Plosives Vowels: No Obstruction of Airstream in the Vocal Tract BUT: Matter of Degree (Hardly Any Obstruction in the 3

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Plosives Vowels: No Obstruction of Airstream in the Vocal Tract BUT: Matter of Degree (Hardly Any Obstruction in the 3 Semester Outline Introduction to English Phonology and Phonetics 1. Phonetics and phonology: basics (& introducing transcription) 2. English consonants Dr. Nadja Nesselhauf 3. English vowels 4. Beyond the phoneme (connected speech, suprasegmentals etc.) 5. Accents of English English Consonants: Outline Vowels vs. Consonants Phonetic distinction: 1. Preliminaries (vowels & cons., fortis & lenis) Consonants: obstruction of airstream in the vocal tract 2. Plosives Vowels: no obstruction of airstream in the vocal tract BUT: matter of degree (hardly any obstruction in the 3. Fricatives articulation of approximants / v/ / q/ / i/) 4. Affricates Phonological distinction: Consonants: sounds that typically occur at the margins 5. Lateral and approximants (onsets and codas) of syllables Vowels: sounds that are typically central in a syllable 6. Nasals (occurring at the peak or nucleus) 1 The Syllable Vowels vs. Consonants Phonetic distinction: Consonants: obstruction of airstream in the vocal tract Syllable Vowels: no obstruction of airstream in the vocal tract BUT: matter of degree (hardly any obstruction in the articulation of approximants / v/ / q/ / i/) Onset Rhyme Phonological distinction: Consonants: sounds that typically occur at the margins Nucleus or Peak Coda (onsets and codas) of syllables Vowels: sounds that are typically central in a syllable (occurring at the peak or nucleus) e.g. P I N DR U NK English Consonants: Classification English Consonants: Fortis vs. Lenis Source: Sauer 2001, 15 Sauer Source: 2001, Fortis sounds: Lenis sounds: articulation with more muscular with less muscular effort effort and greater breath force and less breath force voiceless in all positions may be voiced fortis plosives are aspirated in are unaspirated syllable-initial position vowels are shortened before a vowels have full length fortis consonant before a final lenis cons. 2 English Consonants: Outline English Consonants: Plosives 1. Preliminaries (vowels & cons., fortis & lenis) Stages in the articulation of a plosive: 2. Plosives 1. the approach stage: the articulators come 3. Fricatives together and form the closure 4. Affricates 2. the hold stage: air is compressed behind the closure 5. Lateral and approximants 3. the release stage: the articulators part and the 6. Nasals compressed air is released rapidly English Consonants: Plosives English Consonants: Plosives Stages in the articulation of a plosive: English /p/ and /b/ (hold stage) APPROACH HOLD RELEASE Source: Collins/Mees 2003, 79 3 English Consonants: Plosives English Consonants: Plosives (hold stage) English /k/ and /g/ Question: What sounds (hold stage) are produced here? Source: Collins/Mees 2003, 79 Source: Collins/Mees 2003, 79 Plosives: Allophonic Variation Notation of contexts (Sauer) / in the context of __ space for symbol 1. types of release V vowel C consonant 2. degrees of devoicing FC fortis consonant LC lenis consonant # word boundary 3. degrees of aspiration ! stressed syllable . unstressed syllable ~ usually 4. places of articulation v voiced sound 4 Plosives: Allophonic Variation Plosives: Allophonic Variation Allophones of release (all plosives): Allophones of voice (lenis plosives): oral / __ ~ (put ) fully voiced / v __ v (lobby ) nasal / __ homorganic nasal (button ) part. devoiced / # __ (bus ) lateral / __ lateral (middle ) devoiced / __ # (cab ) none / __ plosive, affricate (wept ) Phonetic notation for devoicing: [afi] Contrasting German and English Contrasting German and English Auslautverhärtung (“final devoicing”) in German: