1 Classification of Nonhuman Primates

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

1 Classification of Nonhuman Primates BLBS036-Voevodin April 8, 2009 13:57 Part I: Introduction to Primatology and Virology COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL BLBS036-Voevodin April 8, 2009 13:57 BLBS036-Voevodin April 8, 2009 13:57 1 Classification of Nonhuman Primates 1.1 Introduction that the animals colloquially known as monkeys and 1.2 Classification and nomenclature of primates apes are primates. From the zoological standpoint, hu- 1.2.1 Higher primate taxa (suborder, infraorder, mans are also apes, although the use of this term is parvorder, superfamily) usually restricted to chimpanzees, gorillas, orangutans, 1.2.2 Molecular taxonomy and molecular and gibbons. identification of nonhuman primates 1.3 Old World monkeys 1.2. CLASSIFICATION AND NOMENCLATURE 1.3.1 Guenons and allies OF PRIMATES 1.3.1.1 African green monkeys The classification of primates, as with any zoological 1.3.1.2 Other guenons classification, is a hierarchical system of taxa (singu- 1.3.2 Baboons and allies lar form—taxon). The primate taxa are ranked in the 1.3.2.1 Baboons and geladas following descending order: 1.3.2.2 Mandrills and drills 1.3.2.3 Mangabeys Order 1.3.3 Macaques Suborder 1.3.4 Colobines Infraorder 1.4 Apes Parvorder 1.4.1 Lesser apes (gibbons and siamangs) Superfamily 1.4.2 Great apes (chimpanzees, gorillas, and Family orangutans) Subfamily 1.5 New World monkeys Tribe 1.5.1 Marmosets and tamarins Genus 1.5.2 Capuchins, owl, and squirrel monkeys Species 1.5.3 Howlers, muriquis, spider, and woolly Subspecies monkeys Species is the “elementary unit” of biodiversity. 1.5.4 Titis, sakis, and uakaris Strictly speaking, the only natural grouping of animals 1.6 Concluding remarks is a population. The species is defined as a group of populations whose individual members interbreed and 1.1. INTRODUCTION produce fertile offspring in their natural habitat. In prac- The order Primata includes humans and animals that tice, the use of this definition may be problematic, for are our closest living “relatives” in the animal kingdom. example, for clearly distinct and fertile hybrids existing There are many definitions of a primate. None of them in the wild. As a result, there are multiple disagreements are completely satisfactory. As a result, there are dis- regarding species rank for the morphologically and/or agreements regarding inclusion of some animal taxa in behaviorally distinguishable nonhuman primate (NHP) the order Primata. At the same time, it is indisputable populations.3,4,10,12 3 BLBS036-Voevodin April 8, 2009 13:57 4 Part I / Introduction to Primatology and Virology The nomenclature of NHP species is binomial, that is, species level, we follow the classification described in the scientific names of the species consist of two Latin literature.4,12 words. The first capitalized word identifies the genus (plural—genera). The second, lowercase word identifies the species within the genus. The scientific names of 1.2.1. Higher Primate Taxa (Suborder, Infraorder, species are italicized. For example, Macaca mulatta is Parvorder, Superfamily) the species mulatta within the genus Macaca. The primates are divided into two suborders: Strep- The nomenclature of NHP subspecies is trinomial. sirrhini and Haplorrhini (Figure 1.1). Strepsirrhini The full scientific name of a subspecies name consists are divided into three