Stages and Processes of Self- Change of Smoking - Toward an Integrative Model of Change

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Stages and Processes of Self- Change of Smoking - Toward an Integrative Model of Change See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/16334721 Stages and Processes of Self- Change of Smoking - Toward An Integrative Model of Change Article in Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology · July 1983 Impact Factor: 4.85 · DOI: 10.1037//0022-006X.51.3.390 · Source: PubMed CITATIONS READS 4,676 12,520 2 authors, including: Carlo Diclemente University of Maryland, Baltim… 212 PUBLICATIONS 27,919 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Available from: Carlo Diclemente Retrieved on: 30 May 2016 Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology Copyright 1983 by the 1983, Vol. 51, No. 3, 390-395 American Psychological Association, Inc. Stages and Processes of Self-Change of Smoking: Toward An Integrative Model of Change James Q Prochaska Carlo C. DiClemente University of Rhode Island Texas Research Institute of Mental Sciences An integrative model of change was applied to the study of 872 subjects changing their smoking habits on their own. The subjects represented the following five stages of change: precontemplation, contemplation, action, maintenance, and relapse. Ten processes of change were expected to receive differential emphases during particular stages of change. Results indicate that self-changers: (a) use the fewest processes of change during precontemplation; (b) emphasize consciousness raising during the contemplation stage; (c) emphasize self-reevaluation in both contemplation and action stages; (d) emphasize self-liberation, a helping rela- tionship, and reinforcement management during the action stage; and (e) use counterconditioning and stimulus control the most in both action and mainte- nance stages. Relapsers were found to respond like a combination of contempla- ters and people in action. Results are discussed in terms of developing a model of self-change of smoking and enhancing a more integrative general model of change. Formalized treatment programs for smok- including history of previous attempts to quit ing fail with a majority of smokers (Hunt, smoking. DiClemente and Prochaska (1982) Barnett, & Branch, 1971). Nevertheless, 30 found that self-changers did differ from ther- million Americans quit smoking in the past apy changers in terms of the processes of decade, with 70% to 80% quitting on their change that were emphasized in recent at- own (Adult Use of Tobacco, 1975). Further- tempts to quit smoking. More importantly, more, 70% of smokers surveyed indicated both self-changers and therapy changers re- that if they were to quit, they would not at- ported retrospectively that they had used af- tend a formal treatment program (McAlister, fective and cognitive processes more during 1975). In spite of the preponderance of and early stages of change and emphasized be- preference for self-change approaches, re- havioral processes during later stages. search on smoking cessation has focused pri- Perri, Richards, and Schulteis (1977) com- marily on formalized treatments. The pres- pleted retrospective interviews with 24 suc- ent study reports on the change processes that cessful and 24 unsuccessful college students were emphasized by 872 self-changers rep- who had made attempts to quit smoking on resenting five different stages of quitting their own. The successful self-changers re- smoking. ported using self-reinforcement procedures In one of the few studies on self-change, significantly more than the relapsers. Al- self-changers did not differ from individuals though encouraging, this study was limited in formalized treatments on smoking habits, by focusing on just two stages and four pro- locus of control, and measures of the Jackson cesses of change. Baer, Foreyt, and Wright Personality Inventory (Pederson & Lefcoe, (1977) analyzed letters describing the quitting 1976). DiClemente and Prochaska (1982) experiences of 51 self-changers who had also found that self-changers did not differ maintained nonsmoking for at least 2 years. from subjects in two types of therapy pro- While most of the self changers used multiple grams in terms of smoking history variables, techniques, the investigators were not able to discover any systematic clustering of their quitting methods. This work was supported by Grant CA 27821 from Research to date on self-change ap- the National Cancer Institute. Requests for reprints should be sent to James O. Pro- proaches to smoking cessation has been lim- chaska, Department of Psychology, University of Rhode ited by inadequate models of change and ret- Island, Kingston, Rhode Island 02881. rospective methodologies. The present re- 390 SELF-CHANGE OF SMOKING 391 search applied the transtheoretical model were assigned to one of the following five groups, de- that has been developed both from the ther- pending on the stage of change they currently were in: Long-term quitters (LTQs). These 247 subjects rep- apy literature (Prochaska, 