Ecological Grief As a Mental Health Response to Climate Change-Related Loss

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Ecological Grief As a Mental Health Response to Climate Change-Related Loss FOCUS | PERSPECTIVE https://doi.org/10.1038/s41558-018-0092-2PERSPECTIVE https://doi.org/10.1038/s41558-018-0092-2 Ecological grief as a mental health response to climate change-related loss Ashlee Cunsolo 1* and Neville R. Ellis 2 Climate change is increasingly understood to impact mental health through multiple pathways of risk, including intense feelings of grief as people suffer climate-related losses to valued species, ecosystems and landscapes. Despite growing research inter- est, ecologically driven grief, or ‘ecological grief’, remains an underdeveloped area of inquiry. We argue that grief is a natural and legitimate response to ecological loss, and one that may become more common as climate impacts worsen. Drawing upon our own research in Northern Canada and the Australian Wheatbelt, combined with a synthesis of the literature, we offer future research directions for the study of ecological grief. ny reading of the ecological literature makes clear that ours aid identification of what climate-related losses matter to people; is a time of significant ecological loss. Evidence accrues that and to identify opportunities to cope with or heal ecological grief Athe sixth mass extinction is well under way, that global eco- and human suffering due to these ecological losses. system productivity is in steep decline, and that the biosphere as We begin by examining the application of ecological grief within a whole is becoming irreparably damaged by human actions1–3. In scientific research exploring the mental health implications of cli- addition to being the subject of intense scientific scrutiny, global mate change. We draw primarily (though not exclusively) upon our environmental change and regional ecological decline are increas- own multi-year research programmes around climate change-driven ingly embedded within everyday experience4, evoking strong men- mental, emotional and grief responses in Northern Canada (by tal and emotional responses. A. Cunsolo) and the Australian Wheatbelt (by N. Ellis) (see also In response, the mental health implications of global environ- Table 1). Building on this research, as well as a synthesis of avail- mental change are gaining increasing research attention. This is able relevant literature, we then offer a broad research agenda for particularly the case over the last decade, which has seen grow- ecological grief that calls for an expansion of the geographic and cul- ing efforts to understand the mental health implications of climate tural application of ecological grief concepts, deep engagement with change. Climate-related weather events and environmental changes, place and land, and engagement with the emerging policy domain for example, have been linked to a wide variety of acute and chronic of climate change loss and damage. Throughout, we note that under- mental health experiences, including: strong emotional responses, standing the multitude of triggers for ecological grief, and the myr- such as sadness, distress, despair, anger, fear, helplessness, hopeless- iad ways in which people experience and express this grief, requires ness and stress; elevated rates of mood disorders, such as depres- a pluralistic and interdisciplinary approach, bringing together at sion, anxiety, and pre- and post-traumatic stress; increased drug times differing and at times complementary research methods, and alcohol usage; increased suicide ideation, attempts and death disciplinary perspectives and lived experiences. We conclude by by suicide; threats and disruptions to sense of place and place reflecting upon what ecological grief means for how we think about attachment; and loss of personal or cultural identity and ways individual and collective mental well-being in the Anthropocene of knowing5–19. era20,21, and for supporting the resourcefulness of individuals and An important concept emerging from peoples’ lived experiences communities increasingly at risk from climate change impacts. of climate change directly related to mental health, but not well rep- resented in the current literature, is what we term ecological grief Situating ecological grief — the grief felt in relation to experienced or anticipated ecologi- Grief is a natural human response to loss. To grieve the loss of a cal losses, including the loss of species, ecosystems and meaning- loved one is a common human experience, and one that all of us will ful landscapes due to acute or chronic environmental change. We encounter throughout the course of our lifetimes22. From a develop- contend that ecological grief is a natural response to ecological mental perspective, grief is the internal physiological and emotional losses, particularly for people who retain close living, working and responses to loss, and mourning is the period of mental, emotional cultural relationships to the natural environment, and one that has and personal transition as people learn to live again in the context of the potential to be felt more strongly and by a growing number of loss23. The processes of grieving and mourning can take many forms, people as we move deeper into the Anthropocene. differ across cultures, vary greatly among individuals, and even be To date, very little research has considered ecological grief an experienced differently by the same individual each time a different area of formal scientific inquiry, although the terms ‘grief’ and loss is encountered. Although processes of grief and mourning are ‘mourning’ are finding increased application in the description of well understood in the psychological literature in response to the