infraorders: Lemuriformes of three words: the binomial species name and the third (lemurs), Chiromyiformes (aye-ayes), and Lorisiformes word identifying the subspecies. For example, the com- (loris). Haplorrhini are divided into two infraorders: mon chimpanzee species (Pan troglodytes) is usually di- Tarsiiformes (tarsiers) and Simiiformes (simians, i.e., vided into four subspecies: Pan troglodytes troglodytes, monkeys and apes). The problematic group is tarsiers, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii, Pan troglodytes verus, also called tree shrews. There is no agreement as and Pan troglodytes vellerosus. The names of the sub- to whether or not they belong to the primate order. species are usually written in abbreviated form, for ex- Placing tarsiers together with simians is also disputed. ample, P. t. troglodytes and P. t. schweinfurthii.The Traditionally, lemurs, aye-ayes, lories, and tarsiers (if notion of subspecies is very useful for understanding the latter are included in the primates) are considered as the natural history of simian viruses. prosimians. In addition to the scientific names, virtually all pri- The simian part of primate classification starts at the mate species and many subspecies have common or ver- parvorder level. The simians are divided into Platyrrhini nacular names. The common names are obviously dif- (literally “broad or flat-nosed”) and Catarrhini (liter- ferent in various languages; however, the English names ally “downward-nosed”). All Platyrrhini species live are used predominantly in the scientific literature.11 For in South America—hence their common name the example, the common name for Macaca mulatta is rhe- New World monkeys (NWMs). There are no Catar- sus monkey; its capitalized version, Rhesus Monkey, is rhine species in the New World, except African green also used. Some simian species have more than one com- monkeys (AGMs), which were introduced to several mon name; for example, Papio hamadryas is called the Caribbean islands (St. Kitts, Nevis, and Barbados) in his- Sacred Baboon or Hamadryas Baboon. Despite the in- torically recent times (seventeenth century). All African trinsic ambiguity of common names, they are useful, for and Asian simian species, except apes, are Old World example, for describing NHPs whose taxonomic status monkeys (OWMs). is undetermined or controversial. Common names are The division into monkeys and apes is formalized at also easier to pronounce and memorize than the Latin the superfamily level: the members of Cercopithecoidea binomial designations. superfamily are monkeys while the members of Homi- Ideally, the hierarchy of primate taxa should reflect noidea superfamily are apes (Figure 1.2). the evolutionary history. In such cases, classification All 152 currently recognized OWM species are in- would be invariant. However, the incompleteness of cur- cluded in one family—Cercopithecidae. This family rent knowledge allows multiple hierarchies of primate is divided into two subfamilies: Cercopithecinae and taxa—hence the existence of different classifications of Colobinae (Figure 1.2). There are 11 Cercopithecinae primates. Most of the inconsistencies between various genera and 10 Colobinae genera. Cercopithecinae gen- classifications are located at levels higher or lower than era with 42 chromosomes diploid karyotype (Papio, species. Taxa whose rank is higher than species are “ar- Theropithecus, Mandrillus, Cercocebus, Lophocebus, tificial” in