticipated several modifications to the standard model (for instance loss- aversion, the endowment effect, reference dependence, framing, the desire for cognitive ease, and status-quo bias) which have been introduced later by behavioural economics. § 3 introduces two problems concerning the normativity of economics, the first of which, at least for Bentham, rests upon a false premise,
    [Show full text]
  • NUMBER 52 Socializing for Modernization in a Multi-Ethnic Elite
    NUMBER 52 Socializing for Modernization in a Multi-Ethnic Elite John A. Armstrong Conference on Entrepreneurship and Economic Innovation in Russia/Soviet Union Sponsored by Kennan Institute for Advanced Russian Studies American Association for the Advancement of Slavic Studies November 16-18, 1978 Washington, D.C. DRAFT PAPER - NOT FOR CITATION SOCIALIZING FOR MODER!~IZATION IN A MULTI-ETHNIC ELITE John A. Armstrong Few historical topics are more important than economic modernization in the Russian polity. 4ike other experiences in economic development, the Russian experience has been highly complex. Certainly one component has been the role of the entrepreneur. Yet, this role itself is a very complex one; failure to separate the many components of the role and to define them analytically has probably caused even more mischief in Russian studies than in examinations of other polities. This essay rests on considerable thought and a limited amount of research on topics close to the theme of entrepreneurial response. There is no intention, however, of presenting a research report on entrepreneurs in the Russian polity, or even laying an adequate groundwork for such research, which ought to be pursued in many directions. The purpose is clarification of the problem, insofar as that preliminary step can be taken by reflection and discussion. At certain points in the analysis a modest element of comparison is introduced. These comparisons are far from fully elaborated, hence they may eventually turn out to be superficial analogies. Given the relative isolation in which so much of Russian area studies has proceeded, even preliminary suggestions of novel analogies to developments elsewhere appear to be appropriate.
    [Show full text]
  • From Sound to Symbol to Meaning Dual Certification Course in Our Phono-Graphix and Kinesiology Cognitive Language Wise Methods
    Learning Theory Epistomology Motivation Memory Child Development Thank you for your participation in our Developmental From Sound to Symbol to Meaning dual certification course in our Phono-Graphix and Kinesiology Cognitive Language Wise methods. The course content and materials for working with new and Psychology remedial readers draw upon theory and research in many fields. The developers of these internationally acclaimed methods understand that among the problems in education is a lack of cross-over from various Neuro-Linguistics areas of investigation and bodies of knowledge in Psychology, Linguistics, and Developmental Kinesiology. In 1993 they set the field of reading research and instruction Pure and Applied on its side with their reseach published in the Orton Annals of Dyslexia (C. McGuinness, et al, Linguistics 1993; Reading Reflex, Simon and Schuster, 1997) in which they demonstrated standard score gains in reading of six times that achieved by other reading methods, by addressing the true nature of the English written code and the three skills needed to teach such a code. In 2000 the McGuinnesses released their Language Wise Verbal Intelligence program (Yale University Press, 2000). The McGuinnesses' latest work, From Sound to Symbol to Meaning, brings the two methods, courses, and materials together in a seamless format for teachers and students. FrFroomm S Soouunndd t too S Syymmbbooll t too MeMeaanniinngg PPhhoonnoo--GGrraapphhiixx a anndd L Laanngguuaaggee WiWissee C Ceerrttiiffiiccaattiioonn C Coouursrsee FrFroomm S Soouunndd t too S Syymmbbooll t too MeMeaanniinngg Phono-Graphiix and Language Wise Duall C Certiifiicatiion Course Four Day Course Syllabus Day One Address the areas of research and practice drawn upon in the theory and practice of Phono-Graphix® reading and spelling instruction.
    [Show full text]
  • Foundational Frameworks of Positive Psychology: Mapping Well-Being Orientations
    Canadian Psychology / Psychologie canadienne © 2015 Canadian Psychological Association 2015, Vol. 56, No. 3, 311–321 0708-5591/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cap0000033 Foundational Frameworks of Positive Psychology: Mapping Well-Being Orientations Louise Lambert Holli-Anne Passmore and Mark D. Holder Canadian University of Dubai University of British Columbia The scientific study of well-being has been strongly influenced by ideas from a number of related fields, including different areas of psychology. Two major philosophical traditions—hedonia and eudaimonia— underscore much of our current understanding of well-being, and are reflected across early and contemporary psychological theories of well-being. These traditions help delineate the various concep- tualisations of well-being and its components; moreover, these traditions influence which research questions are asked, and where and how answers are sought. This has resulted in a plethora of categories and terms referring to similar, yet distinct, concepts such as: well-being, happiness, optimal or positive experiences, life satisfaction, and flourishing. Given the difficulties of distinguishing these concepts, this article aims to provide clarity by delineating the major orientations in positive psychology. We provide a “road-map” to theories and models of well-being found within positive psychology, thereby providing a starting a point from which an integrative framework of theories and models of well-being can be developed. To that end, also included in this review is a selection of well-being models that lie beyond the traditional frameworks. We conclude with a consideration of several criticisms that have been directed at positive psychology, and provide recommendations for future directions.