Lenis plosives / __ # Lenis obstruents (i.e. plosives, fricatives, affricates) regularly become fortis in a syllable-final position English: devoiced, but still lenis in German (but not in English) e.g. Rad ( vs. Räder) : German: devoiced and fortis final plosive fortis [≤`9s ] Los (vs. Lose ) → “Auslautverhärtung ” final fricative fortis [r] Rad – Rat, Bund – bunt BUT: bud vs. but , had vs. hat 5 Plosives: Allophonic Variation Contrasting German and English Hermann the German and his English landlady Meg (adapted from: Eckert & Barry 2005, The Phonetics and Phonology of Aspiration = delayed voice onset time (VOT) English Pronunciation, 50f.) Source: Collins/Mees 2003, 82 2003, Collins/Mees Source: H: I would like a cup of tea. Is this your muck ? M: I beg your pardon? H: Is it ok if I use yours? I can‘t find my muck just now. M: Well, it‘s all over the place… oh sorry, Herman, you mean mug, don‘t you? H: Do I? Eh, could you pass me the slice of cheese that I put in your paperback ? M: What??? Listen, if you need a bookmark I‘d be really grateful if you‘d use a piece of paper and not a slice of cheese. Not in my books anyway. PA: passive articulator H: Who‘s talking about books? I said pa-per-back. AA: active articulator VF: vocal folds Plosives: Allophonic Variation Plosives: Allophonic Variation Allophones of aspiration (fortis plosives): Task: Say the following words holding your hand in front of fully aspirated / __ !V(push ) your mouth: … and devoicing following sound / __ !l, r, j, w ( please ) sting part. aspirated / __ .V(supper ) pain not asp. (exception to above!) /s __ ( spin, spleen, Easter ) rapp er /__ # discussion Notation: [ jg] etc. Do you feel a puff of air coming out of your mouth (yes, no, a slight one)? / Should you feel one? 6 Plosives: Allophonic Variation Plosives: Allophonic Variation Place of articulation – ex: phonemes /t, d/: T-flapping in General American [π]: /t/ → flap / V __ .V (better ) dental / __ C, S/ (eighth, width ) / r __ .V (party ) postalveolar / __ r/ (tray, drive ) / __ kÿÿ, qÿ, lÿ (bottle, shutter, bottom ) (cf. “Assimilation”, Part IV) flap → Ø / n __ .V (winter ) Plosives: Allophonic Variation Source: Fromkin/Rodman 1998, 289 1998, Fromkin/Rodman Source: D-flapping in GA in the same contexts as t-flapping (cf. ladder, weirdo, saddle ) -> NEUTRALISATION of the contrast /t-d/ in the relevant contexts -> homophones such as shutter – shudder, writing - riding etc. 7 English Consonants: Plosives The pronunciation of the (regular) past tense / past participle morpheme : /Hc / / d,t __ (ended ) {p.t.}, {p.p.} /s/ / FC __ (laughed ) /c/ / ~ (sobbed ) Source: Fromkin/Rodman 1998, 295 Transcription Practice Plosives: Glottal Stop Please transcribe the following past tense forms: Glottal stop [ >]: articulation looked decided kissed allowed hummed dreamt Source: Collins/Mees 2003, 29 8 Plosives: Glottal Stop Plosives: Glottal Stop Source: Collins/Mees 2003, 145 2003, Collins/Mees Source: Glottal Stop [ >]: Glottal Stop [ >]: - not a phoneme in RP or GA Some British - serves to reinforce fortis plosives: ( lap ) accents replace Source: Collins/Mees 2003, 82 2003, Collins/Mees Source: /t/ with [ >] in medial and final positions (‘a bit of butter ’) English Consonants: Outline Contrasting German and English German: glottal onset (= glottal stop before 1. Preliminaries (vowels & cons., fortis & lenis) vowels) much more frequent than in English; cf. >Alle >Autos >ärgern mich 2. Plosives In English glottal onset mainly reserved for 3. Fricatives emphasised words ( I‘m >ALWAYS angry at Ann ) 4. Affricates -> Tendency of German-speaking learners of 5. Lateral and approximants English to insert a glottal stop at the beginning of all words which begin with a vowel (cf. also 6. Nasals phrasal verbs: go in , put off) 9 English Consonants: Fricatives English