the sense that their definitions are based on and Macaca) are combined in the tribe Papionini14,35 subjectively chosen criteria. (Figure 1.3). The tribe level is not universally used in In this book, we mainly follow Groves’ classification the classifications of primates. For instance, it is not of primates which is the most widely used.10 This clas- included in Groves’ classification. However, the term sification includes 375 simian species (Tables 1.1–1.4). Papionini is commonly used in the descriptions of Information on NHP subspecies is included only if it simian immunodeficiency virus (SIV) and other simian is relevant in a context of simian virology. At the sub- retrovirus hosts. BLBS036-Voevodin April 8, 2009 13:57 1 / Classification of Nonhuman Primates 5 Figure 1.1. Higher primate taxa (order, suborder, infraorder, and parvorder). Shaded boxes—taxa which include monkeys and apes. Figure 1.2. Catarrhini taxa (down to genus level). BLBS036-Voevodin April 8, 2009 13:57 6 Part I / Introduction to Primatology and Virology Ta b l e 1 . 1 . Old World Monkeys: Subfamily Cercopithecinae Genus/Species Common Name Geographical Distribution Chlorocebus African green monkey C. aethiops Grivet monkey or grivet Sudan, Eritrea, Ethiopia C. cynosuros Malbrouck monkey S DRC,* Angola, N Namibia, Zambia C. djamdjamensis Bale Mountains vervet Ethiopia C. pygerythrus Vervet monkey or vervet Ethiopia, Somalia, Kenya, Tanzania, Zambia, Zimbabwe, RSA† C. sabaeus Green monkey Senegal, Guinea-Bissau, Guinea, Sierra Leone, Liberia, Cote d’Ivoire, Ghana C. tantalus Tantalus monkey Ghana, Togo, Benin, Nigeria, Cameroon, CAR,‡ Kenya Cercopithecus C. albogularis Sykes’ monkey Ethiopia to RSA, S&E DRC, NW Angola C. ascanius Red-tailed monkey Uganda, DRC, Zambia, Angola, CAR, W Kenya C. campbelli Campbell’s monkey Senegal, Liberia, Cote d’Ivoire C. cephus Mustached guenon Gabon, R of Congo,§ S Cameroon, Equatorial Guinea, SW CAR, NW Angola C. denti Dent’s monkey DRC, Rwanda, W Uganda, CAR C. diana Diana monkey Sierra Leone, Liberia, Cote d’Ivoire C. doggetti Silver monkey DRC, S Burundi, NW Tanzania, Rwanda, S Uganda C. dryas Dryas monkey DRC C. erythrogaster White-throated guenon S Nigeria, Benin C. erythrotis Red-eared guenon S&E Nigeria, Cameroon, Bioko Isl (Equatorial Guinea) C. hamlyni Hamlyn’s monkey E DRC, Rwanda C. kandti Golden monkey DRC, Uganda, Rwanda C. l’hoesti L’Hoest’s monkey E DRC, W Uganda, Rwanda, Burundi C. lowei Lowe’s monkey Cote d’Ivoire, Ghana C. mitis Blue monkey DRC, Kenya, Rwanda, N Angola, NW Zambia C. mona Mona monkey Ghana, Togo, Benin, Nigeria, Cameroon C. neglectus De Brazza’s monkey SE Cameroon, R of Congo, DRC, Equatorial Guinea, Gabon, Uganda, N Angola, W Kenya, SW Ethiopia, S Sudan C. nictitans Greater spot-nosed monkey Liberia, Cote d’Ivoire, Nigeria, DRC, CAR, Equatorial Guinea, Cameroon C. petaurista Lesser spot-nosed monkey Gambia, Guinea, Guinea-Bissau, Sierra Leone, Liberia, Cote d’Ivoire, Ghana, Togo C. pogonias Crowned monkey SE Nigeria, Cameroon, Equatorial Guinea, N&W Gabon, W DRC C. preussi Preuss’s monkey Cameroon, Equatorial Guinea C. roloway Roloway monkey Cote d’Ivoire, Ghana C. sclateri Sclater’s guenon SE Nigeria C. solatus Sun-tailed monkey Gabon C. wolfi Wolf’s monkey DRC, NE Angola BLBS036-Voevodin April 8, 2009 13:57 1 / Classification of Nonhuman Primates 7 Ta b l e 1 . 1 . (Continued) Genus/Species Common Name Geographical Distribution Erythrocebus Patas E.
