Pulse Register Phonation in Diana Monkey Alarm Calls

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Pulse Register Phonation in Diana Monkey Alarm Calls View metadata, citation and similarPublished papers inat core.ac.ukThe Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 113, Issue 5, 2003, p. 2919-2926 brought to you by CORE which should be used for any reference to this work provided by RERO DOC1 Digital Library Pulse register phonation in Diana monkey alarm calls Tobias Riedea) Department of Psychology, 245 Uris Hall, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York 14853 Klaus Zuberbu¨hlerb) School of Psychology, University of St. Andrews, St. Andrews, Fife KY 16 9JU, Scotland, United Kingdom The adult male Diana monkeys ͑Cercopithecus diana͒ produce predator-specific alarm calls in response to two of their predators, the crowned eagles and the leopards. The acoustic structure of these alarm calls is remarkable for a number of theoretical and empirical reasons. First, although pulsed phonation has been described in a variety of mammalian vocalizations, very little is known about the underlying production mechanism. Second, Diana monkey alarm calls are based almost exclusively on this vocal production mechanism to an extent that has never been documented in mammalian vocal behavior. Finally, the Diana monkeys’ pulsed phonation strongly resembles the pulse register in human speech, where fundamental frequency is mainly controlled by subglottal pressure. Here, we report the results of a detailed acoustic analysis to investigate the production mechanism of Diana monkey alarm calls. Within calls, we found a positive correlation between the fundamental frequency and the pulse amplitude, suggesting that both humans and monkeys control fundamental frequency by subglottal pressure. While in humans pulsed phonation is usually considered pathological or artificial, male Diana monkeys rely exclusively on pulsed phonation, suggesting a functional adaptation. Moreover, we were unable to document any nonlinear phenomena, despite the fact that they occur frequently in the vocal repertoire of humans and nonhumans, further suggesting that the very robust Diana monkey pulse production mechanism has evolved for a particular functional purpose. We discuss the implications of these findings for the structural evolution of Diana monkey alarm calls and suggest that the restricted variability in fundamental frequency and robustness of the source signal gave rise to the formant patterns observed in Diana monkey alarm calls, used to convey predator information. I. INTRODUCTION mental frequency of the sound produced by the vocal folds is additionally directly related with the tension of the vocal fold The vocalizations of many mammals are the result of tissue ͑Titze, 1989, 1991͒. two distinct components: the oscillating vocal folds within Adult male Diana monkeys ͑Cercopithecus diana͒ pro- the larynx produce a primary acoustic signal, which then duce acoustically distinct alarm calls to two of their preda- undergoes a filtering process within the vocal tract where tors, the crowned eagle and the leopard ͑Zuberbu¨hler et al., various frequency bands are dampened to different degrees 1997; Zuberbu¨hler, 2000a͒. Playback experiments have ͑van den Berg, 1958; Fant, 1960; Titze, 1994; Owren and shown that nearby listeners respond to these alarm calls as if Linker, 1995͒. Basic vocal fold behavior can be described as the corresponding predator were present, suggesting that the following: Bernoulli forces cause the vocal folds ͑if close these calls inform nearby recipients about important ongoing to each other͒ to be sucked together, creating a closed air- events in the environment ͑Zuberbu¨hler et al., 1999; Zuber- space below the glottis. Continued subglottal air pressure bu¨hler, 2000b͒. Acoustically, the Diana monkeys’ alarm vo- from the lungs builds up underneath the closed folds. Once calizations consist of a bout of calls. Bouts vary in the num- this pressure becomes high enough, the folds are blown out- ber of calls from one to more than a dozen. Individual calls ward, thus opening the glottis and releasing a single ‘‘puff’’ are characterized by a highly stereotypic pulse pattern and ͑ ͒ of air ͑van den Berg, 1958͒. As the subglottal pressure in- calls are interspersed by short harmonic elements Fig. 1 . creases, two effects can be observed. First, the motion of the The single pulses within each call resemble a damped vocal folds becomes faster ͑demonstrated in computer