people’s lived experiences and personal responses to environmen- loss of a loved person, rarely are these concepts extended to losses tal change20. In this Perspective, we argue that ecological grief is encountered in the natural world20. an important emergent area for psychological and geographical We consider ecological grief to be a form of “disenfranchised inquiry that has potential to shed light on personal and collective grief” or a grief that isn’t publicly or openly acknowledged24. Indeed, responses to ecological loss. Further, a better understanding of eco- ecological grief, and the associated work of mourning, experienced logical grief has the potential to enhance understanding of the emo- in response to ecological losses are often left unconsidered, or tional and psychological dimensions of climate change impacts; to entirely absent, in climate change narratives, policy and research25,26. 1Labrador Institute of Memorial University, Happy Valley-Goose Bay, Labrador, Canada. 2UWA School of Agriculture and Environment, The University of Western Australia, Perth, Western Australia, Australia. *e-mail: [email protected] NATURE CLIMATE CHANGE | VOL 8 | APRIL 2018 | 275–281 | www.nature.com/natureclimatechange 275 © 2018 Macmillan Publishers Limited, part of Springer Nature. All rights reserved. PERSPECTIVE | FOCUS NATURE CLIMATE CHANGE Yet,https://doi.org/10.1038/s41558-018-0092-2 acknowledging hitherto unacknowledged forms of grief brings mourning were prominent and recurring themes as people strug- to light values and objects that are often considered outside the gled to make sense of the environmental changes wrought upon scope of human care and, by association, ethical responsibility. As their loved home environments10,11,13,35. discussed by Butler27, for example, experiences of grief and mourn- In a synthesis of our own research, the available literature, reports ing illuminate our relational ties and fundamental dependency and the media, we highlight three climate-related contexts in which upon complex ecological communities and, in turn, our ethical ecological grief has been reported: grief associated with physical eco- and political responsibilities to these systems. For Butler, and oth- logical losses (land, ecosystems and species), grief associated with dis- ers28–30, grief and mourning have ‘we-creating’ capacities, exposing ruptions to environmental knowledge and loss of identity, and grief our known, unknown and unacknowledged connections to others, associated with anticipated future ecological losses (Table 1). These and allowing for opportunities to reach across differences to con- categories are ordered from most researched (or most reported) in nect with others. In this light, grief and mourning can also question the literature to the least. It is important to note that these categories fundamental assumptions about what we choose to value — and overlap, and are unlikely to fully capture the entire range and com- what we choose to grieve and mourn — including climate change- plexity of ecological grief experiences; however, they provide a useful induced ecological loss and degradation. starting point for future deliberations and research inquiry. Among the first to describe the emotional pain of experienced ecological loss was ecologist and conservationist Aldo Leopold Grief associated with physical ecological losses. This form of eco- (1953)31, who noted that “one of the penalties of an ecological educa- logical grief is associated with the physical disappearance, degradation tion is that one lives alone in a world of wounds”. Similar sentiments and/or death of species, ecosystems and landscapes, and is driven by have been expressed more recently by eminent ecologists, social climate change in several ways. First, ecological grief has been shown scientists and climate researchers32–34, who invoke concepts of grief to emerge in the aftermath of acute weather-related disasters (that and
Recommended publications
  • Navigating the Genealogies of Trauma, Guilt, and Affect: an Interview with Ruth Leys
    Navigating the Genealogies of Trauma, Guilt, and Affect: An Interview with Ruth Leys Ruth Leys Marlene Goldman University of Toronto Quarterly, Volume 79, Number 2, Spring 2010, pp. 656-679 (Article) Published by University of Toronto Press For additional information about this article http://muse.jhu.edu/journals/utq/summary/v079/79.2.leys.html Access Provided by Duke University Libraries at 08/29/11 12:38PM GMT RUTH LEYS AND MARLENE GOLDMAN Navigating the Genealogies of Trauma, Guilt, and Affect: An Interview with Ruth Leys ABSTRACT In this interview, Ruth Leys discusses her career as a historian of science and her research on contemporary developments in the human sciences, including Trauma: A Genealogy, From Guilt to Shame: Auschwitz and After, and her current work on the genealogy of experimental and theoretical approaches to the affects from the 1960s to the present. Among the topics she covers are her inves- tigation of the role of imitation or mimesis in trauma theory; why shame has replaced guilt as a dominant emotional reference in the West; the ways in which the shift from notions of guilt to notions of shame has involved a shift from concern about actions, or what you do, to a concern about identity, or who you are; why the shift from agency to identity has produced as one of its consequences the replacement of the idea of the meaning of a person’s inten- tions and actions by the idea of the primacy of a person’s affective experience; the significance of the recent “turn to affect” in cultural theory; and why the new affect theorists are committed to the view that the affect system is funda- mentally independent of intention and meaning because they view it is a material system of the body.