Recommended publications
  • Description of a New Species of Hoolock Gibbon (Primates: Hylobatidae) Based
    1 Title: Description of a new species of Hoolock gibbon (Primates: Hylobatidae) based 2 on integrative taxonomy 3 Peng-Fei Fan1,2#,*, Kai He3,4,#, *, Xing Chen3,#, Alejandra Ortiz5,6,7, Bin Zhang3, Chao 4 Zhao8, Yun-Qiao Li9, Hai-Bo Zhang10, Clare Kimock5,6, Wen-Zhi Wang3, Colin 5 Groves11, Samuel T. Turvey12, Christian Roos13, Kris M. Helgen4, Xue-Long Jiang3* 6 7 8 9 1. School of Life Sciences, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, 510275, P.R. China 10 2. Institute of Eastern-Himalaya Biodiversity Research, Dali University, Dali, 671003, 11 P.R. China 12 3. Kunming Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Kunming, 650223, 13 P.R. China 14 4. Department of Vertebrate Zoology, National Museum of Natural History, 15 Smithsonian Institution, Washington, D.C., 20013, USA 16 5. Center for the Study of Human Origins, Department of Anthropology, New York 17 University, New York, 10003, USA 18 6. New York Consortium in Evolutionary Primatology (NYCEP), New York, 10024, 19 USA 20 7. Institute of Human Origins, School of Human Evolution and Social Change, 21 Arizona State University, Tempe, 85281, USA. 22 8. Cloud Mountain Conservation, Dali, 671003, P.R. China 1 23 9. Kunming Zoo, Kunming, 650021, P. R. China 24 10. Beijing Zoo, Beijing, 100044, P.R. China 25 11. School of Archaeology & Anthropology, Australian National University, Acton, 26 ACT 2601, Australia 27 12. Institute of Zoology, Zoological Society of London, NW1 4RY, London, UK 28 13. Gene Bank of Primates and Primate Genetics Laboratory, German Primate Center, 29 Leibniz Institute for Primate Research, Kellnerweg 4, 37077 Göttingen, Germany 30 31 32 Short title: A new species of small ape 33 #: These authors contributed equally to this work.
    [Show full text]
  • Urban Ecology of the Vervet Monkey Chlorocebus Pygerythrus in Kwazulu-Natal, South Africa ______
    Urban Ecology of the Vervet Monkey Chlorocebus pygerythrus in KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa __________________________________ Lindsay L Patterson A thesis presented in fulfilment of the academic requirements for the degree of Doctorate of Philosophy in Ecological Sciences At the University of KwaZulu-Natal, Pietermaritzburg, South Africa August 2017 ABSTRACT The spread of development globally is extensively modifying habitats and often results in competition for space and resources between humans and wildlife. For the last few decades a central goal of urban ecology research has been to deepen our understanding of how wildlife communities respond to urbanisation. In the KwaZulu-Natal Province of South Africa, urban and rural transformation has reduced and fragmented natural foraging grounds for vervet monkeys Chlorocebus pygerythrus. However, no data on vervet urban landscape use exist. They are regarded as successful urban exploiters, yet little data have been obtained prior to support this. This research investigated aspects of the urban ecology of vervet monkeys in three municipalities of KwaZulu-Natal (KZN), as well as factors that may predict human-monkey conflict. Firstly, through conducting an urban wildlife survey, we were able to assess residents’ attitudes towards, observations of and conflict with vervet monkeys, investigating the potential drivers of intragroup variation in spatial ecology, and identifying predators of birds’ nests. We analysed 602 surveys submitted online and, using ordinal regression models, we ascertained that respondents’ attitudes towards vervets were most influenced by whether or not they had had aggressive interactions with them, by the belief that vervet monkeys pose a health risk and by the presence of bird nests, refuse bins and house raiding on their properties.
    [Show full text]
  • The Taxonomy of Primates in the Laboratory Context
    P0800261_01 7/14/05 8:00 AM Page 3 C HAPTER 1 The Taxonomy of Primates T HE T in the Laboratory Context AXONOMY OF P Colin Groves RIMATES School of Archaeology and Anthropology, Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia 3 What are species? D Taxonomy: EFINITION OF THE The biological Organizing nature species concept Taxonomy means classifying organisms. It is nowadays commonly used as a synonym for systematics, though Disagreement as to what precisely constitutes a species P strictly speaking systematics is a much broader sphere is to be expected, given that the concept serves so many RIMATE of interest – interrelationships, and biodiversity. At the functions (Vane-Wright, 1992). We may be interested basis of taxonomy lies that much-debated concept, the in classification as such, or in the evolutionary implica- species. tions of species; in the theory of species, or in simply M ODEL Because there is so much misunderstanding about how to recognize them; or in their reproductive, phys- what a species is, it is necessary to give some space to iological, or husbandry status. discussion of the concept. The importance of what we Most non-specialists probably have some vague mean by the word “species” goes way beyond taxonomy idea that species are defined by not interbreeding with as such: it affects such diverse fields as genetics, biogeog- each other; usually, that hybrids between different species raphy, population biology, ecology, ethology, and bio- are sterile, or that they are incapable of hybridizing at diversity; in an era in which threats to the natural all. Such an impression ultimately derives from the def- world and its biodiversity are accelerating, it affects inition by Mayr (1940), whereby species are “groups of conservation strategies (Rojas, 1992).