Consonants: Fortis vs. Lenis English fricative phonemes: Fortis sounds: Lenis sounds: articulation with more muscular with less muscular effort labio-dental / e,u/ effort and greater breath force and less breath force voiceless in all positions may be voiced dental / S,C/ (seal )(zeal ) alveolar / r,y/ --- --- palato-alveolar / R,Y/ vowels are shortened before a vowels have full length fortis consonant before a final lenis cons. glottal / g/ (safe )(save ) Contrasting English and German Fricatives /f,v/ German: Source: Collins/MeesSource: 2008, 88 <v>, <w> → /v/ (Vase , Wind ) English: <v> → /v/ (vase ) <w> → /w/ (wind ) → both a general use of /v/ and hypercorrective use of /w/ occur among German learners 10 /v/ vs. /w/ Fricatives Task: Read the following sentences aloud to 86 2003, Collins/Mees Source: your neighbour and have her/him check Question: which whether all the <v>s are pronounced as fricatives /v/ and all the <w>s as /w/. are produced here? This is the worst university building I‘ve ever walked into. But at least in winter, it‘s very warm. Fricatives / SSS,CCC/ Pronouncing the Grapheme <th> Source: Collins/Mees 2003, 86 2003, Collins/Mees Source: I word-initially: /S/ in lexical words (think, theatre, thunder ) /C/ in function words ( they, the, that, their ) II word-medially: /S/ usu. in words from Greek/Latin ( author, method, lethal ) /C/ usu. in Germanic words ( father, leather, mother, other; BUT: rhythm ) III word-finally: /S/ often if spelled <th> (both, path, mouth, breath ) /C/ often if spelled <the> ( breathe, clothe; BUT: smooth ) 11 /SSS/ or / CCC/? Fricatives /s,z/ Source: Collins/Mees 2003, 86 2003, Collins/Mees Source: Task: Read the following sentence and indicate how the grapheme <th> is pronounced in each instance: Then, the eldest brother thought they were both trying to attract their father‘s sympathy. English Consonants: Fricatives Pronouncing the Grapheme <s> I word-initially: /e,u/ slit fricatives (tongue is /s/ almost always (otherwise <z>) ( sing, sand ) exceptions: sure, sugar /S,C/ relatively flat) II word-medially: /z/ mostly, esp. between vowels & esp. in Germanic words ( busy, ---------------------------------------------------
Recommended publications
  • LINGUISTICS 221 LECTURE #3 the BASIC SOUNDS of ENGLISH 1. STOPS a Stop Consonant Is Produced with a Complete Closure of Airflow
    LINGUISTICS 221 LECTURE #3 Introduction to Phonetics and Phonology THE BASIC SOUNDS OF ENGLISH 1. STOPS A stop consonant is produced with a complete closure of airflow in the vocal tract; the air pressure has built up behind the closure; the air rushes out with an explosive sound when released. The term plosive is also used for oral stops. ORAL STOPS: e.g., [b] [t] (= plosives) NASAL STOPS: e.g., [m] [n] (= nasals) There are three phases of stop articulation: i. CLOSING PHASE (approach or shutting phase) The articulators are moving from an open state to a closed state; ii. CLOSURE PHASE (= occlusion) Blockage of the airflow in the oral tract; iii. RELEASE PHASE Sudden reopening; it may be accompanied by a burst of air. ORAL STOPS IN ENGLISH a. BILABIAL STOPS: The blockage is made with the two lips. spot [p] voiceless baby [b] voiced 1 b. ALVEOLAR STOPS: The blade (or the tip) of the tongue makes a closure with the alveolar ridge; the sides of the tongue are along the upper teeth. lamino-alveolar stops or Check your apico-alveolar stops pronunciation! stake [t] voiceless deep [d] voiced c. VELAR STOPS: The closure is between the back of the tongue (= dorsum) and the velum. dorso-velar stops scar [k] voiceless goose [g] voiced 2. NASALS (= nasal stops) The air is stopped in the oral tract, but the velum is lowered so that the airflow can go through the nasal tract. All nasals are voiced. NASALS IN ENGLISH a. BILABIAL NASAL: made [m] b. ALVEOLAR NASAL: need [n] c.