1979; Prochaska resented the maintenance stage, since they had main- & DiClemente, 1982) and from data on self- tained their nonsmoking for at least 6 months. The mean changers (DiClemente and Prochaska, 1982; duration of maintenance was 5.9 years. The mean age Prochaska, DiClemente, Velicer, & Zwick, was 44 years, and there were 133 females and 114 males. They had begun smoking^at a mean age of 17.2 years. Note 1). The present research applied the Recent quitters (RQs). These 134 subjects represented model in a cross-sectional design to study the action stage, since they had quit smoking on their self-changers who were in one of the following own within 6 months of entering the study. The mean five stages of change: precontemplation, con- duration of time since they had quit was 2.2 months. templation, action, maintenance, and re- The mean age of these subjects was 35 years, and there were 80 females and 54 males. They had begun smoking lapse. at a mean age of 16.6 years. The transtheoretical model involves 10 Contemplators (Cs). These 187 subjects represented processes of change receiving differential ap- the contemplation stage, since they were smoking reg- plication during the five stages of change ularly for the past year but reported that they were se- riously thinking about quitting smoking in the next year. (Prochaska & DiClemente, 1982). The 10 The mean age of these subjects was 40 years, and there processes of change are as follows: conscious- were 113 females and 74 males, They had begun smoking ness raising, self-liberation, social liberation, at a mean age of 17.4 years. self-reevaluation, environmental reevalua- Immotives (I's). These 108 smokers represented the tion, counterconditioning, stimulus control, precontemplation stage, since they reported that they had no intention of quitting smoking in the next year. reinforcement management, dramatic relief, The mean age of this group was 38 years, and there were and helping relationships. 74 females and 34 males. Their mean age of beginning Based on the transtheoretical model (Pro- smoking was 16.3 years. chaska & DiClemente, 1982) and previous Relapsers (RLs). An exploratory group of 196 relap- sers was included to investigate how individuals u s e par- research (DiClemente & Prochaska, 1982), ticular change processes after having failed within the the following p r e d i c t i o n s were made. Because past year in their attempt to quit smoking. The mean precontemplators tend to be defensive and age of this group was 36 years, and there were 129 fe- avoid changing their thinking and behavior, males and 67 males. Their mean age of beginning smok- they would use the change processes signifi- ing was 17.3 years. Basic demographic data on the subjects indicated that cantly less than subjects in other stages. Be- they were middle-age and middle-class adults who began cause contemplators are seriously thinking smoking as teenagers (M = 17 years). The mean age was about changing their smoking behavior, they 40 and the median 37 years. Of the total sample 62% would use consciousness raising the most to were married, 27% single, 16.5% divorced, and 5.8% sep- arated or widowed. O f the total sample, 19.3% completed gather further information about their smok- high school or less, 41.7% had attended some college ing. Because self-reevaluation appears to be classes, 17.8% had bachelor degrees, and 19.3% had some a process that bridges contemplation and ac- postgraduate education or a graduate degree. Approxi- tion, self-reevaluation would be used most in mately one half of the subjects had incomes of less than the contemplation and action stages. Because $15,000, and 8% had incomes of more than $30,000. subjects in the action stage are most com- mitted to making behavioral changes, they Measures would use self-liberation, counter-condition- The processes of change test. This test is a 40-item ing, stimulus control, and reinforcement questionnaire that measures 10 processes of change in management the most. No clear predictions a statistically well-defined and highly reliable manner had emerged from previous research on (Prochaska et al., Note 1).' Table 1 presents a sample which processes would be emphasized during item and the alpha coefficient for each process. There the maintenance and relapse stages. are four items representing each of the 10 processes. Subjects were asked to rate on a 5-point Likert scale how frequently they employed each item in the past month Method (1 = not at all; 5 = repeatedly). Smoking-status measures. Saliva samples were taken Subjects from each subject to increase validity of self-reports via the bogus pipeline phenomenon (Jones & Sigall, 1971). There were 872 subjects from Rhode Island and Hous- ton, Texas who volunteered to participate in the study in response to newspaper articles and ads. All subjects ' Copies of the test are available from the authors. 392 JAMES O. PROCHASKA AND CARLO C. DiCLEMENTE Table 1 Sample Items and Alpha Coefficients for the 10 Processes of Change Processes Alpha Sample item Consciousness raising .88 I look for information related to smoking.