    [Show full text]
  • The Behavioral Ecology of the Tibetan Macaque
    Fascinating Life Sciences Jin-Hua Li · Lixing Sun Peter M. Kappeler Editors The Behavioral Ecology of the Tibetan Macaque Fascinating Life Sciences This interdisciplinary series brings together the most essential and captivating topics in the life sciences. They range from the plant sciences to zoology, from the microbiome to macrobiome, and from basic biology to biotechnology. The series not only highlights fascinating research; it also discusses major challenges associ- ated with the life sciences and related disciplines and outlines future research directions. Individual volumes provide in-depth information, are richly illustrated with photographs, illustrations, and maps, and feature suggestions for further reading or glossaries where appropriate. Interested researchers in all areas of the life sciences, as well as biology enthu- siasts, will find the series’ interdisciplinary focus and highly readable volumes especially appealing. More information about this series at http://www.springer.com/series/15408 Jin-Hua Li • Lixing Sun • Peter M. Kappeler Editors The Behavioral Ecology of the Tibetan Macaque Editors Jin-Hua Li Lixing Sun School of Resources Department of Biological Sciences, Primate and Environmental Engineering Behavior and Ecology Program Anhui University Central Washington University Hefei, Anhui, China Ellensburg, WA, USA International Collaborative Research Center for Huangshan Biodiversity and Tibetan Macaque Behavioral Ecology Anhui, China School of Life Sciences Hefei Normal University Hefei, Anhui, China Peter M. Kappeler Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Unit, German Primate Center Leibniz Institute for Primate Research Göttingen, Germany Department of Anthropology/Sociobiology University of Göttingen Göttingen, Germany ISSN 2509-6745 ISSN 2509-6753 (electronic) Fascinating Life Sciences ISBN 978-3-030-27919-6 ISBN 978-3-030-27920-2 (eBook) https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-27920-2 This book is an open access publication.
    [Show full text]
  • Primates of the Southern Mentawai Islands
    Primate Conservation 2018 (32): 193-203 The Status of Primates in the Southern Mentawai Islands, Indonesia Ahmad Yanuar1 and Jatna Supriatna2 1Department of Biology and Post-graduate Program in Biology Conservation, Tropical Biodiversity Conservation Center- Universitas Nasional, Jl. RM. Harsono, Jakarta, Indonesia 2Department of Biology, FMIPA and Research Center for Climate Change, University of Indonesia, Depok, Indonesia Abstract: Populations of the primates native to the Mentawai Islands—Kloss’ gibbon Hylobates klossii, the Mentawai langur Presbytis potenziani, the Mentawai pig-tailed macaque Macaca pagensis, and the snub-nosed pig-tailed monkey Simias con- color—persist in disturbed and undisturbed forests and forest patches in Sipora, North Pagai and South Pagai. We used the line-transect method to survey primates in Sipora and the Pagai Islands and estimate their population densities. We walked 157.5 km and 185.6 km of line transects on Sipora and on the Pagai Islands, respectively, and obtained 93 sightings on Sipora and 109 sightings on the Pagai Islands. On Sipora, we estimated population densities for H. klossii, P. potenziani, and S. concolor in an area of 9.5 km², and M. pagensis in an area of 12.6 km². On the Pagai Islands, we estimated the population densities of the four primates in an area of 11.1 km². Simias concolor was found to have the lowest group densities on Sipora, whilst P. potenziani had the highest group densities. On the Pagai Islands, H. klossii was the least abundant and M. pagensis had the highest group densities. Primate populations, notably of the snub-nosed pig-tailed monkey and Kloss’ gibbon, are reduced and threatened on the southern Mentawai Islands.