    [Show full text]
  • English Phonetic Vowel Shortening and Lengthening As Perceptually Active for the Poles
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE Title: English phonetic vowel shortening and lengthening as perceptually active for the Poles Author: Arkadiusz Rojczyk Citation style: Rojczyk Arkadiusz. (2007). English phonetic vowel shortening and lengthening as perceptually active for the Poles. W: J. Arabski (red.), "On foreign language acquisition and effective learning" (S. 237-249). Katowice : Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Śląskiego Phonological subsystem English phonetic vowel shortening and lengthening as perceptually active for the Poles Arkadiusz Rojczyk University of Silesia, Katowice 1. Introduction Auditory perception is one of the most thriving and active domains of psy­ cholinguistics. Recent years have witnessed manifold attempts to investigate and understand how humans perceive linguistic sounds and what principles govern the process. The major assumption underlying the studies on auditory perception is a simple fact that linguistic sounds are means of conveying meaning, encoded by a speaker, to a hearer. The speaker, via articulatory gestures, transmits a message encoded in acoustic waves. This is the hearer, on the other hand, who, using his perceptual apparatus, reads acoustic signals and decodes them into the meaning. Therefore, in phonetic terms, the whole communicative act can be divided into three stages. Articulatory - the speaker encodes the message by sound production. Acoustic - the message is transmitted to the hearer as acous­ tic waves. Auditory - the hearer perceives sounds and decodes them into the meaning. Each of the three stages is indispensable for effective communication. Of the three aforementioned stages of communication, the auditory percep­ tion is the least amenable to precise depiction and understanding.
    [Show full text]
  • Part 1: Introduction to The
    PREVIEW OF THE IPA HANDBOOK Handbook of the International Phonetic Association: A guide to the use of the International Phonetic Alphabet PARTI Introduction to the IPA 1. What is the International Phonetic Alphabet? The aim of the International Phonetic Association is to promote the scientific study of phonetics and the various practical applications of that science. For both these it is necessary to have a consistent way of representing the sounds of language in written form. From its foundation in 1886 the Association has been concerned to develop a system of notation which would be convenient to use, but comprehensive enough to cope with the wide variety of sounds found in the languages of the world; and to encourage the use of thjs notation as widely as possible among those concerned with language. The system is generally known as the International Phonetic Alphabet. Both the Association and its Alphabet are widely referred to by the abbreviation IPA, but here 'IPA' will be used only for the Alphabet. The IPA is based on the Roman alphabet, which has the advantage of being widely familiar, but also includes letters and additional symbols from a variety of other sources. These additions are necessary because the variety of sounds in languages is much greater than the number of letters in the Roman alphabet. The use of sequences of phonetic symbols to represent speech is known as transcription. The IPA can be used for many different purposes. For instance, it can be used as a way to show pronunciation in a dictionary, to record a language in linguistic fieldwork, to form the basis of a writing system for a language, or to annotate acoustic and other displays in the analysis of speech.
    [Show full text]
  • An Acoustic Account of the Allophonic Realization of /T/ Amber King St
    Linguistic Portfolios Volume 1 Article 12 2012 An Acoustic Account of the Allophonic Realization of /T/ Amber King St. Cloud State University Ettien Koffi St. Cloud State University Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.stcloudstate.edu/stcloud_ling Part of the Applied Linguistics Commons Recommended Citation King, Amber and Koffi, Ettien (2012) "An Acoustic Account of the Allophonic Realization of /T/," Linguistic Portfolios: Vol. 1 , Article 12. Available at: https://repository.stcloudstate.edu/stcloud_ling/vol1/iss1/12 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by theRepository at St. Cloud State. It has been accepted for inclusion in Linguistic Portfolios by an authorized editor of theRepository at St. Cloud State. For more information, please contact [email protected]. King and Koffi: An Acoustic Account of the Allophonic Realization of /T/ AN ACOUSTIC ACCOUNT OF THE ALLOPHONIC REALIZATIONS OF /T/ AMBER KING AND ETTIEN KOFFI 1.0 Introduction This paper is a laboratory phonology account of the different pronunciations of the phoneme /t/. Laboratory phonology is a relatively new analytical tool that is being used to validate and verify claims made by phonologists about the pronunciation of sounds. It is customary for phonologists to predict on the basis of auditory impressions and intuition alone that allophones exist for such and such phonemes. An allophone is defined as different realizations of the same phoneme based on the environments in which it occurs. For instance, it has been proposed that the phoneme /t/ has anywhere from four to eight allophones in General American English (GAE). To verify this claim Amber, one of the co-author of this paper recorded herself saying the words <still>, <Tim>, <kit>, <bitter>, <kitten>, <winter>, <fruition>, <furniture>, and <listen>.