    [Show full text]
  • The Affective Turn in Ethnomusicology
    Ana Hofman The Affective Turn in Ethnomusicology DOI: 10.2298/MUZ1518035H UDK: 781.7:159.942 The Affective Turn in Ethnomusicology Ana Hofman1 The Institute of Cultural and Memory Studies Research Centre of Slovenian Academy of Sciences and Arts Abstract The affective turn, which has already questioned dominant paradigms in many disciplinary fields including cultural studies, philosophy, political theory, anthropology, psychology and neuroscience, has started to attract more attention in the field of ethnomusicology, becoming a particularly vibrant stream of thought. Drawing on the voices that call for the historicisation of and critical deliberation on the field of affect studies, the article strives to show how theories of affect might expand dominant paradigms in ethnomusicology and also points to their limitations. Key words Affective turn, ethnomusicology, sonic affect, musical emotions, affect-emotion relationship “Everything that occurs without praiseworthy affects [in music] can be considered nothing, does nothing and means nothing” (Der vollkommene Capellmeister, Johann Mattheson, 1739). Music has often been taken as an example of the power of affect. Its ubiquitously distributive affective potential has become pivotal in the works of scholars advocating the affective turn.2 Spurred by the poststructuralist orientation towards language, representation, deconstruction and psychoanalysis, a perspective in which body, emotions and embodiment have been neglected (see Clough and Halley 2007), the affective turn shifted the focus to pre-, extra-, and paralinguistic aspects and introduced a non-discursive, non- representational approach (see Thrift 2007). Developed mainly in the 2000s and partly inspired by research on the emotions and the body conducted in feminist and queer studies, affect theory is currently attracting growing interest in a variety of disciplinary fields.
    [Show full text]
  • Dissertation Clarifying the Construct of Compassion
    DISSERTATION CLARIFYING THE CONSTRUCT OF COMPASSION: ABILITY TO DOWNREGULATE EMOTION AS A POTENTIAL MEDIATOR BETWEEN EMPATHIC AROUSAL AND COMPASSION Submitted by Leslie A. Merriman Department of Psychology In partial fulfillment of the requirements For the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy Colorado State University Fort Collins, Colorado Spring 2017 Doctoral Committee: Advisor: Kathryn Rickard Chris Allen Ernest Chavez Caridad Souza Copyright by Leslie A. Merriman 2017 All Rights Reserved ABSTRACT CLARIFYING THE CONSTRUCT OF COMPASSION: ABILITY TO DOWNREGULATE EMOTION AS A POTENTIAL MEDIATOR BETWEEN EMPATHIC AROUSAL AND COMPASSION The association between empathy and compassion was examined in a sample of Americans aged 35 to 86, using national survey and phone interview data, biological data, and neuropsychological data. Given the postulation that empathy is a necessary, but not sufficient, condition for compassion to emerge, compassion is conceptualized here as an emergent process that is contingent upon empathic arousal. The degree to which an experience of empathic arousal translates into compassion is hypothesized to depend upon an individual's ability to downregulate the emotional response associated with empathic arousal, which is conceptualized as physiological upregulation in response to witnessing another's suffering. If this hypothesis is supported, then the ability to downregulate physiological processes associated with empathic arousal should mediate a positive association between the activation of empathic feelings and engagement with compassionate behavior. While empathic arousal was found to predict compassion, we were unable to infer that downregulation processes mediated the relationship. The results of this study present preliminary findings that may inform future work aiming to clarify the construct of compassion.