mod- oscillation: a rapid, transient change in signal amplitude from els: Ishizaka and Flanagan, 1972; Steinecke and Herzel, a baseline value to a higher or lower value, followed by a 1995, and in vitro: Titze, 1989͒. Second, the sound pressure rapid return to the baseline value. Elsewhere, we showed that level increases ͑Gramming, 1988; Titze, 1994͒. The funda- the formant peak frequency and formant transition of the pulse elements is the single most important parameter to dif- ferentiate eagle versus leopard alarm calls ͑Riede and Zuber- a͒ Present address and address for correspondence: Tobias Riede, 315 Jordan bu¨hler, in press͒, suggesting that similar to human speech Hall, School of Medicine, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405. Electronic mail: [email protected] sounds, some primate vocalizations convey important se- b͒Electronic mail: [email protected] mantic information by formant structures. Although research 2 completely adducted and a small vocal fold excursion ͑Hol- lien et al., 1977͒. The fundamental frequency is affected by different fac- tors in each of the three registers. In the modal register, the fundamental frequency is mainly determined by changes in vocal fold length and stiffness ͑Murry and Brown, 1971͒. Moreover, there is a positive correlation between vocal fold thickness ͑i.e., mass, length, and stiffness͒ and fundamental frequency ͓reviewed in Titze ͑1994͔͒. This relationship is absent in the pulse register ͑Hollien et al., 1969; Allen and Hollien, 1973͒. Instead, the fundamental frequency of the pulse register appears to be predominantly determined by changes in subglottal air pressure. To investigate the vocal production mechanism of the Diana monkey, we analyzed the relationship between call amplitude ͑a reliable estimator of subglottal pressure͒ and fundamental frequency. We predicted a positive relationship FIG. 1. Time domain and spectrogram of a leopard alarm bout, consisting of between these two parameters if Diana monkey alarm calls seven calls. Basic unit of the call is the pulse as shown in the ‘‘zoom in’’ picture of Fig. 2. are the product of the same source production mechanism that is responsible for the human pulse register. on the structural evolution of animal vocalizations is not new A second aim of this study was to investigate the role of ͑Morton, 1977͒, comparatively little is still known about how nonlinear phenomena in the vocalizations of Diana monkey natural and sexual selection affected the acoustic structure of alarm calls. Nonlinear phenomena are relevant in this context primate alarm calls ͑Zuberbu¨hler, 2003͒. Here, we provide a because they can be directly related to events at the laryngeal detailed acoustic analysis of the source characteristics of Di- source. Several lines of research suggest that nonlinear phe- ana monkey alarm calls to elucidate the adaptive significance nomena are common and ubiquitous in mammalian vocaliza- tion behavior ͑Wilden et al., 1998; Mergell et al., 1999; and physiological constraints of this remarkable vocalization. ͒ Human speech sounds can be produced using three dif- Riede et al., 1997, 2000; Fischer et al., 2000 . Phenomena ferent registers. A register can be described by the frequency such as frequency jumps, subharmonics, biphonation, and range covered and by the specific mode of vocal fold behav- deterministic chaos are commonly observed, usually the re- ior by which it is produced ͑e.g., Hollien, 1974; Titze, 1994; sult of deviations from the regular harmonic vibration pattern Svec et al., 1999͒. Although each register covers a certain of the vocal folds, such as nonsynchronously oscillating left and right vocal folds or simultaneously oscillating horizontal frequency range, neighboring registers overlap significantly. ͑ Normal speech is delivered in the so-called modal ͑or chest͒ and vertical components of the vocal folds e.g., Herzel ͑ ͒ et al., 1994; Berry, 2001; Berry et al., 1994; Steinecke and register fundamental frequency range 100–300 Hz . Hu- ͒ mans are also capable to produce speech using either the Herzel, 1995; Tigges et al., 1997; Neubauer et al., 2001 . falsetto ͑or flagolet͒ register ͑fundamental frequency Ͼ300 The two combined approaches are likely to yield important Hz͒ or the pulse ͑or vocal fry͒ register ͓fundamental fre- insights into the sound production mechanism underlying quency Ͻ100 Hz ͑Blomgren et al., 1998͔͒. Recent studies male Diana monkey alarm calls. suggest the existence of a separate fourth register, i.e., the vocal-ventricular