    [Show full text]
  • Consonants Consonants Vs. Vowels Formant Frequencies Place Of
    The Acoustics of Speech Production: Source-Filter Theory of Speech Consonants Production Source Filter Speech Speech production can be divided into two independent parts •Sources of sound (i.e., signals) such as the larynx •Filters that modify the source (i.e., systems) such as the vocal tract Consonants Consonants Vs. Vowels All three sources are used • Frication Vowels Consonants • Aspiration • Voicing • Slow changes in • Rapid changes in articulators articulators Articulations change resonances of the vocal tract • Resonances of the vocal tract are called formants • Produced by with a • Produced by making • Moving the tongue, lips and jaw change the shape of the vocal tract relatively open vocal constrictions in the • Changing the shape of the vocal tract changes the formant frequencies tract vocal tract Consonants are created by coordinating changes in the sources with changes in the filter (i.e., formant frequencies) • Only the voicing • Coordination of all source is used three sources (frication, aspiration, voicing) Formant Frequencies Place of Articulation The First Formant (F1) • Affected by the size of Velar Alveolar the constriction • Cue for manner • Unrelated to place Bilabial The second and third formants (F2 and F3) • Affected by place of articulation /AdA/ 1 Place of Articulation Place of Articulation Bilabials (e.g., /b/, /p/, /m/) -- Low Frequencies • Lower F2 • Lower F3 Alveolars (e.g., /d/, /n/, /s/) -- High Frequencies • Higher F2 • Higher F3 Velars (e.g., /g/, /k/) -- Middle Frequencies • Higher F2 /AdA/ /AgA/ • Lower
    [Show full text]
  • Nasal Consonant the Basic Characteristic of a Nasal Consonant Is That the Air Escapes Through the Nose
    Nasal Consonant The basic characteristic of a nasal consonant is that the air escapes through the nose. For this to happen, the soft palate must be lowered; in the case of all the other consonants, and all vowels, the soft palate is raised and air cannot pass through the nose, in nasal consonants, hoever, the air does not pass through the mouth; it is prevented by a complete closure in the mouth at some point. /m/ and /n/ are simple , straightforward consonants with distributions like those of the plosives. There is in fact little to describe. However, /η/ is a different matter. It is a sound that gives considerable problems to foreign learners, and one that is so unusual in its phonological aspect that some people argue that it is not one of the phonemes of English at all. There are three phonemes in English which are represented by nasal consonants, /m/ , /n/ and /η/. In all nasal consonants the soft palate is lowered and at the same time the mouth passage blocked at some point, so that all the air pushed out of the nose. /m/ and /n/ All languages have consonants which are similar to / m/ and /n/ in English. Notice: 1- the soft palate is lowered for both /m/ and /n/. 2- for /m/ the mouth is blocked by closing the two lips, for /n/ by pressing the tip of the tongue against the alveolar ridge, and the sides of the tongue against the sides of the palate. 3- Both sounds are voice in English, as they are in other languages, and the voiced air passes out through the nose.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 9 Consonant Substitution in Child Language (Ikwere) Roseline I
    Chapter 9 Consonant substitution in child language (Ikwere) Roseline I. C. Alerechi University of Port Harcourt The Ikwere language is spoken in four out of the twenty-three Local Government Areas (LGAs) of Rivers State of Nigeria, namely, Port Harcourt, Obio/Akpor, Emohua and Ikwerre LGAs. Like Kana, Kalabari and Ekpeye, it is one of the major languages of Rivers State of Nigeria used in broadcasting in the electronic media. The Ikwere language is classified asan Igboid language of the West Benue-Congo family of the Niger-Congo phylum of languages (Williamson 1988: 67, 71, Williamson & Blench 2000: 31). This paper treats consonant substi- tution in the speech of the Ikwere child. It demonstrates that children use of a language can contribute to the divergent nature of that language as they always strive for simplification of the target language. Using simple descriptive method of data analysis, the paper identifies the various substitutions of consonant sounds, which characterize the Ikwere children’s ut- terances. It stresses that the substitutions are regular and rule governed and hence implies the operation of some phonological processes. Some of the processes are strengthening and weakening of consonants, loss of suction of labial implosives causing them to become labial plosives, devoicing of voiced consonants, etc. While some of these processes are identical with the adult language, others are peculiar to children, demonstrating the relationships between the phonological processes in both forms of speech. It is worthy of note that high- lighting the relationships and differences will make for effective communication between children and adults. 1 Introduction The Ikwere language is spoken in four out of the twenty-three Local Government Areas (LGAs) of Rivers State of Nigeria, namely, Port Harcourt, Obio/Akpor, Emohua and Ik- werre LGAs.
