Publ 21947 Issue 5122 Page

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Publ 21947 Issue 5122 Page The Dative Dispositional Construction in Russian Dganit Jenia Kim Tal Siloni The article sheds new light on the so-called dative dispositional con- struction in Russian. We revise the characterization of the classes of verbs able to feed the construction, showing that its input is best de- fined in terms of the Theta System (Reinhart’s (2002, 2016) approach to thematic relations). To resolve controversies in the literature regard- ing the licensing conditions of the construction and its possible inter- pretations, we ran two surveys whose design and findings we report. We then discuss the properties of the construction comparatively, de- fine the operation deriving it, and provide evidence that the operation is lexical. This has consequences regarding the nature of the lexical component. Keywords: dative dispositional construction, argument structure, Ex- periencer, Theta System 1 Introduction Verbal alternations are pervasive across languages, and their study not only sheds light on ques- tions of argument structure, but also has consequences regarding the architecture of grammar and the division of labor between its components. This article concentrates on a verbal alternation common in Slavic languages. Specifically, it deals with the dative dispositional alternation in Russian, illustrated in (1b). (1a) involves a basic, unergative verb, while (1b) realizes the related verbal form in the dative dispositional construction (DDC).1 As is obvious and will be discussed in detail, the two verbal forms in (1) differ in both morphosyntactic structure and meaning.2 (1) a. Ja ploxo rabotaju. I.NOM badly work.PRES.1SG ‘I work badly.’ / ‘I’m working badly.’ For helpful comments, we would like to thank Irena Botwinik, Julia Horvath, Roni Katzir, Fred Landman, Aya Meltzer-Asscher, Eric Reuland, three anonymous LI reviewers, and the audience at the Interdisciplinary Colloquium of the Department of Linguistics at Tel Aviv University. We are grateful to Elena Shpigner, Slava Kim, and Senja Krol for their help with writing and distributing the questionnaires, and Lawrence Vriend, Julie Fadlon, and Simon Vriend for their help with the statistical analysis of the results. 1 The DDC is also known in the literature as the feel-like construction, the dative impersonal reflexive construction, the involuntary state construction, the dative existential disclosure, the desiderative inversion, and the dative habitual construction. 2 All examples provided in this article are in Russian unless otherwise specified. The abbreviations used throughout ס feminine, GEN ס distributive, FM ס dative, DIST ס auxiliary, DAT ס agreement, AUX ס accusative, AGR ס are ACC neuter, NOM ס negation, NEU ס masculine, NEG ס instrumental, MS ס infinitive, INST ס gerund, INF ס genitive, GER ס question, SG ס nonpast tense, QUEST ס plural, PRES ס perfective, PL ס past tense, PERF ס nominative, PAST ס singular. Linguistic Inquiry, Volume 51, Number 2, Spring 2020 237–279 ᭧ 2019 by the Massachusetts Institute of Technology https://doi.org/10.1162/ling_a_00341 237 Downloaded from http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/pdf/10.1162/ling_a_00341 by guest on 28 September 2021 238 DGANIT JENIA KIM AND TAL SILONI b. Mne ploxo rabotaetsja. I.DAT badly work.PRES.3SG.SJA ‘I don’t feel like working.’ / ‘I can’t work (due to my psychological circumstances).’ / ‘I evaluate my working negatively.’ The Russian DDC has received much attention in the literature (e.g., Benedicto 1995, Franks 1995, Maru'i? and äaucer 2006, Zeldowicz 2011, Rivero and Arregui 2012). Still, there is no consensus regarding its basic properties, that is, the environments in which the DDC is licensed and the interpretations it allows. Further, it is standardly assumed that pairs such as (1a–b) are derivationally related, but there is no agreement about what the relation is. For example, Franks (1995) suggests that the DDC is derived by a lexical operation on the base verbal entry’s ␪-grid. In contrast, Benedicto (1995), Maru'i? and äaucer (2006), and Rivero and Arregui (2012) propose that the derivation is syntactic, the VP projected by the base entry being embedded under a null (modal or psych) head. Up to now, nonsystematic searches in corpora and intuitions of authors and/or of a small number of speakers have been used in studies of the DDC, but these have not resulted in a clear description of the construction. Therefore, we carried out two surveys with a large number of native Russian speakers to resolve the basic controversies. Further, we explored and revised the definition of the set of verbs feeding the DDC. The new findings have led to a new analysis of the construction and its derivation, and have consequences regarding the nature of the