    [Show full text]
  • Power Feels Before It Thinks: Affect Theory and Critical Animal Studies
    Power Feels Before It Thinks Affect Theory and Critical Animal Studies in Religious Affects by Donovan O. Schaefer Eleonora Adorni Independent Researcher [email protected] Donovan O. Schaefer. 2015. Religious Affects: Animality, Evolution, and Power. Durham: Duke University Press Books. 304 pp. $ 24.95. ISBN 978-0-8223-5990-6. According to Aristotle, man is zóon logòn échon, the animal for whom lan- guage is the main etiological and constitutive element. From the Greek phi- losopher to our age, the primacy of language has affected all field of human knowledge, from philosophy to cognitive science, marking the border between what is rational – and therefore worthy of being studied – and what belongs to instinct, irrationality, primitivity. Donovan O. Schaefer wants to show the fallacy of this perspective, and opens his ambitious Religious Affects with a famous ethological anecdote: the famous “waterfall dance” recognized by primatologist Jane Goodall as the proof of religious beliefs also among non-human animals, typically considered devoid of language. The chimpanzees of Gombe (Tanzania) observed by the scholar since the sixties have shown a series of behaviors (rituals, responses to death, social interactions) very similar to those that in human terms are considered forms “religion”. “For ten minutes or more they may perform this magnifi- cent ‘dance’. Why? Is it not possible that the chimpanzees are responding to some feeling like awe? A feeling generated by the mystery of the water” (Goodall 1999, 189). Religious Affects starts from here to show that bodili- ness, and not language, is the common ground on which one should rethink religion: one should focus on the body as a prelinguistic element, which Michel Foucault already proved to be the vehicle of any power dynamic.
    [Show full text]
  • Timing of Affect Epistemologies, Aesthetics, Politics
    TIMING OF AFFECT EPISTEMOLOGIES, AESTHETICS, POLITICS EDITED BY MARIE-lUISEANGERER. BERND BOSEl AND MICHAELA on DIAPHANES I I TABLE OF CONTENTS Marie-Luise Angerer, Bernd Bosel, Michaela Ott Introduction 7 Moira Gatens Affective Transitions and Spinoza's Art of Joyful Deliberation 17 Michaela Ott Dividual Affections 35 Steven Shaviro Discognition 49 Mark B. N. Hansen Feelings without Feelers, or Affectivity as Environmental Force 65 Bernd Bdsel Affective Synchronization, Rhythmanalysis and the Polyphonic Qualities of the Present Moment 87 Marie-Luise Angerer Affective Knowledge Movement, Interval, Plasticity 103 Orit Halpern The Neural Network Temporality, Rationality, and Affect in Cybernetics 119 Wolfgang Ernst Temporalizing Presence and "Re-Presencing" the Past The Techno-Traumatic Affect 145 Luciana Parisi Digital Automation and Affect 161 Anna Tuschling The Age of Affective Computing 179 Rolf GroBmann Sensory Engineering Affects and the Mechanics of Musical Time 191 ; Wiebke Trost : Time Flow and Musical Emotions The Role of Rhythmic Entrainment 207 i ,- I Chris Salter INTRODUCTION Atmospheres of Affect 225 Christoph Brunner Since the 1990S, a discussion of affectivity has been conducted across many disciplines, Affective Politics of Timing On Emergent Collectivity in Ragnar Kjartansson's The Visitors 245 driven by cultural and feminist studies. This conspicuous turn towards affect and emo- tion, which can be observed in cultural, media, film and gender studies, in the social Hermann Kappelhoff and Sarah Greifenstein sciences,in cognitive psychology and neurology, in political science,in ethnography, but Feeling Gloomy or Riding High Timings of Melodrama and Comedy 263 also in philosophy, has taken place in the context of a critique of the primacy oflanguage and representation.1 Patricia Ticineto Clough In this focus on affect, various traditional concepts and discourses have been The Object's Affects revived," often updated with new semantic charges.
