Hiding Feelings: the Acute Effects of Inhibiting Negative and Positive Emotion

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Hiding Feelings: the Acute Effects of Inhibiting Negative and Positive Emotion ;ournal uf -\bnonnaJ P<;)'chology Copynght 1997 by the Amencan ?<iYCOOIOglCaJ Assoclaoon. IN'. ,'i'/7. Vol. 106. So. 1.95-103 oo21-1l41)(/97IS3oo Hiding Feelings: The Acute Effects of Inhibiting Negative and Positive Emotion James J. Gross Robert VV. Levenson Stanford University University of California, Berkeley Emotion regulation plays a central role in mental health and illness, but little is known about even the most basic forms of emotion regulation. To examine the acute effects of inhibiting negative and positive emotion. we asked 180 female participants to watch sad, neutral. and amusing films under I of 2 conditions. Suppression participants (N = 90) inhibited their expressive behavior while watching the films; no suppression participants (N = 90) simply watched the films. Suppression diminished expressive behavior in all 3 films and decreased amusement self-reports in sad and amusing films. Physiologically. suppression had no effect in the neutral film, but clear effects in both negative and positive emotional films. including increased sympathetic activation of the cardiovascular system. On the basis of these findings, we suggest several ways emotional inhibition may influence psychological functioning. Emotion regulation and dysregulation figure prominently in processes simply overwhelms any attempt at scientific analysis. mental health and illness (Gross & Munoz. 1995). Indeed. by If this view is correct, the reason that little has been done is our count, over half of the nonsubstance related Axis I disorders that little can be done. and all of the Axis II personality disorders involve some form Without minimizing its complexity. we propose that emotion of emotion dysregulation (American Psychiatric Association. regulation can indeed be broken down into pieces that are ame­ 1994; see also Thoits. 1985). Thus. for example. major de­ nable to empirical study. One way to do this is to study specific pressive disorder is characterized by a deficit of positive emotion clinical populations. Examples of this approach include studies and/or a surplus of negative emotion; generalized anxiety disor­ that examine regional lateralization of brain activity in de­ der by heightened levels of anxiety; schizophrenia. disorganized pressed individuals (Henriques. & Davidson. 1991) or assess type, by inappropriate emotional responses; and hisQionic per­ blunted affect in people with schizophrenia (Kring & Neale. in sonality disorder by excessive emotionality. press). A second, complementary approach examines aspects Despite the manifest importance of emotion regulation to of emotion regulation in nonclinical populations that hwe par­ psychological well-being. surprisingly little has been done to ticular relevance to clinical syndromes. Such studies both eluci­ document adults' attempts to influence which emotions they date the fundamental nature of these emotional responses and have, when they have them. or how these emotions are experi­ help establish norms necessary for subsequent determination of enced or expressed. I This relative neglect is quite puzzling and whether and how these responses are altered in clinical popula­ invites speculation. Is emotion regulation so ubiquitous that we tions. Examples of this approach can be found in work studying already know all there is to know about it? This seems unlikely. processes of emotional suppression (Gross & Levenson. 1993) in that commonsense views of emotion regulation are remark­ or rumination (Nolen-Hoeksema. 1993) in nonclinical or sub­ ably inconsistent (e.g., the injunction to count to 10 before clinical populations. In this article. we take the second approach. acting so that your anger will disappear seems to contradict the with a particular focus on the inhibition of negative and positive conventional wisdom that bottling up your anger will hurt you). emotion. A second possibility is that the diversity of emotion regulatory Emotional Inhibition: For Better or for Worse? Civilization seems to require that we inhibit the free play of James 1. Gross. Department of Psychology. Stanford University; Rob­ our emotions, and many have wondered what consequences such ert W. Levenson. Department of Psychology. University of California. emotional inhibition might have (Elias. 1978; Freud, 1961; Tom­ Berkeley. kins. 1984). Some commentators have feared for the worst. A This study was submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements of century ago, for example, Freud argued that emotional inhibition James J. Gross's doctoral degree at the University of California. Berke­ was an important cause of psychological illness. and his talk ley. This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health therapy was designed to release "strangulated affect" whose (NIMH) National Research Service Award MHl003401. National Insti­ expression. for one reason or ano~ had been severely cur­ tute on Aging Grant AG07476, and NIMH Grant MH39895. We thank Joseph Campos, Paul Ekman, Stephen Hinshaw. and George Lakoff for tailed (Breuer & Freud. 