lexical component. The article is organized as follows. In section 2, we present the basic morphosyntactic and semantic characteristics of the DDC, introduce the controversies regarding its distribution and interpretation, and briefly discuss the two surveys we carried out and their results. Section 3 deals with the set of verbs feeding the formation of the dative dispositional alternant. We show that the common view taking the input to be the set of unergative verbs is inaccurate, and we offer a revised characterization of the input. Section 4 focuses on the operation forming the DDC. We present representative syntactic analyses of the construction and show that in contrast to their claims, the input verb does not merge as is, nor does its argument. We then argue that our findings point in the direction of a lexical derivation. We define the operation and draw the theoretical consequences our analysis bears. Section 5 concludes. The online appendices discuss the two surveys in detail and give the full list of items they included (https://www.mitpressjournals.org /doi/suppl/10.1162/ling_a_00341). 2 The Dative Dispositional Construction 2.1 Morphosyntactic and Semantic Characteristics Compare (1a), repeated in (2a), with its corresponding DDC (1b), repeated in (2b). (2) a. Ja ploxo rabotaju. I.NOM badly work.PRES.1SG ‘I work badly.’ / ‘I’m working badly.’ Downloaded from http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/pdf/10.1162/ling_a_00341 by guest on 28 September 2021 THE DATIVE DISPOSITIONAL CONSTRUCTION IN RUSSIAN 239 b. Mne ploxo rabotaetsja. I.DAT badly work.PRES.3SG.SJA ‘I don’t feel like working.’ / ‘I can’t work (due to my psychological circumstances).’ / ‘I evaluate my working negatively.’ The members of the alternation in (2) differ morphologically. First, unlike the verb in (2a), the verb in the DDC (2b) is suffixed with -sja, the so-called reflexive morpheme (parallel to the clitic se found in other Slavic languages and Romance). The suffix is realized as -sja following a consonant, and as -s’ following a vowel. This morphological marking is found on different types of verbs in Russian, most notably on reflexives, reciprocals, unaccusatives, middles, and passives (for a survey, see Gerritsen 1990). Second, while verbs in Russian agree with their nominative subject in number and person in the present (“nonpast”) tense (2a) and in number and gender in the past, the verb in the DDC invariably shows the “default” or “impersonal” 3rd person singular inflection in the present (2b), and singular neuter in the past. In fact, there is no nominative argument in (2b). While the sole argument in (2a) is a nomina- tive subject, in the DDC (2b) the corresponding argument bears dative case. Schoorlemmer (1993) and Benedicto (1995) argue that the dative is the subject, as it can control into gerunds (3c) and antecede anaphors (4c), like nominative subjects ((3a) and (4a), respectively), and unlike dative indirect objects ((3b), (4b)). (3) a. Jak ?itaju PROk sidja u okna. I.NOM read.PRES.1SG sit.GER by window ‘I am reading while sitting by the window.’ b. Ma'ak mnej ?itaet PROk/*j sidja u okna. Masha.NOM I.DAT read.PRES.3SG sit.GER by window ‘Masha is reading to me while she is sitting by the window.’ c. Mnek ne ?itaetsja PROk sidja u okna. I.DAT NEG read.PRES.3SG.SJA sit.GER by window ‘I can’t read / I don’t feel like reading / I evaluate my reading negatively, when I’m sitting by the window.’ (4) a. Jak ?itaju v svoejk komnate. I.NOM read.PRES.1SG in own room ‘I read in my own room.’ / ‘I’m reading in my own room.’ b. Ma'ak mnej ?itaet v svoejk/*j komnate. Masha.NOM I.DAT read.PRES.3SG in own room ‘Masha is reading to me in her own room.’ c. Mnek ne ?itaetsja v svoejk komnate. I.DAT NEG read.PRES.3SG.SJA in own room ‘I can’t read / I don’t feel like reading / I evaluate my reading negatively, when I’m in my room.’ However, as observed by Greenberg and Franks (1991) and Komar (1999), nonsubjects can also control into gerunds and antecede anaphors in Russian. In (5a), the anaphor is bound by a genitive Downloaded from http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/pdf/10.1162/ling_a_00341 by guest on 28 September 2021 240 DGANIT JENIA KIM AND TAL SILONI noun phrase; in (5b), it is bound by an accusative one. In (6a), a genitive noun phrase controls into the gerund, and in (6b), an accusative one does so. (5) a. Skol’ko u neei bylo s soboji/*neji deneg? how.much at her.GEN was.AGR with self/*her money ‘How much money did she have with her(self)?’ (Komar 1999:254, (16)) b. On zastal menjai v svoeji komnate. he.NOM found me.ACC in own room ‘He found me in my room.’ (Greenberg and Franks 1991:77, (22)) (6) a. Proxodja mimo krasivogo cvetnika, na ix ustalyx licax pojavljaetsja crossing.GER by beautiful garden on their.GEN tired faces appears dobraja blagodarstvennaja ulybka.