Recommended publications
  • A Comparison of Stimulus Control Procedures and Progressive Relaxation in the Treatment of Insomnia Mark L
    Eastern Illinois University The Keep Masters Theses Student Theses & Publications 1979 A Comparison of Stimulus Control Procedures and Progressive Relaxation in the Treatment of Insomnia Mark L. Balen Eastern Illinois University This research is a product of the graduate program in Psychology at Eastern Illinois University. Find out more about the program. Recommended Citation Balen, Mark L., "A Comparison of Stimulus Control Procedures and Progressive Relaxation in the Treatment of Insomnia" (1979). Masters Theses. 3170. https://thekeep.eiu.edu/theses/3170 This is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Theses & Publications at The Keep. It has been accepted for inclusion in Masters Theses by an authorized administrator of The Keep. For more information, please contact [email protected]. PAPJ<:R CERTIFICATE #2 TO: Graduate Degree Candidates who have written formal theses. SUBJECT: Permission to reproduce theses. The University Library is receiving a number of requests from other institutions asking permission to reproduce dissertations for inclusion in their library holdings. Although no copyright laws are involved, we feel that professional courtesy demands that permission be obtained from the author before we allow theses to be copied. Please sign one of the following statements: Bo(!th Library of Eastern Illinois University has my permission to lend my thesis to a reputable college or university for the purpose of copying it for inclusion in that institution's library or research holdings. Date Author I respectfully request Booth Library of Eastern Illinois University not allow my thesis be reproduced because Date Author pdm A Comparison of Stimu1us ControlProcedures and Progressive Relaxation in the Treatment of Insomnia (TITLE) BY Mark L.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Insomnia
    Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Insomnia Melanie K. Leggett, PhD, CBSM Duke University Medical Center Disclosures ▪ I have no relevant financial relationship with the manufacturers of any commercial products and/or providers of commercial services discussed in this CME activity. ▪ Neither I or any member of my immediate family has a financial relationship or interest with any proprietary entity producing health care goods or services related to the content of this CME activity. ▪ My content may include reference to commercial products; however, generic and alternative products will be discussed whenever possible. I do not intend to discuss any unapproved or investigative use of commercial products or devices. ▪ This presentation does not represent the views of the Department of Veterans Affairs or the United States Government. 2 Overview ▪ Development and model of insomnia ▪ CBTI components ▪ CBTI therapy ▪ Outcomes ▪ Dissemination 3 Objectives ▪ Participants will understand the conceptual model of insomnia. ▪ Participants will be able to define CBTI, describe the treatment components, and understand the therapeutic rationale. ▪ Participants will understand the indications and contraindications for CBTI and be able to describe treatment efficacy. 4 Development of Insomnia 5 Definition of Insomnia ▪ Insomnia characterized by: ▫ Trouble falling asleep ▫ Trouble staying asleep ▫ Early morning awakenings ▪ Distress/impairment in daytime functioning ▪ Adequate opportunity & circumstances for sleep ▪ At least 3x/week for 3 months ▪ Not better explained by another sleep disorder 6 Sleep Processes Homeostatic System Circadian System Sleep Arousal System 7 Spielman’s Conceptual Model of Insomnia 8 CBTI Components 9 What is CBTI? ▪ Multicomponent regimen 1. Sleep restriction 2. Stimulus control 3. Cognitive therapy 4. Sleep hygiene 5.