1957/1895, p. (7). Despite the substan- their help in formulating this study. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to James J. Gross. Department of Psychology. Jordan Hall, Building 420. Stanford I This is true despite a recent explosion of interest in the study of University. Stanford. California 94305-2130. Electronic mail may be developmental aspects of emotion regulation (e.g.• Fox, 1994; Garber & sent via Internet to [email protected]. Dodge. 1991). 95 96 GROSS AND LEVENSON tial changes from Freud's original theory of pathogenesis that a sample of healthy volunteers using a similar methodology to occurred over the ensuing decades, the idea that emotional inhi­ the one we used previously to study disgust (Gross &: Levenson, bition may lead to psychological distress remains a central tenet 1993). This time, however, we examined both a negative emo­ of psychodynamic psychotherapy. Accordingly, an important tion (sadness) and a positive emotion (aImscment), as well as goal of expressive therapies continues to be the fuller expression a control neutral emotional state. (e.g., representation in conscious awareness; experience and Our decision to study healthy participants was dictated by a expression in the context of the therapeutic relationship) of desire to establish nonnative data relevant to this form of emo­ inhibited emotional responses (Alexander &: French. 1946; tion regulation before engaging the compleXities of clinical sam­ Brenner. 1982; Levy. 1990). ples. Our selection of target emotions was dictated by the desire Recently, others have cast emotional inhibition in a more to extend our previous study of the negative emotion of disgust positive light. arguing that this and other forms of emotion regu­ to a more clinically relevant negative emotion (sadness), whose lation represent an essential developmental milestone (Kopp. regulation is centrally implicated in mood disorders such as 1989; Saami, 1990; Thompson. 1991). This view is consistent major depressive disorder: Given the theoretical importance with the notion that healthy adults often IDlSt inhibit (to varying attached to distinctions between negative and positive emotions, degrees) their ongoing emotion-expressive behavior (lbmkins. we also wished to include a positive emotion (aImsement) that 1984 ). It also accords weU with the evidence concerning the also IDlSt at times be suppressed (e.g., to facilitate task focus). potent destructive effects of unregulated emotional responding, In addition, to address the possibility that the observed pattern as in the precipitous expression of anger in intermittent explosive of findings might result from inhibiting any behavior (rather disorder: On this view, emotional inhibition is not uniformly than emotional inhibition per se), we included a control condi­ pathogenic. Indeed. in many circumstances, it may be the failure tion in which participants were instructed to inhibit their expres­ of emotional inhibition that is problematic. sive behavior while in a neutral emotional state. Obviously, there is a middle ground between these two per­ spectives, one that holds that there is an optimal level of emo­ Hypotheses tional regulation-somewhere between total strangulation and completely unfettered expression. The point we wish to empha­ We view emotional inhibition as an active, effortful affair in size here. howev~ is the extremely important role that emo­ which inhibitory processes are recruited and then pitted against tional inhibition bas J)layed historically in clinical theory. ongoing emotional responses. This leads to the prediction that some signs of emotion may be visible even as higher order inhibitory processes are activated. In addition. if emotional inhi­ Empirical Findings bition requires effort, it should have physiological conse­ Despite long-standing clinical interest in the topic of emo­ quences. This leads to the prediction that participants who in­ tional inhibition, the relevant empirical literature is impover­ hibit ongoing emotion-expressive behavior should show greater ished and inconsistent (for a review, see Gross &: Levenson, signs of physiological activation that participants who do not 1993 ). EveJl if we limit ourselves to questions about the inhibi­ engage in emotional inhibition. FInally, given the marked behav­ tion of emotion-expressive behavior (as opposed to the inhibi­ ioral and physiological changes we anticipated would be tion of subjective emotional experience), clear conclusions
Recommended publications
  • Navigating the Genealogies of Trauma, Guilt, and Affect: an Interview with Ruth Leys