    [Show full text]
  • The Nature of Learning and Memory
    Arlo Clark-Foos, Ph.D. 2 October 2017 1 • Life Without Memory (Clive Wearing) – Video Clive’s Diary “10:08 a.m.: Now I am superlatively awake. First time aware for years.” “10:13 a.m.: Now I am overwhelmingly awake.” “10:28 a.m.: Actually I am now the first time awake for years.” 10/2/2017 12:41 PM 2 • Genes determine the possible range. • Reflex actions, simple behaviors – Knee-jerk, swallow, suck, grip John B. Watson and Rosalie Rayner 2 October 2017 3 • Experience (and memory of it) determines our individual differences and allows us to improve upon initial behaviors and reflexes. 2 October 2017 4 • How well can you read these sentences? The quick brown fox jumps over the lazy dog. Pack my box with five dozen liquor jugs. 2 October 2017 5 • Context and Expectations Group 1 Group 2 2 October 2017 6 Bugelski & Alampay (1961) 2 October 2017 7 Jastrow (1899) 2 October 2017 8 Kremen (2010) 2 October 2017 9 2 October 2017 10 2 October 2017 11 • Introspection, Logic, & Philosophy • Plato’s Aviary metaphor 2 October 2017 12 “other animals (as well as man) have memory, but … none … except man, shares in the faculty of recollection” • Observation and Data Theories • Contiguity, Frequency, Similarity • All knowledge is innate, • Memory The Republic – Replication of sensory perception – Passive re-perception • Familiarity? • Intuition and Logic • Reminiscence – Replaying an entire experience – Temporal contiguity • Recollection? 2 October 2017 13 Mind Body Cogito ergo sum Stimulus, Response (reflex arc) (Descartes, 1637) Like a machine/clock Animal “Spirits” flow Knowledge is mostly innate 2 October 2017 14 • Absolute Power of the Monarchy – Isaac Newton’s Light and Robert Boyle’s chemicals • Associationism (Green, Bitter/Sour vs.
    [Show full text]
  • The Cultural Psychology of Suffering: the Many Meanings of Health in Orissa, India (And Elsewhere)
    60 ETHOS The Cultural Psychology of Suffering: The Many Meanings of Health in Orissa, India (and Elsewhere) Richard A. Shweder Abstract In this article, I honor Jerome Bruner’s meaning-centered and person-centered approach to the study of cultural psychology by describing aspects of the cultural psychology of suffering in and around a Hindu temple town in Orissa, India. I also outline the ‘‘big three’’ explanations of illness (biomedical, interpersonal, and moral) on a worldwide scale and recount some of the many meanings associated with the word health, as in the English language survey question ‘‘How would you rate your overall health?’’ [cultural psychology; explanations of illness; meanings of health; Orissa, India] Jerome Bruner is famous for his meaning-centered and person-centered approach to the study of cultural psychology (see, e.g., Bruner 1986, 1990, 1993).1 In this article, I seek to honor the life and work of my former teacher with a thickly substantive (and partly analytic) article on the many meanings of health for people in Orissa, India, where I started conducting research on cultural psychology while still a student taking courses with Jerome Bruner at Harvard University during the late 1960s. Before fully engaging my topic—the cultural psychology of suffering—allow me to glance back a few decades and share a pleasurable memory. At Harvard University in the late 1960s, the Department of Social Relations (where I was a student in the ‘‘Social Anthropology Wing’’) and the Department of Psychology (where Jerry Bruner was a faculty member and the director of the ‘‘Center for Cognitive Studies’’) were located in the vertically and intellectually segmented white towering structure, William James Hall.
    [Show full text]
  • An Introduction to Late British Associationism and Its Context This Is a Story About Philosophy. Or About Science
    Chapter 1: An Introduction to Late British Associationism and Its Context This is a story about philosophy. Or about science. Or about philosophy transforming into science. Or about science and philosophy, and how they are related. Or were, in a particular time and place. At least, that is the general area that the following narrative will explore, I hope with sufficient subtlety. The matter is rendered non-transparent by the fact that that the conclusions one draws with regard to such questions are, in part, matters of discretionary perspective – as I will try to demonstrate. My specific historical focus will be on the propagation of a complex intellectual tradition concerned with human sensation, perception, and mental function in early nineteenth century Britain. Not only philosophical opinion, but all aspects of British intellectual – and practical – life, were in the process of significant transformation during this time. This cultural flux further confuses recovery of the situated significance of the intellectual tradition I am investigating. The study of the mind not only was influenced by a set of broad social shifts, but it also participated in them fully as both stimulus to and recipient of changing conditions. One indication of this is simply the variety of terms used to identify the ‘philosophy of mind’ as an intellectual enterprise in the eighteenth and nineteenth centuries.1 But the issue goes far deeper into the fluid constitutive features of the enterprise - including associated conceptual systems, methods, intentions of practitioners, and institutional affiliations. In order to understand philosophy of mind in its time, we must put all these factors into play.