    [Show full text]
  • Creating Stimulus Control 1
    Running head: CREATING STIMULUS CONTROL 1 Creating Stimulus Control In a Classroom for Children with Autism Emily Levy California State University, Fresno Emily This is wonderful, best in the class, doesn’t need editing….very very good. CREATING STIMULUS CONTROL 2 CREATING STIMULUS CONTROL 3 Abstract Human behavior, throughout the day, is guided by certain stimuli that signal a person what response or action is required. The term “stimulus control” is used to describe the situation when a behavior occurs in the presence of some stimuli and not others (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2006). Stimulus control can be created through differential reinforcement. It is commonly used with children with autism during discrete trial lessons. Stimulus control can be used in a classroom environment to increase the on-task behavior of the students. Keywords: stimulus control, differential reinforcement, classroom, autism CREATING STIMULUS CONTROL 4 Creating Stimulus Control: In a Classroom for Children with Autism It is estimated that one in 110 children in the United States have autism spectrum disorder (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2011). Autism is a developmental disability that affects most children by the age of three years (American Psychiatric Association [DSM-IV-TR], 2000). There is no known cause for autism. The three main characteristics for a diagnosis of autism are deficits in language, deficits in social interactions, and restrictive, repetitive behavior. The field of Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) implements a behavioral model that is acknowledged as an empirically validated treatment for children with autism (Schreibman, 2000). This behavioral model composes about two-thirds of the established, effective treatments for children with autism as found in the National Standards Project by the National Autism Center (2009).
    [Show full text]
  • Behavior Modification: Principles and Procedures, 4Th
    Your students can perform “classic” experiments that closely resemble those discussed in Miltenberger’s text Sniffy™ the Virtual Rat Lite, Version 2.0, Second Edition By Tom Alloway, Greg Wilson, and Jeff Graham Order the text packaged with Sniffy Lite and save your students money! Use: 0-495-42745-4 Students realize all of the benefi ts of using a real rat with none of the drawbacks as they master the concepts of classical and operant conditioning by training Sniffy Bring the theories of learning to life using Sniffy the Virtual Rat. Instructors across the country have praised Sniffy, a realistic digital rat in a Skinner Box, for his ability to give students hands- on experience in setting up and conducting experiments that demonstrate the phenomena of classical and operant conditioning. Users begin by training Sniffy to press a bar to obtain food. Then, they progress to studies of more complex learning phenomena. Throughout each study, a series of “Mind Windows” enables students to visualize how Sniffy’s experiences in the chamber produce the psychological changes that they’re learning in your course. Available on a cross-platform CD-ROM, the Lite version of Sniffy includes 16 exercises that cover the essential phenomena of learning psychology. Students grow to love Sniffy as he helps them learn to: • Simulate classical conditioning phenomena including acquisition, extinction, and spontaneous recovery. • Simulate operant conditioning phenomena including magazine training; shaping; extinction; primary and secondary reinforcement; variable-interval, variable-ratio, fi xed-interval, and fi xed-ratio schedule effects; and the partial-reinforcement effect. • Measure responses employed by research psychologists in a data form that can be printed and turned in for class assignments.
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction to Behavior Therapy Terminology
    Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Master's Theses Graduate College 8-1973 Introduction to Behavior Therapy Terminology Alexandre Pacheo e Silva Nucci Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/masters_theses Part of the Psychoanalysis and Psychotherapy Commons Recommended Citation Nucci, Alexandre Pacheo e Silva, "Introduction to Behavior Therapy Terminology" (1973). Master's Theses. 2670. https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/masters_theses/2670 This Masters Thesis-Open Access is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate College at ScholarWorks at WMU. It has been accepted for inclusion in Master's Theses by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at WMU. For more information, please contact [email protected]. INTRODUCTION TO BEHAVIOR THERAPY TERMINOLOGY fcy Alexandre Pacheco e Silva Nucci A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of The Graduate College in partial fulfillment of the Degree of Master of Arts Western Michigan University Kalamazoo, Michigan August 1973 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. ACKNOWLEDGEMENT S First I wish to express my appreciation to Dr. Fred S. Keller who was the first person to consider my idea of writing this paper and who encouraged me in proceeding with it. Appreciation is also extended to Dr. Wade Hitzing, thesis advisor, and the other two members of this thesis committee, Dr. David Lyon and Dp, Paul Mount joy for their assistance to this project. Considering my staying in the United States of America as a foreign student, I want to express my full gratitude to Mr. and Mrs. B. E. Bensinger who accepted me in their home as a member of their own family.