    [Show full text]
  • Velar Segments in Old English and Old Irish
    In: Jacek Fisiak (ed.) Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Historical Linguistics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 1985, 267-79. Velar segments in Old English and Old Irish Raymond Hickey University of Bonn The purpose of this paper is to look at a section of the phoneme inventories of the oldest attested stage of English and Irish, velar segments, to see how they are manifested phonetically and to consider how they relate to each other on the phonological level. The reason I have chosen to look at two languages is that it is precisely when one compares two language systems that one notices that structural differences between languages on one level will be correlated by differences on other levels demonstrating their interrelatedness. Furthermore it is necessary to view segments on a given level in relation to other segments. The group under consideration here is just one of several groups. Velar segments viewed within the phonological system of both Old English and Old Irish cor relate with three other major groups, defined by place of articulation: palatals, dentals, and labials. The relationship between these groups is not the same in each language for reasons which are morphological: in Old Irish changes in grammatical category are frequently indicated by palatalizing a final non-palatal segment (labial, dental, or velar). The same function in Old English is fulfilled by suffixes and /or prefixes. This has meant that for Old English the phonetically natural and lower-level alternation of velar elements with palatal elements in a palatal environ ment was to be found whereas in Old Irish this alternation had been denaturalized and had lost its automatic character.
    [Show full text]
  • Locutour Guide to Letters, Sounds, and Symbols
    LOCUTOUROUR® Guide to Letters, Sounds, and Symbols LOCUTOUR ABEL LASSIFICATION LACE OR RTICULATION PELLED AS XAMPLES S PELLINGP E L L I N G L C P A IPA S E Consonants p bilabial plosive voiceless lips /p/ p, pp pit, puppy b bilabial plosive voiced lips /b/ b, bb bat, ebb t lingua-alveolar plosive voiceless tongue tip + upper gum ridge /t/ t, ed, gth, th, tt, tw tan, tipped, tight, thyme, attic, two d lingua-alveolar plosive voiced tongue tip + upper gum ridge /d/ d, dd, ed dad, ladder, bagged k lingua-velar plosive voiceless back of tongue and soft palate /k/ c, cc, ch, ck cab, occur, school, duck que, k mosque, kit g lingua-velar plosive voiced back of tongue and soft palate /g/ g, gg, gh, gu, gue hug, bagged, Ghana, guy, morgue f labiodental fricative voiceless lower lip + upper teeth /f/ f, ff , gh, ph feet, fl u uff, enough, phone v labiodental fricative voiced lower lip + upper teeth /v/ v, vv, f, ph vet, savvy, of, Stephen th linguadental fricative voiceless tongue + teeth /T/ th thin th linguadental fricative voiced tongue + teeth /D/ th, the them, bathe s lingua-alveolar fricative voiceless tongue tip + upper gum ridge or /s/ s, ss, sc, ce, ci, sit, hiss, scenic, ace, city tongue tip + lower gum ridge cy, ps, z cycle, psychology, pizza z lingua-alveolar fricative voiced tongue tip + upper gum ridge or /z/ z, zz, s, ss, x, cz Zen, buzz, is, scissors, tongue tip + lower gum ridge Xerox, czar sh linguapalatal fricative voiceless tongue blade and hard palate /S/ sh, ce, ch, ci, sch, si, sheep, ocean, chef, glacier, kirsch ss, su,
    [Show full text]
  • Comparative Analysis of American English and Mexican Spanish Consonants for Computer Assisted Pronunciation Training*
    REVISTA SIGNOS. ESTUDIOS DE LINGÜÍSTICA ISSN 0718-0934 © 2017 PUCV, Chile DOI: 10.4067/S0718-09342017000200195 50(94) 195-216 Comparative analysis of American English and Mexican Spanish consonants for Computer Assisted Pronunciation Training* Análisis comparativo de las consonantes del inglés americano y español mexicano para la enseñanza de la pronunciación del inglés asistida por computadora Olga Kolesnikova INSTITUTO POLITÉCNICO NACIONAL MÉXICO [email protected] Recibido: 17-X-2014 / Aceptado: 30-VIII-2016 Abstract The objective of this work is two-fold. Firstly, we aim at detecting similarities and differences between the consonant systems of two languages, namely, American English and Mexican Spanish. To achieve this, we perform a theoretic comparative analysis of consonants of the two languages at the level of both phonemes and allophones. Secondly, a possible practical usage of our results is considered; therefore, as an example of an application, we consider computer-assisted pronunciation training (CAPT) for teaching American English pronunciation to Mexican Spanish speakers. In particular, we took advantage of the results of our analysis to define some hypothetic error patterns which can be used as a starting point for diagnosing possible mispronunciations, their posterior verification, and adjustment taking into account the principles of phonotactics and empirical phonetic analysis of the English learners’ speech. The latter will result in error rules to be applied in a CAPT system for error identification and generation of appropriate corrective feedback. An adequate choice of correcting techniques will improve English pronunciation acquisition and help learners to develop less accented speech. Also, similarities found between the two consonant systems make it possible to organize and present the pronunciation teaching material using a stress-free method of helping learners to adjust their speech organs to new sounds building on the phonetic habits of their first language.