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction: Empathy, Emotional Politics and Transnationality
    Notes Introduction: Empathy, Emotional Politics and Transnationality 1 . I discuss the ways in which the categories ‘emotion’ and ‘affect’ themselves have been distinguished later in the Introduction. 2 . As Marjorie Garber notes, while empathy ‘is a modern word’, it has a Greek analogue: ‘empatheia’ (2004: 24). 3 . Susan Leigh Foster explains that ‘empathy’ was ‘originally coined in 1873 by the German aesthetician Robert Vischer as Einfühlung and translated into English by Edward Titchener in 1909’ (2010: 127). Vischer and Titchner were interested in analysing the affective and kinesthetic experience involved in ‘the act of viewing painting and sculpture’ (10). See also Coplan and Goldie (2011), and Currie (2011). 4 . As Garber explains, ‘Sympathy’s roots are Greek and Latin: it literally trans- lates as “having fellow feeling,” from sym plus pathos , “suffering together”’ (original italics, 2004: 23). In this vein, Foster discusses how, during the 1900s, ‘sympathy was most often theorized as a form of “fellow-feeling”, the product of “delicate nerve fibres”, reacting to the sorrow or joy of another. Both the individual’s expression and demeanour, and also the entire scene affecting the object of one’s sympathy needed to be evaluated in order for a sympathetic reaction to occur’ (2010: 10). 5 . See also Bozarth (2011), and Coplan and Goldie (2011). 6 . See, for example, Meyers (1994), Coplan (2011), and Coplan and Goldie (2011). 7 . See Bartky (1996), Engle and Khanna (1997), Spelman (1997), Koehn (1998), Ahmed (2004, 2010), Pedwell (2007, 2010, 2012a, b, 2013), and Hemmings (2011, 2012). 8 . Drawing on earlier influential feminist work by Nancy Chodorow and Jessica Benjamin, Meyers offers an ‘account of critical moral reflection that situates empathic understanding of others in a context structured by the values of mutual-recognition and self-recognition’ (1994: 16).
    [Show full text]
  • Empathy and Affect: What Can Empathied Bodies Do? Medical Humanities
    Postprint This is a pre-copyedited, author-produced PDF of an article accepted for publication in [Medical Humanities] following peer review. The definitive publisher-authenticated version [Marshall GR, Hooker C. Empathy and affect: what can empathied bodies do? Medical Humanities. 2016 Feb 8:medhum-2015. doi:10.1136/medhum-2015-010818 ] is available online at http://mh.bmj.com/content/early/2016/02/08/medhum-2015-010818.abstract (paywalled). Please cite as: Marshall GR, Hooker C. Empathy and affect: what can empathied bodies do? Medical Humanities. 2016 Feb 8:medhum-2015. doi:10.1136/medhum-2015-010818 EMPATHY AND AFFECT: WHAT CAN EMPATHIED BODIES DO? George Marshall and Claire Hooker (2016) Corresponding Author Dr George Robert Ellison Marshall School of Medicine Sydney University of Notre Dame Australia Darlinghurst NSW 2010 AUSTRALIA Mobile +61405001907 Telephone +61290874600 Fax +61290874611 Email [email protected] Author details George Robert Ellison Marshall, School of Medicine Sydney, University of Notre Dame Australia, Darlinghurst, NSW 2010, Australia Claire Hooker, Centre for Values, Ethics and Law in Medicine, University of Sydney, K25, Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia Keywords: Empathy, Medicine, Affect, Philosophy, Subjectivity Abstract While there has been much interest in the apparent benefits of empathy in improving outcomes of medical care, there is continuing concern over the philosophical nature of empathy. We suggest that part of the difficulty in coming-to-terms with empathy is due to the modernist dichotomies that have structured Western medical discourse, such that doctor and patient, knower and known, cognitive and emotional, subject and object, are situated in oppositional terms, with the result that such accounts cannot coherently encompass an emotional doctor, or a patient as knower, or empathy as other than a possession or a trait.