phonation mode, like pulse register cover- II. MATERIAL AND METHODS ing the frequency range below 100 Hz but unlike pulse reg- A. Study site and subjects ister involving the ventricular folds ͑‘‘false folds’’͒ into the mode of production ͑Fuks et al., 1998; Lindestad et al., Data were collected in an approximately 40-km2 study- 2001͒. According to this terminology we used the term area of primary rain forest surrounding the Centre en ‘‘pulse register’’ to describe the Diana monkey calls, because Recherche d’Ecologie ͑University of Cocody, Abidjan͒ re- these vocalizations strongly resemble the pulse register of search station ͑5°50ЈN, 7°21ЈW͒ in the Taı¨ National Park, humans ͓see
Recommended publications
  • World's Most Endangered Primates
    Primates in Peril The World’s 25 Most Endangered Primates 2016–2018 Edited by Christoph Schwitzer, Russell A. Mittermeier, Anthony B. Rylands, Federica Chiozza, Elizabeth A. Williamson, Elizabeth J. Macfie, Janette Wallis and Alison Cotton Illustrations by Stephen D. Nash IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group (PSG) International Primatological Society (IPS) Conservation International (CI) Bristol Zoological Society (BZS) Published by: IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group (PSG), International Primatological Society (IPS), Conservation International (CI), Bristol Zoological Society (BZS) Copyright: ©2017 Conservation International All rights reserved. No part of this report may be reproduced in any form or by any means without permission in writing from the publisher. Inquiries to the publisher should be directed to the following address: Russell A. Mittermeier, Chair, IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group, Conservation International, 2011 Crystal Drive, Suite 500, Arlington, VA 22202, USA. Citation (report): Schwitzer, C., Mittermeier, R.A., Rylands, A.B., Chiozza, F., Williamson, E.A., Macfie, E.J., Wallis, J. and Cotton, A. (eds.). 2017. Primates in Peril: The World’s 25 Most Endangered Primates 2016–2018. IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group (PSG), International Primatological Society (IPS), Conservation International (CI), and Bristol Zoological Society, Arlington, VA. 99 pp. Citation (species): Salmona, J., Patel, E.R., Chikhi, L. and Banks, M.A. 2017. Propithecus perrieri (Lavauden, 1931). In: C. Schwitzer, R.A. Mittermeier, A.B. Rylands, F. Chiozza, E.A. Williamson, E.J. Macfie, J. Wallis and A. Cotton (eds.), Primates in Peril: The World’s 25 Most Endangered Primates 2016–2018, pp. 40-43. IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group (PSG), International Primatological Society (IPS), Conservation International (CI), and Bristol Zoological Society, Arlington, VA.
    [Show full text]
  • AFRICAN PRIMATES the Journal of the Africa Section of the IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group
    Volume 9 2014 ISSN 1093-8966 AFRICAN PRIMATES The Journal of the Africa Section of the IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group Editor-in-Chief: Janette Wallis PSG Chairman: Russell A. Mittermeier PSG Deputy Chair: Anthony B. Rylands Red List Authorities: Sanjay Molur, Christoph Schwitzer, and Liz Williamson African Primates The Journal of the Africa Section of the IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group ISSN 1093-8966 African Primates Editorial Board IUCN/SSC Primate Specialist Group Janette Wallis – Editor-in-Chief Chairman: Russell A. Mittermeier Deputy Chair: Anthony B. Rylands University of Oklahoma, Norman, OK USA Simon Bearder Vice Chair, Section on Great Apes:Liz Williamson Oxford Brookes University, Oxford, UK Vice-Chair, Section on Small Apes: Benjamin M. Rawson R. Patrick Boundja Regional Vice-Chairs – Neotropics Wildlife Conservation Society, Congo; Univ of Mass, USA Mesoamerica: Liliana Cortés-Ortiz Thomas M. Butynski Andean Countries: Erwin Palacios and Eckhard W. Heymann Sustainability Centre Eastern Africa, Nanyuki, Kenya Brazil and the Guianas: M. Cecília M. Kierulff, Fabiano Rodrigues Phillip Cronje de Melo, and Maurício Talebi Jane Goodall Institute, Mpumalanga, South Africa Regional Vice Chairs – Africa Edem A. Eniang W. Scott McGraw, David N. M. Mbora, and Janette Wallis Biodiversity Preservation Center, Calabar, Nigeria Colin Groves Regional Vice Chairs – Madagascar Christoph Schwitzer and Jonah Ratsimbazafy Australian National University, Canberra, Australia Michael A. Huffman Regional Vice Chairs – Asia Kyoto University, Inuyama,
    [Show full text]