Recommended publications
  • Studies on Language Change. Working Papers in Linguistics No. 34
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 286 382 FL 016 932 AUTHOR Joseph, Brian D., Ed. TITLE Studies on Language Change. Working Papers in Linguistics No. 34. INSTITUTION Ohio State Univ., Columbus. Dept. of Linguistics. PUB DATE Dec 86 NOTE 171p. PUB TYPE Reports - Evaluative/Feasibility (142) -- Collected Works - General (020) EDRS PRICE MF01/PC07 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS Arabic; Diachronic Linguistics; Dialects; *Diglossia; English; Estonian; *Etymology; Finnish; Foreign Countries; Language Variation; Linguistic Borrowing; *Linguistic Theory; *Morphemes; *Morphology (Languages); Old English; Sanskrit; Sociolinguistics; Syntax; *Uncommonly Taught Languages; Word Frequency IDENTIFIERS Saame ABSTRACT A collection of papers relevant to historical linguistics and description and explanation of language change includes: "Decliticization and Deaffixation in Saame: Abessive 'taga'" (Joel A. Nevis); "Decliticization in Old Estonian" (Joel A. Nevis); "On Automatic and Simultaneous Syntactic Changes" (Brian D. Joseph); "Loss of Nominal Case Endings in the Modern Arabic Sedentary Dialects" (Ann M. Miller); "One Rule or Many? Sanskrit Reduplication as Fragmented Affixation" (Richard D. Janda, Brian D. Joseph); "Fragmentation of Strong Verb Ablaut in Old English" (Keith Johnson); "The Etymology of 'bum': Mere Child's Play" (Mary E. Clark, Brian D. Joseph); "Small Group Lexical Innovation: Some Examples" (Christopher Kupec); "Word Frequency and Dialect Borrowing" (Debra A. Stollenwerk); "Introspection into a Stable Case of Variation in Finnish" (Riitta Valimaa-Blum);
    [Show full text]
  • 7. Pragmatics
    Oxford Handbook of Computational Linguistics, 2nd edn. Drafted Apr 20, 2011; revised Mar 4, 2014 7. Pragmatics Christopher Potts Department of Linguistics, Stanford University Abstract This chapter reviews core empirical phenomena and technical concepts from linguistic pragmatics, including context dependence, common ground, indexicality, conversational implicature, presupposition, and speech acts. I seek to identify unifying themes among these concepts, provide a high-level guide to the primary literature, and relate the phenomena to issues in computational linguistics. Keywords context dependence, common ground, indexicality, conversational implica- ture, presupposition, speech acts 7.1 Introduction The linguistic objects that speakers use in their utterances vastly underdetermine the contributions that those utterances make to discourse. To borrow an analogy from Levinson(2000: 4), “an utterance is not [... ] a veridical model or ‘snapshot’ of the scene it describes”. Rather, the encoded content merely sketches what speakers intend and hearers perceive. The fundamental questions of pragmatics concern how semantic content, contextual information, and general communicative pressures interact to guide discourse participants in filling in these sketches: (i) How do language users represent the context of utterance, and which aspects of the context are central to communication? (ii) We often “mean more than we say”. What principles guide this pragmatic enrichment, and how do semantic values (conventional aspects of meaning) enable and constrain
    [Show full text]
  • Teaching (Theoretical) Linguistics in the Second Language Classroom: Beyond Language Improvement