    Navigating the Genealogies of Trauma, Guilt, and Affect: An Interview with Ruth Leys Ruth Leys Marlene Goldman University of Toronto Quarterly, Volume 79, Number 2, Spring 2010, pp. 656-679 (Article) Published by University of Toronto Press For additional information about this article http://muse.jhu.edu/journals/utq/summary/v079/79.2.leys.html Access Provided by Duke University Libraries at 08/29/11 12:38PM GMT RUTH LEYS AND MARLENE GOLDMAN Navigating the Genealogies of Trauma, Guilt, and Affect: An Interview with Ruth Leys ABSTRACT In this interview, Ruth Leys discusses her career as a historian of science and her research on contemporary developments in the human sciences, including Trauma: A Genealogy, From Guilt to Shame: Auschwitz and After, and her current work on the genealogy of experimental and theoretical approaches to the affects from the 1960s to the present. Among the topics she covers are her inves- tigation of the role of imitation or mimesis in trauma theory; why shame has replaced guilt as a dominant emotional reference in the West; the ways in which the shift from notions of guilt to notions of shame has involved a shift from concern about actions, or what you do, to a concern about identity, or who you are; why the shift from agency to identity has produced as one of its consequences the replacement of the idea of the meaning of a person’s inten- tions and actions by the idea of the primacy of a person’s affective experience; the significance of the recent “turn to affect” in cultural theory; and why the new affect theorists are committed to the view that the affect system is funda- mentally independent of intention and meaning because they view it is a material system of the body.
    [Show full text]
  • Decoding Emotions from Nonverbal Vocalizations: How Much Voice Signal Is Enough?
    Motivation and Emotion https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-019-09783-9 ORIGINAL PAPER Decoding emotions from nonverbal vocalizations: How much voice signal is enough? Paula Castiajo1 · Ana P. Pinheiro1,2 © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019 Abstract How much acoustic signal is enough for an accurate recognition of nonverbal emotional vocalizations? Using a gating paradigm (7 gates from 100 to 700 ms), the current study probed the efect of stimulus duration on recognition accuracy of emotional vocalizations expressing anger, disgust, fear, amusement, sadness and neutral states. Participants (n = 52) judged the emotional meaning of vocalizations presented at each gate. Increased recognition accuracy was observed from gates 2 to 3 for all types of vocalizations. Neutral vocalizations were identifed with the shortest amount of acoustic information relative to all other types of vocalizations. A shorter acoustic signal was required to decode amusement compared to fear, anger and sadness, whereas anger and fear required equivalent amounts of acoustic information to be accurately recognized. These fndings confrm that the time course of successful recognition of discrete vocal emotions varies by emotion type. Compared to prior studies, they additionally indicate that the type of auditory signal (speech prosody vs. nonverbal vocaliza- tions) determines how quickly listeners recognize emotions from a speaker’s voice. Keywords Nonverbal vocalizations · Emotion · Duration · Gate · Recognition Introduction F0, intensity) serving the expression of emotional mean- ing. Nonetheless, compared to speech prosody, nonverbal The voice is likely the most important sound category in a vocalizations are considered to represent more primitive social environment. It carries not only verbal information, expressions of emotions and an auditory analogue of facial but also socially relevant cues about the speaker, such as his/ emotions (Belin et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Interim Guidance for Amusement and Water Parks During the Covid-19 Public Health Emergency
    INTERIM GUIDANCE FOR AMUSEMENT AND WATER PARKS DURING THE COVID-19 PUBLIC HEALTH EMERGENCY When you have read this document, you can affirm at the bottom. As of April 9, 2021 Purpose This Interim Guidance for Amusement and Water Parks during the COVID-19 Public Health Emergency (“Interim COVID-19 Guidance for Amusement and Water Parks”) was created to provide owners/operators of amusement parks, theme parks, and/or water parks and their employees, contractors, vendors, and patrons with precautions to help protect against the spread of COVID-19. This guidance applies to all outdoor activities at amusement parks, theme parks, and/or water parks, including any rides, games, or other attractions, such as redemption games of skill and chance, obstacle or recreational courses, outdoor trampolines, outdoor merry-go-rounds or carousels, outdoor train rides, outdoor roller coasters, and/or outdoor bumper cars. Outdoor amusement activities or attractions where appropriate social distancing and cleaning and disinfection standards provided in this document cannot be achieved (e.g., ball pits) must remain closed. Amusement parks, theme parks, and water parks may reopen beginning Friday, April 9, 2021, in accordance with this guidance. Responsible Parties – as defined below – must submit a reopening plan to the respective county health department or local public health authority, within two weeks of reopening, including specific health protocols in place to meet the requirements set forth in this guidance. Temporary, travel outdoor amusement activities (e.g., carnivals) may reopen in accordance with this guidance; provided, however, that the Responsible Parties must submit a reopening plan to the respective county health department or local public health authority at least 14 days prior to reopening, including specific health protocols in place to meet the requirements set forth in this guidance (e.g., health screening, social distancing, face coverings, controlled movement, hand hygiene, cleaning and disinfection, and communication).