    [Show full text]
  • The Buddhist Perspective of Interpritivism As a Philosophical Base for Social Science Research
    International Journal of Science and Research (IJSR) ISSN: 2319-7064 ResearchGate Impact Factor (2018): 0.28 | SJIF (2018): 7.426 The Buddhist Perspective of Interpritivism as a Philosophical Base for Social Science Research MM Jayawardena1, WAAK Amaratunga2 1PhD (Colombo), MA (Peradeniya), BA Hons in Economics (Peradeniya), Dip in Psychology and Counselling (SLNIPC) 2MPhil (Kelaniya), MA in Linguistics (Kelaniya), BA Hons in English (Peradeniya), Faculty of Management, Social Sciences and Humanities, General Sir John Kotelawala Defence University, Sri Lanka Abstract: The practicability and the deliverability of research outcomes in social sciences have been rather questionable. In this context, interpretivism has become popular in social science research as it enables to capture more crucial qualitative information, which is based on inferences of sensory expressions rather than on direct information gathered through sensory organs. Although qualitative information cannot be numerically assessed, it is important in the deliverability of research outcomes. So, interpretivists believe that through a study of individual inferences, the reality of the research context can be better captured as it goes in-depth into the research issue while digging into the roots of individual human behaviour. Therefore, interpretivists argue that they can reach practicable outcomes that deliver a valid contribution at individual level as well as at societal level. On the other hand, some argue that as interpretivism does not have a constructive and strong theoretical base like in positivism, the research outcomes become subjective and less scientific. Addressing this criticism, Max Weber articulated "verstehen" (intuitive doctrine) and the "Ideal Type" referring to the society of the research context focused on by the researcher.
    [Show full text]
  • What Are Emotions? and How Can They Be Measured?
    Trends and developments: research on emotions Courants et tendances: recherche sur les e´motions Klaus R. Scherer What are emotions? And how can they be measured? Abstract. Defining ‘‘emotion’’ is a notorious problem. Without consensual conceptualization and operationalization of exactly what phenomenon is to be studied, progress in theory and research is difficult to achieve and fruitless debates are likely to proliferate. A particularly unfortunate example is William James’s asking the question ‘‘What is an emotion?’’ when he really meant ‘‘feeling’’, a misnomer that started a debate which is still ongoing, more than a century later. This contribution attempts to sensitize researchers in the social and behavioral sciences to the importance of definitional issues and their consequences for distinguishing related but fundamentally different affective processes, states, and traits. Links between scientific and folk concepts of emotion are explored and ways to measure emotion and its components are discussed. Key words. Affective processes – Emotion – Feeling – Folk concepts of emotion – Measurement of emotion – Scientific concepts of emotion Re´sume´. De´finir les emotions est un proble`me bien connu. Sans consensus quant a` la conceptualisation et l’ope´rationnalisation du phe´nome`ne exact que l’on e´tudie, tout The elaboration of the design feature definition of different affective phenomena has been con- ducted as part of the HUMAINE Network of Excellence (6th European Framework). The development of the Geneva Emotion Wheel was supported by a grant from the Daimler- Benz Foundation. The development of the Geneva Affect Label Coder was supported by the University of Geneva. The work of the Geneva Emotion Research Group is supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation.
    [Show full text]
  • Self-Report Captures 27 Distinct Categories of Emotion Bridged By
    Self-report captures 27 distinct categories of emotion PNAS PLUS bridged by continuous gradients Alan S. Cowena,1 and Dacher Keltnera aBerkeley Social Interaction Laboratory, Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, CA 94720 Edited by Joseph E. LeDoux, New York University, New York, NY, and approved August 7, 2017 (received for review February 9, 2017) Emotions are centered in subjective experiences that people rep- tional experiences of similar valence and arousal, such as anger resent, in part, with hundreds, if not thousands, of semantic and fear, or hope and pride (1, 14, 19, 22–24). Varying combi- terms. Claims about the distribution of reported emotional states nations of such dimensions have been the focus of hundreds of and the boundaries between emotion categories—that is, the studies linking reported emotional experience to behavior, phys- geometric organization of the semantic space of emotion—have iology, and brain activity (25–36). sparked intense debate. Here we introduce a conceptual frame- A second approach to emotional experience details how spe- work to analyze reported emotional states elicited by 2,185 short cific emotion categories, such as awe, fear, and envy, describe videos, examining the richest array of reported emotional expe- discrete clusters of states within a presupposed semantic space. riences studied to date and the extent to which reported expe- More precisely, basic emotion theories posit that a limited num- riences of emotion are structured by discrete and dimensional ber of clusters, ranging in theoretical accounts from 6 to 15, geometries. Across self-report methods, we find that the videos describe the distribution of all emotional states (16, 37, 38).