    [Show full text]
  • Transfer of Stimulus Control Across Instrumental Responses Is Attenuated by Extinction in Human Instru- Mental Conditioning
    International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy 2005, Vol. 5, Nº 3, pp. 207-222 Transfer of Stimulus Control across Instrumental Responses is attenuated by Extinction in Human Instru- mental Conditioning A. Matías Gámez and Juan M. Rosas* Universidad de Jaén, España ABSTRACT D Transfer of behavioral control by a discriminative stimulus (S ) between different instru- mental responses trained with the same outcome has been consistently observed in nonhuman animals, regardless of whether the discriminative stimulus and the instrumental response have undergone extinction. Based on this result, it has been proposed that extinction of D nonhuman instrumental learning does not affect S -outcome associations. Two experiments explored whether this was the case in human instrumental conditioning. Both experiments used a video-game task where participants pressed different colored keys to defend Andalusia from the attack of ships and planes. Key-pressing was reinforced with the destruction of the ship or plane. In Experiment 1 removing the response-contingent outcome extinguished the instrumental response, while responding to a non-extinguished response remained high. D As in the animal literature, Experiment 2 found positive transfer of an S between different D responses that produce the same outcome, suggesting the formation of a S -outcome association in human instrumental conditioning. Contrary to what has been found with nonhuman D animals, extinction of the response in the presence of the S eliminated the transfer effect, D suggesting that the S -outcome association is deteriorated in human instrumental extinction. Key-words: Human instrumental conditioning; extinction, contents of learning, transfer test. RESUMEN La transferencia en el control de estímulos en la respuesta instrumental es atenuada por la extinción en el condicionamiento instrumental humano.
    [Show full text]
  • Paradoxical Intention Therapy Colin A
    Chapter 6 Paradoxical Intention Therapy Colin A. Espie University of Glasgow Sleep Centre, Sackler Institute of Psychobiological Research, University of Glasgow, Scotland, UK PROTOCOL NAME Paradoxical Intention (PI) therapy. GROSS INDICATION Paradoxical intention is thought to be ideal for insomnia disorder, particu- larly where there is intense preoccupation about sleep, sleep loss, and its con- sequences. The psychophysiological insomnia phenotype, as characterized in ICSD-2, couples sleep preoccupation with the notion of “striving” to sleep, and the maladaptive relationship between effort to sleep and ability to sleep [1]. SPECIFIC INDICATION There is no evidence to suggestion that Paradoxical Intention (PI) therapy is differentially effective in the sleep onset and sleep maintenance insomnia sub- types. The rationale for PI therapy, however, seems particularly appropriate for the psychophysiological insomnia phenotype (as stated above). There are two studies that suggest that reduction of sleep-related anxiety and performance effort may be the mechanism by which PI has its effect [2,3], but there are no formal clinical trials that demonstrate greater efficacy of PI in those insomnia patients who exhibit greater sleep effort. CONTRAINDICATIONS Two related notes of caution when considering PI therapy. First, PI is not suitable for patients who adopt a very concrete approach [4], or who are cognitively impaired. PI instructions (such as “remain awake for as long as possible”) are not literal commands to be implemented in an unthinking, mechanistic way. Rather, PI presents a cognitively challenging perspective on insomnia. It is important that patients understand that PI is a vehicle for a change in outlook as well as a change in behavior.