    PORTA LINGUARUM 22, junio 2014 161-171 Teaching (Theoretical) Linguistics in the Second Language Classroom: Beyond Language Improvement MAITE CORREA Colorado State University Received: 13 July 2012 / Accepted: 7 February 2013 ISSN: 1697-7467 ABSTRACT: Linguistics enjoys a paradoxical place in second language (SL) teaching. Un- like students majoring in Linguistics who see it as “study of the mind”, those who take it as part of a language degree see it as relevant as far as it supports language learning. However, a good command of the language is not sufficient for effective teaching. I address the need to revise the place of linguistics in SL curricula and present an innovative teaching approach that analyzes language in a scientific, systematic manner while focusing on those aspects of language that are of direct relevance to future language teachers. Keywords: Theoretical linguistics, pedagogy, teacher education, teacher learning, educa- tional linguistics. La enseñanza de lingüística (teórica) en la clase de ELE: más allá de la mejora comu- nicativa RESUMEN: La lingüística ocupa un lugar paradójico en los departamentos de didáctica de la lengua extranjera: aunque en los departamentos de Filología se ve como una “ventana a la mente”, en los de didáctica se ve como un apoyo al aprendizaje de la lengua por parte del futuro profesor. En este artículo reviso el lugar de la lingüística en el currículo de enseñan- za de segundas lenguas y presento un enfoque a la enseñanza de la lingüística que, siendo sistemático y científico, también se enfoca en aquellos aspectos relevantes para los futuros maestros de lengua extranjera.
    [Show full text]
  • Critical Analysis of Social Genetics and Linguistic Inquiry
    International Journal of Language and Linguistics 2020; 8(1): 11-16 http://www.sciencepublishinggroup.com/j/ijll doi: 10.11648/j.ijll.20200801.12 ISSN: 2330-0205 (Print); ISSN: 2330-0221 (Online) Critical Analysis of Social Genetics and Linguistic Inquiry Samuel Leykun Department of Linguistics, Ambo University, Ambo, Ethiopia Email address: To cite this article: Samuel Leykun. Critical Analysis of Social Genetics and Linguistic Inquiry. International Journal of Language and Linguistics . Vol. 8, No. 1, 2020, pp. 11-16. doi: 10.11648/j.ijll.20200801.12 Received : August 27, 2019; Accepted : October 24, 2019; Published : January 7, 2020 Abstract: Human being on Earth has undergone several transitions during its history, from the fire age to modern technology. There has been notable progress in linguistic development and social development. Sociolinguistics and Social genetics as a newlly emerging discipline have strong relationship and enaged with one another. This study tried to analyzed critically the social genetics and linguistic inquiry of human being regardless of anything. The objectives of the study were to introduce this complex idea to the biologists and linguists to reduce their racial baises and to make sense of social genetics and sociolinguistics are the major parameter of human race and genetics than hereditary genetics. The critical paradigm is used as paradigm of the study. Regarding techniques of data analysis, the qualitative approach is utilized in this study. Concerning data collection tools, the secondary data were collected from journals, findings of different researches, books, and proceedings. Therefore, this research is a desktop research. To mention some of the findings, the concept of hereditary genetics distinguish an individual or a group of individual from the other is a refutational way that triggers a misconception, and incorrect.