  • Predator-Specific Alarm Calls in Campbell's Monkeys, Cercopithecus Campbelli
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by RERO DOC Digital Library Published in Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 50, No. 5, 414-422, 1 2001 which should be used for any reference to this work Klaus Zuberbühler Predator-specific alarm calls in Campbell’s monkeys, Cercopithecus campbelli Abstract One of the most prominent behavioural fea- to produce and respond to referential labels for external tures of many forest primates are the loud calls given by events. the adult males. Early observational studies repeatedly postulated that these calls function in intragroup spacing Keywords Alarm call · Predation · Semantic · or intergroup avoidance. More recent field experiments Non-human primate · Communication · Animal with Diana monkeys (Cercopithecus diana) of Ta Forest, cognition Ivory Coast, have clearly shown that loud male calls function as predator alarm calls because calls reliably (1) label different predator classes and (2) convey semantic Introduction information about the predator type present. Here, I test the alarm call hypothesis another primate, the Camp- Vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops) are known to bell’s monkey (C. campbelli). Like Diana monkeys, male produce acoustically distinct alarm calls in response to Campbell’s monkeys produce conspicuous loud calls to leopards, eagles or snakes (Struhsaker 1967). Field play- crowned hawk eagles (Stephanoaetus coronatus) and back experiments demonstrated that these alarm calls leopards (Panthera pardus), two of their main predators. were alone sufficient to elicit adaptive anti-predator re- Playback experiments showed that monkeys responded sponses from recipients in the same way as the corre- to the predator category represented by the different sponding predator normally would (Seyfarth et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Old World Monkeys in Mixed Species Exhibits
    Old World Monkeys in Mixed Species Exhibits (GaiaPark Kerkrade, 2010) Elwin Kraaij & Patricia ter Maat Old World Monkeys in Mixed Species Exhibits Factors influencing the success of old world monkeys in mixed species exhibits Authors: Elwin Kraaij & Patricia ter Maat Supervisors Van Hall Larenstein: T. Griede & M. Dobbelaar Client: T. ter Meulen, Apenheul Thesis number: 594000 Van Hall Larenstein Leeuwarden, August 2011 Preface This report was written in the scope of our final thesis as part of the study Animal Management. The research has its origin in a request from Tjerk ter Meulen (vice chair of the Old World Monkey TAG and studbook keeper of Allen’s swamp monkeys and black mangabeys at Apenheul Primate Park, the Netherlands). As studbook keeper of the black mangabey and based on his experiences from his previous position at Gaiapark Kerkrade, the Netherlands, where black mangabeys are successfully combined with gorillas, he requested our help in researching what factors contribute to the success of old world monkeys in mixed species exhibits. We could not have done this research without the knowledge and experience of the contributors and we would therefore like to thank them for their help. First of all Tjerk ter Meulen, the initiator of the research for providing information on the subject and giving feedback on our work. Secondly Tine Griede and Marcella Dobbelaar, being our two supervisors from the study Animal Management, for giving feedback and guidance throughout the project. Finally we would like to thank all zoos that filled in our questionnaire and provided us with the information required to perform this research.
    [Show full text]
  • Campbell's Monkeys Concatenate Vocalizations Into Context-Specific
    Campbell’s monkeys concatenate vocalizations into context-specific call sequences Karim Ouattaraa,b,c, Alban Lemassona,1, and Klaus Zuberbu¨ hlerd,1 aLaboratoire EthoS ‘‘Ethologie Animale et Humaine,’’ Unite´Mixte de Recherche 6552, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Universite´de Rennes 1, Station Biologique, 35380 Paimpont, France; bCentre Suisse de Recherches Scientifiques, Taï Monkey Project, 01 BP1303, Abidjan 01, Coˆte d’Ivoire; cLaboratoire de Zoologie et de Biologie Animale, Universite´de Cocody, 10 BP770, Abidjan 10, Coˆte d’Ivoire; and dSchool of Psychology, University of St. Andrews, KY16 9JP Saint Andrews, Scotland Edited by Charles G. Gross, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, and approved October 26, 2009 (received for review