    [Show full text]
  • Multistep Routes of Capuchin Monkeys in a Laser Pointer Traveling
    American Journal of Primatology 76:828–841 (2014) RESEARCH ARTICLE Multi‐Step Routes of Capuchin Monkeys in a Laser Pointer Traveling Salesman Task ALLISON M. HOWARD* AND DOROTHY M. FRAGASZY Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, Georgia Prior studies have claimed that nonhuman primates plan their routes multiple steps in advance. However, a recent reexamination of multi‐step route planning in nonhuman primates indicated that there is no evidence for planning more than one step ahead. We tested multi‐step route planning in capuchin monkeys using a pointing device to “travel” to distal targets while stationary. This device enabled us to determine whether capuchins distinguish the spatial relationship between goals and themselves and spatial relationships between goals and the laser dot, allocentrically. In Experiment 1, two subjects were presented with identical food items in Near‐Far (one item nearer to subject) and Equidistant (both items equidistant from subject) conditions with a laser dot visible between the items. Subjects moved the laser dot to the items using a joystick. In the Near‐Far condition, one subject demonstrated a bias for items closest to self but the other subject chose efficiently. In the second experiment, subjects retrieved three food items in similar Near‐Far and Equidistant arrangements. Both subjects preferred food items nearest the laser dot and showed no evidence of multi‐step route planning. We conclude that these capuchins do not make choices on the basis of multi‐step look ahead strategies. Am. J. Primatol. 76:828–841, 2014. © 2014 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Key words: spatial cognition; traveling salesman problem; multi‐step route; capuchins INTRODUCTION destinations to be visited increases.
    [Show full text]
  • Consequences of Color Vision Variation on Performance and Fitness in Capuchin Monkeys
    University of Montana ScholarWorks at University of Montana Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers Graduate School 2014 Consequences of color vision variation on performance and fitness in capuchin monkeys Andrea Theresa Green Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd Let us know how access to this document benefits ou.y Recommended Citation Green, Andrea Theresa, "Consequences of color vision variation on performance and fitness in capuchin monkeys" (2014). Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers. 10766. https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd/10766 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at ScholarWorks at University of Montana. It has been accepted for inclusion in Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at University of Montana. For more information, please contact [email protected]. CONSEQUENCES OF COLOR VISION VARIATION ON PERFORMANCE AND FITNESS IN CAPUCHIN MONKEYS By ANDREA THERESA GREEN Masters of Arts, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, 2007 Bachelors of Science, Warren Wilson College, Asheville, NC, 1997 Dissertation Paper presented in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Organismal Biology and Ecology The University of Montana Missoula, MT May 2014 Approved by: Sandy Ross, Dean of The Graduate School Graduate School Charles H. Janson, Chair Division of Biological Sciences Erick Greene Division of Biological Sciences Doug J. Emlen Division of Biological Sciences Scott R. Miller Division of Biological Sciences Gerald H. Jacobs Psychological & Brain Sciences-UCSB UMI Number: 3628945 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted.