    [Show full text]
  • The History of Associationism and British Medical Psychology
    THE HISTORY OF ASSOCIATIONISM AND BRITISH MEDICAL PSYCHOLOGY by ROBERT HOELDTKE AT the beginning ofthe eighteenth century the time-honoured identification of mental disease with derangements of the humoral factors still prevailed. Another closely related, but less well known tradition localized diseases of the mind to specific mental faculties. Diseases ofthe imagination were thus distinct from diseases ofthejudgment, memory, or will. These ideas were based upon an implicit vitalism; i.e. the faculties were envisioned as free and active aspects of the mind in some way independent of the laws governing the material and biological world. Faculty psychology and its assumed vitalism are of particular importance to the present study since it was this school ofthought that the eighteenth century psychology of associations attempted to discredit. The complex history of this new approach is described in Warren's History ofAssociationism. Several distinct themes are present, the first and most fundamental element being John Locke's assertion that knowledge was based upon sensation. David Hume further developed this simple statement in his observation that the sensation of related objects became associated in the mind so that the subsequent sensation of only one of the related objects would bring about the recollection of the whole series. Finally Hartley explained associating sensation in terms of the Newtonian concepts of vibration and gravitation. In so doing he emphasized the materialistic flavour of the psychology, making it acceptable to the new science of physics, although still finding it possible to evade the charge of materialism. Despite this pious and ambivalent concession to the past, Hartley's thought represented a radical departure from the psychology of active faculties, which gradually became discredited by eighteenth-century physicians.
    [Show full text]
  • Hedonic Treadmill———419
    H-Baumeister (Encyc)-45348.qxd 7/24/2007 6:16 PM Page 419 Hedonic Treadmill———419 life-span perspective, from the time of conception report themselves as being much happier than other (How do prenatal stressors influence birth outcomes?) people, and those who were paralyzed in an accident to the end of life (How can clinicians help people die do not report themselves as being much less happy. with dignity?). Others are now discovering how cul- Similarly, as nations get wealthier, the reported well- tural and gender differences in lifestyles, stress reac- being of its citizens does not increase. tivity, and coping can influence health outcomes. As The lack of evidence for a relation between objec- the population ages and many develop chronic dis- tive circumstances and reported well-being has given eases, it will be increasingly important to focus on rise to the concept of a hedonic treadmill, on which health promotion and how to help individuals cope humans’ happiness remains stationary, despite efforts with their diagnoses and improve their quality of life. or interventions to advance it. The metaphor is also As the biopsychosocial model gains acceptance in interpreted to mean that humans’ happiness will decline the medical community, health psychologists have if their material circumstances remain constant. increasingly important roles to play on interdiscipli- The hedonic treadmill metaphor draws support nary teams of health care providers. Health psycholo- from adaptation in other domains. Pleasant smells gists have the potential to have a dramatic impact on usually become less intense (and less pleasurable) the health of individuals by conducting research that with continued exposure, and a 70° Fahrenheit room contributes knowledge of how psychosocial factors that initially feels delightful when one comes in from can influence behavioral and disease processes and by the cold ceases to confer pleasure after one has been intervening to promote health and prevent illness.
    [Show full text]
  • International Journal of Comparative Psychology, Vol
    International Journal of Comparative Psychology, Vol. 1 1, No. 3, 1998 MAKING PLACE FOR A COMPARATIVE SCIENTIST: ROBERT MEARNS YERKES AT HARVARD, 1902-1917 Randall D. Wight and Kristi A. Smith Ouachita Baptist University, U.S.A. ABSTRACT: During the first decade of this century, Robert Meams Yerkes struggled to make a place for himself within Harvard University's Division of Philosophy as a comparative scientist. From the perspective of a young assistant professor, Yerkes' diary and letters permit a glimpse into the ail-too familiar struggle of establishing career and family. Comparative psychology at the turn of the twentieth century was a relatively new discipline in the United States. The idea of combining psychology and biology into a single domain of study was foreign to many and few such researchers existed. Robert Meams Yerkes. Yerkes, who preferred being called a "psychobiologist" rather than a "psychologist" (e.g., Yerkes, 1932), focused his talent on advancing the comparative, psychological study of animals, on bridging the chasm between psychological and biological science, and on developing and promoting objective methodology (Yerkes, 1950). When Yerkes entered Harvard in the fall of 1887, he entered as a special undergraduate. He applied to Harvard to study medicine but was denied admission because Harvard did not then recognize the bachelor of arts degree Yerkes had earned the previous spring from Ursinus College in Pennsylvania. Thi; ^mporary setback would lead Yerkes to psychology. Yerkes' Harvaro advisor, and one of his favorite teachers, was Josiah Royce. Royce recognized Yerkes' abilities and interests. As Yerkes' undergraduate days grew to a close, Royce suggested Yerkes combine his biological, psychological, and Address correspondence to Randall D.
    [Show full text]