July 20, 2009) Primate vocal behavior is often considered irrelevant in modeling some cases, these structures possess hierarchical organization, human language evolution, mainly because of the caller’s limited although very little is known about the relationship between vocal control and apparent lack of intentional signaling. Here, we acoustic structure and communicative function. A typical finding present the results of a long-term study on Campbell’s monkeys, is that if the structure of a sequence is artificially altered, for which has revealed an unrivaled degree of vocal complexity. Adult example by changing the composition or order of elements, the males produced six different loud call types, which they combined signal tends to loose its communicative function (28–30). An- into various sequences in highly context-specific ways. We found other relevant point is that nonhuman primates are perfectly stereotyped sequences that were strongly associated with cohe- capable of discriminating human speech composed in different sion and travel, falling trees, neighboring groups, nonpredatory ways [e.g., tamarins (31)] and of comprehending simplified animals, unspecific predatory threat, and specific predator classes.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Classification of Nonhuman Primates
    BLBS036-Voevodin April 8, 2009 13:57 Part I: Introduction to Primatology and Virology COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL BLBS036-Voevodin April 8, 2009 13:57 BLBS036-Voevodin April 8, 2009 13:57 1 Classification of Nonhuman Primates 1.1 Introduction that the animals colloquially known as monkeys and 1.2 Classification and nomenclature of primates apes are primates. From the zoological standpoint, hu- 1.2.1 Higher primate taxa (suborder, infraorder, mans are also apes, although the use of this term is parvorder, superfamily) usually restricted to chimpanzees, gorillas, orangutans, 1.2.2 Molecular taxonomy and molecular and gibbons. identification of nonhuman primates 1.3 Old World monkeys 1.2. CLASSIFICATION AND NOMENCLATURE 1.3.1 Guenons and allies OF PRIMATES 1.3.1.1 African green monkeys The classification of primates, as with any zoological 1.3.1.2 Other guenons classification, is a hierarchical system of taxa (singu- 1.3.2 Baboons and allies lar form—taxon). The primate taxa are ranked in the 1.3.2.1 Baboons and geladas following descending order: 1.3.2.2 Mandrills and drills 1.3.2.3 Mangabeys Order 1.3.3 Macaques Suborder 1.3.4 Colobines Infraorder 1.4 Apes Parvorder 1.4.1 Lesser apes (gibbons and siamangs) Superfamily 1.4.2 Great apes (chimpanzees, gorillas, and Family orangutans) Subfamily 1.5 New World monkeys Tribe 1.5.1 Marmosets and tamarins Genus 1.5.2 Capuchins, owl, and squirrel monkeys Species 1.5.3 Howlers, muriquis, spider, and woolly Subspecies monkeys Species is the “elementary unit” of biodiversity.
    [Show full text]
  • (12) United States Patent (10) Patent No.: US 9.260,522 B2 Kufer Et Al
    US009260522B2 (12) United States Patent (10) Patent No.: US 9.260,522 B2 Kufer et al. (45) Date of Patent: Feb. 16, 2016 (54) BISPECIFIC SINGLE CHAIN ANTIBODIES WO WO 2008, 119565 A2 10/2008 WITH SPECIFICITY FOR HIGH WO WO 2008, 119566 A2 10/2008 MOLECULAR WEIGHT TARGET ANTIGENS WO WO 20089567 A2 102008 OTHER PUBLICATIONS (75) Inventors: Peter Kufer, Munich (DE); Claudia Blimel, Munich (DE); Roman Kischel, Sist etal (r. NA i. S. 2. Munich (DE) 139-159).*ariuZZa et al. eV. Ophy S. Ophy S. e. : (73) Assignee: AMGEN RESEARCH (MUNICH) syst al. (Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. May 1987; 84 (9): 2926 GMBH, Munich (DE) Chien et al. (Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. Jul. 1989: 86 (14): 5532 5536).* (*) Notice: Subject to any disclaimer, the term of this Caldas et al. (Mol. Immunol. May 2003; 39 (15): 941-952).* patent is extended or adjusted under 35 Wils a systs, lig,...si:18): U.S.C. 154(b) by 553 days. 5.adoSeal. Elia? J. VTOl. (J. Immunol.S1Ol. Jul. 2002;, 169 (6): 3076-3084).*: (21) Appl. No.: 13/122,271 WuCasset et al. et (J.t Mol.(Biochem. Biol. Nov.Biophys. 19, 1999;Res. &R294 (1): 151-162).*Jul. 2003; 307 (1): 198-205).* (22) PCT Filed: Oct. 1, 2009 MacCallum et al. (J. Mol. Biol. Oct. 11, 1996; 262 (5): 732-745).* Holmetal. (Mol. Immunol. Feb. 2007; 44 (6): 1075-1084).* (86) PCT NO.: PCT/EP2009/062794 ClinicalTrials.gov archive, "Phase II Study of the BiTE(R) Blinatumomab (MT103) in Patients With Minimal Residual Disease S371 (c)(1), of B-Precursor Acute ALL.” View of NCT00560794 on Aug.