    [Show full text]
  • Empathic Concern Is Part of a More General Communal Emotion
    1 Empathic Concern is Part of A More General Communal Emotion Janis H. Zickfeld1*, Thomas W. Schubert12, Beate Seibt12, Alan P. Fiske3 1Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway. 2Instituto Universitário de Lisboa (ISCTE-IUL), Lisboa, Portugal. 3Department of Anthropology, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA. In press at Frontiers. This paper is not the copy of record and may not exactly replicate the authoritative document published in Frontiers. *Correspondence: Janis H. Zickfeld [email protected] 2 Abstract Seeing someone in need may evoke a particular kind of closeness that has been conceptualized as sympathy or empathic concern (which is distinct from other empathy constructs). In other contexts, when people suddenly feel close to others, or observe others suddenly feeling closer to each other, this sudden closeness tends to evoke an emotion often labeled in vernacular English as being moved, touched, or heart-warming feelings. Recent theory and empirical work indicates that this is a distinct emotion; the construct is named kama muta. Is empathic concern for people in need simply an expression of the much broader tendency to respond with kama muta to all kinds of situations that afford closeness, such as reunions, kindness, and expressions of love? Across 16 studies sampling 2918 participants, we explored whether empathic concern is associated with kama muta. Meta-analyzing the association between ratings of state being moved and trait empathic concern revealed an effect size of, r(3631) = .35 [95% CI: .29, .41]. In addition, trait empathic concern was also associated with self-reports of the three sensations that have been shown to be reliably indicative of kama muta: weeping, chills, and bodily feelings of warmth.
    [Show full text]
  • Positive Emotion Dispositions Differentially Associated with Big Five Personality and Attachment Style
    The Journal of Positive Psychology, April 2006; 1(2): 61–71 Positive emotion dispositions differentially associated with Big Five personality and attachment style MICHELLE N. SHIOTA, DACHER KELTNER, & OLIVER P. JOHN University of California at Berkeley, USA Abstract Although theorists have proposed the existence of multiple distinct varieties of positive emotion, dispositional positive affect is typically treated as a unidimensional variable in personality research. We present data elaborating conceptual and empirical differences among seven positive emotion dispositions in their relationships with two core personality constructs, the ‘‘Big Five’’ and adult attachment style. We found that the positive emotion dispositions were differentially associated with self- and peer-rated Extraversion, Conscientiousness, Agreeableness, Openness to Experience, and Neuroticism. We also found that different adult attachment styles were associated with different kinds of emotional rewards. Findings support the theoretical utility of differentiating among several dispositional positive emotion constructs in personality research. Keywords: Emotion; positive emotion; positive psychology; personality; Big Five; attachment Downloaded By: [CDL Journals Account] At: 22:51 20 December 2007 Introduction Shiota, Campos, Keltner, & Hertenstein, 2004). In the present investigation we explored distinctions Philosophers and writers have long debated the nature among the major personality correlates of several of happiness, reaching a wide range of conclusions, corresponding positive emotion dispositions. Prior but never a consensually accepted definition. studies have documented robust relationships Recently scientists have joined this enterprise, creat- between global positive affect and the Big Five trait ing a flourishing line of inquiry: a Psycinfo search Extraversion, as well as secure adult attachment for ‘‘happiness’’ now yields over 4,500 citations.