    [Show full text]
  • Hypnotic Relaxation and Insomnia: a Simple Solution? Harry E
    Hypnotic Relaxation and Insomnia: A Simple Solution? Harry E. Stanton, Ph.D. Objective: After a brief review of the use of behavioral strategies in the treatment of sleep onset insomnia, attention is centered upon one such strategy, hypnotic relaxation. Methods: A specific technique embracing visualization of a garden scene; letting go of problems; and a special place visualization, is described and its application to the problem of insomnia illustrated by means of three case studies. Results: Each of these studies deals with a different type of insomnia: 1) slow sleep onset, 2) waking during the night, and 3) difficulty in sleeping during the day. Conclusions: The success of the technique with all three cases suggests it might well be used more widely in the treatment of insomnia. (Sleep and Hypnosis 1999;1:64-67) Key words: hypnosis, relaxation, insomnia, therapy INTRODUCTION relaxation, stimulus control, paradoxical intention, and a placebo treatment on mild, moderate, and severe ue to uneasiness over the serious negative side effects insomniacs. Their findings demonstrated that behavioral D of sleep medications, considerable research effort has treatment approaches, particularly stimulus control, were been directed into the search for alternative, nondrug effective in reducing sleep onset insomnia, irrespective of interventions capable of alleviating insomnia. Behavioral severity level. With non-drug interventions proving to be strategies such as progressive relaxation, cognitive successful in treating insomnia, it is not suprising that the relaxation, biofeedback, stimulus control, and paradoxical effect of hypnotic relaxation methods has come under intention are one type of intervention that has produced investigation (9,10). In the latter study, progressive promising results (1-3).
    [Show full text]
  • Behavioral Humanism. TNSTTTUTION Stanford Univ., Calif
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 064 253 95 SP 005 797 AUTHOR Thoresen, Carl E. TITLE Behavioral Humanism. TNSTTTUTION stanford Univ., Calif. Stanford Center for Research and Development in Teaching. SPONS AGENCY Office of Education (DHEW), Washington, D.C. REPORT NO ROM-88 BUREAU NO BR-5-0252 PUB DATE Apr 72 CONTRACT OW-6-10-078 NOTE 35p. EDRS PRICE MF-$0.6F HC-$3.29 DESCRIPTORS *Behavioral Sciences; A4Behavior Theories; *Individual Development; *Psychology; *Self Control ABSTRACT Behavioral humanism is defined as the synthesis of behav4.oral techniques with humanistic goals. Contemporary humanism, especially humanistic psychology, offers directions for the kind of behavior that individuals should be able to engage in; contemporary behaviorism offers principles and procedures to help individuals increase their humanistic actions. The intensive experimental study of the individual (N-1)is discussed as an intimate research strategy appropriate to humanistic concerns. Freedom is viewed as the power to control the variables that infillence one's behavior. Behavioral self-control by means of self-observation, individual programming, and environmental planning is offered as a means of developing humanistic behavior. An initial translation of humanistic concerns into action (response) is nffered. The purpose of the research reported here is to create and empirically validate techniques to help individuals develop self-control skills. An extensive bibliography is i.ncluded. (Author) U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH, STANFORD CENTER EDUCATION & WELFARE FOR RESEARCH AND DEVELOPMENT OFFICE OF EDUCATION THIS DOCUMENT HAS BEEN REPRO- IN TEACHING DUCED EXACTLY AS RECEIVED FROM THE PERSON OR ORGANIZATION ORIG- INATING IT POINTS OF VIEW OR OPIN- IONS STATED DO NOT NECESSARILY REPRESENT OFFICIAL OFFICE OF EDU- CATION POSITION OR POLICY.