    [Show full text]
  • Behavioral Patterns and Space Utilization in A
    BEHAVIORAL PATTERNS AND SPACE UTILIZATION IN A CAPTIVE GROUP OF DIANA MONKEYS, CERCOPITHECUS DIANA By MARSHA G. WILLIAMS Bachelor of Science University of Oklahoma Norman, Oklahoma 1994 Submitted to the Faculty of the Graduate College of the Oklahoma State Un i v e rsity in partial fulfillment of the requirements f o r the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE May, 1997 BEHAV I ORAL PATTERNS AND SPACE UTILIZATION IN A CAPTIVE GROUP OF DIANA MONKEYS, CERCOPITHECUS DIANA Thesis Approved: ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank my advisor, Dr. James H. Shaw, for his guidance and support throughout my graduate studies. In addition, I would like to thank Dr. Tammie L. Bettinger of the Tulsa Zoo for her help with all aspects of this thesis and my graduate research. Tammie was instrumental in creating and funding my thesis research. I would also like to acknowledge the other member of my thesis committee , Dr. Tracy Carter, for her assistance with various aspects of this thesis. I am grateful to Tulsa Zoo (Tulsa, OK, USA) and the zoo's current Director, Dave Zucconi, for allowing me access to their facilities any time I needed. In a ddition, I would like to thank Gary Dunn and the many keep ers a t Tulsa Zoo for keeping daily records of the animal collection which made a portion of this thesis possible . Tuls a Zoo Frie nds provided the funding for my project and to them I am d eeply grateful. I would like to thank my husband, Lance Williams, for being an active participant in my research and helping with all aspects of this thesis.
    [Show full text]
  • Diana Monkey Long-Distance Calls: Messages for Conspecifics and Predators
    Anim. Behav., 1997, 53, 589–604 Diana monkey long-distance calls: messages for conspecifics and predators KLAUS ZUBERBU} HLER*, RONALD NOˆ & ROBERT M. SEYFARTH* *Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania †Max Planck Institut für Verhaltensphysiologie (Received 17 November 1995; initial acceptance 25 February 1996; final acceptance 13 June 1996; MS. number: 7457) Abstract. Primate long-distance calls have typically been interpreted as communication signals between conspecific groups (the ‘resource defence hypothesis’), but their potential role as anti-predator alarm calls has received comparably little attention. Male diana monkeys, Cercopithecus diana diana,inthe Taï forest of Côte d’Ivoire often utter long-distance calls, either spontaneously or in reaction to a variety of stimuli, including predators and non-predators. The present study focuses only on predation contexts and provides evidence for communication to both predators and conspecifics. Males called only in response to predators whose hunting success depends on unprepared prey, that is, leopards and crowned hawk eagles, but not in response to pursuit hunters, such as chimpanzees and humans, which can pursue the caller in the canopy. Calling was regularly combined with approaching the predator. Both observations suggest that male long-distance calls are used to signal detection to the predator (‘perception advertisement hypothesis’). Analysis of male long-distance calls given to leopards and eagles showed that they differed according to a number of acoustic parameters. The two call variants were played to different diana monkey groups; conspecifics responded to them as though the original predator were present. We conclude that, in addition to their function in perception advertisement, diana monkey long-distance calls function as within-group semantic signals that denote different types of predators.
    [Show full text]
  • Roloway Guenon
    e osyst m & Ec E f c o o l g a r a n r p Bi et al., J Ecosys Ecograph 2013, 3:3 u h y o J Journal of Ecosystem & Ecography DOI: 10.4172/2157-7625.1000126 ISSN: 2157-7625 Research Article OpenOpen Access Access Roloway Guenon (Cercopithecus diana roloway) and White-Naped Mangabey (Cercocebus atys lunulatus) Prefer Mangrove Habitats in Tanoé Forest, South-Eastern Ivory Coast Sery Gonedelé Bi1,2*, J-C Koffi Bené2,4, E. A. Bitty2,3, B. K. Kassé3, A. N’Guessan2,3, A. D. Koffi2,3, Bertin Akpatou2,3 and Inza Koné2,3 1Department of Genetics, University Felix Houphouet Boigny, Abidjan, Ivory Coast, 22 BP 582 Abidjan 22 2Swiss Center for Scientific Research in Ivory Coast, 01 BP 1303 Abidjan 01 3Laboratory of Zoology and Animal Biology, Felix Houphouet Boigny, Abidjan, Ivory Coast University, 22 BP 582 Abidjan 22 4Cytology Laboratory and Animal Biology, University Jean Lorougnon Guede Daloa, BP 150 Daloa Abstract There are three primate taxa which have the same range in West Africa (eastern Ivory Coast and western Ghana): Miss Waldron’s red colobus Piliocolobus badius waldronae, the Roloway guenon Cercopithecus diana roloway and the white-naped mangabey Cercocebus atys lunulatus. They were previously listed as critically endangered; however, Cercocebus atys lunulatus was recently downgraded to endanger. A series of surveys have been conducted since the early 1990s in the tropical forests of Ghana and Ivory Coast to survey thesee taxa. In 2006, these studies led to the conclusion that the Tanoé forest, south eastern Ivory Coast should be considered a top priority site for primate conservation in West Africa.