    [Show full text]
  • Hiding Feelings: the Acute Effects of Inhibiting Negative and Positive Emotion
    ;ournal uf -\bnonnaJ P<;)'chology Copynght 1997 by the Amencan ?<iYCOOIOglCaJ Assoclaoon. IN'. ,'i'/7. Vol. 106. So. 1.95-103 oo21-1l41)(/97IS3oo Hiding Feelings: The Acute Effects of Inhibiting Negative and Positive Emotion James J. Gross Robert VV. Levenson Stanford University University of California, Berkeley Emotion regulation plays a central role in mental health and illness, but little is known about even the most basic forms of emotion regulation. To examine the acute effects of inhibiting negative and positive emotion. we asked 180 female participants to watch sad, neutral. and amusing films under I of 2 conditions. Suppression participants (N = 90) inhibited their expressive behavior while watching the films; no suppression participants (N = 90) simply watched the films. Suppression diminished expressive behavior in all 3 films and decreased amusement self-reports in sad and amusing films. Physiologically. suppression had no effect in the neutral film, but clear effects in both negative and positive emotional films. including increased sympathetic activation of the cardiovascular system. On the basis of these findings, we suggest several ways emotional inhibition may influence psychological functioning. Emotion regulation and dysregulation figure prominently in processes simply overwhelms any attempt at scientific analysis. mental health and illness (Gross & Munoz. 1995). Indeed. by If this view is correct, the reason that little has been done is our count, over half of the nonsubstance related Axis I disorders that little can be done. and all of the Axis II personality disorders involve some form Without minimizing its complexity. we propose that emotion of emotion dysregulation (American Psychiatric Association. regulation can indeed be broken down into pieces that are ame­ 1994; see also Thoits.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Psychology Ofemotion Darren Ellis Ian Tucker
    Social Psychology of Emotion Darren Ellis Ian Tucker 00_Ellis & Tucker Prelims.indd 3 3/14/2015 12:44:20 PM 9 Affect Theory: Post- Structuralist Accounts Key Aims In this chapter we discuss some of the post-structural developments of emotion studies known as ‘affect theories’. In doing so we will include: • A view of Freud’s and later Tomkins’ affect theories • A discussion of Deleuze’s process of philosophy and affect • A critical review of Massumi’s notions of affect • A consideration of the place of affect in subjectivity and social psychology Introduction What is post-structuralism? Let us briefly remind ourselves. It can be described as a body of work which was essentially a response to structuralism. Structuralism viewed human culture as being understood through the signs and symbols which structure it. Structuralists tend to reject notions of human freedom, autonomy and free will, instead there is a focus on the ways that human behaviour is deter- mined by various structures, like language systems. Indeed the very term ‘subjectivity’ tends to be concerned with the way that systems of language are related to power and shape the individual’s outlook on the world; in other words, the individual is subject to structures of language (for example discourses) which speak through the person. For Karl Marx (1818–1883) human existence could be understood by analysing economic structures. Freud has also been claimed to be a structuralist as he described human functioning in terms of the structure of the psyche. Although Michel Foucault (1926–1984) is often considered to be allied with structuralism because his philosophy tends to agree with the notion that language and society are governed by various systems, he was sceptical of the endeavour to find underlying structures and was also concerned that we could never put ourselves outside of the power of discourse 10_Ellis & Tucker_Ch 09.indd 159 3/14/2015 12:44:44 PM 160 SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF EMOTION to think through these systems objectively.
    [Show full text]