    [Show full text]
  • Fascinating Primates 3/4/13 8:09 AM Ancient Egyptians Used Traits of an Ibis Or a Hamadryas Used Traits Egyptians Ancient ) to Represent Their God Thoth
    © Copyright, Princeton University Press. No part of this book may be distributed, posted, or reproduced in any form by digital or mechanical means without prior written permission of the publisher. Fascinating Primates Fascinating The Beginning of an Adventure Ever since the time of the fi rst civilizations, nonhuman primates and people have oc- cupied overlapping habitats, and it is easy to imagine how important these fi rst contacts were for our ancestors’ philosophical refl ections. Long ago, adopting a quasi- scientifi c view, some people accordingly regarded pri- mates as transformed humans. Others, by contrast, respected them as distinct be- ings, seen either as bearers of sacred properties or, conversely, as diabolical creatures. A Rapid Tour around the World In Egypt under the pharaohs, science and religion were still incompletely separated. Priests saw the Papio hamadryas living around them as “brother baboons” guarding their temples. In fact, the Egyptian god Thoth was a complex deity combining qualities of monkeys and those of other wild animal species living in rice paddies next to temples, all able to sound the alarm if thieves were skulking nearby. At fi rst, baboons represented a local god in the Nile delta who guarded sacred sites. The associated cult then spread through middle Egypt. Even- tually, this god was assimilated by the Greeks into Hermes Trismegistus, the deity measuring and interpreting time, the messenger of the gods. One conse- quence of this deifi cation was that many animals were mummifi ed after death to honor them. Ancient Egyptians used traits of an ibis or a Hamadryas Baboon (Papio hamadryas) to represent their god Thoth.
    [Show full text]
  • The Threads of Evolutionary, Behavioural and Conservation Research
    Taxonomic Tapestries The Threads of Evolutionary, Behavioural and Conservation Research Taxonomic Tapestries The Threads of Evolutionary, Behavioural and Conservation Research Edited by Alison M Behie and Marc F Oxenham Chapters written in honour of Professor Colin P Groves Published by ANU Press The Australian National University Acton ACT 2601, Australia Email: [email protected] This title is also available online at http://press.anu.edu.au National Library of Australia Cataloguing-in-Publication entry Title: Taxonomic tapestries : the threads of evolutionary, behavioural and conservation research / Alison M Behie and Marc F Oxenham, editors. ISBN: 9781925022360 (paperback) 9781925022377 (ebook) Subjects: Biology--Classification. Biology--Philosophy. Human ecology--Research. Coexistence of species--Research. Evolution (Biology)--Research. Taxonomists. Other Creators/Contributors: Behie, Alison M., editor. Oxenham, Marc F., editor. Dewey Number: 578.012 All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying or otherwise, without the prior permission of the publisher. Cover design and layout by ANU Press Cover photograph courtesy of Hajarimanitra Rambeloarivony Printed by Griffin Press This edition © 2015 ANU Press Contents List of Contributors . .vii List of Figures and Tables . ix PART I 1. The Groves effect: 50 years of influence on behaviour, evolution and conservation research . 3 Alison M Behie and Marc F Oxenham PART II 2 . Characterisation of the endemic Sulawesi Lenomys meyeri (Muridae, Murinae) and the description of a new species of Lenomys . 13 Guy G Musser 3 . Gibbons and hominoid ancestry . 51 Peter Andrews and Richard J Johnson 4 .
    [Show full text]
  • Ranging Bonnet Macaques, Macaca Radiata
    Flexibility in Food Extraction Techniques in Urban Free- Ranging Bonnet Macaques, Macaca radiata Madhur Mangalam1, Mewa Singh1,2* 1 Biopsychology Laboratory, University of Mysore, Mysore, India, 2 Evolutionary & Organismal Biology Unit, Jawaharlal Nehru Centre for Advanced Scientific Research, Bangalore, India Abstract Non-human primate populations, other than responding appropriately to naturally occurring challenges, also need to cope with anthropogenic factors such as environmental pollution, resource depletion, and habitat destruction. Populations and individuals are likely to show considerable variations in food extraction abilities, with some populations and individuals more efficient than others at exploiting a set of resources. In this study, we examined among urban free-ranging bonnet macaques, Macaca radiata (a) local differences in food extraction abilities, (b) between-individual variation and within-individual consistency in problem-solving success and the underlying problem-solving characteristics, and (c) behavioral patterns associated with higher efficiency in food extraction. When presented with novel food extraction tasks, the urban macaques having more frequent exposure to novel physical objects in their surroundings, extracted food material from PET bottles and also solved another food extraction task (i.e., extracting an orange from a wire mesh box), more often than those living under more natural conditions. Adults solved the tasks more frequently than juveniles, and females more frequently than males. Both solution-technique and problem-solving characteristics varied across individuals but remained consistent within each individual across the successive presentations of PET bottles. The macaques that solved the tasks showed lesser within-individual variation in their food extraction behavior as compared to those that failed to solve the tasks.