    [Show full text]
  • Laboratory Primate Newsletter
    LABORATORY PRIMATE NEWSLETTER Vol. 49, No. 3 July 2010 JUDITH E. SCHRIER, EDITOR JAMES S. HARPER, GORDON J. HANKINSON AND LARRY HULSEBOS, ASSOCIATE EDITORS MORRIS L. POVAR AND JASON MACHAN, CONSULTING EDITORS ELVA MATHIESEN, ASSISTANT EDITOR ALLAN M. SCHRIER, FOUNDING EDITOR, 1962-1987 Published Quarterly by the Schrier Research Laboratory Psychology Department, Brown University Providence, Rhode Island ISSN 0023-6861 POLICY STATEMENT The Laboratory Primate Newsletter provides a central source of information about nonhuman primates and related matters to scientists who use these animals in their research and those whose work supports such research. The Newsletter (1) provides information on care and breeding of nonhuman primates for laboratory research, (2) disseminates general information and news about the world of primate research (such as announcements of meetings, research projects, sources of information, nomenclature changes), (3) helps meet the special research needs of individual investigators by publishing requests for research material or for information related to specific research problems, and (4) serves the cause of conservation of nonhuman primates by publishing information on that topic. As a rule, research articles or summaries accepted for the Newsletter have some practical implications or provide general information likely to be of interest to investigators in a variety of areas of primate research. However, special consideration will be given to articles containing data on primates not conveniently publishable elsewhere. General descriptions of current research projects on primates will also be welcome. The Newsletter appears quarterly and is intended primarily for persons doing research with nonhuman primates. Back issues may be purchased for $10.00 each. We are no longer printing paper issues, except those we will send to subscribers who have paid in advance.
    [Show full text]
  • Genetic Diversity of Lentiviruses in Non-Human Primates
    AIDSRev2001,' 3: 3-10 Genetic Diversity of Lentiviruses in Non-Human Primates M. Peetets, V. Courgnaud and B. Abela LBborBtoire Rlttrovirus, IRa Mort/p illar, FranC8 Abstract Simian immunodeficiency viruses (SIVs) can be found naturally in a large number of African primate species; already 31 species have been identified with serological evidence of SIV infection, and in 21 this was confirmed bypartial or full-length genome sequencing. So far, the primate lentiviruses, for which full­ length genome sequences are available, fall into six approximately equidistant majorlineages and are represented by, 1) the HIV-1/SIVcpz lineage, 2) the HIV­ 21SIVsm lineage, 3)the SIVagm lineage from African green monkeys, 4) the SIVsyk lineage from Sykes' monkeys, 5) the SIVlhoest lineage including viruses from mandrills, I'Hoest and sun-tailed monkeys and, 6) the SIVcol lineage from a colobus monkey. SIVs from otherAfrican primates have been partially characterised, butthe exact phylogenetic relationship between these SIVs and other nonhuman primate lentiviruses requires the analysis of the complete genome. Most of the SIV-positive primates are the natural hosts of these viruses, and do not seem to develop any clinical symptoms. Nevertheless, if cross-species transmission occurs, the virus may be pathogenic for the new host. The two major viraltypes infecting humans, HIV-1 and HIV-2, represent zoonotic transmissions from chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and sooty mangabeys (Cercocebus atys) respectively. Therefore, the identification and characterisation of new SIV strains are important to better understand the origins of HIV-1 and-2 and to assess the potential risk for additionallentiviruses into the human population.
    [Show full text]