    [Show full text]
  • Grammatical Information in Dictionaries Eric Hoekstra1 Fryske Akademy, Leeuwarden, the Netherlands
    Grammatical information in dictionaries Eric Hoekstra1 Fryske Akademy, Leeuwarden, The Netherlands A dictionary is an encyclopaedia of linguistic information about words. It presents to a target group of laymen and professionals general information about words belonging to various disciplines of linguistics such as semantics (the meaning of words and phrases) phonology (the pronunciation of words) syntax (the syntactic category of words and the collocations in which they partake) My contribution discusses the question whether (new) insights from these disciplines may change the content of dictionaries, seeing that an evaluation of these insights does not take place very often. It is a shortcoming of dictionaries that a paraphrase of the meaning of function words is often not very insightful with respect to their use (Coffey 2006). What is the meaning of Dutch er ‘there’? What is the meaning of articles like the? What is the meaning of the complementiser that? Some Dutch dictionaries muddle the description of the various uses of er, ignoring the distinctions drawn by de Algemene Nederlandse Spraakkunst, the Standard Dutch Grammar (Haeseryn et al. 1997). Those distinctions are practical and well-motivated (Hoekstra 2000). It is proposed to use syntactic knowledge to structure articles about function words. In addition, dictionaries can covertly use example sentences to illustrate syntactic phenomena. Such measures strengthen the encyclopaedic character of a dictionary. 1. Dictionary information is encyclopaedic and multi-disciplinary in nature This contribution focuses on the type of grammatical information about words which is represented in dictionaries, and, more specifically, how function words are dealt with. In a way, a dictionary can be likened to an encyclopaedia.
    [Show full text]
  • SKY Journal of Linguistics Is Published by the Linguistic Association of Finland (One Issue Per Year)
    SKY Journal of Linguistics is published by the Linguistic Association of Finland (one issue per year). Notes for Contributors Policy: SKY Journal of Linguistics welcomes unpublished original works from authors of all nationalities and theoretical persuasions. Every manuscript is reviewed by at least two anonymous referees. In addition to full-length articles, the journal also accepts short (3-5 pages) ‘squibs’ as well as book reviews. Language of Publication: Contributions should be written in either English, French, or German. If the article is not written in the native language of the author, the language should be checked by a qualified native speaker. Style Sheet: A detailed style sheet is available from the editors, as well as via WWW at http://www.ling.helsinki.fi/sky/skystyle.shtml. Abstracts: Abstracts of the published papers are included in Linguistics Abstracts and Cambridge Scientific Abstracts. SKY JoL is also indexed in the MLA Bibliography. Editors’ Addresses (2005): Pentti Haddington, Department Finnish, Information Studies and Logopedics, P.O. Box 1000, FIN-90014 University of Oulu, Finland (e-mail [email protected]) Jouni Rostila, German Language and Culture Studies, FIN-33014 University of Tampere, Finland (e-mail [email protected]) Ulla Tuomarla, Department of Romance Languages, P.O. Box 24 (Unioninkatu 40B), FIN-00014 University of Helsinki, Finland (e-mail [email protected]) Publisher: The Linguistic Association of Finland c/o Department of Linguistics P.O. Box 4 FIN-00014 University of Helsinki Finland http://www.ling.helsinki.fi/sky The Linguistic Association of Finland was founded in 1977 to promote linguistic research in Finland by offering a forum for the discussion and dissemination of research in linguistics, both in Finland and abroad.
    [Show full text]
  • Foundations-Of-Pragmatics.Pdf
    Foundations of Pragmatics HoPs 1 Handbooks of Pragmatics Editors Wolfram Bublitz Andreas H. Jucker Klaus P. Schneider Volume 1 De Gruyter Mouton Foundations of Pragmatics Edited by Wolfram Bublitz Neal R. Norrick De Gruyter Mouton ISBN 978-3-11-021425-3 e-ISBN 978-3-11-021426-0 Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Foundations of pragmatics / edited by Wolfram Bublitz, Neal R. Norrick. p. cm. Ϫ (Handbook of pragmatics; 1) Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 978-3-11-021425-3 (alk. paper) 1. Pragmatics. I. Bublitz, Wolfram. II. Norrick, Neal R. P99.4.P72F68 2011 4011.45Ϫdc22 2011013980 Bibliographic information published by the Deutsche Nationalbibliothek The Deutsche Nationalbibliothek lists this publication in the Deutsche Nationalbibliografie; detailed bibliographic data are available in the Internet at http://dnb.d-nb.de. ” 2011 Walter de Gruyter GmbH & Co. KG, Berlin/Boston Cover