    [Show full text]
  • Studies in African Linguistics Volume 21, Number 3, December 1990
    Studies in African Linguistics Volume 21, Number 3, December 1990 CONTEXTUAL LABIALIZATION IN NA WURI* Roderic F. Casali Ghana Institute of Linguistics Literacy and Bible Translation and UCLA A spectrographic investigation into the non-contrastive labialization of consonants before round vowels in Nawuri (a Kwa language of Ghana) sup­ ports the notion that this labialization is the result of a phonological, feature­ spreading rule and not simply an automatic transitional process. This as­ sumption is further warranted in that it allows for a more natural treatment of some other phonological processes in the language. The fact that labial­ ization before round vowels is generally not very audible is explained in terms of a principle of speech perception. A final topic addressed is the question of why (both in Nawuri and apparently in a number of other Ghanaian languages as well) contextual labialization does tend to be more perceptible in certain restricted environments. o. Introduction This paper deals with the allophonic labialization of consonants before round vowels in Nawuri, a Kwa language of Ghana.! While such labialization is gener­ ally not very audible, spectrographic evidence suggests that it is strongly present, * The spectrograms in this study were produced at the phonetics lab of the University of Texas at Arlington using equipment provided through a grant of the Permanent University Fund of the University of Texas system. I would like to thank the following people for their valuable comments and suggestions: Joan Baart, Don Burquest, Mike Cahill, Jerry Edmondson, Norris McKinney, Bob Mugele, Tony Naden, and Keith Snider. I would also like to express my appreciation to Russell Schuh and an anonymous referee for this journal for their helpful criticism of an earlier version, and to Mary Steele for some helpful discussion concerning labialization in Konkomba.
    [Show full text]
  • Ling 205 (T.Nearey) Summary of English Allophone Rules Page 1 of 3
    Ling 205 (T.Nearey) Summary of English allophone rules Page 1 of 3 Summary of important allophones and their most likely environments N= may be tested in ‘live’ narrow transcription G = ‘general knowledge’ may be tested in multiple choice, short answer or other focused questions (and may appear in homework assignments) CONSONANTS Aspiration of voiceless stops (N, G) Aspiration of voiceless stops (transcribed with raised h). Voiceless stops are: 1) Always aspirated: word initially OR at beginning a syllable with a stressed vowel. 2) Optionally aspirated word finally before a pause. 3) NEVER aspirated before a vowel or sonorant following /s/ in same syllable. 4) Usually NOT aspirated it occurs in other environments it is usually unaspirated or weakly aspirated. 5) Optionally aspirated elsewhere ( but not often, except for emphasis). Flapping (N,G) Alveolar stops /t/ and /d/ are realized as flaps (taps): 6) Usually: when both after a primary stressed vowel and before a reduced syllable. 7) Optionally: when both after a primary stressed vowel and a non-primary stressed vowel. Inaudible release of stops (G, N with directed attention) Stops show inaudible release or ‘are unreleased’ ( e.g. [p|, t|, d|, g|] , etc). 8) Usually inaudibly released: when preceding another stop. 9) Optionally inaudibly released: word finally before pause. Glottal stop 10) Almost always: /t/ is realized as a glottal stop [ /], (or perhaps as a simultaneously articulated [/] + [t] ) before syllabic [n`] , as in ‘button’ /»b!t´n/ --> [»b!/n`] or [»b!!"t n`] Special release (G) 11) Always: /d/ before /n/ or syllabic /n/ shows nasal release [d<], 12) Always: /d/ before /l/ (and possibly syllabic /l/ shows lateral release [dl] Note: Some dialects may also allow /t/ to show lateral release.
    [Show full text]