    [Show full text]
  • The Affective Turn in Ethnomusicology
    Ana Hofman The Affective Turn in Ethnomusicology DOI: 10.2298/MUZ1518035H UDK: 781.7:159.942 The Affective Turn in Ethnomusicology Ana Hofman1 The Institute of Cultural and Memory Studies Research Centre of Slovenian Academy of Sciences and Arts Abstract The affective turn, which has already questioned dominant paradigms in many disciplinary fields including cultural studies, philosophy, political theory, anthropology, psychology and neuroscience, has started to attract more attention in the field of ethnomusicology, becoming a particularly vibrant stream of thought. Drawing on the voices that call for the historicisation of and critical deliberation on the field of affect studies, the article strives to show how theories of affect might expand dominant paradigms in ethnomusicology and also points to their limitations. Key words Affective turn, ethnomusicology, sonic affect, musical emotions, affect-emotion relationship “Everything that occurs without praiseworthy affects [in music] can be considered nothing, does nothing and means nothing” (Der vollkommene Capellmeister, Johann Mattheson, 1739). Music has often been taken as an example of the power of affect. Its ubiquitously distributive affective potential has become pivotal in the works of scholars advocating the affective turn.2 Spurred by the poststructuralist orientation towards language, representation, deconstruction and psychoanalysis, a perspective in which body, emotions and embodiment have been neglected (see Clough and Halley 2007), the affective turn shifted the focus to pre-, extra-, and paralinguistic aspects and introduced a non-discursive, non- representational approach (see Thrift 2007). Developed mainly in the 2000s and partly inspired by research on the emotions and the body conducted in feminist and queer studies, affect theory is currently attracting growing interest in a variety of disciplinary fields.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 the RATIONALITY of EMOTIONAL CHANGE: TOWARD a PROCESS VIEW1 Oded Na'aman the Hebrew University of Jerusalem This Is The
    THE RATIONALITY OF EMOTIONAL CHANGE: TOWARD A PROCESS VIEW1 Oded Na’aman The Hebrew University of Jerusalem This is the penultimate draft of an article that is forthcoming in Noûs. Please cite published version. Abstract The paper argues against a widely held synchronic view of emotional rationality. I begin by considering recent philosophical literature on various backward-looking emotions, such as regret, grief, resentment, and anger. I articulate the general problem these accounts grapple with: a certain diminution in backward-looking emotions seems fitting while the reasons for these emotions seem to persist. The problem, I argue, rests on the assumption that if the facts that give reason for an emotion remain unchanged, the emotion remains fitting. However, I argue there are rationally self-consuming attitudes: affective attitudes that become less fitting the longer they endure while the facts that give reason for them persist. A widely held synchronic view of fitting affective attitudes denies that fittingness at a time depends on the agent’s attitudes at different times and therefore denies that the fittingness of an affective attitude can depend on its duration. Once we reject the synchronic view, we may see that affective attitudes are often fitting due to the fitting processes of which they are part. These fitting processes explain the fitting diminution of backward-looking emotions as well as other diachronic aspects of the fittingness of emotions. Like actions and choices, attitudes are subject to normative standards. From belief and desire to envy, admiration, grief, amusement, and lust—we evaluate, criticize, disown, and endorse attitudes. Though such attitudes are not voluntary, they are rational in the sense that they can succeed or fail to meet the normative standards we apply to them.2 Furthermore, it is thought that the rationality of these attitudes is related to the fact that they are about an object or directed toward it.