image: Vetta Collection/iStockphoto Typesetting: Dörlemann Satz GmbH & Co. KG, Lemförde Printing: Hubert & Co. GmbH & Co. KG, Göttingen ϱ Printed on acid-free paper Printed in Germany www.degruyter.com Preface to the handbook series Wolfram Bublitz, Andreas H. Jucker and Klaus P. Schneider The series Handbooks of Pragmatics, which comprises nine self-contained vol- umes, provides a comprehensive overview of the entire field of pragmatics. It is meant to reflect the substantial and wide-ranging significance of pragmatics as a genuinely multi- and transdisciplinary field for nearly all areas of language de- scription, and also to account for its remarkable and continuously rising popularity in linguistics and adjoining disciplines. All nine handbooks share the same wide understanding of pragmatics as the scientific study of all aspects of linguistic behaviour.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonological Weight
    Phonological weight The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters Citation Ryan, Kevin M. 2016. “Phonological Weight.” Language and Linguistics Compass 10 (12) (December): 720–733. doi:10.1111/ lnc3.12229. Published Version 10.1111/lnc3.12229 Citable link http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:32086283 Terms of Use This article was downloaded from Harvard University’s DASH repository, and is made available under the terms and conditions applicable to Open Access Policy Articles, as set forth at http:// nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:dash.current.terms-of- use#OAP Kevin M. Ryan Preprint (published 2016, Language and Linguistics Compass 10:720{33) Phonological weight Abstract Grammars frequently categorize syllables for prosodic purposes, treating one class as heavier (e.g. more stress-attracting) than another. While such categorization is usually dichotomous, complex and gradient scales are also attested, with various or- ganizational criteria. This article reviews the range of phenomena that invoke weight distinctions and introduces some current debates concerning weight, touching on topics such as the syllable versus interval as the domain of weight, rich scalarity, process and position specificities, the role of onsets, the phonetic basis of categorization, and the mora. In order to capture generalizations about the phonological forms of words and phrases, it is often necessary to divide syllables into classes, such that one class patterns as prosodically \heavier" than another. For example, in many languages, the location of stress in words is determined by weight, such that stress skips over one or more light syllables in order to land on a heavy one.
    [Show full text]
  • Questioning Theoretical Primitives in Linguistic Inquiry
    studies in functional and structural linguistics 76 Questioning Theoretical Primitives in Linguistic Inquiry edited by Naomi L. Shin Daniel Erker John Benjamins Publishing Company Questioning Theoretical Primitives in Linguistic Inquiry Studies in Functional and Structural Linguistics (SFSL) issn 1385-7916 Taking the broadest and most general definitions of the terms functional and structural, this series aims to present linguistic and interdisciplinary research that relates language structure – at any level of analysis from phonology to discourse – to broader functional considerations, whether cognitive, communicative, pragmatic or sociocultural. Preference will be given to studies that focus on data from actual discourse, whether speech, writing or other nonvocal medium. The series was formerly known as Linguistic & Literary Studies in Eastern Europe (LLSEE). For an overview of all books published in this series, please see http://benjamins.com/catalog/sfsl Founding Editor Honorary Editors John Odmark Eva Hajičová Petr Sgall Charles University Charles University General Editors Yishai Tobin Bob de Jonge Ben-Gurion University of the Negev Groningen University Editorial Board Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald James A. Matisoff La Trobe University University of California, Berkeley Joan L. Bybee Jim Miller University of New Mexico Emeritus, University of Edinburgh Ellen Contini-Morava Marianne Mithun University of Virginia University of California, at Santa Barbara Nicholas Evans Lawrence J. Raphael University of Melbourne CUNY and Adelphi University Victor A. Friedman Olga Mišeska Tomić University of Chicago Leiden University Anatoly Liberman Olga T. Yokoyama University of Minnesota UCLA Volume 76 Questioning Theoretical Primitives in Linguistic Inquiry Papers in honor of Ricardo Otheguy Edited by Naomi L. Shin and Daniel Erker Questioning Theoretical Primitives in Linguistic Inquiry Papers in honor of Ricardo Otheguy Edited by Naomi L.
    [Show full text]
  • Linguistic Inquiry in the Classroom: a Missing Link?