    [Show full text]
  • Relapse Prevention
    Relapse Prevention An Overview of Marlatt’s Cognitive-Behavioral Model Mary E. Larimer, Ph.D., Rebekka S. Palmer, and G. Alan Marlatt, Ph.D. Relapse prevention (RP) is an important component of alcoholism treatment. The RP model proposed by Marlatt and Gordon suggests that both immediate determinants (e.g., high-risk situations, coping skills, outcome expectancies, and the abstinence violation effect) and covert antecedents (e.g., lifestyle factors and urges and cravings) can contribute to relapse. The RP model also incorporates numerous specific and global intervention strategies that allow therapist and client to address each step of the relapse process. Specific interventions include identifying specific high-risk situations for each client and enhancing the client’s skills for coping with those situations, increasing the client’s self-efficacy, eliminating myths regarding alcohol’s effects, managing lapses, and restructuring the client’s perceptions of the relapse process. Global strategies comprise balancing the client’s lifestyle and helping him or her develop positive addictions, employing stimulus control techniques and urge- management techniques, and developing relapse road maps. Several studies have provided theoretical and practical support for the RP model. KEY WORDS:AODD (alcohol and other drug dependence) relapse; relapse prevention; treatment model; cognitive therapy; behavior therapy; risk factors; coping skills; self efficacy; expectancy; AOD (alcohol and other drug) abstinence; lifestyle; AOD craving; intervention; alcohol cue; reliability (research methods); validity (research methods); literature review elapse, or the return to heavy cess, a series of events that unfold over alcohol use following a period of time (Annis 1986; Litman et al. 1979; MARY E.
    [Show full text]
  • Acceptance and Mindfulness in Cognitive Behavior Therapy
    Acceptance and Mindfulness in Cognitive Behavior Therapy Understanding and Applying the New Therapies Edited by James D. Herbert Evan M. Forman John Wiley & Sons, Inc. JWBT357-FM_i-x.indd i 10/7/10 2:41:53 PM This book is printed on acid-free paper. ϱ Copyright © 2011 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc. All rights reserved. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Inc., Hoboken, New Jersey. Published simultaneously in Canada. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning, or otherwise, except as permitted under Section 107 or 108 of the 1976 United States Copyright Act, without either the prior written permission of the publisher, or authorization through payment of the appropriate per-copy fee to the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc., 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, (978) 750-8400, fax (978) 646-8600, or on the web at www. copyright.com. Requests to the publisher for permission should be addressed to the Permissions Department, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 111 River Street, Hoboken, NJ 07030, (201) 748-6011, fax (201) 748-6008. Limit of Liability/Disclaimer of Warranty: While the publisher and author have used their best efforts in preparing this book, they make no representations or warranties with respect to the accuracy or completeness of the contents of this book and specifically disclaim any implied warranties of merchantability or fitness for a particular purpose. No warranty may be created or extended by sales representatives or written sales materials. The advice and strategies contained herein may not be suitable for your situation.
    [Show full text]
  • An Exploratory Study of Seven Procedures to Encourage College Student Studying Behavior
    Loyola University Chicago Loyola eCommons Master's Theses Theses and Dissertations 1977 An Exploratory Study of Seven Procedures to Encourage College Student Studying Behavior Calvin Edward Hainzinger Loyola University Chicago Follow this and additional works at: https://ecommons.luc.edu/luc_theses Part of the Education Commons Recommended Citation Hainzinger, Calvin Edward, "An Exploratory Study of Seven Procedures to Encourage College Student Studying Behavior" (1977). Master's Theses. 2972. https://ecommons.luc.edu/luc_theses/2972 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Theses and Dissertations at Loyola eCommons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Master's Theses by an authorized administrator of Loyola eCommons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-Noncommercial-No Derivative Works 3.0 License. Copyright © 1977 Calvin Edward Hainzinger AN EXPLORATORY STUDY OF SEVEN PROCEDURES TO ENCOURAGE COLLEGE STUDENT STUDYING BEHAVIOR by Calvin Hainzinger A Thesis Submitted to the-Faculty of the Graduate School of Loyola University of Chicago in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for. the Degree of Master of Arts July 1977 .. VITA Calvin Edward Hainzinger was born in Chicago, Illinois on January 22, 1953. His elementary and secondary education was ob­ tained in the public schools of Niles, Illinois and Park Ridge, Illinois. He was granted a Bachelor of Arts Degree from the Uni­ versity of Illinois at Chicago Circle with a major in psychology in December, 1974. In February, 1975, he entered the Master of Arts program at Loyola University of Chicago where he pursued the study of educational psychology.
    [Show full text]