    [Show full text]
  • Dissertation Clarifying the Construct of Compassion
    DISSERTATION CLARIFYING THE CONSTRUCT OF COMPASSION: ABILITY TO DOWNREGULATE EMOTION AS A POTENTIAL MEDIATOR BETWEEN EMPATHIC AROUSAL AND COMPASSION Submitted by Leslie A. Merriman Department of Psychology In partial fulfillment of the requirements For the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy Colorado State University Fort Collins, Colorado Spring 2017 Doctoral Committee: Advisor: Kathryn Rickard Chris Allen Ernest Chavez Caridad Souza Copyright by Leslie A. Merriman 2017 All Rights Reserved ABSTRACT CLARIFYING THE CONSTRUCT OF COMPASSION: ABILITY TO DOWNREGULATE EMOTION AS A POTENTIAL MEDIATOR BETWEEN EMPATHIC AROUSAL AND COMPASSION The association between empathy and compassion was examined in a sample of Americans aged 35 to 86, using national survey and phone interview data, biological data, and neuropsychological data. Given the postulation that empathy is a necessary, but not sufficient, condition for compassion to emerge, compassion is conceptualized here as an emergent process that is contingent upon empathic arousal. The degree to which an experience of empathic arousal translates into compassion is hypothesized to depend upon an individual's ability to downregulate the emotional response associated with empathic arousal, which is conceptualized as physiological upregulation in response to witnessing another's suffering. If this hypothesis is supported, then the ability to downregulate physiological processes associated with empathic arousal should mediate a positive association between the activation of empathic feelings and engagement with compassionate behavior. While empathic arousal was found to predict compassion, we were unable to infer that downregulation processes mediated the relationship. The results of this study present preliminary findings that may inform future work aiming to clarify the construct of compassion.
    [Show full text]
  • Power Feels Before It Thinks: Affect Theory and Critical Animal Studies
    Power Feels Before It Thinks Affect Theory and Critical Animal Studies in Religious Affects by Donovan O. Schaefer Eleonora Adorni Independent Researcher [email protected] Donovan O. Schaefer. 2015. Religious Affects: Animality, Evolution, and Power. Durham: Duke University Press Books. 304 pp. $ 24.95. ISBN 978-0-8223-5990-6. According to Aristotle, man is zóon logòn échon, the animal for whom lan- guage is the main etiological and constitutive element. From the Greek phi- losopher to our age, the primacy of language has affected all field of human knowledge, from philosophy to cognitive science, marking the border between what is rational – and therefore worthy of being studied – and what belongs to instinct, irrationality, primitivity. Donovan O. Schaefer wants to show the fallacy of this perspective, and opens his ambitious Religious Affects with a famous ethological anecdote: the famous “waterfall dance” recognized by primatologist Jane Goodall as the proof of religious beliefs also among non-human animals, typically considered devoid of language. The chimpanzees of Gombe (Tanzania) observed by the scholar since the sixties have shown a series of behaviors (rituals, responses to death, social interactions) very similar to those that in human terms are considered forms “religion”. “For ten minutes or more they may perform this magnifi- cent ‘dance’. Why? Is it not possible that the chimpanzees are responding to some feeling like awe? A feeling generated by the mystery of the water” (Goodall 1999, 189). Religious Affects starts from here to show that bodili- ness, and not language, is the common ground on which one should rethink religion: one should focus on the body as a prelinguistic element, which Michel Foucault already proved to be the vehicle of any power dynamic.