    [Show full text]
  • Colobus Angolensis Palliatus) in an East African Coastal Forest
    African Primates 7 (2): 203-210 (2012) Activity Budgets of Peters’ Angola Black-and-White Colobus (Colobus angolensis palliatus) in an East African Coastal Forest Zeno Wijtten1, Emma Hankinson1, Timothy Pellissier2, Matthew Nuttall1 & Richard Lemarkat3 1Global Vision International (GVI) Kenya 2University of Winnipeg, Winnipeg, Canada 3Kenyan Wildlife Services (KWS), Kenya Abstract: Activity budgets of primates are commonly associated with strategies of energy conservation and are affected by a range of variables. In order to establish a solid basis for studies of colobine monkey food preference, food availability, group size, competition and movement, and also to aid conservation efforts, we studied activity of individuals from social groups of Colobus angolensis palliatus in a coastal forest patch in southeastern Kenya. Our observations (N = 461 hours) were conducted year-round, over a period of three years. In our Colobus angolensis palliatus study population, there was a relatively low mean group size of 5.6 ± SD 2.7. Resting, feeding, moving and socializing took up 64%, 22%, 3% and 4% of their time, respectively. In the dry season, as opposed to the wet season, the colobus increased the time they spent feeding, traveling, and being alert, and decreased the time they spent resting. General activity levels and group sizes are low compared to those for other populations of Colobus spp. We suggest that Peters’ Angola black-and-white colobus often live (including at our study site) under low preferred-food availability conditions and, as a result, are adapted to lower activity levels. With East African coastal forest declining rapidly, comparative studies focusing on C.
    [Show full text]
  • Information Sheet on Ramsar Wetlands (RIS) – 2009-2012 Version Available for Download From
    Information Sheet on Ramsar Wetlands (RIS) – 2009-2012 version Available for download from http://www.ramsar.org/ris/key_ris_index.htm. Categories approved by Recommendation 4.7 (1990), as amended by Resolution VIII.13 of the 8th Conference of the Contracting Parties (2002) and Resolutions IX.1 Annex B, IX.6, IX.21 and IX. 22 of the 9th Conference of the Contracting Parties (2005). Notes for compilers: 1. The RIS should be completed in accordance with the attached Explanatory Notes and Guidelines for completing the Information Sheet on Ramsar Wetlands. Compilers are strongly advised to read this guidance before filling in the RIS. 2. Further information and guidance in support of Ramsar site designations are provided in the Strategic Framework and guidelines for the future development of the List of Wetlands of International Importance (Ramsar Wise Use Handbook 14, 3rd edition). A 4th edition of the Handbook is in preparation and will be available in 2009. 3. Once completed, the RIS (and accompanying map(s)) should be submitted to the Ramsar Secretariat. Compilers should provide an electronic (MS Word) copy of the RIS and, where possible, digital copies of all maps. 1. Name and address of the compiler of this form: FOR OFFICE USE ONLY. DD MM YY Beatriz de Aquino Ribeiro - Bióloga - Analista Ambiental / [email protected], (95) Designation date Site Reference Number 99136-0940. Antonio Lisboa - Geógrafo - MSc. Biogeografia - Analista Ambiental / [email protected], (95) 99137-1192. Instituto Chico Mendes de Conservação da Biodiversidade - ICMBio Rua Alfredo Cruz, 283, Centro, Boa Vista -RR. CEP: 69.301-140 2.
    [Show full text]