    Linguistic Inquiry in the Classroom: A Missing Link? Kristin Bieber Domm Mount Saint Vincent University A thesis submitted to the Department of Education in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts (Education) May 2007 Copyright 2007 by Kristin Bieber Domm Abstract Linguistic inquiry is the exploration of language—what language is, how language works, and how language is used. Students themselves engage in linguistic inquiry by observing language use, collecting language data, and investigating what they find. This research creation asks the question: Is linguistic inquiry a miss- ing link between students, teachers, language, and learning? Can linguistic inquiry enable students to become more aware of their own language exper- tise, more respectful of language diversity, more engaged as second lan- guage learners, more effective as writers and readers, and more confident in using academic language? As a language arts and ESL teacher with twenty-five years’ experi- ence (P-12), I have explored linguistic inquiry and its possibilities in the classroom by creating a book about language for young people. This book is my writing to discover how to recognize, highlight, and investigate lan- guage itself—in order to understand how linguistic exploration might en- hance learning in school. 1 Part I of the thesis contains the thesis overview, a description of my linguistic location, and the literature review. Part II, Talking Up a Storm: Linguistics for Kids, is a nine-chapter nonfiction manuscript written for stu- dents in Grades 4 - 8. Chapters include topics such as language acquisition, endangered languages, writing systems, the history of English, code- switching, and the poetry of language.
    [Show full text]
  • TEACHING LINGUISTICS Linguistics in Middle School: Incorporating Linguistics Into Project-Based Learning Nicoleta Bateman
    TEACHING LINGUISTICS Linguistics in middle school: Incorporating linguistics into project-based learning Nicoleta Bateman California State University San Marcos This article argues that linguistics can be effectively incorporated into project-based learn - ing (PBL) as an innovative approach to introducing young students to linguistics. It draws from eighteen months of a collaboration between a linguist and a middle-school humanities teacher, and builds on prior efforts by linguists to incorporate linguistics into the K–12 curriculum. The article reviews relevant literature, introduces PBL and linguistics in PBL as a model, and presents de - tailed descriptions of how the study of language and the linguist’s ways of knowing were incorpo - rated into two student projects. Evidence of student learning and the impact of linguistics is also presented. The article concludes by suggesting that this approach to incorporating linguistics into K–12 education can address the goals both of linguists—changing attitudes toward and under - standing of language—and of educators—improving academic performance and addressing con - tent standards.* Keywords : linguistics, K–12 education, project-based learning, middle-school linguistics 1. Introduction. Linguistics should play a significant role in primary and second - ary education, particularly as the widely adopted Common Core State Standards (CCSS) have an entire strand titled ‘Language’ within the standards for English Lan - guage Arts and Literacy. This presents an opportunity for linguistically informed curric - ula to be widely used in classrooms to further the goals of educators, linguists, and administrators alike (Denham 2015). However, linguistics has traditionally been a higher-education discipline. Nonlinguists do not have a clear understanding of what lin - guistics is, its potential applications, or how to utilize it to meet the standards (much less how to use it as a teaching tool for new learners).
    [Show full text]
  • A Probabilistic Generative Model of Linguistic Typology
    A Probabilistic Generative Model of Linguistic Typology Johannes Bjerva@ Yova Kementchedjhieva@ Ryan CotterellP,H Isabelle Augenstein@ @Department of Computer Science, University of Copenhagen PDepartment of Computer Science, Johns Hopkins University HDepartment of Computer Science and Technology, University of Cambridge bjerva,yova,[email protected], [email protected] Abstract In the principles-and-parameters framework, the structural features of languages depend on parameters that may be toggled on or off, with a single parameter often dictating the status of multiple features. The implied co- variance between features inspires our prob- abilisation of this line of linguistic inquiry— we develop a generative model of language based on exponential-family matrix factorisa- tion. By modelling all languages and fea- tures within the same architecture, we show how structural similarities between languages can be exploited to predict typological features Figure 1: Correlations between selected typological with near-perfect accuracy, outperforming sev- parameters. Feature values are classified according eral baselines on the task of predicting held- to head-directionality (head-initial +, head-final -, no out features. Furthermore, we show that lan- dominant order o). For instance, ++ under Affixation guage embeddings pre-trained on monolingual means strongly suffixing. text allow for generalisation to unobserved lan- guages. This finding has clear practical and also theoretical implications: the results con- firm what linguists have hypothesised, i.e. that to identify the parameters that serve as axes, along there are significant correlations between typo- which languages may vary. logical features and languages. It is not enough, however, to simply write down the set of parameters available to language.
    [Show full text]