    [Show full text]
  • Reward and Emotion: an Affective Neuroscience Approach
    Reward and emotion: An affective neuroscience approach David Sander1 & Lauri Nummenmaa2 1Swiss Center for Affective Sciences (CISA), Campus Biotech, and Laboratory for the Study of Emotion Elicitation and Expression, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences (FPSE), University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland 2TurKu PET Centre, TurKu University Hospital, and Department of Psychology, University of TurKu, Finland Address Correspondence to: Lauri Nummenmaa Turku PET Centre c/o Turku University Hospital FI-20520 Turku, Finland Email: [email protected] Tel: +358 50 574 7933 Acknowlegements This study was supported by the Academy oF Finland (grants #294897 and #332225), Sigrid Juselius stiftelse and Signe och Anet Gyllenberg’s stiftelse, and by the Swiss National Science Foundation (grant 100019_188966). DS and LN thank Brian Knutson For in-depth discussions concerning several aspects of this paper. Conflicts of interest None Abstract Pleasure and reward are central for motivation, learning, feeling and allostasis. Although reward is without any doubt an affective phenomenon, there is no consensus concerning its relationship with emotion. In this mini-review we discuss this conceptual issue both from the perspective of theories of reward and emotion as well as human systems neuroimaging. We first describe how the reward process can be understood and dissected as intertwined with the emotion process, in particular in light of the appraisal theories, and then discuss how different facets of the reward process can be studied using neuroimaging and neurostimulation techniques. We conclude that future worK needs to focus on mapping the similarities and differences across stimuli and mechanisms that are involved in reward processing and in emotional processing, and propose that an integrative affective sciences approach would provide means for studying the emotional nature of reward.
    [Show full text]
  • Compounded Loss During This Pandemic
    Compounded Loss During this Pandemic Written by Amy Monzingo, MS, NCC, LMHP, LMHC, Best Care EAP Counselor Unchartered waters… Throughout the last couple of months I’ve been feeling a little off. This strange feeling of loss has kind of snuck up on me and it finally dawned on me that I’m feeling grief. Grief is what we call that process we go through with loss. Usually we associate grief with the death of a loved one, but it can really be about anything related to loss. A breakup in a relationship, the loss of a job, coming in last in that shuffleboard tournament, misplacing your lucky shirt, forgetting how to make your grandma’s cookies, moving away, not making the team, getting passed over for a promotion….the list is endless. Oftentimes when we go through a loss, we experience one loss at a time. For instance, somebody dies. You process through that grief while the other parts of your life continue on or stay mostly stable. (As much as they can when you lose someone you love.) But, the point is, you pour a lot of your energy into grieving for your loved one and as you begin to heal, you slowly turn more attention back on to the other parts of your life that have been kept in this sort of neutral state until you could come back. Well, what happens when another loss happens on top of that death? Suppose the week after that death, you find out the company you work for is closing its doors? That’s a second loss.
    [Show full text]
  • 13. Yanina Michelini. OK.Indd
    Trends in Psychology / Temas em Psicologia DOI: 10.9788/TP2019.2-13 ISSN 2358-1883 (online edition) Article LATEMO-E: A Film Database to Elicit Discrete Emotions and Evaluate Emotional Dimensions in Latin-Americans Yanina Michelini*, 1, 2 Orcid.org/0000-0003-0768-7645 Ignacio Acuña1, 2 Orcid.org/0000-0001-6152-6530 Juan Ignacio Guzmán1, 2 Orcid.org/0000-0002-9996-6694 Juan Carlos Godoy1, 2 Orcid.org/0000-0002-1622-1647 –––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––– 1Universidad Nacional de Córdoba, Córdoba, Argentina 2Instituto de Investigaciones Psicológicas, IIPsi, Unidad Ejecutora CONICET, Córdoba, Argentina Abstract Introduction: Film clips are the most used stimuli to induce diff erent emotional states. Objective: Identify, select, and evaluate a set of fi lm clips according to dimensionality and discreteness of emotions. Film experts suggested 437 fragments, but only 70 met the criteria to be included in the LATEMO-E fi lm-clip database. Study 1: The 70 fi lm clips were tested for their ability to induce disgust, anger, fear, sadness, amusement, tenderness, and neutral (n = 147 participants; mean age = 20.64 years, SD ±2.21 years). Results: A total of 28 fi lm clips presented adequate scores on intensity and discreteness. Study 2: A total of 28 selected fi lm clips were assessed again in a new sample of 106 participants (mean age = 20.96 years, SD ±2.45 years). Results: Anger and amusement fi lm clips were the most extreme in the valence and activation dimensions. Anger and fear fi lm clips were the most extreme categories in the intensity, certainty, and control dimensions. Conclusions: These stimuli were useful for inducing emotional states that can be judged from both the dimensional perspective of emotions and the basic emotions perspective.
    [Show full text]